Abstract
Voltage-sensitive dye imaging experiments in primary visual cortex (V1) have shown that local, oriented visual stimuli elicit stable orientation-selective activation within the stimulus retinotopic footprint. The cortical activation dynamically extends far beyond the retinotopic footprint, but the peripheral spread stays non-selective—a surprising finding given a number of anatomo-functional studies showing the orientation specificity of long-range connections. Here we use a computational model to investigate this apparent discrepancy by studying the expected population response using known published anatomical constraints. The dynamics of input-driven localized states were simulated in a planar neural field model with multiple sub-populations encoding orientation. The realistic connectivity profile has parameters controlling the clustering of long-range connections and their orientation bias. We found substantial overlap between the anatomically relevant parameter range and a steep decay in orientation selective activation that is consistent with the imaging experiments. In this way our study reconciles the reported orientation bias of long-range connections with the functional expression of orientation selective neural activity. Our results demonstrate this sharp decay is contingent on three factors, that long-range connections are sufficiently diffuse, that the orientation bias of these connections is in an intermediate range (consistent with anatomy) and that excitation is sufficiently balanced by inhibition. Conversely, our modelling results predict that, for reduced inhibition strength, spurious orientation selective activation could be generated through long-range lateral connections. Furthermore, if the orientation bias of lateral connections is very strong, or if inhibition is particularly weak, the network operates close to an instability leading to unbounded cortical activation.
Author Summary
Optical imaging techniques can reveal the dynamical patterns of cortical activation that encode low-level visual features like position and orientation, which are shaped by both feed-forward projections, recurrent and long-range intra-cortical connections. Anatomical studies have characterized intra-cortical connections, however, it is non-trivial to predict from this data how evoked activity might spread across cortex. Indeed, there remains an apparent conflict between the reported orientation bias of cortical connections, and imaging studies on the propagation of cortical activity. Our study reconciles structure (anatomy) with function (evoked activity) using a dynamic neural field model that predicts the dynamics of cortical activation in a setting both inspired by and parametrically matched to the available anatomical data.
Introduction
Horizontal connections link cells separated within the same cortical area, over a distance of a few millimeters (mm) covering several iso-functional columns [1, 2] and spatially distributed into regular clusters [3–5]. In V1, the lattice-like pattern of connectivity is reminiscent of the spatial regularity observed in orientation maps and has therefore been proposed to link neurons with similar preferred orientation [6]. Functional mapping combined with retrograde labeling [7] has shown that pyramidal cell horizontal axons have a bias to preferentially connect iso-orientation loci (in cat [8, 9], tree shew [10] and monkey [11]). However, this result depends on cell type and location since neurons in layer 4 (L4) [12], pinwheel centers ([13] but see [14, 15] since this result may depend critically on the distance to the pinwheel center) and inhibitory cells [16, 17] connect without orientation bias. As a consequence of this cellular heterogeneity, it is not trivial to predict the selectivity of stimulus-driven horizontal activation.
More information was recently gained from population measures of orientation selectivity at mesoscopic scales. Techniques such as optical imaging has led to the description of cartographic organization of many cortical structures [18, 19]. The development of voltage-sensitive dye [20] has further allowed for investigation of dynamic features and computations arising within these maps, see reviews in [21] and [22]. In [23] voltage sensitive dye imaging (VSDI) was used to study the retinotopic activation with localized oriented inputs in cat V1. A characteristic plateau of activity, coinciding with the retinotopic extent of the stimulus, was independent of stimulus orientation. Within the plateau several peaks of activation would appear, with location strongly dependent on orientation. [24] further explored orientation selectivity outside the retinotopic activation using localized stimuli. Over several hundred milliseconds activation gradually propagated outwards, extending several mm beyond the feedforward footprint (FFF). The dynamic and the spatial range of this activation beyond the FFF is presumably generated by long-range excitatory connections in L2/3 of V1. Note that, although less plausible because their spatial and temporal properties are not in the appropriate range (see discussion in [25] and [24]), alternative circuits such as intra-thalamic, thalamo-cortical divergence and feedback loops cannot be entirely dismissed (but see [26]). Interestingly, only a local component of this activation, circumscribed within the FFF, was found to be orientation selective (Fig 1A), at first glance a surprising finding given the numerous studies showing an iso-orientation bias for long-range connections. However, as discussed in [24] and introduced above, this result may be expected from the anatomy known from studies already published at that time. First, because the iso-orientation connection bias is small and has been quantified only for short horizontal distances (mostly <1–1.5 mm, a distance for which a similar small bias is also reported by [24]). Second, because it can be seen in the few existing intracellular labeling studies that the iso-orientation bias tends to decrease with distance [17, 14]. Thus, the results from [24], although in contradiction with a strict “like-to-like connectivity” principle, call for modification depending on cortical distance: from a like-to-like bias in functional connectivity at short range toward no bias at long range. Importantly, two follow-up studies consolidated these findings with an optogenetics functional confirmation ([27], see also [28, 29]) and a timely anatomical clarification using quantitative and statistical analysis of intracellular labeled neurons [15].
The aim of this modelling study is to explore the relationship between lateral connectivity properties (structure) and the way activity spreads across cortex as evoked by localized, oriented stimuli (function). It remains to reconcile the studies of iso-orientation bias of anatomical connectivity in V1 [7, 9, 14] and the properties of cortical activation observed in other experiments [24, 27, 15]. To achieve this, we have developed a dynamical model to investigate stimulus driven activity in 2D cortical space with a discrete representation of orientation. The neural field equation gives a coarse-grained description of average membrane potential on a continuous domain [30, 31]. This framework has been widely used to model visual cortex (e.g. [32–35]) and cortical dynamics in general (see reviews [36] and [37]). This level of description is well-suited for comparison with VSDI [38–40]. The model’s design allows for connectivity properties such as the spatial profile of excitation and inhibition to be investigated. Model parameters were constrained to be consistent with the level of orientation bias reported in [14]. Our study shows that realistic cortical connectivity schemas [14, 15] govern a spatio-temporal dynamics of cortical activation in accordance with the observed functional dynamics [24, 27]. Our approach allowed us to further elaborate on the non-trivial link between anatomical constraints and predictions of population level activation patterns. To probe the structure-function link, we used our model to make experimentally tractable predictions on the specific role of excitatory-inhibitory balance.
Methods
Outline of the model
Our neural field equation model [30, 31] gives a mesoscopic, continuous description of neural activity on a 2D plane (x, y) parallel with the cortical surface in layer 2/3 of V1. Neural activity is represented by an average membrane potential ui(x, y, t) evolving with time t in four sub-populations, each associated with a different orientation i = {0°, 45°, 90°, 135°} (the ° notation will be dropped in the remainder of the manuscript). The decomposition into sub-populations is a convenient abstraction for modelling purposes; these sub-population outputs ui(x, y, t) will be combined into activity variables that can be compared with experimental data in Conversion of model output to VSD-like signal. The following integro-differential equation describes the dynamics of the ui population:
(1) |
(2) |
(3) |
(4) |
The cortical timescale is τ = 10 ms, and the rate of change of ui is proportional to the right hand side of this equation with the following terms
(1) Decay of the population activity to resting potential
(2) Stimulus driven input with feedforward footprint I, modulated by the orientation map J
(3) Non-selective intra-cortical interactions gated by sigmoidal threshold function S
(4) Orientation-selective interactions, modulated by the orientation map J
Term (1) describes a decay back to an resting potential of ui = 0 (the membrane potential has arbitrary units). Within the sub-population the decay term has strength ρi=j = 1 (an arbitrary choice for this first constant). Between sub-populations the decay term has strength ρi≠j = 0.1 (local, linear cross inhibition). For ρ > 0.2 this cross inhibition can generate undesired above-threshold activation in non-stimulated sub-populations, therefore, a value 50% below this was selected. Term (2) describes localised circular inputs with one of four orientations (e.g. Fig 1B). For an input with say orientation 0, I0 is active and I45, I90 and I135 are zero. The input weighting for a sub-population’s associated orientation ki=j = k1 is larger than the weighting for other orientations ki≠j = k2, with k1 = 2k2. Inputs are modulated by the orientation map Ji with strength βinp = 0.25 (Fig 1D and 1I). For βinp > 0.2 the spatial phase of multi-bump patterns of activity will match the orientation preference map as required, increasing it further has little effect. Term (3) describes non-selective lateral connections within the sub-population ui via a convolution ⋆ over (x, y) (spatial integrals, see (6)) with the components of the spatial connectivity profile. This profile is radially symmetric and describes the average connectivity at any point in cortex. It is broken down into local excitatory and inhibitory wI components, see Fig 1G and Details and equations for connectivity profile. In the convolutions ui is processed through the sigmoidal threshold function S, which transforms membrane potential into a normalized firing rate. Any locations above threshold will influence their neighbors through lateral connections. Weighting constants gex and gin are determined by theoretical constraints, see Details and equations for connectivity profile and (17). Term (4) describes orientation-selective excitatory connections as modulated by the preference map Ji with strength βrec.
An example of the simulated sub-population activity is shown in Fig 1F for an input with orientation 0. The main parameters of interest in this study are RWex, which modifies the spatial width of excitatory peaks (Fig 1G) and βrec (taking values between 0 and 1), which controls the amplitude modulation of long-range excitation by the orientation preference map (Fig 1H). We will also study a parameter C controlling the strength of inhibition (equivalently the balance between excitation and inhibition) as defined by (16) in Details and equations for connectivity profile.
The firing rate (or threshold) function is given by a sigmoid
(5) |
where μ = 2.3 is a slope parameter and θ = 5.6 the threshold. Input strengths k1 = 2.8 and k2 = 1.4 are set such that inputs to the stimulated sub-population are above threshold in (5) μk1 > θ, whilst inputs to other sub-populations are below threshold μk2 < θ. The particular form of S is chosen such that ui = 0 (all populations at resting potential) is a always a solution to the model equations and values of μ and θ are chosen such that this is the only stable solution with no inputs [41]. The spatial convolution terms in (3)–(4) are expressed as integrals, e.g. Term (4) can be computed as one integral
(6) |
with dummy variables x′ and y′. In this case the modulation by Ji is introduced whilst still preserving the convolutional structure, allowing for the numerical methods described in [41], exploiting Fast Fourier Transforms to be applied. The radial inputs Ii(r), , as plotted in Fig 1I(left), are given by
(7) |
where h is the radially shifted Gaussian ring given below in (10). The extent of Ii and its decay at the stimulus border were chosen to match cortical point spread functions measured in [24]. In simulations the input amplitude ramps up linearly from 0 to full amplitude in the interval t ∈ [20, 120] ms, see [40].
For each sub-population a finite differencing scheme for the domain [−L, L] × [−L, L] with L = 30 and N = 128 evenly distributed gridpoints in each spatial direction is used, as described in [41]. Model simulations were run on a domain much larger (2L = 60, relative to Λ = 2π) than the localized patterns of activation studied here, justifying the use of periodic boundary conditions. A standard Runge-Kutta time stepper in Matlab was used for model simulations with default tolerances. The source code for the full implementation of the model has been made available in the Supplemental Information (S1 Code).
Details and equations for connectivity profile
Earlier theoretical works characterized connectivity constraints that lead to stable localized patterns of activation in 1D [42, 43] and in 2D [44, 45]. A common choice of connectivity function is a so-called Mexican hat (e.g. a difference of Gaussians) featuring a broader footprint for inhibition than for excitation [36, 37]. [41] suggested a role for longer-range excitation serving to stabilize larger patterns of activation and equating the separation between peaks in excitation with Λ (hypercolumn separation, [46]) The formulation presented here is further inspired by the way [14] quantified their results so that we can link model parameters with their quanitification or orientation bias for long-range connections (see next section). The model’s connectivity profile is broken down into local excitatory (a local Gaussian bump), lateral excitatory (Gaussian rings centered at Λ and 2Λ) and inhibitory wI (a broad local Gaussian bump) components, which were plotted in Fig 1G. The use of Gaussian rings was inspired by [16] (see their Fig 16), noting the important features that excitatory connections 1) drop in number at a range Λ/2, 2) have a peak at a range Λ and 3) can extend several mm across cortex. The following details give full definitions of these components and their relative scaling with particular attention to the global balance between excitation and inhibition, which will be controlled by a parameter C. When C = 0 excitation and inhibition are balanced (the area under the 2D radial versions of the blue and red curves in Fig 1G would be equal). In this study C is taken to be negative (net inhibition) whilst remaining close to the balanced condition. We note that when C is larger (or even positive) localized patterns of activity are more likely to destabilize and spread across cortex [41], which is investigated in Reduced inhibition leads to orientation selective activation outside stimulus footprint. The length scale Λ is the mean hypercolumn separation.
Radially symmetric functions for the connectivity components are defined in terms of a radial coordinate . We define a 2D Gaussian function with spatial decay rate σ:
(8) |
where the pre-factor normalizes the area. The number of inhibitory connection, based on a diverse class of inhibitory neurons [16], is assumed to have a Gaussian decay with distance from the origin:
(9) |
with RWin = 0.55Λ (a cross-section of this function is plotted red in Fig 1G). This value gives the qualitative feature that there is more excitation than inhibition at ranges Λ and above. The results are not contingent on the exact value chosen, but varying RWin has a similar effect to varying C (inhibition strength, defined below), which is investigated in Reduced inhibition leads to orientation selective activation outside stimulus footprint. Assuming that there are peaks in the number of excitatory connections ever Λ-distance from the origin up to 2Λ, there are rings of excitation at distances {0, Λ, 2Λ}. We assume that the amplitude of the peaks centered at these distances decay within an exponential envelope
where ζ = 0.625Λ. The exact value chosen is not critical, varying ζ by ±20% does not significantly affect the results. We define a radially shifted 2D Gaussian
(10) |
which describes a ring that is maximal at a radius r = r0 and decays away with spatial scale σ (note that ). The local excitatory component (a Gaussian bump centered at 0) is given by
(11) |
where RWex is a free parameter. For RWex < 0.1 the number of excitatory connections at Λ/2 would drop to 0, which is not realistic. Further RWex should be less than RWin; for our parameter exploration, we therefore consider a smaller range [0.1,0.4] based on the anatomical constraints introduced later. The long-range excitatory component (Gaussian rings centered at Λ and 2Λ) is given by
(12) |
The overall profile of excitation is plotted in Fig 1G. Noting the following analytic expression for the zero-mode of the Fourier transform of (10)
(13) |
where erf is the standard error function, we write the normalisation pre-factor for the combined excitatory components
(14) |
We now define the normalized combined excitatory profile as
(15) |
which by design has zero-order Fourier mode of 1. The complete connectivity function is
(16) |
where C is a constant controlling the relative strength of excitation and inhibition. When C = 0 excitation and inhibition are balanced and when C is negative there is net inhibition globally. We introduce a constant P that matches the value of W’s largest Fourier mode with the connectivity used in [41]. This final scaling of the overall connectivity allows us to manipulate any of the connectivity parameters, whilst keeping all non-connectivity parameters constant (e.g. input and threshold function parameters). Failing to do this means that the correct operating region of the model would shift each time, say, RWex was modified. Two constants gex and gin in (3)–(4) are given by
(17) |
Orientation preference map
Retinotopic space is mapped to the surface of V1 [47] and different locations show preference for a variety of low-level visual features such as spatial frequency, ocular dominance, orientation and direction of motion [48, 49]. Cortical neurons exhibiting similar preference for low-level features are organized in a columnar fashion [50, 51]. Orientation preference varies incrementally along the cortical surface [52], defining a quasi-periodic organization characterized by linear zones and pinwheels, singularities about which all orientation preferences are present along a circular path [18]. The orientation maps have a quasi-periodic organisation with a regular length scale of around 0.5–1 mm (depending on species and cortical areas), which can be measured as the mean distance between iso-orientation domains. This length scale, which we denote Λ, is reflected as a peak in the Fourier spectrum of the preference map represented as polar argument [53, 46]. Orientation preference can be deduced from single-electrode recordings [54] or with optical imaging of intrinsic signals [18, 10].
The characteristics, artificial generation and biological development of orientation preference maps have been investigated in various modelling studies [55, 56, 46, 57]. [56] showed the importance of long range connections in generating the quasi-periodic repetition of key map features in a canonical pattern forming system with spatially extended complex representation of orientation. [46] showed that when normalized by the regular length scale, orientation preference maps show a constant pinwheel density; see also [58, 59]. [57] explored mechanisms for the stable development using a Hebbian learning method for connections in a two-stage (LGN, V1) model and found an important role for adaptation and normalisation.
In this study we generated realistic maps specifically for our planar model with discrete representation of orientation (in four-sub-populations). The maps were generated specifically for the periodic domain used in our model using a spatial Hebbian-like learning rule operating on the converged model output from a series of localized inputs Ii with random orientations and at random locations. After each simulation the Ji were updated via the following rule:
Ha is the learning rate, G a smoothing kernel (8) with σ = 0.6Λ, and 〈.〉 rectification on the given interval. The smoothing ensures that regions that already have local structure are modified less than regions without local structure. Learning was initiated from a homogenous initial set of Ji = 0 (where the model produces localized multi-bump states in the stimulated area and sub-population). The learning process converged (e.g. pinwheel density stabilizes) after around 1600 steps as the map takes on structure across the whole domain (after this the term 1 − 〈|Ji,n| ⋆ G〉[0, 1] remains close to 0 across the whole domain, although small changes in the map continue). Final maps (at 6400 steps) were high-pass and low-pass filtered as described in [46]. The maps used in this study are included as part of the simulation code (see Supplemental information S1 Code), thus allowing the results to be independently reproduced. Full details of the method will be the subject of a separate study.
Alternatively, one could use maps obtained experimentally directly with our model, but this would not have any significant effect on the results presented. For the purposes of the present study it is sufficient to show that the maps used in our model show characteristic features of realistic maps including linear zones, pinwheels and the regular length scale Λ. Fig 2A shows the component maps Ji, each associated with a different orientation, that in (1)–(4) modulate inputs with strength βinp and long range excitatory connections with strength βrec. In conjunction, the composite maps combine to give the orientation preference map shown in Fig 2B, which has a regular length scale Λ characterized by a sharp peak in the map’s spectral power curve (Fig 2B).
Anatomically motivated parameter operating range for the model
The effective tuning of lateral connections is computed across ranges of the connectivity parameters RWex (width of peaks in excitation) and βrec (orientation bias of long-range lateral connections). These parameters were chosen as they have a strong effect on the anatomical measure of interest over ranges where the model is well defined. Another choice could be ζ (controlling the decay of peaks in excitation), but this would be redundant with ζ having a similar effect as varying RWex. These computations allow for a direct comparison with the anatomical-data-based model analysis presented in [14], which reported the orientation bias of long-range lateral connections in V1 L2/3. Optical imaging was used to find orientation preference maps and combined with intracellular labeling of lateral projections of pyramidal cells to identify target locations relative to the preference map. The orientation bias was quantified by tuning curves of the orientation preference at axon terminals relative to orientation preference in a region local to the originating cell’s body. Tuning curves were quantified by parameter-fits to a von-Mises distribution (circular normal distribution). Our model was developed to be able to provide a direct point of quantitative comparison with these measures. The distribution is defined on a circular domain and parametrized by a tuning coefficient κ ≥ 0 (0 is untuned, larger is more tightly tuned) and a preferred orientation μ ∈ [0, 180):
(18) |
I0(κ) is the modified Bessel function of order 0. [14] reported values of κ in the range 0.7–1.2 for population-level tracing of lateral connections.
We perform a similar computation for the connectivity function in our model. The computation of the effective orientation tuning of lateral connections, at one specific map location (Fig 3A(top)), is illustrated in Fig 3A–3E. For the orientation preference map one can compute an orientation tuning curve by counting the number of pixels falling within equally sized orientation bins, as shown in Fig 3A (bottom, circular markers). All orientations are represented with equal probability so the profile is untuned and a best-fit von-Mises distribution (solid curve) has κ ≈ 0 (fit determined using a least-squares minimisation with Matlab’s lsqcurvefit for κ and μ in (18)). A spatial weighting for the pixel count can be introduced in order to find the tuning of orientations in some local region. A 2D Gaussian function (Fig 3B(middle)) was used to give the local weighting shown in Fig 3B(top). In most regions of the map this results in a sharp tuning (note large κ) centered at one specific orientation as shown in Fig 3B(bottom). Introducing the radial profile of excitatory connections from the model as a weighting function (Fig 3C(middle), see (15)) one can compute the effective tuning of the lateral connections. With βrec = 0 the weighting function is radially symmetric, nevertheless there is an expected weak tuning of the weighted connections (Fig 3C(bottom)) due to the structure of the orientation preference map; there is a slight bias toward finding similar orientations at a range Λ (annular ring) relative to the origin (star). Increasing βrec introduces an orientation-specific bias in the weighting profile for the long-range connections (Fig 3D–3E(middle)). This results in locations with similar orientations to the origin being targeted specifically by long-range connections (Fig 3D–3E(top)) and a corresponding increase in the tuning strength κ (Fig 3D–3E(bottom)).
As one might expect the tuning strength of connections (κ) increases monotonically with increasing βrec or decreasing RWex, which allows us to define an operating range for the model before running simulations. Fig 3F shows κ (average value from 50 randomly selected map locations) computed at combinations of RWex ∈ [0.1, 0.4] and βrec ∈ [0, 1]. Map locations close to pinwheels (7/50) identified by having a local tuning (computed as in Fig 3B) with κ < 1 were excluded (this exclusion had a very minor effect). The map shows that κ increases with βrec (as expected) and decreases with RWex. Solid white contours show a band of values for RWex and βrec where the tuning of connections in the model is consistent with the anatomical data (κ ∈ [0.7, 1.2]). These contours are later replotted in Orientation selective activation is restricted to stimulus footprint in the anatomical parameter range and Reduced inhibition leads to orientation selective activation outside stimulus footprint for comparison with model simulations. For RWex much beyond 0.4 the anatomical data cannot be matched as βrec must be <1.
Conversion of model output to VSD-like signal
The process of converting the model output from individual simulations into a VSD-like signal, and the method for computing the general and orientation selective activation, is illustrated in Fig 4. For a given oriented simulation, the Ii are weighted toward the specific orientation (Fig 1A). The input is further modulated by Ji if βinp > 0 (Fig 1B) and recurrent connections in each sub-population are modulated by Ji if βrec > 0. The sub-population corresponding to the input orientation responds above threshold in a multi-bump pattern with the location of the bumps determined by Ji, whilst the other sub-populations have a sub-threshold response (Fig 4A). We first transform the sub-population variables ui into a VSD-like signal following a similar method to the one proposed in [38]. The sub-population membrane potentials in Fig 4A are converted into a firing rate, processed through weighted excitatory and inhibitory connectivity profiles, summed across the sub-populations and diffused with a Gaussian profile (representing the attenuation and diffusion of the signal in cortical tissue). The contribution from inhibition is assumed to be in the range 15–20% [60, 61]. The resulting optical imaging signal OI0 in Fig 4B(top left) was computed from the four sub-population responses in Fig 4A. For different orientation inputs, the other OI signals can be computed in a similar fashion (Fig 4B, other panels). The general activation Act is computed as an average across the responses to inputs with four different orientations; the response in Fig 4C was computed as an average of the four responses in Fig 4B. The preference Pref and selectivity Sel (Fig 4D) are computed by transforming the four OI signals into polar coordinates from difference maps (OI0 − OI90 and OI45 − OI135) where the angular coordinate (argument) is the preference and the radial coordinate (magnitude) is the selectivity [62]. Thresholds delineating the general activated area and orientation selective area are defined as a fraction of the average activation or selectivity inside the FFF. The radial profiles (average radial decay) of Act and Sel are characterized by the parameters of best-fit Naka-Rushton functions [63] (a widely-used, smooth, monotonically decaying function (29), which has been used to fit, e.g., contrast response data [64]). Further details and equations of all the processing steps are given below.
Following the method described in [38], to account for the known optical diffusion of the VSD signal (light) in V1 L2/3, we convolved the signal with a Gaussian distribution (8) with σOI = 0.075Λ. Note that the results in this study are not contingent on this specific value, there being little effect of either increasing or decreasing σOI by a factor of 2. The unattenuated imaging signal ui for each sub-population is assumed proportional to the post-synaptic membrane potential. Hence we first computed the dynamics of mean pre-synaptic membrane potential for each sub-population ui as given by (1)–(4), which is converted to a firing rate of the pre-synaptic neurons through S. The postsynatpic population response was then computed via a convolution of the presynaptic firing rate with the connection profile. There is an 85% contribution from excitation and a 15% contribution from inhibition, leading to a weighting for inhibition of pI = 0.177. These are summed across the sub-populations i to give the total unattenuated signal (expression in large parentheses). Finally the optical imaging (OI) signal is computed as a convolution of the total unattenuated signal with a Gaussian:
(19) |
This equation converts the model’s state variables ui for an input stimulus with specific orientation (e.g. 0° as in Fig 4A) into a VSD-like signal (e.g. OI0 in Fig 4B, top left).
We are interested to explore how cortical activation spreads over time. One potential issue with the temporal dynamics in the model, and (19), is the assumption that activity generated through long range lateral connections () propagate instantaneously. Although the model converges to the correct final state, the transient dynamics may not be captured exactly. To solve this, whilst avoiding the introduction of say delay terms in (1)–(4), we assume that there is a slower timescale τlat = 240 ms for the portion of the OI-signal generated through :
(20) |
where t is the time after stimulus onset. The introduction of τlat is done at the post-processing stage only and its value was chosen to match data from [24].
For four sequential simulations, each with an input with different orientation, the OI signal can be computed (Fig 4B). The general activation is the average of these signals:
(21) |
as shown in Fig 4C.
Before computing the preference Pref and selectivity Sel, the VSD signals are normalized by a scale factor that accounts for differences in the maximum value over (x, y) across the four simulations with different orientations. Two difference maps between the normalized VSD signals from simulations with orthogonal inputs are computed:
(22) |
(23) |
The orientation preference of the activation is given by
(24) |
where Arctan is the four quadrant inverse tangent, and the selectivity strength is given by
(25) |
In the results section, all plots of Sel(x, y, t) and Act(x, y, t) are scaled by 1.1× their values at the final time point tfinal in the simulation, thus showing the time-evolution relative to the final state. The thresholds contours Tact and Tsel for activation and selectivity (and corresponding areas Aact and Asel) are set as a fraction of the mean activation and selectivity within the FFF at the final time point,
(26) |
(27) |
where rFF is the FFF boundary. The thresholds are given by
(28) |
where ηAct = 0.2 and ηSel = 0.5. These thresholds were chosen ad hoc. In order to ensure the results obtained weren’t contingent only on these choices, we further characterized the radial decay rates of the general and orientation selective activation.
We also look at radial profiles of the general and selective activation patterns (radial decay of Act and Sel). Radial profiles Act(r, t) and Sel(r, t) are computed by re-meshing Act(x, y, t) and Sel(x, y, t) on radial coordinate system (r, θ) centered at the stimulus center and averaging in the angular coordinate θ. The decreasing Naka-Rushton function NR used here decays to zero as r increases:
(29) |
where the exponent n > 0 describes the steepness, the maximal value is Rmax and the half-max r-value is r50 > 0. Best-fit Naka-Rushton functions were determined by minimising, for example the least-squares distance between Act(r, tfinal) and NR(r) varying the parameters n, Rmax and r50 using Matlab’s lsqcurvefit function.
Results
Radial profile of lateral connections determines the extent of orientation selective activation
The spatio-temporal dynamics are investigated for a specific case with inputs modulated by the orientation preference map (βinp = 0.25) but with radially symmetric lateral connections (βrec = 0, no orientation bias in lateral connections). The dynamics produced by the model will be illustrated for two values of the width of the peaks of excitatory connections RWex. When RWex is small, excitatory connections cluster on tightly on rings at distances Λ and 2Λ away from the origin. When RWex is larger the clustering at these specific ranges is more diffuse (Fig 1G).
Simulations were run, sequentially for four different orientations, with a radial input centered at a specific location in the orientation preference map (Fig 5A). Fig 5B and 5E (top) show that the general (averaged) activation spreads outward from the center of the stimulated region (note that simulations are computed on a domain around 3 times larger than the window shown here). Only some portion of this general spread of activation is orientation selective as shown in the bottom panels (area inside white contour is significantly smaller than area inside grey contour). When, RWex is small the selective activation extends outside the FFF (Fig 5B(bottom,right)), and when RWex is larger it is confined to the FFF (Fig 5E(bottom,right)). The temporal dynamics of the general and orientation selective areas (Fig 5C and 5F) shows that the rate of the area increase slows down after roughly the first 100 ms. Nevertheless, when RWex is small the spread of activation persists after 550 ms as shown by the blue/green curves still increasing in Fig 5C (eventually converges at ∼ 700 ms). When RWex is larger the orientation selective activation converges after around 200 ms although the general activation continues to increase slowly (Fig 5F). This latter behavior is compatible with the observations made using VSDI in [24]. The radial profile of the general and selective activation shows the decay with distance from the center of the stimulated region (Fig 5D and 5G). When RWex is small, the profile of the general and selective activation exhibits a similar decay profile as marked by similar values of the exponent n in a best-fit Naka-Rushton function for the two profiles (Fig 5D). For a larger RWex the exponent for the orientation selective activation is substantially larger (nSel = 14.47), indicating a steep transition from high to low selectivity, than for the general activation (Fig 5G).
Even with lateral connections not modulated by the orientation preference map (no orientation bias in lateral connections), orientation selective activation can be generated outside the FFF of the stimulus due to convergent excitation generated from regions directly stimulated within the FFF. Indeed, activated locations with the same preference mutually excite each other at a range Λ. These activated regions can further generate overlapping excitation at Λ-equidistant points, either inside the FFF, or potentially outside the FFF. Orientation selective activation generated in this way, outside the FFF, does not necessarily agree with the orientation preference map (Fig 5B, bottom right, region inside the white contour but not the yellow). This occurs if the range of peaks in excitation are highly specific (small RWex) as in Fig 5B–5D. These findings illustrate that with RWex excessively small, the ringed connectivity can lead to undesired behaviour that is inconsistent with activation observed in experiments. We note that later in section Reduced inhibition leads to orientation selective activation outside stimulus footprint we find that spurious activation (not agreeing with the preference map) can be generated by another mechanism (destabilization of activity). When the peaks in excitation are broader (RWex > 0.2), the orientation selective activation decays quickly at the border of the stimulated region and no such miss-tuned activation (as generated by convergent excitation) is observed outside the FFF.
We have seen how our modelling results allow us to distinguish between patterns of activation that are consistent with imaging studies in terms of 1) the area and range of general (grey) and selective (white) activation relative to the FFF (red) in Fig 5B and 5E(bottom); 2) whether activation reflects the correct orientation with respect to the underlying preference map (difference between yellow and white contours in Fig 5B and 5E(bottom,right); and 3) the rate of decay of general and selective activation in Fig 5D and 5G as quantified by n (larger is a steeper decay). The simulation shown in Fig 5E–5G is consistent with [23] and [24] because 1) in E(bottom,right) the grey contour extends much further than the white, 2) in E(bottom,right) the yellow and white contours agree closely, 3) in G the exponent n is larger for the selective activation (blue) than for the general activation (green). Further, this steep decay of selectivity is consistent with there being a plateau of highly selective activation inside the FFF that transitions sharply to low selectivity outside the FFF.
Orientation bias of lateral connections increases the range of orientation selective activation
The effect of introducing an orientation bias to the long-range lateral connections (βrec > 0) is shown in Fig 6. For another location in the orientation preference map (Fig 6A) the orientation selective activation is shown for different values of βrec in Fig 6B–6D with the radial profile shown in F–I. The parameter values for panels Fig 6B and 6F are the same as those for Fig 5E–5G (RWex = 0.25 and βrec = 0), the only difference is the location in the orientation preference map. These two examples show a qualitatively similar spatial profile, and in general, the resulting spatio-temporal dynamics are independent of the location in the orientation preference map (five locations were randomly chosen for the results in this paper). Across the five locations there is a large range of nSel (between 6 and 15) when RWex = 0.25 and βrec = 0. However, this variability in nSel across locations is much less (between 5 and 7) when βrec is increased to say 0.5. As βrec is increased, more orientation selective activation is observed increasing from small isolated patches outside the FFF at intermediate values (Fig 6C–6D) to many larger for βrec = 0.9 (Fig 6E). In contrast with the orientation selective activation outside the FFF observed in Fig 5B(bottom), which does not necessarily reflect the local orientation from the preference map, the activation outside the FFF in Fig 6E reflects the preference map (white and yellow contours agree) as it is generated by orientation-biased long-range connections. The increased selective activation outside of the FFF coincides with a reduced slope (smaller exponent n) in the radial decay of selective activation shown in Fig 6F–6I.
Again, the results shown here for βrec in the range [0, 0.5] are consistent with imaging studies [23, 24], as the region of selective activation is predominantly confined to the FFF (limited small selective regions outside the red circle), the preference agrees with the underlying map (yellow and white contours are very similar) and the exponent n is larger for the selective activation.
Orientation selective activation is restricted to stimulus footprint in the anatomical parameter range
The measures of the orientation selective component of the lateral spread of activation as studied in individual simulations in Figs 5 and 6 are now quantified over a range of RWex and βrec. The ranges of these parameters consistent with anatomical data is indicated in Figs 7–9 (between the white iso-κ contours reproduced from Fig 3). The agreement of model simulations with qualitative features from imaging data will be assessed in this and the following section. Fig 7A shows a map of normalized area (relative to the FFF area) of the selective activation, where each value is an average from simulations at 5 randomly selected map locations. Within the red contour (darker regions) the selective area is roughly equal to or smaller than the FFF of the stimulated region (e.g. Fig 7E, top). Outside this region the selective region is larger than the FFF (e.g. Fig 7D, top). Fig 7B shows a greyscale map of the proportion of the selective area with the correct orientation with respect to the underlying orientation preference map. Within the white contour (darker regions) the majority of the selective region has the correct orientation. For small RWex or small βrec a significant proportion (more than 15%) of the selective activation is spurious (not in agreement with the preference map). Fig 7C shows a map of the ratio of the slopes (Naka-Rushton exponent) of the selective activation nSel and the general activation nAct. When this ratio nSel/nAct is large there is steeper radial decay of the selective activation (relative to the general activation) and a sharp transition to non-selectivity at the border of the FFF. Note that nAct and nSel are independent of the choice of ηSel and ηAct defined in (28).
In each case (Fig 7A–7C) there is a significant overlap between the regions with orientation selectivity confined to the FFF, with this selective activation having the correct orientation, with a characteristically steep decay of this region, and the anatomical range for the lateral connections in the model. In the green triangular region (overlayed in Fig 7A–7C), all of these characteristics are satisfied. We highlight that the operating range matching anatomy and functional data covers more than 20% of the (conservative) permissible range of RWex (0.1 < RWex < 0.55 = RWin).
The extent of the operating region for the model depends on values of the thresholds used in Fig 7A–7C. A value of 1.05 for normalized selective area is consistent with observations from [24], that the selective area is close to or marginally larger than the FFF area. A value of 85% for the proportion correct orientation was chosen heuristically, for smaller values isolated spurious selectivity occurs outside the FFF, e.g. compare Fig 7E and 7F. The value nSel/nAct = 1.3, also chosen heuristically, gives a sharp drop-off in the selectivity close to the FFF border. Reducing the threshold on the selective area, or the threshold on nSel/nAct, by 10% would give a reduced by still existing operating region for the model, but increasing the proportion correct orientation threshold much beyond 85% rapidly reduces the operating region. Fig 8 illustrates the dependence of the operating region limits on each of the thresholds. The specific choice ηSel = 0.5 could also affect the size of the model’s operating region for the normalised selective area. For example, increasing ηSel could potentially increase the operating region for a fixed value of the threshold on the normalized selective area. However, the constraint on the ratio nSel/nAct (also providing an upper bound on the green region) would be unaffected as it is independent of the choice of ηsel.
Reduced inhibition leads to orientation selective activation outside stimulus footprint
Although an anatomical bias towards iso-orientation in the model’s connectivity profile was introduced through βrec, the bias is not necessarily evident at the population functional level (in the patterns of activity observed in simulations). This may originate from the non-linearities in the functional expression that combines excitatory and inhibitory activation. To further explore the role of the relative balance between E/I in this functional expression, we manipulate the strength of inhibition in the model (controlled by C in (16)). Recall that when C = 0 there is global E/I balance and for the reduced inhibition case here we reduce C toward 0 (from its standard value C = −0.4 to C = −0.2). Fig 9 (computed in the same way as Fig 7A) shows that with reduced inhibition, the region of the (RWex, βrec)-plane with orientation selective activation confined to the FFF footprint (inside the red contour) is smaller. In general, maintaining the same values of other parameters, but reducing inhibition leads to a wider spread of orientation selective activation. Compare Fig 9B (bottom) where the stable pattern of activation extends outside the FFF with Fig 7B (bottom) where the activation is confined within the FFF. Furthermore, with strong orientation bias in the model’s lateral connections (large βrec), the spread of activation can destabilize and continue indefinitely, see Fig 9B (top). This activation outside the stimulated region has spurious orientation preference and is due to destabilized activity that spreads far beyond the boundary of the stimulated region. Note that this a distinct mechanism from the convergent excitation that generates activation with incorrect orientation when RWex is too small (see Fig 5B). For the case illustrated in Fig 9B (top), of the four stimulus presentations with I0, I45, I90 and I135, the condition I0 resulted in an continual spread of activation down and to the right of the FFF (blue-green regions), I45 above the FFF (purple regions), whilst I90 and I135 remained constrained to the FFF. These different responses across different orientations arise from a local imbalance in the preference map, which becomes exaggerated when input drives the model close to spatial instability, as is the case with reduced inhibition. With stronger inhibition, small imbalances in the spread of activity can occur across different orientations, but these don’t impact the validity of tuning of the response (e.g. see the slightly larger response to 135° in Fig 4B). We note that the destabilization is not contingent on there being local imbalance in the preference map, in general, moving in parameter space to the top left of Fig 7A, the unbounded activation would occur for all input orientations. In general, with reduced inhibition, there is no more overlap in the (RWex, βrec)-plane between the anatomical operating range of the model (between the white contours) and the range where orientation selective activation is confined to the FFF.
With increased inhibition strength (C = −0.6) the operating region for the model increases slightly shifting to lower RWex and extending to larger βrec values (Fig 9C). This illustrates how inhibition constrains the spread of activation to the FFF, even with larger orientation bias of lateral connections. Fixing RWex = 0.25, the operating region in terms of C and βrec is illustrated in Fig 9D, with the largest extent in βrec occurring at C = −0.4. For large values of C the model responses become predominantly feed-forward (input driven) and the profile of decay for the general and selective activation becomes similar (nSel/nAct tends to 1). As discussed above, without sufficient balance from inhibition activity can spread unbounded across cortex (top left region of Fig 9D).
Discussion
This study reported on a neural field model of L2/3 of primary visual cortex, investigating the dynamics of stimulus driven, localized patterns of cortical activity. The aim was to understand the relationship between the anatomical properties of lateral connections and the functional propagation of cortical activation. The model featured a coarse-grained description of average membrane potential in 2D, with discrete sub-populations for different orientations. Orientation was encoded using a realistic orientation preference map that modulates inputs from LGN via L4 and/or the lateral connections within L2/3 sub-populations. Anatomical data quantifying the orientation bias of lateral connections was used to constrain model parameters [14].
VSDI experiments in V1 have shown that local, oriented visual stimuli elicit stable orientation-selective activation within the retinotopic footprint [24]. Recently two studies confirmed these findings, one using optogenetic stimulation [27] and another anatomofunctional study [15]. The cortical activation dynamically extends far beyond the retinotopic footprint, but the peripheral spread stays non-selective, which could be surprising given anatomical studies showing the orientation specificity of long-range connections [7,10,8,11,9]. However, this result could actually be expected given that (i) the quantified orientation bias is small (ii) decreases with distance [14] and (iii) holds only for a sub-part the functionally activated hyper-column (L4, pinwheel and interneuron cells do not show this bias [12, 65], but see [14, 15] for the pinwheel dependence).
Based on these anatomical studies, we designed a new connectivity function, flexibly parametrized to investigate clustering of connections, their orientation bias and balance between excitation and inhibition. We adopted the non-orientation specific nature of local excitatory connections [14] and inhibitory connections [16]. Taking motivation from [16], longer-range excitatory connections are proposed here to, although decaying with distance, form in rings at multiples of Λ (hypercolumn separation). This allows for the following important features to be captured: that excitatory connections 1) drop in number at a range Λ/2, 2) have a peak at a range Λ and 3) can extend several mm across cortex. Two parameters were tuned to agree with the available data from [14], the width of peaks in number of excitatory connections and their orientation bias (note we did not include an explicit representation of L4 nor a special connectivity rule at pinwheel locations). Similar results and conclusions would be expected if other parameters had been varied, e.g. those controlling the amplitude of excitatory peaks, or the width of inhibition.
We found a significant overlap between the anatomically relevant parameter range and patterns of cortical activation consistent with imaging experiments. Hence the imaging results can be reconciled with the reported level of orientation bias from anatomical studies. Specifically, [24] found a sharp decay of orientation selective activation at the stimulus retinotopic footprint border, resulting in peripheral activation that was not orientation selective. Our results demonstrate that this sharp decay is contingent on three factors: the diffuse clustering of long-range connections, the intermediate range (consistent with anatomy) of their orientation bias and the sufficient balance between excitation and inhibition. We note that although long-range connections are orientation biased, the recruitment at the target of a local network without orientation bias could result in a general non-orientation specific activation [27]. Furthermore, if the orientation bias of lateral connections is excessively strong, or if inhibition is particularly weak, the network operates close to an instability leading to unbounded cortical activation (a testable prediction, see below).
Model predictions
Our modelling results predict a qualitative change in the spread of orientation selective activation for localized, oriented inputs if global inhibition strength is reduced. Under those conditions, we should observe selective activation outside of the retinotopic footprint, however, the exact orientation preference may not be preserved. Activation with spurious orientation preference is generated via spatial destablization of the localized activation generated by the input. These predictions could be addressed by manipulating the inhibitory cells pharmacologically [66–68] or optogenetically [27]. To confirm our model’s prediction though, one may have to go close to pathological epileptic conditions [69, 70]. Other approaches could be used by simply comparing anesthetized and awake conditions. Recently, [71] have indeed shown this to be a valuable approach for investigating E/I balance in the integrative properties of the cortical populations. Comparison of the dynamics of propagation of orientation-selective activity in awake or anesthetized conditions could hence provide the appropriate non-pathological test to probe our model’s prediction.
Spurious activation can also be generated by another mechanism. Activated regions inside the stimulated region can generate excitation at an equidistant range outside the stimulated region. The specific range is associated with the peak in the radial excitatory profile. This happens in the model when peaks in excitation are highly specific, in a parameter range that was ruled out from the operating regime of the model. This could be seen as an undesired consequence of the model’s design. However, the fact that, under particular circumstances, the preferred orientation of the horizontal propagation may be at odds with the underlying orientation preference map could unravel some new unexpected computational capacities of the horizontal network, which may be present in visual areas beyond V1/area 17. For instance, the ability to link information of position and orientation for non co-circular filters that could be of importance for processing objects with sharp angles. Inline with this hypothesis, [24] showed that the spread of orientation selective activity is not fixed but can increase when increasing spatial summation.
Comparison with other models
The transition to an unbounded spread across the network, as contingent on a spatial modulation parameter (like βrec controlling orientation bias in our model), has been observed in a one dimensional theoretical study of the neural field equation with purely excitatory connectivity [72]. A more common choice of connectivity function (e.g. difference of Gaussians) features a broader footprint for inhibition than for excitation [36, 37]. In our model excitation extends further than inhibition with additional peaks in excitation away from the origin [44]. The distance between excitation peaks fixes a regular length scale that stabilizes multi-bump patterns. In [41], without an explicit representation of orientation, localized inputs were shown to produce multiple bumps within a stimulated region. It was proposed that the connectivity’s excitatory peak separation could be equated with Λ (hypercolumn separation) and the spatial phase of multi-bump patterns governed by an orientation preference map. In the present work, we have shown that the localized patterns of orientation selective activation observed in [23] and [24] are well described by a superposition of these intrinsic multi-bump states.
Local models of orientation selectivity [32, 33] have been more widely studied than spatial models capturing interactions across columns. Spatial models of map development [55, 73, 46, 74, 57] do not focus on dynamics on sub-second timescales. Indeed, [74] does not consider dynamics or long-range connections within V1; see also commentaries in [75, 76]. The self-organizing map (SOM) model in [57] included, but did not show, the dynamics of stimuli responses. The focus of the study was on the long-timescale dynamics of map development, however, this class of model could be another candidate to investigate functional activation in the future. Integrate-and-fire neuron models of earlier thalamic and cortical processing stages proved successful for capturing the dynamics of orientation selectivity and tuning, but were restricted to a small patch of cortex without considering superficial layers accessible to imaging [77, 78]. [79] explored the role of patchy long-range connections in cortex in a general setting, whilst [80] looked at larger regions of cortex in an integrate-and-fire network, reporting complex spatio-temporal dynamics, but did not model orientation.
The neural field framework used here is an ideal level of description for comparison with VSDI recordings [38] that allowed us to simulate a large spatial domain extending beyond the local region of interest, thus avoiding issues with boundary effects. The four sub-population implementation used here was chosen because it allows for efficient computation of connectivity integrals using convolutions. In turn, this allowed for a comprehensive multi-parameter investigation of the model’s dynamics. Another choice would be to consider a single mixed population, where connection weights depend directly on orientation difference as has been considered in other spatial models [73, 81], including those posed in an elastic net framework [82, 83]. This would have the advantage of allowing direct implementation of the heterogeneity of long-range connections, however, the increased computational burden may not allow such a broad parameter investigation.
Perspectives
[27] used optogenetic stimulation in combination with visual inputs and observed a non-orientation specific linear addition of the two inputs, which could be explored in our modelling framework. The model could also be used to investigate selective recruitment and spatial summation in regions between localized oriented stimuli. Two further properties were described in [24] that can be further investigated. First, as demonstrated with intracellular recordings, the lack of orientation selectivity observed at the mesoscopic level (VSDI) was due to a diversity of microscopic rules: some cells received untuned presynaptic input, but others a tuned presynaptic input with preferred orientation either agreeing with or different from the recorded cell. Such diversity in local cellular rules is to be linked to our observations that, with narrow excitatory peak widths or strong orientation bias (Fig 7B) or reduced inhibition (Fig 8), our model can easily produce spurious selective propagation. Adding diversity in the connectivity rules can thus easily lead to natural diversity in the tuning of the propagation. Second, increasing spatial summation increases the slope of selectivity decay at the stimulus boundary, whilst selective propagation reaches further across cortex; the model could also account for this property. More generally, the model could be used to make predictions to decipher the selective functional connectivity rules that link position and orientation in cortical space. For example, the model could be extended to differentiate inhibitory cell sub-classes as reported in [16]. As such it could generate functional predictions on e.g. the role of long-range basket cell connections that preferentially target cross orientations.
A lumped description of inhibition and excitation was used here with local cross-orientation inhibition. By separating out inhibitory and excitatory populations, one could consider different profiles for cross-orientation interactions including their spatial profile. This may change the model’s behavior and would be important if it is extended to consider inputs with multiple orientations that overlap spatially. Earlier stages of cortical and thalamic processing could be incorporated with differing properties of orientation tuning and bias of connections across V1 layers [84]. The four sub-population implementation used here can be viewed as a coarse discretization from a continuous representation of orientation, which could be considered in future work. For example, a recent paper studied spatio-temporal patterns with continuous orientation, but only on a 1D spatial domain [85]. Theoretical work characterising localized states in 2D space plus orientation would be an important first step. An extended feature space including spatial frequency (SF) could be used to investigate lateral connections in light of recent work showing interesting interactions between orientation and SF maps [86].
Conclusion
Our model of orientation selectivity in V1 is the first of its kind, capturing the spatio-temporal dynamical spread of localized patterns of activation with a representation of orientation. Our study addressed an apparent conflict between the orientation bias of lateral excitatory connections in L2/3 of V1, as characterized in anatomical studies, and imaging studies on the lateral propagation of cortical activity for localized oriented visual stimuli. Simulations with the neural field model illustrated that observed levels of orientation bias in anatomical studies actually predict long-range activation outside of the retinotopic footprint of the stimulus, but with a sharply decaying profile of orientation selectivity, as observed in imaging studies. Without this sharp decay, which might occur with excessive orientation bias or diminished inhibition strength, the network could destabilize leading to unbounded spread of cortical activation.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Zoltán Kisvárday, David McLaughlin, Lyle Muller and Jonathan Touboul for valuable feedback on the manuscript. The authors would like to thank Daniele Avitabile, Gregory Faye and David Lloyd for stimulating discussions during the conceptualisation of the model. Along with the numerical methods described in [41], an in-house numerical continuation scheme written by Daniele Avitabile was used in parameter investigation for a single-sub-population version of the model.
Data Availability
All relevant information to reproduce the results are within the paper. Model code allowing for independent reproduction of the results is available in the supplemental information (S1 Code) and on the following online repository https://github.com/QBME/rankin-chavane-neural-field/.
Funding Statement
JR was supported at NYU on a postdoc fellowship from the Swartz Foundation. FC is supported by ANR project BalaV1 (ANR-13-BSV4-0014-02). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Braitenberg V. A note on myeloarchitectonics. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1962;118(2):141–156. 10.1002/cne.901180202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Morphology and intracortical projections of functionally characterised neurones in the cat visual cortex. Nature. 1979;280(5718):120–125. 10.1038/280120a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Fisken RA, Garey LJ, Powell TPS. The Intrinsic, Association and Commissural Connections of Area 17 of the Visual Cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences. 1975;272(919):487–536. 10.1098/rstb.1975.0099 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Creutzfeldt OD, Garey LJ, Kuroda R, Wolff JR. The distribution of degenerating axons after small lesions in the intact and isolated visual cortex of the cat. Experimental Brain Research. 1977;27(3):419–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Rockland KS, Lund JS. Widespread periodic intrinsic connections in the tree shrew visual cortex. Science. 1982;215(4539):1532–1534. 10.1126/science.7063863 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Mitchison G, Crick F. Long axons within the striate cortex: their distribution, orientation, and patterns of connection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1982;79(11):3661–3665. 10.1073/pnas.79.11.3661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Columnar specificity of intrinsic horizontal and corticocortical connections in cat visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9(7):2432–2442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Kisvárday Z, Toth E, Rausch M, Eysel U. Orientation-specific relationship between populations of excitatory and inhibitory lateral connections in the visual cortex of the cat. Cerebral Cortex. 1997;7(7):605–618. 10.1093/cercor/7.7.605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Schmidt KE, Kim DS, Singer W, Bonhoeffer T, Löwel S. Functional specificity of long-range intrinsic and interhemispheric connections in the visual cortex of strabismic cats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17(14):5480–5492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Bosking WH, Zhang Y, Schofield B, Fitzpatrick D. Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17(6):2112–2127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Malach R, Amir Y, Harel M, Grinvald A. Relationship between intrinsic connections and functional architecture revealed by optical imaging and in vivo targeted biocytin injections in primate striate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1993;90(22):10469–10473. 10.1073/pnas.90.22.10469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Yousef T, Bonhoeffer T, Kim DS, Eysel UT, Tth, Kisvrday ZF. Orientation topography of layer 4 lateral networks revealed by optical imaging in cat visual cortex (area 18). European Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;11(12):4291–4308. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00863.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Yousef T, Tóth E, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Kisvárday ZF. Topography of orientation centre connections in the primary visual cortex of the cat. NeuroReport. 2001;12(8):1693–1699. 10.1097/00001756-200106130-00035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Buzás P, Kovács K, Ferecskó AS, Budd JML, Eysel UT, Kisvárday ZF. Model-based analysis of excitatory lateral connections in the visual cortex. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2006;499(6):861–881. 10.1002/cne.21134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Martin KA, Roth S, Rusch ES. Superficial layer pyramidal cells communicate heterogeneously between multiple functional domains of cat primary visual cortex. Nature communications. 2014;5 10.1038/ncomms6252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Buzás P, Eysel UT, Adorján P, Kisvárday ZF. Axonal topography of cortical basket cells in relation to orientation, direction, and ocular dominance maps. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2001;437(3):259–285. 10.1002/cne.1282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Kisvárday Z, Kim DS, Eysel U, Bonhoeffer T. Relationship Between Lateral Inhibitory Connections and the Topography of the Orientation Map in Cat Visual Cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;6(10):1619–1632. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1994.tb00553.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Bonhoeffer T, Grinvald A. Iso-orientation domains in cat visual cortex are arranged in pinwheel-like patterns. Nature. 1991;353(6343):429–431. 10.1038/353429a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Grinvald A, Hildesheim R. VSDI: a new era in functional imaging of cortical dynamics. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5(11):874–885. 10.1038/nrn1536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Shoham D, Glaser DE, Arieli A, Kenet T, Wijnbergen C, Toledo Y, et al. Imaging Cortical Dynamics at High Spatial and Temporal Resolution with Novel Blue Voltage-Sensitive Dyes. Neuron. 1999;24(4):791–802. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81027-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Chemla S, Chavane F. Voltage-sensitive dye imaging: Technique review and models. Journal of Physiology-Paris. 2010;104(1–2):40–50. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2009.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Chemla S, Muller L, Reynaud A, Takerkart S, Destexhe A, Chavane F. Improving voltage-sensitive dye imaging: with a little help from computational approaches. Neurophotonics. 2017;4(3):031215–031215. 10.1117/1.NPh.4.3.031215 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Sharon D, Jancke D, Chavane F, Na’aman S, Grinvald A. Cortical response field dynamics in cat visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(12):2866–2877. 10.1093/cercor/bhm019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Chavane F, Sharon D, Jancke D, Marre O, Frégnac Y, Grinvald A. Lateral spread of orientation selectivity in V1 is controlled by intracortical cooperativity. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2011;5:4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Bringuier V, Chavane F, Glaeser L, Frgnac Y. Horizontal Propagation of Visual Activity in the Synaptic Integration Field of Area 17 Neurons. Science. 1999;283(5402):695–699. 10.1126/science.283.5402.695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Muller L, Reynaud A, Chavane F, Destexhe A. The stimulus-evoked population response in visual cortex of awake monkey is a propagating wave. Nature communications. 2014;5 10.1038/ncomms4675 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Huang X, Elyada YM, Bosking WH, Walker T, Fitzpatrick D. Optogenetic Assessment of Horizontal Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(14):4976–4990. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4116-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Alonso J, Kremkow J. F1000Prime Recommendation of Chavane et al 2011. F1000Prime. 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Alonso J, Kremkow J. F1000Prime Recommendation of Huang et al 2014. F1000Prime. 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Wilson HR, Cowan JD. Excitatory and inhibitory interactions in localized populations of model neurons. Biophysical journal. 1972;12(1):1–24. 10.1016/S0006-3495(72)86068-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Amari S. Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biological Cybernetics. 1977;27(2):77–87. 10.1007/BF00337259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Ben-Yishai R, Bar-Or RL, Sompolinsky H. Theory of orientation tuning in visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1995;92(9):3844 10.1073/pnas.92.9.3844 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Hansel D, Sompolinsky H. Modeling feature selectivity in local cortical circuits. Methods of neuronal modeling. 1997; p. 499–567. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Bressloff PC, Cowan JD, Golubitsky M, Thomas PJ, Wiener MC. Geometric visual hallucinations, Euclidean symmetry and the functional architecture of striate cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences. 2001;356(1407):299–330. 10.1098/rstb.2000.0769 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Bressloff P. Spontaneous symmetry breaking in self-organizing neural fields. Biological Cybernetics. 2005;93:256–274. 10.1007/s00422-005-0002-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Coombes S. Waves, bumps, and patterns in neural field theories. Biological Cybernetics. 2005;93:91–108. 10.1007/s00422-005-0574-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Bressloff PC. Spatiotemporal dynamics of continuum neural fields. Journal of Physics A: Mathematical and Theoretical. 2012;45(3):033001 10.1088/1751-8113/45/3/033001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Grimbert F. Mesoscopic models of cortical structures. University of Nice Sophia-Antipolis; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Markounikau V, Igel C, Grinvald A, Jancke D. A dynamic neural field model of mesoscopic cortical activity captured with voltage-sensitive dye imaging. PLOS Computational Biology. 2010;6(9):e1000919 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Chemla S, Chavane F. A biophysical cortical column model to study the multi-component origin of the VSDI signal. NeuroImage. 2010;53(2):420–438. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Rankin J, Avitabile D, Baladron J, Faye G, Lloyd DJ. Continuation of localized coherent structures in nonlocal neural field equations. SIAM Journal on Scientific Computing. 2014;36(1):B70–B93. 10.1137/130918721 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Laing CR, Troy WC, Gutkin B, Ermentrout GB. Multiple bumps in a neuronal model of working memory. SIAM Journal on Applied Mathematics. 2002;63(1):62–97. 10.1137/S0036139901389495 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Faye G, Rankin J, Chossat P. Localized states in an unbounded neural field equation with smooth firing rate function: a multi-parameter analysis. Journal of mathematical biology. 2013;66(6):1303–1338. 10.1007/s00285-012-0532-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Laing CR, Troy WC. PDE methods for nonlocal models. SIAM Journal on Applied Dynamical Systems. 2003;2(3):487–516. 10.1137/030600040 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Faye G, Rankin J, Lloyd DJ. Localized radial bumps of a neural field equation on the Euclidean plane and the Poincaré disc. Nonlinearity. 2013;26(2):437 10.1088/0951-7715/26/2/437 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Kaschube M, Schnabel M, Löwel S, Coppola DM, White LE, Wolf F. Universality in the evolution of orientation columns in the visual cortex. Science. 2010;330(6007):1113–1116. 10.1126/science.1194869 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Uniformity of monkey striate cortex: a parallel relationship between field size, scatter, and magnification factor. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1974;158(3):295–305. 10.1002/cne.901580305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Hübener M, Shoham D, Grinvald A, Bonhoeffer T. Spatial Relationships among Three Columnar Systems in Cat Area 17. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17(23):9270–9284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Shmuel A, Grinvald A. Functional Organization for Direction of Motion and Its Relationship to Orientation Maps in Cat Area 18. Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16(21):6945–6964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Mountcastle VB. Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat’s somatic sensory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1957;20(4):408–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 1968;195(1):215–243. 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Ferrier lecture: Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 1977;198(1130):1–59. 10.1098/rspb.1977.0085 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Tanaka S, Miyashita M, Ribot J. Roles of visual experience and intrinsic mechanism in the activity-dependent self-organization of orientation maps: theory and experiment. Neural networks. 2004;17(8):1363–1375. 10.1016/j.neunet.2004.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Albus K. A quantitative study of the projection area of the central and the paracentral visual field in area 17 of the cat. Experimental Brain Research. 1975;24(2):159–179. 10.1007/BF00234061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Niebur E, Wörgötter F. Design principles of columnar organization in visual cortex. Neural Computation. 1994;6(4):602–614. 10.1162/neco.1994.6.4.602 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Wolf F. Symmetry, multistability, and long-range interactions in brain development. Physical Review Letters. 2005;95(20):208701 10.1103/PhysRevLett.95.208701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Stevens JLR, Law JS, Antolík J, Bednar JA. Mechanisms for stable, robust, and adaptive development of orientation maps in the primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(40):15747–15766. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1037-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Keil W, Kaschube M, Schnabel M, Kisvarday ZF, Lwel S, Coppola DM, et al. Response to Comment on “Universality in the Evolution of Orientation Columns in the Visual Cortex”. Science. 2012;336(6080):413–413. 10.1126/science.1206416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Schottdorf M, Keil W, Coppola D, White LE, Wolf F. Random Wiring, Ganglion Cell Mosaics, and the Functional Architecture of the Visual Cortex. PLOS Computational Biology. 2015;11(11):e1004602 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Gabbott PL, Somogyi P. Quantitative distribution of GABA-immunoreactive neurons in the visual cortex (area 17) of the cat. Experimental Brain Research. 1986;61(2):323–331. 10.1007/BF00239522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Hendry SH, Schwark HD, Jones EG, Yan J. Numbers and proportions of GABA-immunoreactive neurons in different areas of monkey cerebral cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 1987;7(5):1503–1519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Blasdel GG. Orientation selectivity, preference, and continuity in monkey striate cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12(8):3139–3161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Naka K, Rushton W. S-potentials from colour units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae). The Journal of Physiology. 1966;185(3):536 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Sclar G, Maunsell JHR, Lennie P. Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey. Vision Research. 1990;30(1):1–10. 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90123-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Karube F, Kisvárday ZF. Axon topography of layer IV spiny cells to orientation map in the cat primary visual cortex (area 18). Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21(6):1443–1458. 10.1093/cercor/bhq232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Sillito AM. The contribution of inhibitory mechanisms to the receptive field properties of neurones in the striate cortex of the cat. The Journal of Physiology. 1975;250(2):305–329. 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Crook JM, Eysel UT. GABA-induced inactivation of functionally characterized sites in cat visual cortex (area 18): effects on orientation tuning. Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12(5):1816–1825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Crook JM, Kisvrday ZF, Eysel UT. Evidence for a contribution of lateral inhibition to orientation tuning and direction selectivity in cat visual cortex: reversible inactivation of functionally characterized sites combined with neuroanatomical tracing techniques. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;10(6):2056–2075. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00218.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Trevelyan AJ, Sussillo D, Watson BO, Yuste R. Modular Propagation of Epileptiform Activity: Evidence for an Inhibitory Veto in Neocortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(48):12447–12455. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2787-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Trevelyan AJ, Sussillo D, Yuste R. Feedforward Inhibition Contributes to the Control of Epileptiform Propagation Speed. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(13):3383–3387. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0145-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Chemla S, Chavane F. Effects of GABAA kinetics on cortical population activity: computational studies and physiological confirmations. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;115(6):2867–2879. 10.1152/jn.00352.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Avitabile D, Schmidt H. Snakes and ladders in an inhomogeneous neural field model. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena. 2015;294:24–36. 10.1016/j.physd.2014.11.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Blumenfeld B, Bibitchkov D, Tsodyks M. Neural network model of the primary visual cortex: From functional architecture to lateral connectivity and back. Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 2006;20(2):219 10.1007/s10827-006-6307-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Paik SB, Ringach DL. Retinal origin of orientation maps in visual cortex. Nature neuroscience. 2011;14(7):919–925. 10.1038/nn.2824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Hore VRA, Troy JB, Eglen SJ. Parasol cell mosaics are unlikely to drive the formation of structured orientation maps in primary visual cortex. Visual Neuroscience. 2012;29(6):283–299. 10.1017/S0952523812000338 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Schottdorf M, Eglen SJ, Wolf F, Keil W. Can Retinal Ganglion Cell Dipoles Seed Iso-Orientation Domains in the Visual Cortex? PLOS ONE. 2014;9(1):e86139 10.1371/journal.pone.0086139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. McLaughlin D, Shapley R, Shelley M, Wielaard DJ. A neuronal network model of macaque primary visual cortex (V1): Orientation selectivity and dynamics in the input layer 4Ca. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2000;97(14):8087–8092. 10.1073/pnas.110135097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Chariker L, Shapley R, Young LS. Orientation Selectivity from Very Sparse LGN Inputs in a Comprehensive Model of Macaque V1 Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36(49):12368–12384. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2603-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Voges N, Schüz A, Aertsen A, Rotter S. A modeler’s view on the spatial structure of intrinsic horizontal connectivity in the neocortex. Progress in neurobiology. 2010;92(3):277–292. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Voges N, Perrinet L. Complex dynamics in recurrent cortical networks based on spatially realistic connectivities. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 2012;6:41 10.3389/fncom.2012.00041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Baker TI, Cowan JD. Spontaneous pattern formation and pinning in the primary visual cortex. Journal of physiology. 2009;103(1-2):52–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Goodhill GJ, Cimponeriu A. Analysis of the elastic net model applied to the formation of ocular dominance and orientation columns. Network: Computation in Neural Systems. 2000;11(2):153–168. 10.1088/0954-898X_11_2_303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Keil W, Wolf F. Coverage, continuity, and visual cortical architecture. Neural systems & circuits. 2011;1(1):1–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Ringach DL, Shapley RM, Hawken MJ. Orientation Selectivity in Macaque V1: Diversity and Laminar Dependence. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(13):5639–5651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Bressloff P, Carroll S. Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Neural Fields on Product Spaces. SIAM Journal on Applied Dynamical Systems. 2014;13(4):1620–1653. 10.1137/140976339 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Ribot J, Romagnoni A, Milleret C, Bennequin D, Touboul J. Pinwheel-dipole configuration in cat early visual cortex. NeuroImage. 2016;128:63–73. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant information to reproduce the results are within the paper. Model code allowing for independent reproduction of the results is available in the supplemental information (S1 Code) and on the following online repository https://github.com/QBME/rankin-chavane-neural-field/.