Abstract
Olfactory sensory input is detected by receptor neurons in the nose, which then send information to the olfactory bulb (OB), the first brain region for processing olfactory information. Within the OB, many local circuit interneurons, including axonless granule cells, function to facilitate fine odor discrimination. How interneurons interact with principal cells to affect bulbar processing is not known, but the mechanism is likely to be different from that in sensory cortical regions because the OB lacks an obvious topographical organization. Neighboring glomerular columns, representing inputs from different receptor neuron subtypes, typically have different odor tuning. Determining the spatial scale over which interneurons such as granule cells can affect principal cells is a critical step toward understanding how the OB operates. We addressed this question by assaying inhibitory synchrony using intracellular recordings from pairs of principal cells with different intersomatic spacing. We found, in acute rat OB slices from both sexes, that inhibitory synchrony is evident in the spontaneous synaptic input in mitral cells (MCs) separated up to 220 μm (300 μm with elevated K+). At all intersomatic spacing assayed, inhibitory synchrony was dependent on Na+ channels, suggesting that action potentials in granule cells function to coordinate GABA release at relatively distant dendrodendritic synapses formed throughout the dendritic arbor. Our results suggest that individual granule cells are able to influence relatively large groups of MCs and tufted cells belonging to clusters of at least 15 glomerular modules, providing a potential mechanism to integrate signals reflecting a wide variety of odorants.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Inhibitory circuits in the olfactory bulb (OB) play a major role in odor processing, especially during fine odor discrimination. However, how inhibitory networks enhance olfactory function, and over what spatial scale they operate, is not known. Interneurons are potentially able to function on both a highly localized, synapse-specific level and on a larger, spatial scale that encompasses many different glomerular channels. Although recent indirect evidence has suggested a relatively localized functional role for most inhibition in the OB, in the present study, we used paired intracellular recordings to demonstrate directly that inhibitory local circuits operate over large spatial scales by using fast action potentials to link GABA release at many different synaptic contacts formed with principal cells.
Keywords: brain slice, coincident inhibition, patch clamp, synaptic transmission
Introduction
Inhibitory local circuits play a central role in processing olfactory information. In insects, blockade of inhibitory function in the antennal lobe, the first processing region of olfactory information, impairs selectively the normal ability of the these animals to make fine distinctions between related similar odors while leaving intact the ability to distinguish between unrelated olfactory stimuli (Stopfer et al., 1997). Recent work in the olfactory bulb (OB), the mammalian equivalent to the antennal lobe, has demonstrated parallel findings when selectively perturbing inhibition onto the principal neurons mitral cells (MCs) and tufted cells (TCs) (Abraham et al., 2010). These studies suggest that olfactory information can be processed through at least two distinct streams–either a hardwired pathway that does not require extensive local inhibitory interactions but which reveals only relatively coarse distinctions among odors or a more complex circuit involving functions mediated by inhibitory interneurons that facilitates fine distinctions. The latter pathway may also be a key site of olfactory learning within the bulb because previous work has found both LTP- and spike-timing-dependent plasticity on excitatory synapses onto granule cells, the primary type of GABAergic interneuron the OB (Gao and Strowbridge, 2009; Nissant et al., 2009). Granule cells also receive a large proportion of top-down and neuromodulatory input (Luskin and Price, 1983; Davis and Macrides, 1981), suggesting a role of behavioral state in regulating inhibition. However, how inhibitory interneurons such as GCs function to enhance olfactory performance is not known.
Recent experimental and computational studies of OB circuitry has led to two divergent views of GC function: (1) that GCs function through locally mediated inhibition with minimal integration of information across processing streams and (2) that GCs operate by interconnecting principal cells belonging to different sensory channels, enabling the circuit to represent more complex or abstract features than either MCs or TCs (Carey et al., 2015; Fukunaga et al., 2014; Arevian et al., 2008; McIntyre and Cleland, 2016). In the former model, GC function would likely enhance fine odor discrimination via actions on nearby principal cells belonging the same or close glomerular modules, whereas the latter models allows for extensive cross-channel synaptic interactions. Direct imaging of Ca2+ responses in GCs has demonstrated that olfactory stimuli appear to activate subregions within the apical dendritic arbor without strongly depolarizing the interneuron cell body (Wienisch and Murthy, 2016). These results are consistent with previous in vitro imaging and recording studies suggesting that that GC dendrites have the ability to generate spatially localized spikes (or spikelets; Egger et al., 2003, 2005; Zelles et al., 2006; Bywalez et al., 2015), providing a potential mechanism for highly localized MC/GC feedback inhibition. Other work supporting a spatially restricted role for GCs includes the observation that some forms of emergent network oscillations that appear to be dependent on GCs are not phase locked with somatic spiking in GCs (Lagier et al., 2004). Computational models of the circuits that could underlie these oscillations (Brea et al., 2009) support the hypothesis that the primary output mode of GCs is highly localized, perhaps restricted to a single GC spine or a small group of nearby spines on the same GC dendritic branch.
Because the GC dendritic arbor only spans a relatively narrow range of GL (∼115 μm, representing two to four glomeruli; Dhawale et al., 2010; Ke et al., 2013; see Materials and Methods), alternate mechanisms that propose GCs operate across many glomerular columns depend on the ability of spiking activity to trigger GABA release at multiple synapses at remote locations within the dendritic arbor, triggering synchronized inhibitory responses in pairs of principal cells. Although long range, GC mediated inhibition has been shown to shape MC firing in response to distant MC activity (Arevian et al., 2008), direct recordings demonstrating synchronized inhibitory input over different spatial scales have been performed rarely and only under specialized conditions following tetanic electrical stimulation (Schoppa, 2006) or after pharmacological treatments that increase network excitability (Schmidt and Strowbridge, 2014). It is not known to what extent synchronized inhibition functions to couple principal cells under physiological conditions and if such synchronization is spike dependent.
Through paired intracellular recordings from OB principal cells, we demonstrated that inhibitory synchronization occurs frequently and is dependent on fast voltage-gated Na+ channels. Divergent inhibitory inputs can functionally associate principal cells belonging to different subtypes (MCs and TCs) with somatic separations up to 300 μm, likely including principal cells belonging to at least 15 different glomerular columns. This study shows for the first time that inhibition can link a wide spatial range of glomerular columns in the absence of external synchronizing input, potentially providing a mechanism of how population codes for different odorants could be refined by local inhibitory circuits.
Materials and Methods
Slice preparation.
Horizontal OB slices 300 μm thick were made from ketamine-anesthetized postnatal day 14 (P14)–P25 Sprague Dawley rats of both sexes as described previously (Balu et al., 2007; Pressler et al., 2013). Slices were incubated for 30 min at 30°C and then at room temperature until use. All experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines approved by the Case Western Reserve University Animal Care and Use Committee.
Electrophysiology.
Slices were placed in a recording chamber and superfused with oxygenated artificial CSF (ACSF) at a rate of 1.5 ml/min. Recordings were made between 29°C and 32°C. ACSF consisted of the following (in mm): 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.23 NaH2PO4, 1.2 MgSO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, and 2.5 CaCl2 equilibrated with 95% O2/5% CO2. The K+ concentration was elevated in “high-K ASCF” by increasing the KCl concentration to 6 mm. All whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were made with Axopatch 1C or 1D amplifiers (Molecular Devices) using borosilicate glass pipettes (WPI) of impedances ranging from 2 to 5 MΩ pulled on a P-97 pipette puller (Sutter Instruments). Recordings were low-pass filtered at 5 kHz (FLA-01; Cygus Technology) and digitized at 10 kHz using an ITC-18 computer interface (Instrutech) connected to a PC operating Windows 7 using custom software.
Slices were imaged using IR-DIC optics on Zeiss Axioskop FS1 or Olympus BX51WI upright microscopes. Live two-photon imaging was performed using a custom-built laser-scanning system, as described previously (Balu et al., 2007; Gao and Strowbridge, 2009; Pressler and Strowbridge, 2006). Neuronal cell type was determined based on IR-DIC morphology and soma laminar location. Cell type classification was confirmed in a subset of experiments using two-photon reconstructions as described in the Cellular Reconstructions section of the Methods. Somatic separation was measured using a calibrated eyepiece reticule. We used two-photon imaging to reconstruct the apical dendritic trees in four MC/MC paired recordings, including three paired recordings that had above-chance coincident inhibition.
Voltage-clamp recordings were made with an internal solution containing the following (in mm): 140 CsCl, 4 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 2 EGTA, 4 MgATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 10 phosphocreatine, and 5 QX-314 with a pH of 7.3 and an osmolarity of 290 mmol/kg. The cesium-chloride solution was used to reverse the chloride gradient. Alexa Fluor 594 (10 μM; Invitrogen) was added to the internal solution in experiments using live two-photon visualization. All drugs were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich except TTX, which was from Calbiochem, and gabazine, which was from Ascent. All drugs were prepared from aliquots stored at −20°C except for TTX, which was prepared from a stock solution kept at 4°C. The drugs were added to the bath by changing the external solution source.
Experimental design and statistical analysis.
Spontaneous IPSCs were detected and measured automatically using a custom algorithm used in previous studies (Schmidt and Strowbridge, 2014; Larimer and Strowbridge, 2008; Larimer and Strowbridge, 2010; Hyde and Strowbridge, 2012). Detected events were confirmed by visual analysis. Event detection and data analysis routines were implemented in Python (version 3.5). Multiple trials (typically 10–20 episodes each lasting 10 s; mean 16.9 trials) were acquired for each condition in each experiment. Summary data are presented as mean ± SEM except where noted. Membrane variance was computed over 250 ms duration windows.
Cross-correlograms (examples are shown in Figs. 2D, 4D) were computed from the onset lags between all detected IPSC pairs and analyzed between −10 and 10 ms in 0.4 ms time bins. To determine statistical significance, the maximal cross-correlation value (averaged over 3 bins, 1.2 ms) reported in the actual data was compared with surrogate data created by permuting inter-IPSC intervals. The mean of 100 interval permutation runs are presented in example cross-correlation plots (e.g., Fig. 2D, left, gray plot). The probability that the maximal cross-correlation metric in the actual data was larger than expected by chance was computed empirically using three methods. In the first approach, we generated a distribution of 2000 maximal cross-correlation values from surrogate data in which the inter-IPSC intervals were permuted. For example, if the peak cross-correlation value obtained in one experiment was larger than 1900 of 2000 cross-correlation values obtained in the interval permutation runs, then the probability of inhibitory coincidence would be assigned as 0.05 (100/2000). This analysis procedure was repeated five times for each experiment with the median recorded as the interval permutation-based p-value. Throughout the study, we express randomization-derived p-values as −1 * ln(P) where more positive numbers reflect lower p-values. The upper limit on this metric [7.6 = −1 * ln(1/2000)] reflects experiments in which the peak cross-correlation values were lower in all randomization runs than in the actual (nonpermuted) dataset.
In the second randomization control method, we used trial shuffling to estimate whether each experiment had beyond-chance rates of inhibitory coincidence. In this test, we randomly selected different trials for cell A and for cell B from the same experiment, creating a dataset composed exclusively of nonsimultaneous records (e.g., trial 1 from cell A compared with trial 3 from cell B). This process was repeated 2000 times in each experiment to generate a p-value based on the distribution of trial shuffled recordings. As with the interval permutation control procedure, we repeated the trial-shuffling analysis five times and recorded the median of those runs as the trial shuffling-based p-value. Both the interval permutation and trial shuffling control procedures abolished the peak near 0 ms present in experiments with obvious inhibitory synchrony determined by visual inspection of raw intracellular records. Figures 2 and 4 illustrate cross-correlation plots after interval permutation and trial shuffling on two example paired recordings with frequent near-coincident IPSCs.
Both interval permutation and trial-shuffling approaches generated similar metrics expressing the degree of inhibitory synchrony evident our primary dataset of 89 paired MC/MC recordings assayed under standard ACSF conditions (r = 0.92; p < 10−30; correlation performed on the −ln(P) metric as used throughout the study). We also computed a dual randomization p-value where we combined both randomization methods by computing bootstrap distributions based on interval permutations of trial shuffled data. Using a standard 0.05 threshold for statistical significance, the dual randomization p-value generated the most conservative ranking of inhibitory synchrony across experiments (N = 17 experiments with empirical p-values lower than this threshold compared with N = 21 for the interval permutation approach and N = 23 using only trial shuffling. Sixteen experiments had p-values < 0.05 in all three randomization tests. Rather than report separate results from interval permutation and trial shuffling procedures, we only report only the final dual randomization p-value in the text and in the illustrations.
We set a threshold for statistical significance at 0.015 using the dual randomization p-value throughout the study. This value balanced competing goals of minimizing the false rejection rate while controlling the familywise error rate. A more conservative approach, such as Bonferroni corrections, would adjust the significance threshold to 0.05/N where N is the number of independent tests performed. This simple approach proved unworkable because the very low thresholds in our large N dataset (p = 0.00056 for our population of 89 MC/MC paired recordings) rejected many experiments with obvious inhibitory coincidence and central peaks on cross-correlation plots that were abolished by both interval permutation and trial shuffling. As noted in other work, this approach often rejects true positives and therefore tends to reduce statistical power (Gelman et al., 2012). Using a Bonferroni correction approach also would lead to different thresholds for statistical significance across the different groups of experiments. The empirical p-value approach that we followed has been used in previous studies (Sham and Purcell, 2014; Westfall, 2011; Westfall and Young, 1993; Narayanan and Laubach, 2009) and allowed us to specify a uniform threshold (0.015) that was more stringent than the p-value traditionally used with single tests (0.05). Using all three randomization approaches, we found a large drop-off in p-values near this threshold (from <0.0101 in all experiments labeled significant to 0.042 in the best experiment labeled as nonsignificant). In our primary dataset with 89 paired MC/MC recordings, using this threshold resulted in 16 experiments with significant inhibitory synchrony. In Figure 2E, we also estimated the degree of inhibitory synchrony using the clipped cross-intensity function approach (CIF; Hahnloser, 2007) that we have used in a related study (Schmidt and Strowbridge, 2014). This metric was based on the count of near-coincident IPSPs (onset times within 3 ms) divided by the total number of IPSCs in the cell with the fewest IPSCs. As with the cross-correlation method, we computed p-values for each experiment empirically based on a distribution of CIF metrics generated in surrogate data in which inter-IPSC intervals were permuted.
The estimates of the rates of above-chance inhibitory coincidence presented in Figure 7D were computed by subtracting the rates of near-coincident IPSCs (onset latencies within 3 ms) before and after trial shuffling. We plotted the rate of near-coincident IPSCs beyond expected by chance as a percentage of the TTX-sensitive IPSC rate in each cell. For example, if the actual rate of near-coincident IPSCs was 2 Hz which then dropped to 0.5 Hz after trial shuffling, then the rate of “beyond-chance” near-coincident IPSCs would be 1.5 Hz. If the spike-dependent rate of spontaneous IPSCs was 15 Hz in one of the cells in the paired recording (reflecting 20 Hz spontaneous IPSCs recorded in ACSF and 5 Hz in TTX), then “beyond chance” coincident IPSCs would constitute 10% of the spike-driven spontaneous IPSCs in that neuron.
Cellular reconstructions.
The horizontal span of the GC dendritic arbor was calculated from 19 published GC reconstructions shown in the following figures: Figure 2 in Price and Powell (1970); Figures 1C, 2A, 2H, and 3A in Burton and Urban (2015); Figure 2 in Egger et al. (2005); Figure 1 in Wellis and Scott (1990); Figures 4A, 5A, and 6A in Woolf et al. (1991); and Figure 1M–O in Petreanu and Alvarez-Buylla (2002). To account for shrinkage during fixation, the measured width was increased by 15% (Tukker et al., 2013; Korogod et al., 2015). The mean GC dendritic width that we estimated from these published studies was 113 ± 11.1 μm with a SD of 48.5 μm.
We estimated the thickness of the GL in 10 OB slices visualized using a 5× objective (185 ± 21 μm; mean ± SD). We estimated the glomerular packing density using our measured GL thickness and the mean glomerular diameter reported in two anatomical studies (75 μm; Dhawale et al., 2010; Ke et al., 2013). The horizontal span of MC lateral dendrites was estimated from maximal two-photon Z-stack projections (N = 33 lateral dendrites from 21 visualized MCs; 349 ± 180 μm; mean ± SD. The actual length of the lateral dendrites was ∼10% larger than the horizontal span (the component of distance within the mitral cell layer, MCL), reflecting angled trajectory of these dendrites from the cell body into the external plexiform layer (EPL).
Results
We divided our analysis of inhibitory input onto principal cells in this study into two sections, starting with determining the properties of spontaneous ISPCs in MCs and TCs (Fig. 1) and then focusing on measuring coincident inhibition across pairs of intracelluarly recorded principal cells to assess the spatial scale of inhibitory synchrony in the OB (Figs. 2–7). Both MCs and TCs receive frequent spontaneous IPSCs, shown as inward currents in the example voltage-clamp recordings in Figure 1, A and B, which used CsCl-based intracellular solutions that reversed the normal chloride gradient. Spontaneous inwards currents were abolished by the GABA-A receptor antagonist gabazine (10 μM; 96.6% reduction in membrane current variance; Fig. 1C), indicating that spontaneous IPSCs form the majority of the detectable synaptic input onto OB principal cells under these recording conditions. Most of the detected IPSCs in MCs appeared to originate from synapses onto secondary dendrites and not from synapses onto the distal apical dendrite. In a subset of 21 MCs visualized using live two-photon microscopy, we found only a small decrease in the rate of spontaneous IPSCs in MCs with apical dendrites truncated before they reached the GL (12.7 ± 1.5 Hz; N = 15) compared with MCs with intact apical dendrites (14.6 ± 1.6 Hz; N = 6: p = 0.49; unpaired t test; mean ± SEM).
We first compared the baseline frequency and amplitude of inhibitory inputs onto both classes of principal cells. MCs received a significantly higher rate of spontaneous IPSCs than TCs (Fig. 1D,E), consistent with results from a recent comparison of bulbar neurons (Geramita et al., 2016). The mean IPSC amplitude was modestly smaller in TCs than MCs (Fig. 1F), whereas the rising-phase kinetics were similar in both cell types (Fig. 1G). In both cell types, blockade of voltage-gated Na+ channels with 1 μM TTX dramatically decreased spontaneous ISPC frequency (Fig. 1D, middle), suggesting that most synaptic responses resulted from Na+-based spiking in GCs or other classes of GABAergic interneurons. Enhancing GC excitability by stimulating α-adrenegic receptors with phenylephrine (PE; 10 μM) (McLennan, 1971; Salmoiraghi et al., 1964; Araneda and Firestein, 2006; Zimnik et al., 2013) was able to restore basal IPSC rates in both principal cell types even in the presence of TTX (Fig. 1D, right).
Surprisingly, throughout these manipulations, the ratio of spontaneous IPSC frequencies between MCs and TCs remained relatively constant (Fig. 1E), suggesting that the two cell types differed primarily in the density of inhibitory connections with GCs. The proportion of spike-driven IPSCs also was similar in both cell types (mean of 70% in MCs vs 61% in TCs; Fig. 1H). We observed a strong correlation between miniature IPSC rates and basal spontaneous IPSC rates that was more pronounced in MCs (Fig. 1I; r = 0.59; p = 10−7) than TCs (Fig. 1J). As with the other measures of inhibitory function, we found more variability among TC IPSC rates, including an outlier recording with very high spontaneous IPSC rates. Excluding this outlier TC, the correlation between miniature IPSC rates and basal spontaneous IPSC rates was statistically significant (r = 0.532, p = 0.013; r = 0.251 including outlier value). Together, these results suggest that the basal inhibitory synaptic tone in MCs and TCs is determined primarily by the density of GABAergic synapses on the secondary dendrites (and potentially somatic sites). Both cell types likely sample from similar pools of spontaneously active interneurons, accounting for the similar properties of individual IPSCs in MCs and TCs, the similar fraction of TTX-sensitive IPSCs, and the ability of miniature IPSC rates to predict the basal spontaneous IPSC rates. The difference in inhibitory synaptic tone that we and others (Geramita et al., 2016) have observed in OB principal cells likely reflects fewer synaptic contacts on TCs versus MCs, consistent with previous anatomical studies (Mori et al., 1983; Orona et al., 1984).
Coincident inhibition between pairs of OB principal cells
After determining baseline properties of spontaneous IPSCs in MCs and TCs, we next recorded from pairs of principal cells (N = 89 MC/MC and 24 MC/TC) separated by variable distances to determine the spatial scale of over which synchronized inhibition occurs in the OB. A subset (∼15%) of paired MC recording had multiple coincident IPSCs evident by visual inspection of intracellular records, such as the example paired recording in Figure 2, A–C. We used two independent control procedures to determine whether the rate of coincident IPSCs was greater than expected by chance in each paired MC/MC recording. We first computed the cross-correlation based on IPSC onset times in both cells and compared the maximal cross-correlation function (“actual”) to randomized data in which the intervals between recorded IPSCs were permuted (“interval permutation”; Fig. 2D, left). In the second analysis method, we compared the maximal cross-correlation function computed from actual data with the same analysis performed trial shuffled recorded (Fig. 2D, right; see Materials and Methods), generating a dataset with physiological IPSC timing statistics but no actual coincidences because only nonsimultaneously recorded datasets were analyzed (e.g., trial 1 from MC-A with trial 2 from MC-B). We computed the probability that actual maximal cross-correlation function was larger than expected by chance by combining the interval permutation and trial-shuffling procedures 2000 times; the threshold for significance used was p < 0.015; see Materials and Methods for details). Sixteen of 89 MC/MC paired recordings met this criterion for above-chance rates of near-coincident IPSCs. All 16 MC/MC paired recordings identified as having inhibitory synchronization assayed through the cross-correlation method also exhibited statistically significant rates of coincident IPSCs when tested using the cross-intensity function method used in previous work (Fig. 2E; Schmidt and Strowbridge, 2014).
Near-coincident synaptic inputs identified by our analysis (which captured onset latencies that differed by up to 20 ms) were nearly synchronous in the subgroup of 16 MC/MC paired recording classified as statistically significant. The mean latency between IPSC onset times across the two cells was 0.05 ± 0.13 ms (range −1.0 to 1.4 ms; N = 16 experiments; histogram shown in Fig. 2F). There was no difference between the overall mean IPSC rates in the 16 significant MC/MC paired recordings and the other 73 paired recordings (Fig. 2G). The inhibitory synchrony we observe was unlikely to arise from glomerular-layer local circuits because three of the 16 significant MC/MC paired recordings included least one MC with a truncated apical dendrite, reducing the contributions from GL interneurons (example shown in Fig. 2A–C). All three of the paired recordings that included MCs with truncated apical dendrites had highly significant inhibitory synchrony (p < 5 × 10−4, the lowest possible p-value in our bootstrap analysis; somatic separations of 50, 50, and 80 μm), suggesting that coincident inhibitory input does not originate from GL interneurons.
We determined the spatial scale of coincident inhibition by plotting the p-value computed from the interval permutation analysis against the separation between the two MC somata (Fig. 3A,B). Most MC/MC paired recordings with significant inhibitory synchrony occurred between closely spaced neurons (intersoma distances <100 μm). The maximal somatic separation that generated inhibitory synchrony beyond chance levels in standard ACSF was 220 μm. When restricted to intersomatic spacing <220 μm, we still observed no difference in the mean IPSC rate between significant and nonsignificant MC/MCs pairs (p = 0.71; unpaired t test; t(57) = 0.38), suggesting that our metric of inhibitory synchrony was not simply revealing an underlying bias toward MCs with high (or low) spontaneous IPSC rates. The distribution of p-values estimated from interval permutation in trial-shuffled data (all <0.015; Fig. 3B, right; equivalently, >4.2 on the scale in Fig. 3B) was similar to the distribution of the actual nonsignificant MC/MCs (non-trial-shuffled data; Fig. 3B, left, black symbols). These results suggest that our dataset of MC/MC paired recordings comprised two populations: a minority (18%) that received coincident inhibition, presumably reflecting divergent output from one or more spontaneously spiking interneurons, and MC/MC pairs that received no detectable inhibitory coincidence.
Although most of the MC/MC pairs we found that have above-chance rates of coincident IPSCs were relatively closely spaced (mean somatic separation 80.0 ± 14.7 μm; N = 16 pairs), this estimate is biased toward nearby pairs because of slice truncation effects; relatively short-range divergent projections from one interneuron are more likely to be preserved within 300 μm thick OB slices than longer range projections. Therefore, the most biologically relevant parameter reflecting the spatial scale of coincident inhibition is likely to be the furthest extent over which we can detect coincident inhibition. This parameter (up to 220 μm MC somatic separation) suggests that individual interneurons can couple MCs associated with different glomeruli (mean glomerular diameter 75 μm; Dhawale et al., 2010; Ke et al., 2013). We confirmed this was possible by recording and visualizing two MCs with intact apical dendritic tufts that terminated in nearby but different glomeruli (Fig. 4A–C; intersoma distance 40 μm, apical dendritic tufts separated by 130 μm). The cross-correlation function in this paired recording (Fig. 4D, left; p = 0.0005) had a peak at 0.2 ms.
Under basal (unstimulated) conditions, we also observed inhibitory synchrony between heterogeneous pairs of principal cells (experiments including one MC and one TC recording; Fig. 5A). Figure 5B illustrates an example of coincident IPSCs recorded in a MC/TC pair with intersoma spacing of 80 μm (measured as the horizontal distance through the MCL ignoring the different distances in the MCL-to-GL direction). Using the same criteria applied to MC/MC paired recordings, three of 24 MC/TCs had statistically significant inhibitory coincidence (Fig. 5C), including the example recording presented in Figure 5B. The three significant MC/TC pairs were separated by between 20 and 150 μm (span along the MCL). The mean somatic separation in the 3 MC/TC pairs with coincident inhibition was 86.7 ± 33.1 μm. The mean latency between near-coincident IPSCs was 0.2 ± 0.33 ms (range, −0.2 to 1 ms). As with the MC/MC paired recording that showed elevated rates of near-coincident IPSCs, there was no difference in the mean IPSC rates between MC/TC pairs with and without demonstrable inhibitory synchrony (p = 0.09; unpaired t test). These results suggest that not all inhibitory local circuits in the OB are segregated into MC- and TC-specific subgroups. Instead, individual GCs or EPL interneurons (Kato et al., 2013) appear to innervate both populations of principal cells.
Spike-dependent inhibitory synchrony
We next investigated whether near-coincident IPSCs recorded in pairs of principal cells required Na+-based action potentials in the common presynaptic interneurons. Bath application of TTX (1 μM) abolished inhibitory synchrony in all 16 MC/MC pairs with statistically significant rates of near-coincident IPSCs (Fig. 5D). No previously nonsignificant paired recording experiments became significant through this treatment. The loss of inhibitory synchrony did not simply reflect the lower overall rate of spontaneous IPSCs in TTX because addition of the GC-stimulating α receptor agonist PE (10 μM; McLennan, 1971; Salmoiraghi et al., 1964; Araneda and Firestein, 2006; Zimnik et al., 2013) failed to promote synchronous inhibition in MC paired recordings (Fig. 4D; 0/44 experiments). We found similar results in heterogeneous MC/TC pairs in which TTX abolished inhibitory synchrony in the three paired MC/TC recordings with statistically significant rates of near-coincident IPSCs (Fig. 4E). Because voltage-gated Na+ channels were already blocked in MC and TC voltage-clamp recordings (because of QX-314 in the internal solution), these results suggest that synchronous IPSCs resulted from spontaneous spiking in GABAergic interneurons that form divergent synaptic connections onto MC and MC/TC ensembles.
If near-coincident IPSCs recorded in pairs of principal cells reflect spontaneous spiking in presynaptic GABAegic interneurons, we hypothesized that passively depolarizing interneurons by elevating extracellular K+ concentration (from 3 to 6 mm; “high-K” ACSF) would enhance inhibitory synchrony. The relatively weak passive depolarization produced by this treatment, estimated to shift the K+ equilibrium potential by +18 mV, did not preclude repetitive firing in 3/3 GCs tested (AP amplitude reduced by 3.5 ± 0.5 mV and AP full width increased by 0.39 ± 0.04 ms in 6 mm K+). As shown in the example records in Figure 6A, high-K ACSF increased both the rate of spontaneous IPSCs in MCs and also the incidence of near-coincident IPSCs. Of the 19 paired MC experiments in high-K ACSF, nine (47%) had statistically significant inhibitory synchrony, including the paired recording shown in Figure 6A. Two of the nine significant MC/MC recordings already had significant rates of inhibitory synchrony in control conditions before treatment with high-K ACSF (Fig. 6B). In the remaining seven significant MC/MC recordings, no inhibitory synchrony was evident except in high-K ACSF. The mean IPSC latency in the 9 MC paired recordings with near-coincident IPSCs in high-K ACSF was 0.24 ± 0.19 ms (range −0.6 to 1.4 ms). The ability of slightly elevated K+ levels to increase the frequency that we observed coincident inhibition suggests that, under physiological conditions (3 mm [K+]), the slow rate of GC spontaneous spiking limits our ability to detect synchronous inhibition.
Our results thus far demonstrated divergent effects of passive depolarization with high-K ACSF, which promoted inhibitory synchrony, and activation of adrenergic receptors with PE, which stimulated GABA release (increasing miniature IPSC rates), but did not trigger more frequent coincident IPSCs. Consistent with this model, PE combined with high-K ACSF increased spontaneous IPSC rates (Fig. 6C) but abolished most inhibitory synchrony (only 1/10 experiments with PE + high-K ACSF has statistically significant inhibitory synchrony; Fig. 6D). As with our earlier results suggesting a requirement for Na+-based action potentials, inhibitory synchrony present in high-K ACSF was abolished in TTX (Fig. 6D; zero of nine experiments including three MC paired recordings that had statistically significant inhibitory synchrony in high-K ACSF).
Although high-K ACSF did not affect the amplitudes of spontaneous IPSCs recorded in MCs, PE triggered a modest but statistically significant decrease in mean IPSC amplitude (29% decrease; Fig. 6E). This difference primarily reflected an overrepresentation of IPSCs with very small amplitudes (see IPSC amplitude histograms in Fig. 6F, inset). Therefore, we tested whether the lack of statistically significant inhibitory synchrony following treatment with PE and high-K ACSF reflected a “dilution” effect that prevented detecting above-chance inhibitory synchrony. We tested for this possibility by restricting our cross-correlation analysis to spontaneous IPSCs greater than an arbitrary minimum amplitude. As shown in the plot in Figure 6F, we failed to find statistically significant inhibitory synchrony when analyzing different subsets of large-amplitude IPSCs in the 10 MC pairs with no evident inhibitory synchrony in high-K ACSF. Eliminating spontaneous IPSCs <50 pA also abolished inhibitory synchrony in the one MC/MC paired recording with statistically significant rates of near-coincident IPSCs (Fig. 6G), suggesting that some synchronous inhibitory inputs generated relatively small-amplitude (<50 pA) IPSCs.
Passively depolarizing bulbar neurons with high-K ACSF also increased the spatial extent of inhibitory synchrony. As shown in Figure 7A, MC pairs with intersoma distances up to 300 μm exhibited statistically significant inhibitory synchrony (versus up to 220 μm in control ACSF). As in previous control analyses, trial shuffling eliminated all statistically significant inhibitory synchrony (Fig. 7A, right). The overall proportion of MC/MC paired recordings with significant inhibitory synchrony increased from 18% in control ACSF to 47% in high-K ACSF (Fig. 7B). We found an even more pronounced increase in the proportion of experiments with significant inhibitory synchrony when only considering MC pairs with intersoma separations >100 μm (Fig. 7B, right bars). Consistent with this hypothesis, we found a statistically significant difference between plots of cumulative intersoma distance in experiments with inhibitory synchrony in control and high-K ACSF (Fig. 7C, left). The midpoint in MC separations with inhibitory synchrony (similar to EC50 estimates) shifted from 60 μm in control to 140 μm in high-K ACSF (arrowheads along the x-axis in Fig. 7C, left). The somatic separation where we found a 50% probability of detecting above-chance coincident IPSCs in control ACSF (60 μm) was similar to the mean somatic separation in the 16 MC/MC pairs coincident inhibition described above (80 μm). There was no difference in the cumulative intersoma distance plots when considering all MC/MC pairs (regardless of whether they had inhibitory synchrony) between control and high-K ACSF (p = 0.71; Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; Fig. 7C, right).
Although high-K ACSF increased the fraction of experiments with inhibitory synchrony and the spatial extent of inhibitory synchrony, this treatment did not affect the proportion of putative synchronized IPSCs within each recording. Actual measurements of near-coincident IPSCs (e.g., number of IPSCs in one recording with a corresponding IPSC within 3 ms in the other, simultaneous recording) reflect the sum of two different frequencies: the rate of real (“biological”) coincident IPSCs, presumably corresponding to coordinated release of GABA at two different synapses from the same interneuron, and the rate of chance coincidences. We calculated the “excess coincidence rate” reflecting an estimate of the underlying frequency of actual coordinated GABA release events by subtracting the rate of random coincidences (computed by permuting inter-IPSC intervals) from the measured rate of near-coincident IPSCs. The excess inhibitory coincidence rate modestly increased from 0.70 ± 0.14 Hz in the 16 MC/MC with statistically significant inhibitory synchrony in control ACSF to 1.62 ± 0.69 Hz in the 9 MC/MC pairs with inhibitory synchrony recorded in high-K ACSF (p = 0.12; unpaired t test). However, expressed as a proportion of the ongoing spontaneous IPSC frequency in each condition, there was almost no difference in the rate of excess IPSCs between control (8.4% of all spontaneous IPSCs) and high-K ACSF (7.5%; Fig. 7D). As expected, we found near-zero rates of excess coincident IPSCs after trial shuffling or when assaying MC/MC pairs that did not have statistically significant inhibitory synchrony (Fig. 7D, right columns).
The similar percentage of near-coincident events detected under different levels of rates of ongoing spontaneous IPSCs suggests that inhibitory synchrony reflected inputs from small but constant fraction of presynaptic inputs. This 8% estimate could arise from a single spontaneously active interneuron that innervated both MCs assuming each MC also receives input from ∼12 other active interneurons (or from two convergent interneurons and ∼23 other active interneurons, etc.). This percentage would remain constant during passive depolarization tests if the treatment had a similar effect in all presynaptic GCs. If the inhibitory coincidence that we report arises on average from a single active interneuron, then this provides an estimate of the total number of active presynaptic interneurons each MC receives (∼13).
Urban and colleagues (Galán et al., 2006) have studied the functional effect of coincident inhibition onto pairs of OB principal cells by injecting fixed patterns of randomly timed α functions (mimicking GABAergic IPSCs) with defined percentages of coincident inputs applied to both cells. They found that even relatively low percentages of coincident hyperpolarizing inputs (20%, similar to the ∼8% we observe) were effective in generating synchronized MC spikes (see Fig. 4A in Galán et al., 2006). Presumably, the brief hyperpolarizing current injections used in that study evoked synchronized rebound spiking in both MCs (Balu and Strowbridge, 2007, Balu et al., 2007; Schoppa, 2006), although the specific biophysical mechanisms involved were not defined. Together with the present results, this work suggests that even the relatively low frequency of coincident inhibition generated by spontaneously active presynaptic interneurons can generate a functionally significant signature in the spike output of the OB (transiently synchronized MC spikes).
Discussion
In this study, we assayed OB synaptic connectivity patterns using paired recordings and make three primary conclusions. First, we find that the frequency of spontaneous IPSCs is strongly correlated with the frequency of TTX-resistant IPSC in MCs, suggesting that most inhibitory inputs to MCs behave as a uniform population. Second, we find that Na+-spike-driven coincident inhibition can link pairs of principal cells belonging to different glomerular columns as well as different types of principal cells (MCs and TCs). These results provide the first direct demonstration that distant pairs of principal cells can be functionally linked by coincident inhibition under physiological conditions. This finding is consistent with previous results from Schoppa (2006), who observed inhibitory coupling between distant MCs after tetanic electrical stimulation with near-coincident IPSCs evident in ∼40% of nearby MCs based on the presence of a central peak in cross-correlation plots. However, direct pathway stimulation can trigger synchronized firing in multiple interneurons, potentially generating coincident inhibitory postsynaptic responses in MCs. These types of “common driver” mechanisms are unlikely to account for inhibitory synchrony apparent under resting (nonstimulated) conditions used in the present study, leaving spontaneous Na+ spiking in GCs that are presynaptic to both MCs as the most likely explanation for above-chance rates of coincident IPSC. Finally, we found that coincident inhibition onto MCs depends on Na+ spiking in GCs and can be disrupted by strong depolarizing stimuli. Together, these results suggest that bulbar interneurons can play an important role in the olfactory system by coordinating responses in principal cells belonging to different glomerular channels. These synaptic interactions can function to modulate MC and TC firing patterns to reflect contextual information associated with the larger glomerular network activated by an odor.
Difference in inhibitory tone between MCs and TCs
To our knowledge, our study is the first to compare inhibitory tone in MCs and TCs under normal physiological conditions and when Na+-based spiking is abolished by TTX. Our finding that TCs receive less pronounced inhibition (approximately threefold fewer IPSCs) is consistent with several previous reports that assayed the rate of spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic responses (Geramita et al., 2016; Ezeh et al., 1993). This different in spontaneous IPSC/P rate could be explained by preferential innervation of MCs by more active interneurons compared with TCs or by a difference in overall density of inhibitory innervation in the principal cell types. Our observation that the threefold difference in IPSC rate between MCs and TCs is maintained when Na+ spiking is blocked with TTX and then when GC excitability is enhanced by PE suggests that this difference reflects primarily a higher density of inhibitory synaptic inputs on MCs. Because our ability to detect spontaneous IPSCs is likely limited to the synapses within 100–150 μm of the somata (Lowe, 2002) differences in the length of the secondary dendrites between MCs and TCs are unlikely to influence the incidence of coincident inhibition detected.
Secondary dendrites of MCs and TCs also arborize in different zones within the EPL, where they contact different subpopulations of GCs (Orona et al., 1983; Mori et al., 1983). Our finding that coincident inhibition can be detected in MC/TC pairs suggests that the distinction between MC- and TC-targeting GCs is not absolute. Diminished inhibitory tone in TCs may reflect a lower density of reciprocal contacts on TC-targeting GCs. Although Greer and colleagues (Bartel et al., 2015) found a similar density of reciprocal synapses throughout the EPL, to our knowledge, the density of dendrodendritic synapses formed by different subclasses of GCs has not be been defined. Urban and colleagues (Geramita et al., 2016) found that TCs were more excitable than MCs after glomerular stimulation, suggesting that any reduction in TC-to-GC synaptic density may be compensated for by strong excitatory inputs to TCs.
Synchronized inhibition links principal cells across multiple glomerular columns
We detected synchronized inhibition using standard methods (Narayanan and Laubach, 2009) based on cross-correlation analysis of IPSC onset times and used two different control procedures to estimate the frequency of coincident IPSCs expected by chance in each experiment (interval permutation and trial shuffling). The spontaneous IPSC frequency was only modestly reduced in visualized MCs with apical dendrites truncated before reaching the GL compared with intact MCs, suggesting that most of the inhibitory tone detected in somatic recordings from OB principal cells originates from interneurons outside of the GL, presumably GCs. Although several populations of non-GC interneurons exist in the GCL, these appear to target either TCs (Burton and Urban, 2015) or GCs (Pressler and Strowbridge, 2006; Burton et al., 2017). A recent study (Kato et al., 2013) has found that parvalbumin (PV)-positive interneurons located in the EPL form reciprocal synapses with MCs and therefore could also contribute to the inhibitory synchrony that we observed. However, PV EPL interneurons appear to fire at high rates (>40 Hz in Kato et al., 2013), which is inconsistent with the very low rates of beyond-chance synchronized IPSCs in our MC/MC paired recordings (∼0.7 Hz in ACSF).
Coincident inhibition onto MC/MC and MC/TC pairs was abolished in TTX, suggesting that they likely originate from GABA release sites coupled by Na+-based action potentials in GCs. The absence of detectable inhibitory synchrony in our study was unlikely to occur simply because of the lower IPSC rate in TTX because we also observed no detectable coincident inhibition (beyond chance levels) when GCs were stimulated with PE after Na+ spikes were abolished. This treatment restored near-control rates of spontaneous IPSCs in both MCs and TCs but did not reveal inhibitory synchrony. This finding suggests that, in the absence of Na+-based APs, direct depolarization of GCs does not trigger coordinated GABA release at multiple dendritic spines that we can detect using paired recordings.
Granule cells generate APs in response to depolarizing stimuli that propagate throughout the dendritic arbor (Egger et al., 2003). Because many of the MC pairs that we found that receive synchronized inhibition were separated by distances that are the far extreme of what should be possible given the electrotonic length constant of MC secondary dendrites assayed from focal GABA uncaging (∼75 μm; Lowe, 2002), it is likely that Na+-based APs coupled GABA release at different spines within the GC dendritic tree. This mechanism would explain the ability of TTX to abolish inhibitory coupling in MC/MC and MC/TC paired recordings and the inability of increasing excitability in TTX with PE to recover inhibitory synchrony in pairs known to share a common presynaptic interneuron.
Functional (Wienisch and Murthy, 2016) and computational (Brea et al., 2009) studies have suggested an important role for spatially localized Na+ and Ca2+ spikes in GCs (Egger et al., 2005; Zelles et al., 2006; Bywalez et al., 2015). The extent of spike propagation within GC dendrites represents an attractive mechanism to regulate the degree of inhibitory synchrony in principal cells. At the smallest extreme, individual GC spines are likely able to release GABA autonomously, leading to an increase in asynchronously inhibition in MCs. Removing extracellular Mg2+ greatly facilitates GABA release from GCs by enhancing Ca2+ influx through NMDA receptors located in dendritic spines (Isaacson and Strowbridge, 1998; Schmidt and Strowbridge, 2014). However, this treatment does not lead to an increase in coincident IPSPs, even between nearby pairs of MCs (Schmidt and Strowbridge, 2014), likely reflecting uncoordinated release events at different spines. In contrast to low-Mg2+ treatment, the present study found that increasing extracellular K+ increased both IPSC frequency and the incidence of coincident inhibition. Presumably, elevated K+ increased the frequency of spontaneous spiking in GCs and therefore the frequency of near-simultaneous release of GABA on spines that synapse onto different principal cells. Further increasing the excitability of GCs by combining elevated K+ with PE greatly diminished the incidence of synchronized inhibition. This effect probably reflected depolarization blockade of AP generation in GCs rather than dilution of synchronized IPSCs by an elevated frequency of small-amplitude asynchronous events because restricting our analysis to large-amplitude IPSCs failed to recover evidence for coincident inhibition.
Functional significance of coincident inhibition onto OB principal cells
The central finding in this study is that individual GCs can generate coordinated inhibition onto relatively distant principal cells. This result suggests that GCs can function to influence firing patterns of output neurons belonging to different glomerular columns. Although we demonstrate directly that coincidence inhibition occurs in MCs associated with nearby glomeruli (Fig. 4), the spatial extent over which we observed synchronized inhibition suggests that individual GCs influence activity over multiple glomerular columns. The average diameter of a glomerulus is ∼75 μm (range 50–120 μm; Pinching and Powell, 1971) and a rough estimate of the 2D packing density is 2.5 glomeruli per 100 μm span parallel to the MCL (185 μm estimated GL width). Assuming at least two layers of intact glomeruli per slice (but likely three or four in some slices), leads to a maximal span of 14 glomerular columns per GC (Fig. 7E) and an estimated average connectivity of seven columns per GC. This analysis is based solely on detecting the incidence of coincident IPSCs exceeding chance, so it likely represents an underestimate because infrequently discharging GCs will generate too few coincident IPSCs to be detected in our assay. This analysis also does not account for the variable trajectory of the apical dendrite as it passed through the EPL, which also could enhance the effective glomerular span of GC-mediated inhibition.
Because there is very little chemotopic structure within the GL (Soucy et al., 2009; Fantana et al., 2008; Luo and Katz, 2001; Ma et al., 2012), it is possible for an individual GC to affect the firing patterns of principal cells responding to a wide range of odorants. Inhibitory postsynaptic responses can trigger rebound spikes in MCs (Balu and Strowbridge, 2007, Balu et al., 2007; Desmaisons et al., 1999; Schoppa, 2006), including synchronized APs in pairs of MCs in response to coincident inhibition (Galán et al., 2006). Through synchronous IPSP-triggered rebound spikes, the functional impact of coincident inhibition onto subsets of principal cells is likely magnified and transmitted to downstream cortical regions that detect transiently synchronized MC inputs (Salinas and Sejnowski, 2000). In addition to triggering synchronized firing, coordinated inhibition also may function to sculpt MC discharges to enhance subtle differences evoked by sensory input, facilitating the decorrelation of related odor responses (Friedrich and Laurent, 2001; Cazakoff et al., 2014; Shepherd et al., 2007; Friedrich and Wiechert, 2014; Gschwend et al., 2015). Because most MC/GC and TC/GC synaptic connections in the EPL are reciprocal (Isaacson and Strowbridge, 1998; Rall et al., 1966), our results suggest that individual GCs also receive excitatory input from a relatively large range of sensory neuron subtypes, helping to explain the complex sensory responses observed in GC recordings in vivo (Kato et al., 2012; Fukunaga et al., 2014; Cazakoff et al., 2014; Cang and Isaacson, 2003; Tan et al., 2010; Luo and Katz, 2001; Wellis and Scott, 1990).
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grant R01-DC04285 to B.W.S.). We thank Todd Pressler and Chris Ford for constructive comments on this manuscript.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Abraham NM, Egger V, Shimshek DR, Renden R, Fukunaga I, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH, Klugmann M, Margrie TW, Schaefer AT, Kuner T (2010) Synaptic inhibition in the olfactory bulb accelerates odor discrimination in mice. Neuron 65:399–411. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Araneda RC, Firestein S (2006) Adrenergic enhancement of inhibitory transmission in the accessory olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 26:3292–3298. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4768-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arevian AC, Kapoor V, Urban NN (2008) Activity-dependent gating of lateral inhibition in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 11:80–87. 10.1038/nn2030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balu R, Strowbridge BW (2007) Opposing inward and outward conductances regulate rebound discharges in olfactory mitral cells. J Neurophysiol 97:1959–1968. 10.1152/jn.01115.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balu R, Pressler RT, Strowbridge BW (2007) Multiple modes of synaptic excitation of olfactory bulb granule cells. J Neurosci 27:5621–5632. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4630-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartel DL, Rela L, Hsieh L, Greer CA (2015) Dendrodendritic synapses in the mouse olfactory bulb external plexiform layer. J Comp Neurol 523:1145–1161. 10.1002/cne.23714 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brea JN, Kay LM, Kopell NJ (2009) Biophysical model for gamma rhythms in the olfactory bulb via subthreshold oscillations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:21954–21959. 10.1073/pnas.0910964106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton SD, Urban NN (2015) Rapid feedforward inhibition and asynchronous excitation regulate granule cell activity in the mammalian main olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 35:14103–14122. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0746-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton SD, LaRocca G, Liu A, Cheetham CE, Urban NN (2017) Olfactory bulb deep short-axon cells mediate widespread inhibition of tufted cell apical dendrites. J Neurosci 37:1117–1138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bywalez WG, Patirniche D, Rupprecht V, Stemmler M, Herz AV, Pálfi D, Rózsa B, Egger V (2015) Local postsynaptic voltage-gated sodium channel activation in dendritic spines of olfactory bulb granule cells. Neuron 85:590–601. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cang J, Isaacson JS (2003) In vivo whole-cell recording of odor-evoked synaptic transmission in the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 23:4108–4116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey RM, Sherwood WE, Shipley MT, Borisyuk A, Wachowiak M (2015) Role of intraglomerular circuits in shaping temporally structured responses to naturalistic inhalation-driven sensory input to the olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol 113:3112–3129. 10.1152/jn.00394.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cazakoff BN, Lau BY, Crump KL, Demmer HS, Shea SD (2014) Broadly tuned and respiration-independent inhibition in the olfactory bulb of awake mice. Nat Neurosci 17:569–576. 10.1038/nn.3669 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis BJ, Macrides F (1981) The organization of centrifugal projections from the anterior olfactory nucleus, ventral hippocampal rudiment, and piriform cortex to the main olfactory bulb in the hamster: an autoradiographic study. J Comp Neurol 203:475–493. 10.1002/cne.902030310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desmaisons D, Vincent JD, Lledo PM (1999) Control of action potential timing by intrinsic subthreshold oscillations in olfactory bulb output neurons. J Neurosci 19:10727–10737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhawale AK, Hagiwara A, Bhalla US, Murthy VN, Albeanu DF (2010) Non-redundant odor coding by sister mitral cells revealed by light addressable glomeruli in the mouse. Nat Neurosci 13:1404–1412. 10.1038/nn.2673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger V, Svoboda K, Mainen ZF (2003) Mechanisms of lateral inhibition in the olfactory bulb: efficiency and modulation of spike-evoked calcium influx into granule cells. J Neurosci 23:7551–7558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger V, Svoboda K, Mainen ZF (2005) Dendrodendritic synaptic signals in olfactory bulb granule cells: local spine boost and global low-threshold spike. J Neurosci 25:3521–3530. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4746-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ezeh PI, Wellis DP, Scott JW (1993) Organization of inhibition in the rat olfactory bulb external plexiform layer. J Neurophysiol 70:263–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fantana AL, Soucy ER, Meister M (2008) Rat olfactory bulb mitral cells receive sparse glomerular inputs. Neuron 59:802–814. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich RW, Laurent G (2001) Dynamic optimization of odor representations by slow temporal patterning of mitral cell activity. Science 291:889–894. 10.1126/science.291.5505.889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich RW, Wiechert MT (2014) Neuronal circuits and computations: pattern decorrelation in the olfactory bulb. FEBS Lett 588:2504–2513. 10.1016/j.febslet.2014.05.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukunaga I, Herb JT, Kollo M, Boyden ES, Schaefer AT (2014) Independent control of gamma and theta activity by distinct interneuron networks in the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 17:1208–1216. 10.1038/nn.3760 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galán RF, Fourcaud-Trocmé N, Ermentrout GB, Urban NN (2006) Correlation-induced synchronization of oscillations in olfactory bulb neurons. J Neurosci 26:3646–3655. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4605-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao Y, Strowbridge BW (2009) Long-term plasticity of excitatory inputs to granule cells in the rat olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 12:731–733. 10.1038/nn.2319 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelman A, Hill J, Yajima M (2012) Why we (usually) don't have to worry about multiple comparisons. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness 5:189–211. 10.1080/19345747.2011.618213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Geramita MA, Burton SD, Urban NN (2016) Distinct lateral inhibitory circuits drive parallel processing of sensory information in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Elife 5: pii: e16039. 10.7554/eLife.16039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gschwend O, Abraham NM, Lagier S, Begnaud F, Rodriguez I, Carleton A (2015) Neuronal pattern separation in the olfactory bulb improves odor discrimination learning. Nat Neurosci 18:1474–1482. 10.1038/nn.4089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH. (2007) Cross-intensity functions and the estimate of spike-time jitter. Biol Cybern 96:497–506. 10.1007/s00422-007-0143-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyde RA, Strowbridge BW (2012) Mnemonic representations of transient stimuli and temporal sequences in the rodent hippocampus in vitro. Nat Neurosci 15:1430–1438. 10.1038/nn.3208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacson JS, Strowbridge BW (1998) Olfactory reciprocal synapses: dendritic signaling in the CNS. Neuron 20:749–761. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81013-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kato HK, Chu MW, Isaacson JS, Komiyama T (2012) Dynamic sensory representations in the olfactory bulb: modulation by wakefulness and experience. Neuron 76:962–975. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kato HK, Gillet SN, Peters AJ, Isaacson JS, Komiyama T (2013) Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly control olfactory bulb output. Neuron 80:1218–1231. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ke MT, Fujimoto S, Imai T (2013) SeeDB: A simple and morphology-preserving optical clearing agent for neuronal circuit reconstruction. Nat Neurosci 16:1154–1161. 10.1038/nn.3447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korogod N, Petersen CC, Knott GW (2015) Ultrastructural analysis of adult mouse neocortex comparing aldehyde perfusion with cryo fixation. Elife 4. 10.7554/eLife.05793 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lagier S, Carleton A, Lledo PM (2004) Interplay between local gabaergic interneurons and relay neurons generates gamma oscillations in the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 24:4382–4392. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5570-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larimer P, Strowbridge BW (2008) Nonrandom local circuits in the dentate gyrus. J Neurosci 28:12212–12223. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3612-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larimer P, Strowbridge BW (2010) Representing information in cell assemblies: persistent activity mediated by semilunar granule cells. Nat Neurosci 13:213–222. 10.1038/nn.2458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe G. (2002) Inhibition of backpropagating action potentials in mitral cell secondary dendrites. J Neurophysiol 88:64–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo M, Katz LC (2001) Response correlation maps of neurons in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron 32:1165–1179. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00537-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luskin MB, Price JL (1983) The topographic organization of associational fibers of the olfactory system in the rat, including centrifugal fibers to the olfactory bulb. J Comp Neurol 216:264–291. 10.1002/cne.902160305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma L, Qiu Q, Gradwohl S, Scott A, Yu EQ, Alexander R, Wiegraebe W, Yu CR (2012) Distributed representation of chemical features and tunotopic organization of glomeruli in the mouse olfactory bulb. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:5481–5486. 10.1073/pnas.1117491109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntyre AB, Cleland TA (2016) Biophysical constraints on lateral inhibition in the olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol 115:2937–2949. 10.1152/jn.00671.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLennan H. (1971) The pharmacology of inhibition of mitral cells in the olfactory bulb. Brain Res 29:177–184. 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori K, Kishi K, Ojima H (1983) Distribution of dendrites of mitral, displaced mitral, tufted, and granule cells in the rabbit olfactory bulb. J Comp Neurol 219:339–355. 10.1002/cne.902190308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narayanan NS, Laubach M (2009) Methods for studying functional interactions among neuronal populations. Methods Mol Biol 489:135–165. 10.1007/978-1-59745-543-5_7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nissant A, Bardy C, Katagiri H, Murray K, Lledo PM (2009) Adult neurogenesis promotes synaptic plasticity in the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 12:728–730. 10.1038/nn.2298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orona E, Scott JW, Rainer EC (1983) Different granule cell populations innervate superficial and deep regions of the external plexiform layer in rat olfactory bulb. J Comp Neurol 217:227–237. 10.1002/cne.902170209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orona E, Rainer EC, Scott JW (1984) Dendritic and axonal organization of mitral and tufted cells in the rat olfactory bulb. J Comp Neurol 226:346–356. 10.1002/cne.902260305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petreanu L, Alvarez-Buylla A (2002) Maturation and death of adult-born olfactory bulb granule neurons: role of olfaction. J Neurosci 22:6106–6113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinching AJ, Powell TP (1971) The neuropil of the glomeruli of the olfactory bulb. J Cell Sci 9:347–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pressler RT, Strowbridge BW (2006) Blanes cells mediate persistent feedforward inhibition onto granule cells in the olfactory bulb. Neuron 49:889–904. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pressler RT, Rozman PA, Strowbridge BW (2013) Voltage-dependent intrinsic bursting in olfactory bulb Golgi cells. Learn Mem 20:459–466. 10.1101/lm.031856.113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JL, Powell TP (1970) The morphology of the granule cells of the olfactory bulb. J Cell Sci 7:91–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rall W, Shepherd GM, Reese TS, Brightman MW (1966) Dendrodendritic synaptic pathway for inhibition in the olfactory bulb. Exp Neurol 14:44–56. 10.1016/0014-4886(66)90023-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salinas E, Sejnowski TJ (2000) Impact of correlated synaptic input on output firing rate and variability in simple neuronal models. J Neurosci 20:6193–6209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmoiraghi GC, Bloom FE, Costa E (1964) Adrenergic mechanisms in rabbit olfactory bulb. Am J Physiol 207:1417–1424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt LJ, Strowbridge BW (2014) Modulation of olfactory bulb network activity by serotonin: synchronous inhibition of mitral cells mediated by spatially localized GABAergic microcircuits. Learn Mem 21:406–416. 10.1101/lm.035659.114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoppa NE. (2006) Synchronization of olfactory bulb mitral cells by precisely timed inhibitory inputs. Neuron 49:271–283. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sham PC, Purcell SM (2014) Statistical power and significance testing in large-scale genetic studies. Nat Rev Genet 15:335–346. 10.1038/nrg3706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd GM, Chen WR, Willhite D, Migliore M, Greer CA (2007) The olfactory granule cell: from classical enigma to central role in olfactory processing. Brain Res Rev 55:373–382. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soucy ER, Albeanu DF, Fantana AL, Murthy VN, Meister M (2009) Precision and diversity in an odor map on the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 12:210–220. 10.1038/nn.2262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stopfer M, Bhagavan S, Smith BH, Laurent G (1997) Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature 390:70–74. 10.1038/36335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan J, Savigner A, Ma M, Luo M (2010) Odor information processing by the olfactory bulb analyzed in gene-targeted mice. Neuron 65:912–926. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tukker JJ, Lasztóczi B, Katona L, Roberts JD, Pissadaki EK, Dalezios Y, Márton L, Zhang L, Klausberger T, Somogyi P (2013) Distinct dendritic arborization and in vivo firing patterns of parvalbumin-expressing basket cells in the hippocampal area ca3. J Neurosci 33:6809–6825. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5052-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wellis DP, Scott JW (1990) Intracellular responses of identified rat olfactory bulb interneurons to electrical and odor stimulation. J Neurophysiol 64:932–947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westfall PH. (2011) On using the bootstrap for multiple comparisons. J Biopharm Stat 21:1187–1205. 10.1080/10543406.2011.607751 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westfall PH, Young SS (1993) Resampling-based multiple testing: examples and methods for p-value adjustment. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Wienisch M, Murthy VN (2016) Population imaging at subcellular resolution supports specific and local inhibition by granule cells in the olfactory bulb. Sci Rep 6:29308. 10.1038/srep29308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolf TB, Shepherd GM, Greer CA (1991) Local information processing in dendritic trees: Subsets of spines in granule cells of the mammalian olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 11:1837–1854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zelles T, Boyd JD, Hardy AB, Delaney KR (2006) Branch-specific ca2+ influx from na+-dependent dendritic spikes in olfactory granule cells. J Neurosci 26:30–40. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1419-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimnik NC, Treadway T, Smith RS, Araneda RC (2013) Alpha (1A)-adrenergic regulation of inhibition in the olfactory bulb. J Physiol 591:1631–1643. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.248591 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]