Abstract
Objective
The level of hippocampal atrophy in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is typically less than that observed in Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, it is not known how the cognitive phenotype of DLB is influenced by hippocampal atrophy or the atrophy of adjacent medial temporal lobe structures.
Methods
Dementia with Lewy bodies (n = 65), AD (n = 76) and control (n = 63) participants underwent 3T magnetic resonance imaging and cognitive Cambridge Cognitive Examination and Mini‐Mental State Examination (CAMCOG and MMSE) assessments. Hippocampal volume, and parahippocampal, entorhinal and temporal pole cortical thickness, was compared between groups. Regression models were used to investigate whether hippocampal volume and cortical thickness associated with global cognition and cognitive subdomains.
Results
Dementia with Lewy bodies, AD and control participants showed significantly different hippocampal, parahippocampal and entorhinal cortical thinning, where atrophy was greatest in AD and intermediate in DLB. Temporal pole thickness was reduced in DLB and AD compared with control participants. In DLB, but not AD, hippocampal volume associated with total CAMCOG, CAMCOG memory and MMSE scores. In DLB, parahippocampal, entorhinal and temporal pole thickness associated with total CAMCOG and CAMCOG memory scores, parahippocampal thickness associated with MMSE scores, and entorhinal thickness associated with CAMCOG executive function scores.
Conclusions
In this large sample, these results are in agreement with other studies indicating that hippocampal atrophy is less severe in DLB than AD. Hippocampal atrophy and medial temporal lobe cortical thickness were associated with the severity of cognitive symptoms, suggesting that atrophy in these structures, as a potential proxy of AD pathology, may partly mediate specific DLB cognitive symptoms. © 2017 The Authors. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Keywords: hippocampus, dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer's disease, magnetic resonance imaging
Introduction
Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is the second most common cause of dementia in older people after Alzheimer's disease (AD), and accounts for 15–20% of all dementia cases at post mortem (McKeith et al., 2005). DLB is characterised by symptoms including motor and neuropsychiatric problems, executive dysfunction and visuospatial deficits, visual hallucinations, autonomic dysfunction and marked fluctuations in alertness (McKeith et al., 2005; McKeith, 2006). There are differences between the symptom profile of DLB and AD, where memory problems are typically more pronounced in AD, and attentional and visuoperceptual difficulties are more prominent in DLB (Calderon et al., 2001; Ferman et al., 2006).
To date, the use of magnetic resonance (MR) imaging has been useful in understanding the differences between DLB and AD, despite the low‐to‐intermediate co‐existence of AD neuropathology in individuals with DLB (Merdes et al., 2003; McKeith et al., 2005; Schneider et al., 2012). One particular region of relevance is the hippocampus, which is located within the medial temporal lobe and has a functional role in declarative memory, emotional memory and spatial navigation (Eichenbaum, 2004; Strange et al., 2014). Hippocampal atrophy is a marker of AD pathology, as hippocampal volume has been shown to negatively associate with the tau and amyloid beta burden in AD (Apostolova et al., 2015) and hippocampal atrophy can predate a diagnosis of dementia by a period of up to several years (Tondelli et al., 2012; de Flores et al., 2015) also making it a potentially important prodromal biomarker.
Hippocampal AD pathology is also a feature of DLB, as previous imaging studies have consistently demonstrated that the level of hippocampal atrophy observed in DLB is less than in AD (Hashimoto et al., 1998; Tam et al., 2005; Burton et al., 2009; Watson et al., 2012), and the level of atrophy relates to concurrent AD pathology measured post mortem (Burton et al., 2009; Kantarci et al., 2012). The extent of the atrophy may also influence the subsequent clinical course of DLB, as individuals with higher levels of hippocampal atrophy have a shorter survival time compared with those with lower levels of atrophy (Graff‐Radford et al., 2016).
It is not known how hippocampal atrophy, or the thickness of adjacent medial temporal lobe structures, influences the cognitive phenotype of DLB. Three relevant subregions are (i) the parahippocampal gyrus, which has a predominant role in episodic memory and an involvement in visuospatial processing (Aminoff et al., 2013); (ii) the entorhinal cortex, which relays information from sensory cortical areas of the hippocampus, and from the hippocampus to neocortical storage areas (Heinemann et al., 2000); and (iii) the temporal pole, which has a role in cognition and memory function and particularly declarative memory (Blaizot et al., 2010). Whilst one previous study showed that compared with healthy control participants, DLB participants showed reductions in the thickness of the right entorhinal, right perirhinal and right parahippocampal areas; there were no differences compared with AD participants and no associations between these regions and cognitive variables in DLB (Delli Pizzi et al., 2016). However, the main limitation of this study was in the relatively modest sample size.
Therefore, the main aim of the present study was to investigate whether hippocampal atrophy, as a proxy of AD pathology, is associated with cognitive deficits in DLB, in a large sample of dementia patients. A secondary aim was to examine the association between the thickness of the parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex and temporal pole, and measures of cognition in DLB. It was hypothesised that DLB patients would display a level of hippocampal atrophy that was greater in severity than healthy control individuals but less than that observed in AD, and individuals with DLB would display a greater level of surface atrophy in adjacent medial temporal lobe subregions, as compared with healthy controls, but less than that observed in AD.
Method
Participants
A total of 204 participants (M age = 77.75 years; SDage = 6.76 years), including 76 with probable AD (McKhann et al., 1984), 65 with probable DLB (McKeith et al., 2005) and 63 similarly older healthy controls, were recruited from a community‐dwelling population of patients referred to Old Age Psychiatry, Geriatric Medicine or Neurology services. Control participants were recruited from the relatives and friends of patients with dementia and from a bank of volunteer participants held by Newcastle University. Exclusion criteria for all participants included contra‐indications for MR imaging, a previous history of alcohol or substance misuse, a significant neurological history or psychiatric illness, focal brain lesions, or the presence of other relevant severe or uncontrolled illnesses. Participants, or their nearest relative or carer, provided informed consent. The diagnosis of DLB was confirmed by two senior experienced clinicians. The study was approved by the local research ethics committee.
Measures
Global cognition was assessed using the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) and the Cambridge Cognitive Examination (CAMCOG; Roth et al., 1986). In DLB and AD patients, the severity of parkinsonian motor symptoms were assessed using Part III of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS‐III; Goetz et al., 2008), and with the help of a carer/informant, the severity of visual hallucinations and depressive symptoms in the previous four weeks were assessed using the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI; Cummings et al., 1994), and the severity of cognitive fluctuations in the previous four weeks were assessed using the Clinician Assessment of Fluctuation Scale (Walker et al., 2000).
Magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition
All participants underwent MR imaging within 2 months of completing neuropsychiatric assessments. T1‐weighted whole brain scans were obtained using a 3T Intera Achieva scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Eindhoven, Netherlands) with an eight‐channel head coil. The imaging sequence protocol was as follows: 3D MPRAGE, sagittal acquisition, 1‐mm isotropic resolution, matrix size of 240 (anterior–posterior) × 216 (superior–inferior) × 180 (right–left); repetition time = 8.3 ms, echo time = 4.6 ms; SENSE factor = 2; flip angle = 8°. The acquired volume was angulated such that the axial slice orientation was standardised to align with the AC–PC line.
Calculation of hippocampal and total grey matter volumes and cortical thickness
Estimates of hippocampal and total grey matter (GM) volumes as well as regional cortical thickness of specific medial temporal lobe structures (parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex and temporal pole) were obtained for each participant using freesurfer (v.5.1.0, http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). The technical aspects of these methods have been described elsewhere (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999; Fischl and Dale, 2000). In brief, the processing stream involves intensity non‐uniformity correction, Talairach registration, the removal of non‐brain tissue (skull stripping), segmentation of white matter (WM) and subcortical GM structures (Fischl et al., 2002), tessellation of the GM‐WM boundary then surface deformation following GM‐cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) intensity gradients to optimally place GM‐WM and GM‐CSF borders (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl and Dale, 2000). Once cortical models were generated, surface inflation, transformation to a spherical atlas and parcellation of the cerebral cortex into regions based on gyral and sulcal structure were undertaken (Makris et al., 2006). This technique used both intensity and continuity information from the entire 3D MR volume in the segmentation and deformation procedures to produce representations of cortical thickness, calculated as the closest distance from the GM‐WM to GM‐CSF boundaries at each vertex on the tessellated surface (Fischl et al., 2004). Total GM volume was calculated as the sum of subcortical, cortical (left and right) and cerebellum GM.
Visual inspection of images at each step of the freesurfer processing stream was carefully conducted by a single operator (S. J. C.) to ensure segmentation accuracy of the subcortical structures, as well as WM/pial surfaces and tissue classifications. Images with segmentation errors, which were unable to be corrected, were excluded from subsequent analyses.
Data analysis
Cambridge Cognitive Examination total, memory and executive function, MMSE, UPDRS‐III, NPI depression, NPI visual hallucinations and Clinician Assessment of Fluctuation Scale scores were compared between DLB, AD and control participants using a one‐way analysis of variance or independent‐samples t‐tests, where appropriate. Chi‐squared tests were used to assess group differences in age and gender. The volume of the left, right and total hippocampus (mm3) were compared between‐groups using an analysis of covariance, controlling for intracranial volume. Left, right and total cortical thickness measures of the parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex and temporal pole (mm) were compared between DLB, AD and control groups using a series of one‐way analysis of variances. Post‐hoc comparisons were Bonferroni‐corrected (p = .017), where appropriate.
Separate linear regression models were used, with the inclusion of age and intracranial volume as covariates, to investigate whether total hippocampal volume was associated with cognitive scores (CAMCOG total, CAMCOG memory, CAMCOG executive function and MMSE). In addition, linear regression models, with the inclusion of age as a covariate, examined whether total cortical thickness measures of the parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal and temporal poles were associated with cognitive scores. Additional exploratory analyses were also conducted using linear regression models, with the inclusion of age as a covariate, to examine whether total parahippocampal, entorhinal and temporal pole cortical thickness was associated with other CAMCOG subscales (orientation, language, attention, calculation, praxis, abstract thinking and perception), and with three further memory subdomains (remote memory, recent memory and learning memory).
Results
Demographic and clinical results
Demographic and clinical results are shown in Table 1. DLB, AD and control groups did not differ in age or gender (p > .05). There were significant between‐group differences in CAMCOG total scores (F(2,203) = 132.18, p < .001, η 2 p = .57), CAMCOG memory (F(2,203) = 182.57, p < .001, η 2 p = .65) and CAMCOG executive function subscores (F(2,203) = 108.78, p < .001, η 2 p = .52). Compared with control participants, CAMCOG scores were significantly lower in DLB and AD, but there were no differences between the dementia groups. All three groups showed significant differences on CAMCOG memory scores, where control participants showed the lowest levels of impairment, DLB participants intermediate impairment and AD the greatest levels of impairment. For CAMCOG executive function scores, post‐hoc comparisons showed that DLB participants had lowest scores, AD participants displayed intermediate scores and controls had the highest scores. MMSE scores were significantly different between DLB, AD and control groups (F(2,203) = 101.97, p < .001, η 2 p = .50). Post‐hoc comparisons indicated that control participants obtained highest scores but that there were no differences between DLB and AD groups.
Table 1.
Demographic and group characteristics
DLB (n = 65) | AD (n = 76) | Control (n = 63) | Group comparisons | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Age | 78.40 | 6.47 | 77.99 | 7.51 | 76.81 | 6.05 | p > .05 |
Disease duration (months) | 35.96 | 27.74 | 32.61 | 21.83 | N/A | N/A | p > .05 |
Gender (male/female) | 45/20 | 48/28 | 36/27 | p > .05 | |||
CAMCOG total | 67.94 | 15.27 | 67.09 | 12.96 | 96.76 | 3.77 | p < .001a , b |
CAMCOG memory score | 15.40 | 4.99 | 10.42 | 4.45 | 23.52 | 1.79 | p < .001a , b , c |
CAMCOG executive function | 11.66 | 5.15 | 13.67 | 4.64 | 22.36 | 2.95 * | p < .001a , b , d |
MMSE | 20.46 | 5.23 | 20.25 | 4.32 | 29.00 | 1.00 * | p < .001a , b |
UPDRS‐III total | 20.51 | 13.14 | 3.79 | 3.68 | 6.19 | 12.81 * | p < .001 |
CAF | 5.47 | 4.03 | 1.85 | 3.40 | N/A | N/A * | p < .001 |
NPI hallucinations | 2.30 | 2.55 | .25 | .81 | N/A | N/A * | p < .001 |
NPI depression | 1.17 | 2.42 | .97 | 1.57 | N/A | N/A | p > .05 |
AD, Alzheimer's disease; DLB, dementia with Lewy bodies; CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Examination; CAF, Clinician Assessment of Fluctuation Scale; MMSE, Mini‐Mental State Examination; NPI, Neuropsychiatric Inventory; SD, standard deviation; UPDRS‐III, Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (Part III).
DLB < control.
AD < control.
AD < DLB.
DLB < AD.
p < .001.
As expected, there were significant differences between all three groups in terms of UPDRS‐III scores (F(2,199) = 48.73, p < .001, η 2 p = .33), where motor impairments were greater in DLB compared with AD and control participants. Similarly, cognitive fluctuations were more severe in DLB than in AD participants (t(100.71) = 5.30, p < .001). DLB participants also had significantly more visual hallucinations in the previous month compared with AD participants (t(59.70) = 5.66, p < .001) but not depressive symptoms (t(111) = .55, p > .05). Neuropsychiatric symptoms were not associated with hippocampal volume (all p‐values > .05).
Magnetic resonance imaging
There were significant differences in intracranial volume between DLB, AD and control groups (F(2,204) = 4.46, p < .05, η 2 p = .04), where DLB participants had a significantly greater volume than control but not AD participants. Significant differences were observed in the left hippocampus (F(2,203) = 67.12, p < .001, η 2 p = .40), right hippocampus (F(2,203) = 52.95, p < .001, η 2 p = .35) and in total hippocampal volumes (F(2,203) = 65.53, p < .001, η 2 p = .40). Post‐hoc comparisons indicated that hippocampal volume was lowest in individuals with AD, moderate in individuals with DLB and greatest in control participants (p < .001; Table 2).
Table 2.
Comparison of hippocampal volume between DLB, AD and control groups
DLB (n = 65) | AD (n = 76) | Control (n = 63) | Group comparisons | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Left Hippocampus volume (mm3) | 3062.00 | 596.26 | 2536.38 | 626.28 | 3564.75 | 469.21 | p < .001a , b , c |
Right Hippocampus volume (mm3) | 3121.40 | 580.33 | 2598.74 | 626.26 | 3539.70 | 558.45 | p < .001a , b , c |
Total Hippocampus volume (mm3) | 6183.40 | 1134.45 | 5135.12 | 1199.19 | 7104.44 | 981.17 | p < .001a , b , c |
Total Intracranial Volume (mm3) | 1318687.47 | 261299.23 | 1288641.50 | 247635.81 | 1198843.29 | 192283.20 | p < .05a |
AD, Alzheimer's disease; DLB, dementia with Lewy bodies; SD, standard deviation.
DLB < control.
AD < control.
AD < DLB.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Significant differences in cortical thickness were observed between DLB, AD and control groups in the parahippocampal gyrus, left (F(2,203) = 20.03, p < .001, η 2 p = .17), right (F(2,203) = 20.13, p < .001, η 2 p = .17) and total thickness (F(2,203) = 23.88, p < .001, η 2 p = .19); in entorhinal areas, left (F(2,203) = 42.96, p < .001, η 2 p = .30), right (F(2,203) = 28.22, p < .001, η 2 p = .22) and total thickness (F(2,203) = 40.30, p < .001, η 2 p = .29); and temporal pole, left (F(2,203) = 8.44, p < .001, η 2 p = .08), right (F(2,203) = 12.67, p < .001, η 2 p = .11) and total thickness (F(2,203) = 13.73, p < .001, η 2 p = .12). Post‐hoc comparisons indicated a consistent difference in profile (controls > DLB > AD) with the exception of the temporal pole, where DLB and AD participants both significantly differed from controls but not from each other (Table 3).
Table 3.
Comparison of medial temporal subregions between DLB, AD and control groups
DLB (n = 65) | AD (n = 76) | Control (n = 63) | Group comparisons | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Left parahippocampal thickness (mm) | 2.30 | .35 | 2.12 | .28 | 2.46 | .32 | p < .001a , b , c |
Right parahippocampal thickness (mm) | 2.28 | .30 | 2.15 | .31 | 2.47 | .28 | p < .001a , b , c |
Left entorhinal thickness (mm) | 2.79 | .42 | 2.41 | .42 | 3.01 | .32 | p < .001a , b , c |
Right entorhinal thickness (mm) | 2.79 | .47 | 2.47 | .51 | 3.06 | .39 | p < .001a , b , c |
Left temporal pole thickness (mm) | 3.32 | .38 | 3.23 | .49 | 3.51 | .33 | p < .001a , b |
Right temporal pole thickness (mm) | 3.28 | .42 | 3.14 | .56 | 3.55 | .41 | p < .001a , b |
Total parahippocampal thickness (mm) | 4.58 | .60 | 4.27 | .55 | 4.93 | .54 | p < .001a , b , c |
Total entorhinal thickness (mm) | 5.58 | .82 | 4.88 | .86 | 6.07 | .66 | p < .001a , b , c |
Total temporal pole thickness (mm) | 6.60 | .72 | 6.37 | .94 | 7.06 | .61 | p < .001a , b |
DLB < control;
AD < control;
AD < DLB.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
In DLB participants, total hippocampal volume was significantly associated with total CAMCOG (R 2 = .21, F(3,64) = 5.24, p < .01), CAMCOG memory (R 2 = .38, F(3,64) = 12.25, p < .001), CAMCOG executive function (R 2 = .13, F(3,64) = 3.07, p < .05) and MMSE scores (R 2 = .13, F(3,64) = 2.95, p < .05; Table 4). In AD, total hippocampal volume was not associated with total CAMCOG, CAMCOG memory, CAMCOG executive function, or MMSE scores (all p‐values > .05; Table S1).
Table 4.
Associations between total hippocampal volume and cognition in DLB (n = 65)
B | SE B | β | |
---|---|---|---|
CAMCOG total | .01 | .00 | .47** |
CAMCOG memory | .00 | .00 | .68*** |
CAMCOG executive function | .00 | .00 | .36 |
MMSE | .00 | .00 | .37** |
CAMCOG: Cambridge Cognitive Examination, MMSE: Mini‐Mental State Examination
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001.
In DLB, total parahippocampal thickness was associated with total CAMCOG (R 2 = .15, F(2,64) = 5.64, p < .01), CAMCOG memory (R 2 = .21, F(2,64) = 8.05, p < .01) and MMSE (R 2 = .12, F(2,64) = 4.30, p < .05), but not CAMCOG executive function scores (p > .05). Total entorhinal thickness in DLB was associated with total CAMCOG (R 2 = .22, F(2,64) = 8.77, p < .001), CAMCOG memory (R 2 = .41, F(2,64) = 21.36, p < .001), CAMCOG executive function (R 2 = .11, F(2,64) = 3.93, p < .05) but not MMSE scores (p > .05). Total temporal pole thickness in the DLB group was associated with total CAMCOG (R 2 = .16, F(2,64) = 5.71, p < .01), CAMCOG memory (R 2 = .26, F(2,64) = 10.79, p < .001), but not CAMCOG executive function or MMSE scores (p‐values > .05; Table 5). In AD, parahippocampal, entorhinal and temporal pole thickness was not associated with total CAMCOG, CAMCOG memory, CAMCOG executive function or MMSE scores (all p‐values > .05; Table S2).
Table 5.
Associations between medial temporal subregion cortical thickness and cognition in DLB (n = 65)
B | SE B | β | |
---|---|---|---|
Total parahippocampal thickness | 9.54 | 3.02 | .38** |
CAMCOG total | 3.15 | .96 | .38** |
CAMCOG memory | 1.72 | 1.08 | .20 |
CAMCOG executive function | 3.09 | 1.05 | .36** |
MMSE | |||
Total entorhinal thickness | 8.93 | 2.22 | .48*** |
CAMCOG total | 3.78 | .63 | .63*** |
CAMCOG memory | 2.22 | .80 | .36** |
CAMCOG executive function | 2.04 | .82 | .32 |
MMSE | |||
Total temporal pole thickness | 8.14 | 2.56 | .39** |
CAMCOG total | 3.13 | .78 | .46*** |
CAMCOG memory | 1.45 | .91 | .20 |
CAMCOG executive function | 1.75 | .93 | .24 |
MMSE | 9.54 | 3.02 | .38** |
CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Examination; DLB, dementia with Lewy bodies; MMSE, Mini‐Mental State Examination.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Examination of CAMCOG subscales indicated that in DLB participants, total parahippocampal thickness was associated with language expression, attention, calculation and all three memory subdomains (remote, recent and learning memory); entorhinal thickness was associated with orientation, language expression, and all three memory domains, and temporal pole thickness was associated with language expression, calculation and the recent memory and learning memory subdomains (model p‐values < .05).
Discussion
The results of the present study indicate that hippocampal atrophy, as a proxy of AD pathology, is present in DLB but is less severe than in AD. DLB, AD and control groups showed significant differences in left, right and total parahippocampal, entorhinal and temporal pole surface thickness, where the thickness was greatest in controls, intermediate in DLB and lowest in AD. One exception to this was in the left temporal pole, where the surface thickness was greatest in controls but did not differ between DLB and AD groups. These results also indicated that hippocampal atrophy was associated with the severity of specific cognitive deficits in DLB, as total hippocampal volume was associated with total CAMCOG and MMSE scores, as global measures of cognition and with CAMCOG memory subscores. In DLB, parahippocampal surface thickness was associated with total CAMCOG, CAMCOG memory and MMSE scores; entorhinal surface thickness was associated with CAMCOG total, CAMCOG memory and executive function scores and temporal pole surface thickness was associated with CAMCOG total and CAMCOG memory scores. In AD, parahippocampal, entorhinal and temporal pole thickness was not associated with measures of cognition.
These results are in agreement with previous studies, which have observed that the hippocampus is relatively preserved in DLB as compared with AD (Hashimoto et al., 1998; Tam et al., 2005; Burton et al., 2009; Watson et al., 2012). The present study also indicates that other regions of the medial temporal lobe display an intermediate level of atrophy in DLB, specifically in the parahippocampal, entorhinal and temporal pole areas. With the exception of the left temporal lobe area, DLB and AD groups showed differential patterns of surface atrophy. These results are partially in agreement with a previous study that demonstrated that compared with controls, right entorhinal and parahippocampal thickness was reduced (Delli Pizzi et al., 2016). However, this previous study may have been underpowered and thus unable to detect subtle differences between DLB and AD. In the current study, no relationship was observed between surface thickness and cognitive measures in AD. Whilst relationships between cognition and medial temporal atrophy, and hippocampal volume, have been observed in previous studies in individuals with mild AD (Dickerson et al., 2009; Arlt et al., 2013), the lack of a relationship in the present study may reflect differences in AD participants, compared with the present sample, and in the differing neuropsychological measures used in these studies.
Overall, these results indicate that despite the relative preservation of the hippocampus in DLB, hippocampal atrophy is associated with global cognitive and memory impairment, and these relationships may represent AD pathology in the hippocampus. However, other pathologies may contribute to the cognitive symptoms of DLB and should be investigated further; one study has previously observed that cognitive decline in LBD may be a consequence of alpha‐synuclein, amyloid‐beta and phosphorylated tau pathology (Howlett et al., 2015), and additionally, one study of the medial temporal lobe showed that alpha‐synuclein pathology, rather than AD pathology, had a role in the atrophy of the amygdala (Burton et al., 2012). Hippocampal volume showed the strongest association with the CAMCOG memory score, which is in keeping with the memory function of the hippocampus (Eichenbaum, 2004; Strange et al., 2014). These results also indicate that atrophy to adjacent medial temporal lobe subregions can also influence the DLB cognitive phenotype, with an overlap of symptoms: parahippocampal surface thickness was associated with CAMCOG cognition and memory subscores, but not executive function, the entorhinal surface thickness was associated with total CAMCOG scores, as well as CAMCOG memory and executive function subscores. No association was observed between the MMSE, as a measure of global cognition, and entorhinal thickness. Although AD pathology typically first appears in entorhinal regions (Schonheit et al., 2004) and the entorhinal cortex projects to the hippocampus, it is likely that the MMSE is less sensitive to changes in entorhinal surface thickness in DLB, given the large sample size of DLB patients in the present study. In DLB, temporal pole surface thickness was associated with total CAMCOG and CAMCOG memory scores, most likely representing the primary memory role of this subregion (Blaizot et al., 2010). The lack of association between hippocampal atrophy and common neuropsychiatric symptoms in DLB, specifically depressive symptoms and hallucinations, as well as cognitive fluctuations, reinforce arguments that these are likely to be driven by pathologies in other regions.
It is a specific strength of the study that a relatively large AD and DLB cohort was included, and that imaging data was acquired using a 3T scanner as compared with the lower field strength of previous studies (Tam et al., 2005; Burton et al., 2009), and that a standardised image analysis tool was used to avoid the limitations of manually delineating the hippocampus. The main limitation of the study is in the lack of concurrent amyloid biomarker imaging as a measure of AD pathology. A further limitation is the lack of longitudinal follow‐up data; longitudinal data is necessary to clarify whether hippocampal atrophy, and the atrophy of adjacent medial temporal regions, can predict the course of DLB.
Overall, despite similar levels of cognitive impairment in DLB and AD, less hippocampal atrophy was observed in DLB than AD. These results suggest that amnestic deficits in DLB may be, in part, mediated by concurrent AD pathology in the hippocampus and in the adjacent subregions of the medial temporal lobe and may have clinical implications in the stratification of DLB patients, in the future, for anti‐amyloid disease‐modifying treatments. Indirectly, these data suggest a role for non‐AD pathology or AD related pathology, focused in non‐medial temporal lobe regions in the aetiology of other cognitive domains in DLB; further work combined with amyloid imaging may help elucidate these unanswered questions.
Conflict of interest
The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Key points.
It is not known how the cognitive phenotype of DLB is influenced by hippocampal atrophy or adjacent medial temporal lobe structures.
Hippocampal atrophy and medial temporal lobe cortical thickness were associated with the severity of cognitive symptoms.
Atrophy in these structures may partly mediate specific cognitive symptoms in DLB.
Supporting information
Supplementary Table S1: Associations between total hippocampal volume and cognition in AD (n = 76)
Supplementary Table S2: Associations between medial temporal subregion cortical thickness and cognition in AD (n = 76)
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the following: Wellcome Trust Starter Grants scheme for Clinical Lecturers (BH090112 to J‐P.T.); Wellcome Intermediate Clinical Fellowship (BH083281 to J‐P.T.); the Sir Jules Thorn Charitable Trust (05/JTA) and by the National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) Newcastle Biomedical Research Unit based at Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and Newcastle University. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.
Elder, G. J. , Mactier, K. , Colloby, S. J. , Watson, R. , Blamire, A. M. , O'Brien, J. T. , and Taylor, J.‐P. (2017) The influence of hippocampal atrophy on the cognitive phenotype of dementia with Lewy bodies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 32: 1182–1189. doi: 10.1002/gps.4719.
Sponsors of research: Wellcome Trust and Sir Jules Thorn Charitable Trust
References
- Aminoff EM, Kveraga K, Bar M. 2013. The role of the parahippocampal cortex in cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 17: 379–390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apostolova LG, Zarow C, Biado K, et al. 2015. Relationship between hippocampal atrophy and neuropathology markers: A 7T MRI validation study of the EADC‐ADNI Harmonized Hippocampal Segmentation Protocol. Alzheimers Dement 11: 139–150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arlt S, Buchert R, Spies L, et al. 2013. Association between fully automated MRI‐based volumetry of different brain regions and neuropsychological test performance in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin . Neuroscience 263: 335–344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blaizot X, Mansilla F, Insausti AM, et al. 2010. The human parahippocampal region: I. Temporal pole cytoarchitectonic and MRI correlation. Cereb Cortex 20: 2198–2212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton EJ, Barber R, Mukaetova‐Ladinska EB, et al. 2009. Medial temporal lobe atrophy on MRI differentiates Alzheimer's disease from dementia with Lewy bodies and vascular cognitive impairment: a prospective study with pathological verification of diagnosis. Brain 132: 195–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton EJ, Mukaetova‐Ladinska EB, Perry RH, et al. 2012. Neuropathological correlates of volumetric MRI in autopsy‐confirmed Lewy body dementia. Neurobiol Aging 33: 1228–1236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calderon J, Perry RJ, Erzinclioglu SW, et al. 2001. Perception, attention, and working memory are disproportionately impaired in dementia with Lewy bodies compared with Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 70: 157–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al. 1994. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44: 2308–2314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. 1999. Cortical surface‐based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9: 179–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Flores R, La Joie R, Chételat G. 2015. Structural imaging of hippocampal subfields in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neuroscience 309: 29–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delli Pizzi S, Franciotti R, Bubbico G, et al. 2016. Atrophy of hippocampal subfields and adjacent extrahippocampal structures in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging 40: 103–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson BC, Feczko E, Augustinack JC, et al. 2009. Differential effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease on medial temporal lobe cortical thickness and surface area. Neurobiol Aging 30: 432–440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H. 2004. Hippocampus: cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron 44: 109–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferman TJ, Smith GE, Boeve BF, et al. 2006. Neuropsychological differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Clin Neuropsychol 20: 623–636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Dale AM. 2000. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 11050–11055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. 2002. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33: 341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. 1999. Cortical surface‐based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface‐based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9: 195–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, et al. 2004. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 14: 11–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. 1975. “Mini‐mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12: 189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, et al. 2008. Movement Disorder Society‐sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS‐UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord 23: 2129–2170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graff‐Radford J, Lesnick TG, Boeve BF, et al. 2016. Predicting survival in Dementia with Lewy bodies with hippocampal volumetry. Mov Disord 31: 989–994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashimoto M, Kitagaki H, Imamura T, et al. 1998. Medial temporal and whole‐brain atrophy in dementia with Lewy bodies: a volumetric MRI study. Neurology 51: 357–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinemann U, Schmitz D, Eder C, Gloveli T. 2000. Properties of entorhinal cortex projection cells to the hippocampal formation. Ann N Y Acad Sci 911: 112–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howlett DR, Whitfield D, Johnson M, et al. 2015. Regional multiple pathology scores are associated with cognitive decline in Lewy body dementias. Brain Pathol 25: 401–408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kantarci K, Ferman TJ, Boeve BF, et al. 2012. Focal atrophy on MRI and neuropathologic classification of dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 79: 553–560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makris N, Kaiser J, Haselgrove C, et al. 2006. Human cerebral cortex: a system for the integration of volume‐ and surface‐based representations. Neuroimage 33: 139–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeith IG. 2006. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the Consortium on DLB International Workshop. J Alzheimers Dis 9: 417–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, et al. 2005. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB consortium. Neurology 65: 1863–1872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. 1984. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS‐ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 34: 939–944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merdes AR, Hansen LA, Jeste DV, et al. 2003. Influence of Alzheimer pathology on clinical diagnostic accuracy in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 60: 1586–1590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth M, Tym E, Mountjoy CQ, et al. 1986. CAMDEX. A standardised instrument for the diagnosis of mental disorder in the elderly with special reference to the early detection of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 149: 698–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Yu L, et al. 2012. Cognitive impairment, decline and fluctuations in older community‐dwelling subjects with Lewy bodies. Brain 135: 3005–3014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schonheit B, Zarski R, Ohm TG. 2004. Spatial and temporal relationships between plaques and tangles in Alzheimer‐pathology. Neurobiol Aging 25: 697–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strange BA, Witter MP, Lein ES, Moser EI. 2014. Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat Rev Neurosci 15: 655–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam CW, Burton EJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, O'Brien JT. 2005. Temporal lobe atrophy on MRI in Parkinson disease with dementia: a comparison with Alzheimer disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 64: 861–865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tondelli M, Wilcock GK, Nichelli P, et al. 2012. Structural MRI changes detectable up to ten years before clinical Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging 33: 825.e825–825.e836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker MP, Ayre GA, Cummings JL, et al. 2000. The Clinician Assessment of Fluctuation and the One Day Fluctuation Assessment Scale. Two methods to assess fluctuating confusion in dementia. Br J Psychiatry 177: 252–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson R, O'Brien JT, Barber R, Blamire AM. 2012. Patterns of gray matter atrophy in dementia with Lewy bodies: a voxel‐based morphometry study. Int Psychogeriatr 24: 532–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary Table S1: Associations between total hippocampal volume and cognition in AD (n = 76)
Supplementary Table S2: Associations between medial temporal subregion cortical thickness and cognition in AD (n = 76)