Abstract
Background
Despite its high prevalence and associated disability, the neural correlates of emotion processing in patients with functional (psychogenic) tremor (FT), the most common functional movement disorder, remain poorly understood.
Methods
In this cross sectional functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study at 4T, 27 subjects with FT, 16 with essential tremor (ET), and 25 healthy controls (HCs) underwent a finger-tapping motor task, a basic-emotion task, and an intense-emotion task to probe motor and emotion circuitries. Anatomical and functional MRI data were processed with FSL (FMRIB Software Library) and AFNI (Analysis of Functional Neuroimages), followed by seed-to-seed connectivity analyses using anatomical regions defined from the Harvard-Oxford subcortical atlas; all analyses were corrected for multiple comparisons.
Results
After controlling for depression scores and correcting for multiple comparisons, the FT group showed increased activation in the right cerebellum compared to ET during the motor task; and increased activation in the paracingulate gyrus and left Heschl's gyrus compared with HC with decreased activation in the right precentral gyrus compared with ET during the basic-emotion task. No significant differences were found after adjusting for multiple comparisons during the intense-emotion task but increase in connectivity between the left amygdala and left middle frontal gyrus survived corrections in the FT subjects during this task, compared to HC.
Conclusions
In response to emotional stimuli, functional tremor is associated with alterations in activation and functional connectivity in networks involved in emotion processing and theory of mind. These findings may be relevant to the pathophysiology of functional movement disorders.
Abbreviations: AFNI, Analysis of Functional Neuroimages; CPT-END, continuous performance task with emotional and neutral distracters; FSL, FMRIB Software Library; FT, functional tremor; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; HAM-A, Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; HAM-D, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; EPI, echo-planar imaging; MDEFT, modified equilibrium Fourier transform; MINI, Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview
Keywords: Functional tremor, Psychogenic tremor, Functional movement disorders, Conversion disorder, fMRI, Emotion processing
Highlights
-
•
Patients with functional tremor exhibit altered emotion processing circuitry.
-
•
There is increased activation in the paracingulate gyrus after emotional stimuli.
-
•
Increased connectivity between the left amygdala and middle frontal gyrus
-
•
Functional disorders may be associated with disturbances in the theory of mind.
1. Introduction
Functional (psychogenic) tremor (FT), the most common functional movement disorder, is diagnosed by confirming entrainment or full suppressibility of the oscillatory activity, distractibility, co-activation or co-contraction sign, pause of tremor during contralateral ballistic movements, and variability in tremor frequency, axis, and/or topographical distribution (Espay and Lang, 2015). Despite its frequency and the magnitude of disability it imparts, the pathophysiological underpinnings of FT remain poorly understood and no effective treatments have been established.
Neuroimaging studies have suggested that the basal ganglia and limbic systems are integral parts of the neural pathways for processing emotions (Nowak and Fink, 2009). Recent functional neuroimaging studies of patients with functional movement disorders have demonstrated alterations in regional cerebral blood flow during simple motor tasks (Schrag et al., 2013) or in brain activation of the cerebellar vermis, posterior cingulate cortex, and hippocampus on isometric precision-grip contraction tasks (Blakemore et al., 2016) as well as in brain activation of the right amygdala on simple emotional stimuli (n = 10) (Voon et al., 2010) or both amygdala on stimulation on fearful emotional stimuli (n = 12) (Aybek et al., 2015). We hypothesized that patients with FT have an impairment and/or disconnection of cortical and subcortical areas involved in motor and emotion control that may be distinguished from those of essential tremor (ET) and healthy controls (HCs). This hypothesis is also complemented by preliminary findings of differences in emotion processing in other neurological disorders (Allendorfer and Szaflarski, 2014, Szaflarski et al., 2014). ET is the most common tremor disorder, diagnosed in the presence of postural and action hand tremor, often in the context of a positive family history. While it follows none of the diagnostic criteria for FT, ET is considered to represent cerebellar dysfunction although with poorly defined neurobiological boundaries (Espay et al., 2017). We chose basic and intense emotion processing fMRI tasks in order to access the emotional state of the observed individual and as a measure of social intelligence, a concept separate from general (or cognitive) intelligence (Bar-On et al., 2003, Bonora et al., 2011). Further, the brain regions responsible for facial emotion recognition and processing, which includes visual (spatial cognition) and executive (attentional control) networks may be involved directly or indirectly in the generation or maintenance of FT (Calarge et al., 2003).
2. Methods
2.1. Subjects
Twenty-seven consecutive consenting patients with FT met established clinical criteria (Espay and Lang, 2015, Fahn and Williams, 1988, Gupta and Lang, 2009). Tremor needed to be absent or minimal at rest in order to avoid interference with the scanning procedure. Patients were excluded if they had any comorbid neurological disorder or severe depression or anxiety as measured by a Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) > 24 and a Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAM-A) > 25. Subjects were also excluded if they were on benzodiazepines for any reason. We also prospectively recruited 16 consecutive patients with ET as active controls (given the common misdiagnosis of FT with ET and vice versa), and 25 HCs with no history of neurological or general medical conditions. This study was approved by the local IRB and all subjects provided informed consent.
To reach the subject goal of 27 FT patients completing all assessments and rendering high-quality dataset for analyses, we screened a total of 35 FT subjects. Eight screened subjects, 6 with FT and 3 with ET were not recruited due to the following reasons (one each for FT, unless otherwise specified): malingering (rather than conversion), not meeting criteria for FT, patient unwilling to provide consent, unacceptance of diagnosis, prior neurosurgical procedure (unable to undergo fMRI), excessive tremor during scanning, obesity beyond scanner's capacity (1 ET), and inability to get comfortable in scanner (2 ET, 2 FT). Data from 10 subjects was excluded from final analysis due to non-completion of the task or for data quality issues: basic-emotion processing task: 1 HC; intense-emotion processing task: 1 FT, 1 HC; VBM analysis: 4 HCs, 1 ET; volumetric analysis: 2 HCs.
2.2. Clinical measurements
All subjects underwent a 15-minute structured diagnostic interview (Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview; MINI) (Pinninti et al., 2003) developed to screen for axis I DSM-IV and ICD-10 psychiatric disorders (Sheehan et al., 1998). In addition, we administered the 17-item HAM-D (Williams, 1988) to assess depressed mood and vegetative and cognitive symptoms of depression; and the 14-item HAM-A (Maier et al., 1988), to evaluate for psychic and somatic anxiety. These scales were administered as part of a structured interview (Williams et al., 2008).
2.3. Functional MRI procedure
Anatomical and functional brain images were obtained using a 4T MRI/MRS system (Varian Inc.). The behavioral experiment was programmed in E-Prime, version 1 (www.pstnet.com). All participants wore MR-compatible VGA goggles and headphones (Resonance Technologies, Inc.). For each imaging session, once the participant was positioned in the scanner, a three-plane scout scan was performed to confirm isocenter positioning prior to each of the functional tasks. An echo-planar imaging (EPI) was performed while subjects carried out the behavioral paradigms using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo EPI pulse sequence: TR/TE 3000/29 ms, FOV 256 × 256 mm, matrix 64 × 64, slice thickness 4 mm, flip angle 75°. A multi-echo reference scan was performed to correct for geometric distortion and Nyquist ghost artifacts. After completion of all functional MRI (fMRI) tasks a T1-weighted three-dimensional anatomical high-resolution scan using modified equilibrium Fourier transform (MDEFT) sequence (TR/TE 13/6 ms, T(MD) 1.1 s, FOV 192 × 256 × 256 mm, matrix 192 × 256 × 265, slice thickness 1 mm, flip angle 20°) was acquired (Lee et al., 1995). The MRI system triggered the behavioral paradigms to ensure precise timing of the task with respect to image acquisition.
2.4. Imaging paradigms
Three paradigms were used during the functional scans to examine differences in motor and emotional processing between the three groups. The paradigms, a finger-tapping motor task, a basic-emotion task, and an intense-emotion task were presented to each participant in the same order.
Finger-tapping motor task was designed to assess and monitor the motor system while in the scanner. This paced task consisted of a 30-second block of right-only finger tapping, followed by a 30-second block of left-only tapping, followed by a 30-second block of rest, all repeated 4 times. Subjects were instructed to adhere to the provided rate with the visual prompt presented every second. The task required subjects to move a lever using their right or left index finger, according to whether the “R” or “L” was flashing. The total task duration was 6 min. Task adherence was monitored visually. The task was modeled such that the blocks of rest were treated as “baseline” in the analyses.
The “basic-emotion” face recognition task was designed to assess response to basic emotional stimuli. Over the span of 14 min, subjects were presented with 120 different faces, corresponding to unique (non-repeating) facial identities each depicting a particular emotion (sadness, happiness, or fear) or a neutral expression (Szaflarski et al., 2014). Processing of emotional expressions is thought to occur subliminally and automatically but is dependent on attention (Pessoa et al., 2002b, Rees et al., 1997). To monitor attention to the task, subjects were instructed to decide the gender of each face by pressing one of two buttons with the right thumb. Subjects were exposed to 30 prototypically happy, 30 sad, 30 fearful and 30 neutral expressions presented in random order selected from the NimStim set of facial expressions (Tottenham et al., 2009). Each stimulus was presented for 2 s with variable inter-stimulus interval of 3.9 ± 2.4 s; during the delay subjects viewed a fixation cross. Subjects were asked to press button “1” for males and button “2” for females while viewing each image. It is well recognized that activation by faces in some brain areas is strongly affected by attentional condition while in other brain areas it is not (e.g., amygdala response to fearful stimuli) (Bentley et al., 2003, Pessoa et al., 2002a). The event-related design with variable inter-stimulus delay was used to reduce habituation of the activation in regions such as the amygdala, since habituation may occur in block designs with highly repetitive and predictable stimulus presentation (Breiter et al., 1996).
The “intense-emotion” task (continuous performance task with emotional and neutral distracters; CPT-END) consisted of a series of offensive or disgusting images probing intense emotional circuitry (Yamasaki et al., 2002). This task utilized a visual oddball paradigm where 70% of the cues were squares, 10% were circles (targets), 10% were emotionally unpleasant pictures, and 10% were emotionally neutral pictures. Subjects held the same response box as for the basic-emotion task and were asked to press with the right thumb a “2” for circles and “1” for all other images. There were two runs of the task in the imaging session. There were 158 total cues with 3 s per cue and a constant display time of 2.75 s with a 0.25 s interval with fixation cross. Emotional and neutral pictures originated from the International Affective Picture System (University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida) and were selected based on criteria previously utilized (Strakowski et al., 2011).
2.5. Image processing and statistical analysis
Reconstruction of the raw data was performed with 3D Hamming filter using in-house software developed in IDL (www.ittvis.com) (Schmithorst et al., 2001). First-level fMRI data processing was carried out using FSL (FMRIB Software Library) (Jenkinson et al., 2012, Smith et al., 2004) and AFNI (Analysis of Functional Neuroimages) (Cox, 1996).
2.6. Anatomical data
Data were first reoriented to standard orientation using FSL's fslreorient2std. Next, the T1 data were bias corrected and brain extracted using FSL's FAST(Zhang et al., 2001) and BET respectively (Smith, 2002). The brain extracted image was then normalized to the 2 mm isotropic MNI ICBM 152 non-linear 6th Generation template (Grabner et al., 2006) using FSL's FLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002, Jenkinson and Smith, 2001). Subcortical segmentation was performed using FSL's FIRST (Patenaude et al., 2011) on the bias corrected image in native space.
2.7. Functional data
Typical pre-processing steps, such as reorientation, slice timing correction and brain extraction, were carried out using FSL's fslreorient2std, slicetimer and BET (Smith, 2002), respectively. Outlying functional volumes, based on motion and intensity, were detected using FSL's fsl_motion_outliers. Motion correction of the BOLD time-series was carried out using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002). The functional file was interpolated to 2 × 2 × 2 mm voxel size and aligned to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template (Grabner et al., 2006) by first co-registering it with the participant's T1 using FSL's FLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002, Jenkinson and Smith, 2001). The motion related artifacts were then regressed from the data by setting up a general linear model design using 24 motion parameters (6 motion parameters, the 6 motion parameters squared, a first order autoregressive model of the 6 motion parameters and a first order autoregressive model of the 6 motion parameters squared) plus an additional parameter for each detected outlier (Friston et al., 1996). A CompCor regression was also implemented using the eigenvectors from the first five out of ten principle components within a white matter and CSF mask respectively (Behzadi et al., 2007). The residuals from the GLM were high-pass filtered in accordance with the task timing, 0.008 Hz for finger tapping and 0.04 Hz for CPT and faces, and smoothed with a 6 mm FWHM filter using AFNI's 3dBandpass. All task-based group results were corrected for multiple comparisons using FSL's threshold free cluster enhancement (TFCE) (Smith and Nichols, 2009), a non-parametric permutation test, with 5000 iterations. Each of the functional group comparisons was modeled both, with and without the HAM-D score as a covariate of interest. While measures of both anxiety and depression were collected, they are highly correlated, so therefore we chose to use only HAM-D scores in our analysis.
2.8. Seed-to-seed connectivity analysis
Connectivity analysis was performed for the intense-emotion task using anatomical regions defined from the Harvard-Oxford subcortical atlas (Desikan et al., 2006, Frazier et al., 2005, Goldstein et al., 2007, Makris et al., 2006), the sub-thalamic nucleus atlas (Forstmann et al., 2012) and the mean cortical activation of all subjects from the emotional images greater then neutral images contrast (Z > 2.3, p < 0.05 corrected and regions > 20 voxels). The anatomical regions include the anterior cingulate gyrus, posterior cingulate gyrus and left and right regions of the amygdala, thalamus, caudate, putamen, and subthalamic nuclei. The regions from the group activation of the intense-emotion task include regions in the left cerebral cortex, right frontal pole, right postcentral gyrus, left precuneus cortex and right superior frontal gyrus along with two regions in the left lateral occipital cortex, two regions in the left cingulate gyrus, three regions in the right lateral occipital cortex, and regions in the left and right middle frontal gyrus (Fig. 1). The average time course in each seed region was extracted and correlated with the average time course of all other seeds. The correlation coefficients were then converted to Fisher Z scores and used in a GLM analysis to look at differences in functional connectivity between the three groups. For each group comparison, all p values from all seed-seed correlations were corrected for multiple comparisons using the False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).
Fig. 1.
Regions of interest used in connectivity analysis based on the activation patterns from the intense-emotion task and subcortical atlas. The anatomical regions of interest included the anterior cingulate gyrus, posterior cingulate gyrus and left and right regions of the uncus/amygdala, thalamus, caudate, putamen, and subthalamic nuclei. The regions from the group activation of the task included regions in the left cerebral cortex, right frontal pole, right postcentral gyrus, left precuneus cortex and right superior frontal gyrus along with two regions in the left lateral occipital cortex, two regions in the left cingulate gyrus, three regions in the right lateral occipital cortex, and regions in the left and right middle frontal gyrus.
2.9. Volumetric analysis
Subcortical volumetric analysis was performed using the subcortical segmentation of each participant's anatomical image (described in Section 2.6). The volume of each region was extracted from the segmentation and compared across the groups using a two-sample t-test and corrected for multiple comparisons using FDR correction. A comparison of cortical gray matter volume was implemented using FSL-VBM (Douaud et al., 2007, Good et al., 2001). The standard FSL VBM pipeline was used which included gray matter segmentation and non-linear registration (Andersson, 2007) in order to create a study-specific gray matter template that is aligned to the MNI 152 template. Each participants' gray matter segmentation was then non-linearly registered to the template and corrected for local expansion (or contraction) due to the non-linear component of the spatial transformation. The gray matter images were then smoothed with a 3 mm (sigma) isotropic Gaussian kernel. Differences in gray matter volume between the groups was computed using a voxel-wise GLM with non-parametric permutation testing with TFCE and 5000 iterations to correct for multiple comparisons.
3. Results
3.1. Demographics
Twenty-seven subjects with FT (age: 50.9 ± 12.0 years, 23 women, handedness: 1 left, 25 right, 1 ambidextrous) were matched to 25 HCs (48.6 ± 11.4 years, 21 women, handedness: 1 left, 23 right) but not, due to anticipated differences in disease demographics, to 16 subjects with ET (61.7 ± 9.3 years, 5 women, handedness: 1 left, 15 right, 3 unknown) (Table 1). Shorter disease duration in FT compared to ET subjects was expected (4.9 ± 6.0 years [range, 0.5 to 21] vs. 29.5 ± 16.3 years [range, 1.5 to 53]).
Table 1.
Clinical features of study subjects.
Subject/sex | Age (years) | Affected body part | Duration (years) | HAM-A | HAM-D | MINI | Medications | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Functional tremor | 1 F | 52 | BUE, BLE | 18 | 15 | 17 | MDD (P) | Duloxetine, alprazolam, primidone, amitriptyline, propranolol |
2 F | 50 | BUE | 2 | 17 | 17 | None | Milnacipran | |
3 F | 46 | LUE, head | 11 | 8 | 15 | NGSP, OCD (C) | Clonazepam | |
4 F | 56 | BUE | 3 | 27 | 17 | None | Duloxetine, modafinil, methylphenidate, imipramine, lorazepam, gabapentin | |
5 M | 40 | LUE > RUE | 20 | 6 | 19 | None | Carbamazepine, citalopram | |
6 F | 64 | RUE | 11 | 0 | 5 | MDD (C) | None | |
7 F | 49 | BUE, BLE | 21 | 15 | 19 | MDD, PD-Ag, GSP, OCD, PTSD, GAD (C) | Risperidone, clonazepam, propranolol | |
8 F | 60 | BUE | 3 | 3 | 2 | None | Lorazepam, valproate, sertraline | |
9 F | 34 | BUE, head | 3 | 2 | 16 | PD-Ag, GSP, OCD, PTSD, Hypomania (C) | Duloxetine | |
10 F | 55 | BUE | 4 | 20 | 11 | Agoraphobia (C) | Duloxetine, trazodone, alprazolam | |
11 F | 63 | Head, trunk, BLE | 3.5 | 1 | 3 | None | None | |
12 F | 45 | BUE | 1 | 3 | 5 | None | None | |
13 F | 58 | RUE, stuttering | 2 | 28 | 20 | MDD, PD, PTSD, MDSP (C) | Citalopram, quetiapine, clonazepam, lamotrigine, sertraline | |
14 F | 38 | RUE, RLE, eye | 4 | 7 | 10 | None | Citalopram, diazepam, propranolol, topiramate | |
15 F | 38 | BUE, head | 4 | 28 | 26 | MDD, PD-Ag, PTSD, GAD (C) | Cyclobenzaprine, duloxetine | |
16 F | 38 | BUE | 1.2 | 3 | 3 | None | Clonazepam | |
17 F | 43 | BLE, stuttering | 6 | 46 | 43 | MDD, GAD, PD, PTSD (C) | Paroxetine | |
18 M | 27 | RUE | 1 | 0 | 0 | None | Amitriptyline | |
19 M | 44 | BLE > BUE, head | 3.5 | 24 | 18 | PD, PTSD (P) | Gabapentin, fluoxetine, clonazepam, baclofen | |
20 F | 66 | LUE, BLE | 1 | 22 | 11 | None | None | |
21 M | 65 | Head, RUE, LUE | 1.5 | 5 | 7 | None | None | |
22 F | 49 | BUE | 1 | 27 | 29 | MDD, PD-Ag, PTSD (C) | Clonazepam, diazepam, venlafaxine | |
23 F | 52 | BUE, BLE | 0.5 | 23 | 32 | MDD, GSP (C) | Valproate, gabapentin, alprazolam, baclofen | |
24 F | 75 | RUE, RLE | 1 | 13 | 10 | None | Escitalopram | |
25 F | 58 | Head, BUE | 1.5 | 5 | 9 | None | Primidone, propanolol, trazodone | |
26 F | 63 | RUE, RLE | 2.5 | 14 | 15 | PD-Ag, OCD (C) | None | |
27 F | 53 | Face, head, BUE, BLE | 1 | 32 | 23 | MDD, PD, GSP (C) alcohol dependence (P) | Clonazepam, desvenlafaxine, lamotrigine, lithium carbonate | |
Essential tremor | 1 | 68 | BUE | 9 | 0 | 1 | None | Propranolol, hydrocodone, ropinirole |
2 | 46 | BUE | 30 | 0 | 0 | None | Propranolol, bupropion, duloxetine | |
3 | 56 | BUE | 40 | 0 | 0 | None | Atenolol | |
4 | 56 | RUE | 1.5 | 5 | 9 | None | Zolpidem, propranolol, amitriptyline | |
5 | 47 | BUE | 40 | 0 | 1 | None | Amitriptyline, primidone | |
6 | 64 | RUE > LUE | 53 | 0 | 0 | None | Primidone, gabapentin, propranolol, cyclobenzaprine | |
7 | 55 | BUE, neck | 35 | 23 | 20 | MDD, PD | Propranolol, quetiapine, lorazepam, lamotrigine, desvenlafaxine, flurazepam | |
8 | 64 | BUE | 20 | 0 | 0 | None | Unknown | |
9 | 61 | BUE | 49 | 7 | 3 | None | Citalopram, primidone | |
10 | 74 | BUE | 14 | 4 | 4 | None | Diazepam | |
11 | 75 | BUE, voice | 20 | 5 | 2 | None | Citalopram, clonazepam, primidone, topiramate | |
12 | 50 | LUE | 20 | 7 | 4 | GAD | Escitalopram, trazodone | |
13 | 61 | BUE | 21 | 0 | 0 | None | Propranolol | |
14 | 69 | BUE | 50 | 0 | 1 | None | Zolpidem | |
15 | 73 | BUE, head | 20 | 0 | 0 | None | Propranolol | |
16 | 68 | LUE, head | 45 | 0 | 0 | None | Unknown |
HAM-D: 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; HAM-A: 14-item Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; MINI: Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview to screen for axis I DSM-IV and ICD-10 psychiatric disorders; Ag: agoraphobia; OCD: obsessive-compulsive disorder; GAD: generalized anxiety disorder; GSP: generalized social phobia; MDD: major depression; MDSP: Mood Disorder with psychotic features; NGSP: non-generalized social phobia; PD: panic disorder; PTSD: post-traumatic stress disorder. RUE: right upper extremity; LUE: left upper extremity; RLE: right lower extremity; LLE: left lower extremity; RH: right hand; LH: left hand; RF: right foot; LF: left foot; B, bilateral; (C): current; (P): past.
3.2. Psychiatric features
Depression (HAM-D, 14.9 ± 10.1 vs. 2.8 ± 5.2 [HAM-D score 14–18 = moderate depression]) and anxiety scores (HAM-A, 14.6 ± 11.7 vs. 3.2 ± 5.9 [HAM-A score 14–17 = mild anxiety]) were significantly higher in the FT group compared to ET. Healthy controls had normal and low scores (HAM-D, 0.9 ± 1.6; HAM-A, 0.9 ± 2.2).
3.3. Motor processing: finger-tapping task
Two small regions in the left precentral gyrus were found to have reduced activation in FT patients when compared with HC in the right tap greater than left tap contrast (Z > 2.3, p < 0.05 corrected), but this difference did not pass correction for multiple comparison when controlling for HAM-D. When controlling for HAM-D, there remained a significant reduction in the right cerebellum lobule VI in ET compared to FT for the right tapping greater than rest contrast (Fig. 2). No differences were found in the left tapping versus rest contrast. Average head motion for each group, as measured using the relative RMS displacement at a 50 mm radius (Power et al., 2012) was 0.11 ± 0.06 mm HC, 0.17 ± 0.18 FT, and 0.11 ± 0.04 ET with no significant differences between them.
Fig. 2.
Differences in fMRI activation for the finger-tapping task. A region in the right cerebellum with increased activation in the FT group compared to ET during right tapping compared to rest. Images are shown at a threshold of Z > 2.3 and p < 0.05 corrected, after controlling for HAM-D.
3.4. Basic-emotion processing
In the sad faces compared to neutral faces, regional differences were found between the FT vs. ET groups and FT vs. HC groups when controlling for HAM-D. FT patients showed increased activation in the paracingulate gyrus and left Heschl's gyrus compared with HC and decreased activation in two regions in right precentral gyrus when compared with ET (Z > 2.3, p < 0.05 corrected) (Fig. 3). There were no significant differences between groups in the other contrasts. Average head motion for each group was 0.12 ± 0.04 mm HC, 0.16 ± 0.14 FT, and 0.09 ± 0.04 ET with no significant differences between them.
Fig. 3.
Differences in fMRI activation for the basic-emotion task (sad greater than neutral faces). Above, paracingulate gyrus and left Heschl's gyrus showed greater activation in the FT group than HC. Below, right precentral gyrus showed reduced activation in the FT compared to ET. Images are shown at a threshold of Z > 2.3 and p < 0.05 corrected, controlling for HAM-D.
3.5. Intense-emotion processing
No differences between the groups during the intense-emotion processing task passed multiple comparison correction.
3.6. Connectivity analysis
A complete list of differences in pairwise connectivity between groups corrected at p < 0.05, and only significant for the intense-emotion task, is included in Table 2. When controlling for HAM-D, only the increase in connectivity in the FT group relative to HC between the left amygdala and left middle frontal gyrus survives the FDR correction.
Table 2.
Summary of connectivity.
Contrast | ROI 1 | ROI 2 | t value | p value | FDR p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
FT > HC | L putamen | R thalamus | 3.566485696 | 0.000697516 | 0.034709915 |
L putamen | L precuneus cortex | 3.620004807 | 0.000588311 | 0.034709915 | |
L putamen | Post. cingulate G | 4.003263873 | 0.000167205 | 0.034709915 | |
R middle frontal G | R postcentral G | 3.544564582 | 0.000747598 | 0.034709915 | |
R occipital cortex | R occipital cortex 2 | 3.742310385 | 0.000396638 | 0.034709915 | |
R putamen | L precuneus cortex | 3.410815902 | 0.001135415 | 0.043372047 | |
L amygdala | L middle frontal G | 3.337452006 | 0.001422406 | 0.046228209 | |
Ant. cingulate G | L middle frontal G | 3.262964607 | 0.001782948 | 0.048288185 | |
Post. cingulate G | R putamen | 3.267808005 | 0.001757106 | 0.048288185 | |
HC > FT | L putamen | R putamen | 3.392599168 | 0.001201072 | 0.043372047 |
L putamen | R superior frontal G | 3.649168746 | 0.00053587 | 0.034709915 | |
L caudate | R superior frontal G | 3.661756816 | 0.00051464 | 0.034709915 | |
ET > FT | R thalamus | R superior frontal G | 4.940914954 | 6.04E-06 | 0.001964324 |
FT > HC (HAM-D regressed) | L amygdala | L middle frontal G | 4.03383398 | 0.000152953 | 0.049709585 |
HC: healthy controls; FT: finger tapping; ET: essential tremor; G; gyrus; ROI: region of interest; FDR: false discovery rate.
3.7. Volumetric analysis
Compared to HC, patients with FT showed a smaller volume in the left caudate (corrected p < 0.022). Patients with ET showed a larger volume of the right amygdala compared to patients with FT and HCs (corrected p < 0.001 and p < 0.032, respectively). The VBM analysis showed a small region of reduced gray matter in the right postcentral gyrus in FT patients when compared with controls. It also showed reduced gray matter in ET in the left and right cerebellum (VIII) when compared with FT and left cerebellum (VII) and left occipital pole when compared with HC.
4. Discussion
Our main findings are that, after controlling for depression scores and correction for multiple comparisons, FT is associated with increased activation in the right cerebellum compared to ET during the motor task; and increased activation in the paracingulate gyrus and left Heschl's gyrus compared with HC along with decreased activation in the right precentral gyrus when compared with ET during the basic-emotion task (sad faces greater then neutral faces). While there were no significant differences during the intense-emotion task FT subjects exhibited an increase in connectivity between the left amygdala and the left middle frontal gyrus during this task as compared to HC. Collectively, these findings suggest that patients with FT exhibit altered activation of key cognitive and limbic structures in a pattern that is distinct from ET and HC.
This is the first study probing emotional circuitry specifically in patients with FT. Voon and colleagues examined the brain activation patterns using fMRI and a simple facial emotion paradigm in 16 patients with a variety of functional disorders (10 had tremor) and found no group differences on primary analysis compared to HCs even after controlling for concurrent depression and anxiety (Voon et al., 2010). However, they identified greater right amygdala activity during happy stimuli on post-hoc analysis in the functional group. The study did not assess differences between simple and intense emotion processing paradigms, which limit comparisons with ours. More recently, Aybek and colleagues evaluated fMRI changes to stimuli of faces with fearful and sad emotional expressions in comparison to faces with neutral expressions in 12 patients with a variety of neuropsychiatric symptoms qualifying as conversion disorder and found increased amygdala activation to negative emotions compared to healthy controls (Aybek et al., 2015). Using a larger sample and more homogenous functional phenotype, we identified the left amygdala as an area of abnormally increased connectivity (with the middle frontal gyrus) during intense stimuli. Importantly, when correcting for depression, which was more common in the FT cohort, many abnormalities on activation patterns disappeared, suggesting a strong influence of mood states on the neurobiological effects of emotional stimuli. Our study was not designed, however, to properly assess such effect since matching for the presence of major depression, the most common psychiatric comorbidity, was not performed (the MINI screen uncovered major depression in one third of FT patients but in only 2 ET patients).
Several pathophysiologic mechanisms can be entertained. On one level, the differences in connectivity by the amygdala and, therefore, the limbic system and striatum, suggests that abnormal processing of emotional information is associated with limbic overactivation (Harvey et al., 2006). Emotional stimuli may thus affect somatosensory processing by limbic areas associated with emotion and attention (Black et al., 2004). On the other hand, increased activation in the paracingulate gyrus, which is associated with the anterior cingulate cortex, may represent in FT subjects the neurobiological correlate of alexithymia, the inability to identify and describe emotions (Kaplan et al., 2013). Indeed, high alexithymia scores have been shown to increase activity in the anterior cingulate, mediofrontal cortices, and insula during emotional stimuli processing (Deng et al., 2013), a pattern similar to ours. Finally, increased activation of the paracingulate gyrus may represent a compensatory neural mechanism during emotional processing, modulating emotional responsiveness via top-down cognitive control, as recently shown in patients with Parkinson disease (Moonen et al., 2017).
The anterior paracingulate cortex is considered a key prefrontal region subserving the theory of mind, the ability to represent mental states, which is important in understand the intentions of people involved in social interaction and in predicting future intentional social interaction (Walter et al., 2004). The impaired reasoning about our own and other people's mental states has already been identified in another functional disorder, psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (also with increased alexithymic traits) (Schonenberg et al., 2015). Theory of mind can provide a unifying vision of functional neurological disorders given that it may serve to form a representation of mental states by reconciling the perception of the internal states of the body, or interoception, across a variety of emotional states (Shah et al., 2017). In the setting of altered emotion processing (possibly with alexithymia), the ability to predict future events as desired is eroded (Bartsch and Estes, 2014). This impaired “Bayesian prediction” may explain the ostensible contradictions between voluntary and unconscious motor control in FMD, with abnormal predictive beliefs generating movements executed without a sense of agency (Newby et al., 2016).
Our study has limitations. Although it was powered to detect major differences in activation by emotion processing tasks, its relatively small sample precludes an adequate assessment of the effect of psychiatric co-morbidities (such as depression, PTSD, and anxiety) on the activation patterns in the motor and emotional tasks. It is possible that these comorbidities may have contributed to at least some of the differences in activation patterns identified in FT subjects, suggesting maladaptive cognitive interpretation of emotions (Harrison and Critchley, 2007). Similarly, due to sample size, we are unable to determine whether sidedness and topographical involvement (e.g., arm versus neck involvement) may be contributory. Nevertheless, the variance in phenomenological involvement, characteristic of the referral pattern of a specialized movement disorders center, would have diluted the statistical differences between groups rather than create false positive findings. In addition, we did not capture resting-state fMRI data and our findings cannot therefore be compared to those recently reported by Maurer and colleagues demonstrating decreased functional connectivity between the right temporo-parietal junction and bilateral sensorimotor regions (Maurer et al., 2016). Finally, we did not measure alexithymia in our patients, which has been recently shown to contribute to the development of functional disorders (Demartini et al., 2014) and to impaired facial emotion recognition in these patients (Pedrosa Gil et al., 2009), and may have mediated at least some of the cortical activation in response to emotion stimuli.
5. Conclusions
Functional tremor is associated with abnormalities in activation and functional connectivity in networks involved in emotion processing and theory of mind, which may be relevant to the pathophysiology of functional movement disorders. Prospective studies will be needed to determine whether the observed changes are relevant to the pathogenesis of functional disorders or may represent changes that are compensatory or secondary to primary motor or psychological states. It will also be important to examine the extent to which promising interventions, such as cognitive behavioral therapy (LaFrance et al., 2014), are capable of normalizing the abnormalities in emotion processing identified in this study.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by NIH (CTSA KL2 Research Scholars grant KL2 RR026315 and CDA K23 MH092735). The funding source had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Conflicts of interest/financial disclosures
Dr. Espay is supported by a K23 career development award (NIMH, 1K23MH092735) and has received grant support from Great Lakes Neurotechnologies and the Michael J Fox Foundation; personal compensation as a consultant/scientific advisory board member for Abbvie, TEVA, Impax, Merz, Acadia, Cynapsus, Lundbeck, and USWorldMeds; publishing royalties from Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Cambridge University Press, and Springer; and honoraria from Abbvie, UCB, USWorldMeds, Lundbeck, Acadia, the American Academy of Neurology, and the Movement Disorders Society.
Dr. Vannest, Mr. Maloney, Mr. Norris, Dr. Eliassen, and Ms. Neefus have nothing to disclose.
Dr. Allendorfer has received funding from the Shor Foundation for Epilepsy Research. She serves as an associate editor of the journal Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience.
Dr. Lang has served as an advisor for Abbvie, Allon Therapeutics, Avanir Pharmaceuticals, Biogen Idec, Boerhinger-Ingelheim, Ceregene, Lilly, Medtronic, Merck, Novartis, NeuroPhage Pharmaceuticals, Teva and UCB; received honoraria from Medtronic, Teva, UCB, AbbVie; received grants from Brain Canada, Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Edmond J Safra Philanthropic Foundation, Michael J. Fox Foundation, the Ontario Brain Institute, National Parkinson Foundation, Parkinson Society Canada, Tourette Syndrome Association, W. Garfield Weston Foundation; received publishing royalties from Saunders, Wiley-Blackwell, Johns Hopkins Press, and Cambridge University Press; and has served as an expert witness in cases related to the welding industry.
Dr. Szaflarski has received, in the last 24 months, research funding from NIH, Shor Foundation for Epilepsy Research, Epilepsy Foundation of America, Food and Drug Administration, Compumedics Neuroscan, Inc., Department of Defense, and the University of Alabama at Birmingham (UAB). While this research was conducted, he was supported by NIH K23 NS052468. He serves as an associate editor of the journal Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience and on editorial boards of the journals Epilepsy & Behavior, Journal of Epileptology, Folia Medica Copernicana and Journal of Medical Science.
References
- Allendorfer J.B., Szaflarski J.P. Contributions of fMRI towards our understanding of the response to psychosocial stress in epilepsy and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. 2014;35C:19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson M.J.a.S.S. 2007. Non-linear Registration, aka Spatial Normalisation. [Google Scholar]
- Aybek S., Nicholson T.R., O'Daly O., Zelaya F., Kanaan R.A., David A.S. Emotion-motion interactions in conversion disorder: an FMRI study. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0123273. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar-On R., Tranel D., Denburg N.L., Bechara A. Exploring the neurological substrate of emotional and social intelligence. Brain. 2003;126:1790–1800. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartsch K., Estes D. Why we assume it's all good: the role of theory of mind in early inherent feature inferences. Behav. Brain Sci. 2014;37:482. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13003646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behzadi Y., Restom K., Liau J., Liu T.T. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage. 2007;37:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate - a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Bentley P., Vuilleumier P., Thiel C.M., Driver J., Dolan R.J. Effects of attention and emotion on repetition priming and their modulation by cholinergic enhancement. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;90:1171–1181. doi: 10.1152/jn.00776.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Black D.N., Seritan A.L., Taber K.H., Hurley R.A. Conversion hysteria: lessons from functional imaging. J. Neuropsychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 2004;16:245–251. doi: 10.1176/jnp.16.3.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore R.L., Sinanaj I., Galli S., Aybek S., Vuilleumier P. Aversive stimuli exacerbate defensive motor behaviour in motor conversion disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93:229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonora A., Benuzzi F., Monti G., Mirandola L., Pugnaghi M., Nichelli P., Meletti S. Recognition of emotions from faces and voices in medial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2011;20:648–654. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breiter H.C., Etcoff N.L., Whalen P.J., Kennedy W.A., Rauch S.L., Buckner R.L., Strauss M.M., Hyman S.E., Rosen B.R. Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron. 1996;17:875–887. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80219-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calarge C., Andreasen N.C., O'Leary D.S. Visualizing how one brain understands another: a PET study of theory of mind. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2003;160:1954–1964. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox R.W. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demartini B., Petrochilos P., Ricciardi L., Price G., Edwards M.J., Joyce E. The role of alexithymia in the development of functional motor symptoms (conversion disorder) J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2014;85:1132–1137. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-307203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng Y., Ma X., Tang Q. Brain response during visual emotional processing: an fMRI study of alexithymia. Psychiatry Res. 2013;213:225–229. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan R.S., Segonne F., Fischl B., Quinn B.T., Dickerson B.C., Blacker D., Buckner R.L., Dale A.M., Maguire R.P., Hyman B.T., Albert M.S., Killiany R.J. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douaud G., Smith S., Jenkinson M., Behrens T., Johansen-Berg H., Vickers J., James S., Voets N., Watkins K., Matthews P.M., James A. Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain. 2007;130:2375–2386. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Espay A.J., Lang A.E. Phenotype-specific diagnosis of functional (psychogenic) movement disorders. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 2015;15:556. doi: 10.1007/s11910-015-0556-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Espay A.J., Lang A.E., Erro R., Merola A., Fasano A., Berardelli A., Bhatia K.P. Essential pitfalls in “essential” tremor. Mov. Disord. 2017;32(3):325–331. doi: 10.1002/mds.26919. (Mar) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahn S., Williams D.T. Psychogenic dystonia. Adv. Neurol. 1988;50:431–455. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forstmann B.U., Keuken M.C., Jahfari S., Bazin P.L., Neumann J., Schafer A., Anwander A., Turner R. Cortico-subthalamic white matter tract strength predicts interindividual efficacy in stopping a motor response. NeuroImage. 2012;60:370–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frazier J.A., Chiu S., Breeze J.L., Makris N., Lange N., Kennedy D.N., Herbert M.R., Bent E.K., Koneru V.K., Dieterich M.E., Hodge S.M., Rauch S.L., Grant P.E., Cohen B.M., Seidman L.J., Caviness V.S., Biederman J. Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:1256–1265. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K.J., Williams S., Howard R., Frackowiak R.S., Turner R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 1996;35:346–355. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein J.M., Seidman L.J., Makris N., Ahern T., O'Brien L.M., Caviness V.S., Jr., Kennedy D.N., Faraone S.V., Tsuang M.T. Hypothalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia: sex effects and genetic vulnerability. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;61:935–945. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Good C.D., Johnsrude I.S., Ashburner J., Henson R.N., Friston K.J., Frackowiak R.S. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. 2001;14:21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabner G., Janke A.L., Budge M.M., Smith D., Pruessner J., Collins D.L. Symmetric atlasing and model based segmentation: an application to the hippocampus in older adults. Med. Image. Comput. Comput. Assist. Interv. 2006;9:58–66. doi: 10.1007/11866763_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gupta A., Lang A.E. Psychogenic movement disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2009;22:430–436. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32832dc169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison N.A., Critchley H.D. 2nd edition. Elsevier; UK: 2007. Neuroimaging and Emotion. [Google Scholar]
- Harvey S.B., Stanton B.R., David A.S. Conversion disorder: towards a neurobiological understanding. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2006;2:13–20. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M., Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 2001;5:143–156. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M., Bannister P., Brady M., Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002;17:825–841. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M., Beckmann C.F., Behrens T.E., Woolrich M.W., Smith S.M. Fsl. NeuroImage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan M.J., Dwivedi A.K., Privitera M.D., Isaacs K., Hughes C., Bowman M. Comparisons of childhood trauma, alexithymia, and defensive styles in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures vs. epilepsy: implications for the etiology of conversion disorder. J. Psychosom. Res. 2013;75:142–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaFrance W.C., Jr., Baird G.L., Barry J.J., Blum A.S., Frank Webb A., Keitner G.I., Machan J.T., Miller I., Szaflarski J.P., Consortium N.E.S.T.T. Multicenter pilot treatment trial for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiat. 2014;71:997–1005. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J.H., Garwood M., Menon R., Adriany G., Andersen P., Truwit C.L., Ugurbil K. High contrast and fast three-dimensional magnetic resonance imaging at high fields. Magn. Reson. Med. 1995;34:308–312. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier W., Buller R., Philipp M., Heuser I. The Hamilton Anxiety Scale: reliability, validity and sensitivity to change in anxiety and depressive disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 1988;14:61–68. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(88)90072-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makris N., Goldstein J.M., Kennedy D., Hodge S.M., Caviness V.S., Faraone S.V., Tsuang M.T., Seidman L.J. Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2006;83:155–171. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurer C.W., LaFaver K., Ameli R., Epstein S.A., Hallett M., Horovitz S.G. Impaired self-agency in functional movement disorders: a resting-state fMRI study. Neurology. 2016;87:564–570. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moonen A.J.H., Weiss P.H., Wiesing M., Weidner R., Fink G.R., Reijnders J., Weber W.M., Leentjens A.F.G. An fMRI study into emotional processing in Parkinson's disease: does increased medial prefrontal activation compensate for striatal dysfunction? PLoS One. 2017;12:e0177085. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newby R., Alty J., Kempster P. Functional dystonia and the borderland between neurology and psychiatry: new concepts. Mov. Disord. 2016;31:1777–1784. doi: 10.1002/mds.26805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nowak D.A., Fink G.R. Psychogenic movement disorders: aetiology, phenomenology, neuroanatomical correlates and therapeutic approaches. NeuroImage. 2009;47:1015–1025. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patenaude B., Smith S.M., Kennedy D.N., Jenkinson M. A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. NeuroImage. 2011;56:907–922. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedrosa Gil F., Ridout N., Kessler H., Neuffer M., Schoechlin C., Traue H.C., Nickel M. Facial emotion recognition and alexithymia in adults with somatoform disorders. Depress. Anxiety. 2009;26:E26–33. doi: 10.1002/da.20456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessoa L., Kastner S., Ungerleider L.G. Attentional control of the processing of neural and emotional stimuli. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2002;15:31–45. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00214-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessoa L., McKenna M., Gutierrez E., Ungerleider L.G. Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2002;99:11458–11463. doi: 10.1073/pnas.172403899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinninti N.R., Madison H., Musser E., Rissmiller D. MINI International Neuropsychiatric Schedule: clinical utility and patient acceptance. Eur. Psychiatry. 2003;18:361–364. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power J.D., Barnes K.A., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rees G., Frith C.D., Lavie N. Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science. 1997;278:1616–1619. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5343.1616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmithorst V.J., Dardzinski B.J., Holland S.K. Simultaneous correction of ghost and geometric distortion artifacts in EPI using a multiecho reference scan. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2001;20:535–539. doi: 10.1109/42.929619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schonenberg M., Jusyte A., Hohnle N., Mayer S.V., Weber Y., Hautzinger M., Schell C. Theory of mind abilities in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. 2015;53:20–24. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.09.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schrag A.E., Mehta A.R., Bhatia K.P., Brown R.J., Frackowiak R.S., Trimble M.R., Ward N.S., Rowe J.B. The functional neuroimaging correlates of psychogenic versus organic dystonia. Brain. 2013;136:770–781. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shah P., Catmur C., Bird G. From heart to mind: linking interoception, emotion, and theory of mind. Cortex. 2017;93:220–223. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.010. (Aug) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan D.V., Lecrubier Y., Sheehan K.H., Amorim P., Janavs J., Weiller E., Hergueta T., Baker R., Dunbar G.C. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl. 20):22–33. (quiz 34–57) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S.M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002;17:143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S.M., Nichols T.E. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage. 2009;44:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S.M., Jenkinson M., Woolrich M.W., Beckmann C.F., Behrens T.E., Johansen-Berg H., Bannister P.R., De Luca M., Drobnjak I., Flitney D.E., Niazy R.K., Saunders J., Vickers J., Zhang Y., De Stefano N., Brady J.M., Matthews P.M. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23(Suppl. 1):S208–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strakowski S.M., Eliassen J.C., Lamy M., Cerullo M.A., Allendorfer J.B., Madore M., Lee J.H., Welge J.A., DelBello M.P., Fleck D.E., Adler C.M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain activation in bipolar mania: evidence for disruption of the ventrolateral prefrontal-amygdala emotional pathway. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69:381–388. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szaflarski J.P., Allendorfer J.B., Heyse H., Mendoza L., Szaflarski B.A., Cohen N. Functional MRI of facial emotion processing in left temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2014;32:92–99. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N., Tanaka J.W., Leon A.C., McCarry T., Nurse M., Hare T.A., Marcus D.J., Westerlund A., Casey B.J., Nelson C. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168:242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V., Brezing C., Gallea C., Ameli R., Roelofs K., LaFrance W.C., Jr., Hallett M. Emotional stimuli and motor conversion disorder. Brain. 2010;133:1526–1536. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter H., Adenzato M., Ciaramidaro A., Enrici I., Pia L., Bara B.G. Understanding intentions in social interaction: the role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004;16:1854–1863. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams J.B. A structured interview guide for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1988;45:742–747. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800320058007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams J.B., Kobak K.A., Bech P., Engelhardt N., Evans K., Lipsitz J., Olin J., Pearson J., Kalali A. The GRID-HAMD: standardization of the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2008;23:120–129. doi: 10.1097/YIC.0b013e3282f948f5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamasaki H., LaBar K.S., McCarthy G. Dissociable prefrontal brain systems for attention and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2002;99:11447–11451. doi: 10.1073/pnas.182176499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y., Brady M., Smith S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2001;20:45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]