Abstract
Cerebral blood flow (CBF) is related to integrated neuronal activity of the brain whereas EEG provides a more direct measurement of transient neuronal activity. Therefore, we addressed what happens in the brain during sleep, combining CBF and EEG recordings. The dynamic relationship of CBF with slow-wave activity (SWA; EEG sleep intensity marker) corroborated vigilance state specific (i.e., wake, non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep stages N1-N3, wake after sleep) differences of CBF e.g. in the posterior cingulate, basal ganglia, and thalamus, indicating their role in sleep-wake regulation and/or sleep processes. These newly observed dynamic correlations of CBF with SWA – namely a temporal relationship during continuous NREM sleep in individuals – additionally implicate an impact of sleep intensity on the brain’s metabolism. Furthermore, we propose that some of the aforementioned brain areas that also have been shown to be affected in disorders of consciousness might therefore contribute to the emergence of consciousness.
Introduction
Although extensive research has been directed towards the question of why and how we sleep, until now, these issues remain largely unsolved. Amongst other hypotheses, sleep has e.g. been reported to affect responses of the immune system1–3 and to play a role in brain development4. It has also been hypothesized to conserve energy5,6 and has been suggested to act on the ‘downscaling’ of synaptic weights7–9.
While the underlying mechanisms of sleep remain unknown, sleep physiology has been well described and studied, both at baseline and in response to challenges, such as sleep restriction or sleep deprivation (i.e. the curtailment or absence of sleep). A physiological and reliable indicator of sleep pressure is slow-wave activity (SWA; power of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) in the 1–4.5 Hz range), as its level reflects prior sleep-wake history. SWA is increased after sleep restriction or deprivation10,11 and dissipates from high levels at the beginning of sleep to lower levels in the course of sleep. SWA is also a reliable indicator of sleep intensity or sleep depth, with high levels during deep NREM sleep12.
Up to now, positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies revealed mainly decreases of brain activity (assessed by regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) or blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals) during NREM sleep, compared to waking or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep13–18.
Reoccurring throughout these studies, decreased brain activity was reported in subcortical areas (thalamus, brainstem, basal ganglia) and cortical regions (prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, precuneus). Among the brain areas that showed these decreases were neuronal populations involved in arousal (such as the basal ganglia and brainstem) and awakening, as well as structures which are most active during wakefulness (e.g. anterior cingulate, prefrontal cortex and precuneus19,20). The deactivation of these areas during NREM sleep may be interpreted as an involvement of sleep-promoting processes, as well as the occurrence of local cellular homeostasis. This could imply a role of NREM sleep in the recovery of brain energy, which has been hypothesized as a possible function of sleep5,6. Additionally, brain areas as e.g. the brainstem and the cerebellum have been shown to be involved in generation of slow waves during NREM sleep21,22 and showed decreases of rCBF as a function of the level of SWA15,16.
Slow waves (giving rise to SWA for the main part) exist as two types with distinct sites of origin (thalamic and cortical) and different underlying mechanisms of generation23,24. The mechanism of thalamically generated slow waves by way of intrinsic oscillations of thalamocortical neurons is quite well understood25. The generating mechanisms of cortically originating slow waves, however, are less clear but their existence has been demonstrated by their persistence in animals after thalamectomy23.
In animals, EEG slow waves have been linked to so-called up and down states intracellularly, describing periods of no action potential firing (down and off states, in single and multi unit activity, respectively) during the troughs of EEG slow waves and burst of firing (up and on states, in single and multi unit activity, respectively) during the peaks of EEG slow waves, respectively26,27. In epileptic patients with scalp and intracerebral EEG recordings as well as unit firing, it was recently established that the alteration pattern of burst-mode firing and silence periods during slow waves is conserved in humans as well28. Furthermore, this study could show that different brain areas can be in opposite states, i.e., some brain regions might be silent in an off state while at the same time other brain areas are in an on state. It is hypothesized that this burst-mode firing due to its periods of silence might be less energy demanding than tonic firing (see discussion).
Studies have proposed the frontal cortex as the initiation site of slow waves21,22. These results are in line with findings of PET studies that mainly associated prefrontal areas with negative correlations between rCBF and SWA (i.e. increased SWA associated with decreased rCBF) in areas such as the medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex15,16. On the other hand, a BOLD study investigating slow waves in an ‘event-related’ approach observed significant activations associated with large slow waves (>140 μV) in the brainstem, cerebellum, parahippocampal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, precuneus and posterior cingulate gyrus, while slow waves with smaller amplitudes (75–140 μV) were associated with frontal areas29. In this study, no BOLD signal decreases related to slow waves were reported.
We aimed to investigate the modulation of CBF on the one hand in relation to wakefulness and on the other hand as a function of NREM sleep depth. As prior studies suggested a connection between CBF and vigilance stage as well as a relationship between CBF and SWA, a further objective was to investigate CBF across different distinct vigilance stages (namely, pre-sleep wake, NREM sleep stages N1, N2, N3, and wake after sleep). In addition to quantifying mean CBF of different distinct, categorized wake and sleep stages and its topographical aspects, we were interested in how the temporal evolution of CBF correlated with the one of SWA during NREM sleep. Therefore, we exploited the progress that has been made in terms of imaging techniques and employed simultaneous EEG/fMRI-arterial spin labeling (ASL). ASL allows the measurement of absolute CBF, analogous to PET measurements, but with the advantage of being non-invasive and providing a much higher temporal resolution (i.e., one image approximately every 10 s compared to the PET resolution of minutes30,31).
Thus, we addressed two major questions. First, we wanted to confirm with fMRI-ASL recordings mean CBF differences between vigilance stages, both ‘locally’ with respect to differences in specific brain areas as well as ‘globally’ in terms of mean whole brain CBF values. Second, we were interested to establish which brain areas show a temporal relationship between CBF and NREM sleep intensity (for the first time investigating correlations between the continuous time courses of both CBF and SWA in individuals during continuous NREM sleep).
Results
Participants included in the analyses (n = 19) were of average chronotype (no extreme morning or evening types) and showed normal levels of day-time sleepiness (Table S1).
Mean CBF across Vigilance States
To visualize the level of topographic accuracy that can be achieved with CBF images, and to show how different the CBF is during different vigilance states even by pure visual comparison, the mean CBF is illustrated for pre-sleep wake, NREM sleep stage 3 (N3) and post-sleep wake in Fig. 1.
Mean CBF differed in all contrasts assessed (i.e. pre-sleep W – post-sleep W, pre-sleep W - N1/N2/N3, pre-sleep W - N1, pre-sleep W - N2, pre-sleep W - N3, and the inverse of each contrast, see also Methods section; one-way within-subject ANOVA; family-wise error (FWE) corrected for p < 0.05 with a cluster extent of 20 voxels). Details are provided in Figs 2 and 3, in Tables 1 and 2, as well as in the following paragraphs.
Table 1.
Brain regions | L/R | Cluster | MNI coordinates | T value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||
pre-sleep W > N2 | ||||||
Frontal_Sup_R (aal) | R | 4432 | 15 | 59 | 1 | 13.04 |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | L | −21 | 44 | −8 | 12.64 | |
Frontal_Sup_Orb_L (aal) | L | −18 | 32 | −14 | 12.63 | |
SupraMarginal_L (aal) | L | 141 | −54 | −40 | 34 | 8.90 |
SupraMarginal_L (aal) | L | −51 | −46 | 25 | 8.27 | |
Supramarginal Gyrus | L | −39 | −49 | 25 | 7.45 | |
Sub-Gyral (Frontal Lobe) | R | 47 | 24 | −22 | 43 | 8.19 |
Precentral_R (aal) | R | 30 | −19 | 55 | 7.48 | |
Supp_Motor_Area_R (aal) | R | 9 | −19 | 58 | 5.50 | |
Lingual_L (aal) | L | 42 | −18 | −82 | −2 | 7.83 |
Cuneus (Occipital Lobe) | L | −18 | −79 | 7 | 6.56 | |
Cerebellar Tonsil | R | 36 | 30 | −55 | −41 | 6.97 |
Sub-Gyral (Parietal Lobe) | R | 34 | 33 | −52 | 25 | 6.94 |
Cerebelum_8_R (aal) | R | 25 | 12 | −67 | −35 | 6.68 |
Precuneus | R | 20 | 30 | −49 | 52 | 6.67 |
Sub-Gyral (Parietal Lobe) | R | 21 | −46 | 46 | 5.89 | |
pre-sleep W > N3 | ||||||
Frontal_Sup_R (aal) | R | 554 | 15 | 59 | 4 | 11.14 |
Cingulum_Mid_R (aal) | R | 9 | 38 | 31 | 10.10 | |
Cingulum_Ant_R (aal) | R | 9 | 41 | 1 | 9.42 | |
Frontal_Sup_Orb_L (aal) | L | 310 | −18 | 32 | −14 | 10.97 |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | L | −21 | 44 | −8 | 10.80 | |
Frontal_Mid_Orb_L (aal) | L | −30 | 47 | −2 | 9.91 | |
Caudate_L (aal) | L | 146 | −6 | 8 | 7 | 10.44 |
Caudate_L (aal) | L | −12 | −4 | 19 | 5.88 | |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | R | 46 | 33 | 44 | −2 | 8.85 |
Frontal_Inf_Orb_R (aal) | R | 27 | 29 | −14 | 7.44 | |
Frontal_Inf_Oper_R (aal) | R | 93 | 42 | 11 | 7 | 8.43 |
Insula_R (aal) | R | 45 | 2 | −2 | 7.41 | |
Extra-Nuclear (Right Cerebrum) | R | 36 | 8 | −11 | 5.90 | |
Sub-Gyral (Temporal Lobe) | R | 205 | 39 | −28 | −5 | 8.19 |
Sub-Gyral (Temporal Lobe) | R | 36 | −40 | 7 | 7.80 | |
Temporal_Sup_R (aal) | R | 57 | −37 | 7 | 7.30 | |
Frontal_Inf_Orb_R (aal) | R | 37 | 21 | 17 | −17 | 8.04 |
Putamen | R | 18 | 5 | −11 | 6.34 | |
Putamen_R (aal) | R | 21 | 11 | 1 | 6.13 | |
Precentral Gyrus | R | 70 | 45 | −4 | 25 | 7.53 |
Precentral_R (aal) | R | 48 | −4 | 37 | 7.38 | |
Rolandic_Oper_R (aal) | R | 54 | 2 | 10 | 6.45 | |
SupraMarginal_R (aal) | R | 66 | 57 | −28 | 31 | 7.17 |
SupraMarginal_R (aal) | R | 48 | −34 | 28 | 6.70 | |
Postcentral_R (aal) | R | 51 | −25 | 43 | 6.24 | |
Rolandic_Oper_L (aal) | L | 39 | −51 | −1 | 4 | 7.12 |
Temporal_Sup_L (aal) | L | −45 | −7 | −5 | 6.18 | |
N2 > pre-sleep W | ||||||
Calcarine_R (aal) | R | 167 | 18 | −58 | 7 | 10.67 |
Cerebelum_6_R (aal) | R | 55 | 27 | −73 | −17 | 9.22 |
Cerebelum_6_R (aal) | R | 33 | −58 | −23 | 6.45 | |
Cerebelum_6_R (aal) | R | 24 | −58 | −17 | 5.41 | |
Lingual_R (aal) | R | 20 | 15 | −85 | −11 | 6.96 |
N3 > pre-sleep W | ||||||
Calcarine_R (aal) | R | 800 | 18 | −58 | 7 | 11.66 |
Cerebelum_6_R (aal) | R | 27 | −73 | −17 | 11.21 | |
Lingual_R (aal) | R | 12 | −85 | −11 | 10.62 | |
Hippocampus_R (aal) | R | 20 | 27 | −37 | 1 | 9.35 |
Cerebelum_6_L (aal) | L | 45 | −24 | −79 | −20 | 9.32 |
Cingulum_Mid_L (aal) | L | 21 | −12 | −19 | 40 | 8.54 |
Temporal_Mid_R (aal) | R | 44 | 45 | −61 | 7 | 7.93 |
Frontal_Inf_Orb_L (aal) | L | 26 | −48 | 41 | −17 | 7.65 |
Middle Occipital Gyrus | R | 30 | 33 | −79 | 1 | 7.03 |
Occipital_Inf_R (aal) | R | 33 | −82 | −8 | 6.30 | |
Cingulate Gyrus | L | 21 | −12 | 11 | 34 | 6.65 |
Table 2.
Brain regions | L/R | Cluster | MNI coordinates | T value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||
pre-sleep W > post-sleep W | ||||||
Frontal_Mid_Orb_L (aal) | L | 9229 | −33 | 47 | −2 | 13.86 |
Frontal_Inf_Orb_L (aal) | L | −30 | 29 | −14 | 13.68 | |
Frontal_Sup_R (aal) | R | 15 | 59 | 4 | 13.66 | |
Temporal_Mid_L (aal) | L | 67 | −63 | −46 | −11 | 8.01 |
Temporal_Inf_L (aal) | L | −54 | −55 | −20 | 7.85 | |
Calcarine_R (aal) | R | 22 | 24 | −73 | 4 | 7.30 |
Precuneus | L | 24 | −18 | −49 | 40 | 6.82 |
Cerebelum_Crus2_R (aal) | R | 33 | 6 | −88 | −26 | 6.64 |
Cerebelum_Crus2_L (aal) | L | -6 | −88 | −26 | 6.32 | |
post-sleep W > pre-sleep W | ||||||
Precuneus | R | 145 | 18 | −58 | 13 | 10.00 |
Mean CBF Maps
CBF during NREM Sleep Compared to Wake
Figure 2 depicts brain areas with lower CBF (left panel, cold colors) as well as higher CBF (right panel, warm colors) during NREM sleep stages N2 (cyan and yellow) or N3 (blue and red) compared to pre-sleep wake. Table 1 lists all significant clusters in detail.
Areas that showed decreases in CBF during sleep comprised e.g. the precuneus, anterior cingulate, medial frontal gyrus, parts of the basal ganglia (caudate, putamen) and the insula, as well as parts of the parahippocampal gyrus (Fig. 2, left panel; Table 1). Increases in CBF during NREM sleep stages N2 and N3 were mainly located in occipital parts of the brain such as the lingual gyrus and the cerebellum, and in a small cluster in the hippocampus (Fig. 2, right panel; Table 1).
CBF Differences between Pre-Sleep and Post-Sleep Wake
Figure 3 illustrates the brain areas with decreased CBF (left panel, green) as well as higher CBF (right panel, magenta) during pre-sleep wake compared to post-sleep wake. Table 2 provides all significant clusters in detail.
Significantly lower CBF during pre-sleep wake compared to post-sleep wake overlapped with the area of higher CBF during N2/N3 compared to pre-sleep wake, namely a concise part of the posterior cingulate (compare Figs 2 and 3, right panels).
Areas with higher CBF during pre-sleep wake compared to post-sleep wake were very similar to the areas that showed decreased CBF during NREM sleep N2/N3 compared to pre-sleep wake (compare Figs 2 and 3, left panels). These areas comprised the anterior cingulate, medial frontal gyrus and the caudate, as well as the bilateral superior temporal gyri, the parahippocampal gyrus and the hippocampus.
Mean CBF Values
Mean values of whole brain and grey matter (GM) CBF (Fig. S1) differed between vigilance stages (factor ‘condition’ (pre-sleep wake, N1, N2, N3, post-sleep wake) p < 0.01, and p < 0.0001, respectively; mixed model repeated measures ANOVA). Post-hoc comparisons (Tukey-Kramer corrected for multiple comparisons) revealed that CBF was highest during pre-sleep wake, and lowest during post-sleep wake. Further, in N3, CBF was higher than during post-sleep wake both for whole brain and GM measures. GM CBF in N2 was lower than in pre-sleep wake and GM CBF in N1 was higher than in post-sleep wake (Fig. S1).
Correlation of CBF and SWA
Figure 4 displays the topographical distribution of mean correlations of CBF and SWA across participants. The correlation coefficients were derived from the partial correlations between the time course of SWA and CBF in all voxels of the brain. CBF correlated both positively (pink) and negatively (cyan) with SWA in spatially distinct brain areas. Positive correlations (increased CBF with increased SWA) were mainly located in the occipital lobe (lingual gyrus, visual cortex) whereas negative correlations (decreased CBF with increased SWA) seemed to occur more abundantly and were especially present in prefrontal areas (anterior and medial parts of the cingulate cortex), the precuneus, the basal ganglia and the thalamus. Markedly, the areas with strongest positive correlations corresponded to the areas with increased CBF during N3 compared to wake, while the most pronounced negative correlations occurred in those areas that were associated with decreased CBF during N3 compared to wake (compare Figs 2 and 3).
CBF and SWA were significantly correlated in distinct clusters (statistical parametric maps (SPM32) thresholded at p < 0.05 with FWE correction for multiple comparisons and a cluster extent threshold of 20 voxels, see Table 3, Fig. 5; cyan and yellow for negative and positive correlations, respectively). Due to the lower signal to noise ratio (SNR) of the perfusion images, the SPM{t} maps were additionally thresholded at p < 0.001 uncorrected33,34 to display trends of the correlations, also with a cluster extent of 20 (see Table 3, Fig. 5; blue and red for negative and positive correlations, respectively).
Table 3.
Brain regions | L/R | Cluster | MNI coordinates | T value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||
Negative correlations CBF SWA | ||||||
Anterior Cingulate | R | 423 | 6 | 44 | 7 | 14.96 |
Anterior Cingulate | L | −3 | 41 | 7 | 14.88 | |
Cingulum_Ant_L | L | 0 | 41 | 19 | 13.59 | |
Insula_R (aal) | R | 50 | 42 | 5 | −8 | 13.60 |
Caudate_R (aal) | R | 156 | 12 | 14 | 4 | 11.78 |
Sub-Gyral (Frontal Lobe) | R | 21 | 14 | −11 | 9.57 | |
Medial Frontal Gyrus | R | 9 | 17 | −17 | 9.52 | |
Thalamus_R (aal) | R | 64 | 6 | −10 | 4 | 9.67 |
Subthalamic Nucleus | L | −9 | −16 | −5 | 8.68 | |
Midbrain | L | −6 | −25 | −5 | 8.39 | |
Putamen | R | 39 | 30 | −10 | −8 | 9.50 |
Pallidum_R | 24 | −1 | 1 | 9.13 | ||
Caudate_L (aal) | L | 21 | −6 | 14 | 1 | 8.55 |
Caudate_L (aal) | −12 | 11 | 7 | 8.11 | ||
Positive correlations CBF SWA | ||||||
Lingual Gyrus | L | 305 | −15 | −79 | −11 | 11.20 |
Lingual Gyrus | L | −3 | −85 | −14 | 9.86 | |
Lingual_R | R | 18 | −70 | 1 | 9.31 |
Significant positive correlation clusters were located in the lingual gyrus (occipital cortex), while significant negative correlation clusters were observed in the anterior and medial frontal gyrus, in the basal ganglia (caudate and putamen), thalamus and a small cluster in the hippocampus. The positive correlations overlapped in part with the higher CBF observed during NREM sleep compared to wake (compare Fig. 2). On the other hand, negative correlations partly overlapped with regions displaying decreased CBF during NREM sleep compared to wake, such as the caudate and anterior cingulate, as well as the thalamus (compare Fig. 2). The spatial extent of the regions was comparable between correlations and stage difference images.
Discussion
We demonstrated both local brain-area specific CBF changes and global variations of CBF during NREM sleep compared to wake. Furthermore, we could for the first time associate SWA with CBF during sleep with a very high temporal resolution (on the seconds scale) and observed both negative and positive correlations between these two measures in distinct brain areas hypothesized and known to be involved in sleep-wake regulation.
Mean CBF across Vigilance States
We observed significant topographical differences between mean CBF images depending on vigilance stages (Figs 2 and 3). These were even visible by pure comparison (Fig. 1) and were located in areas that have already been associated with different activity during wake and sleep as e.g. the thalamus19.
CBF during NREM Sleep Compared to Wake
Contrary to the notion of sleep as a state of globally reduced brain activity, we observed higher CBF levels in occipital areas (lingual gyrus, posterior cingulate, small part of the parahippocampal gyrus and a small cluster in the hippocampus; see Fig. 2, right panel) during N2/N3 compared to pre-sleep wake. The parahippocampal gyrus has been reported to be a neuronal correlate of slow waves29. The authors interpreted this finding with the role of the parahippocampal gyrus as a relay station between hippocampus and neocortex and suggested an involvement in memory processing occurring during sleep. PET studies have also reported positive correlations of CBF with SWA in the primary visual and auditory cortices16, as well as increased CBF during SWS compared to wake13. These increases might be due to dream-like mentation, as suggested by Hofle et al.16 but could also be related to a preservation of functional connectivity in visual and auditory cortices as observed during anesthesia-induced loss of consciousness35. In their study, the authors found preserved or even higher parametric estimates (reflecting the statistical effect size) in visual and auditory networks during unconsciousness compared to wakefulness. Studies investigating disorders of consciousness as e.g. patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome/in vegetative state reported metabolic dysfunction in a widespread cortical network, also encompassing posterior cingulate and precuneus cortices36,37. Parts of this impaired network have been related to external/sensory awareness and internal/self-awareness38. Therefore, activity in primary cortices might thus resemble a basic form of remaining sensory processing during anesthesia or sleep that could be unique to this reversible state of unconsciousness.
Lower CBF during N3/N2 compared to wake was observed in prefrontal cortical areas (medial frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate) and in the basal ganglia (caudate, putamen; see Fig. 2, left panel) as well as in part of the thalamus. These areas have previously been reported to show decreased CBF in sleep compared to wake based on PET studies13,17,20.
The prefrontal cortex has been proposed as the initiation site of cortically generated slow waves21,22. The decrease of CBF in this area could be due to the burst-mode firing of cortical neurons generating up and down states that give rise to slow waves at the EEG level27. This burst-mode is characterized by longer periods of ‘silence’ (off periods/down states) followed by short activity bursts (on periods/up states), in contrast to the tonic firing during waking. Therefore, during NREM sleep, when CBF is averaged over seconds or minutes the net result of off and on periods might be reduced overall neuronal activity, manifested in a decrease in CBF compared to waking.
The basal ganglia (in particular the striatum) have been proposed to play a role in sleep-wake regulation39, as they constitute part of the ascending reticular arousal system (ARAS) located in the brainstem40. The ARAS integrates signals from e.g. cortical areas as well as from the thalamus and amygdala. Stroke or atrophy affecting the caudate have been associated with distinctive changes in sleep patterns (hypersomnia and reduced SWS, respectively41), and cats with removed caudate showed transient hyposomnia42. Based on its anatomical connectivity, less caudate activation would lead to inhibition of the globus pallidus interna (therefore resulting in higher activation of it), which in turn would cause more inhibition of the thalamic nuclei. This reduced activation of the thalamic nuclei (which is manifested in our data in a reduction of the pulvinar activation) would then decrease excitation of the frontal cortex.
Together, these data suggest a role of the basal ganglia, the prefrontal cortex, the posterior cingulate and the thalamus in sleep-wake regulation, as well as a prefrontal cortex role in slow wave generation. Our data further corroborate such a notion as we observed NREM sleep associated decreases of CBF in the aforementioned brain areas compared to wakefulness. As some of these areas (e.g. the thalamus, prefrontal cortex) are also among those showing an impaired metabolism in patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome/ in the vegetative state36,37 but not in patients in emergence from minimally conscious state38, implications might extend towards neuronal underpinnings of consciousness and its restoration (see related interpretations by He and Raichle43).
CBF Differences between Pre-Sleep Wake and Post-Sleep Wake
The brain regions with lower CBF during pre-sleep wake compared to post-sleep wake (see Fig. 3, right panel) included the area that displayed increased CBF during NREM sleep (compare Fig. 2, right panel), namely the posterior cingulate. Similarly, higher CBF during pre-sleep wake compared to post-sleep wake was mainly observed in regions associated with decreased CBF in NREM sleep (compare Figs 2 and 3, left panels), i.e., in the anterior cingulate, medial frontal gyrus, the basal ganglia and the parahippocampal gyrus. Pre-sleep wake was additionally associated with higher CBF in the bilateral superior temporal gyri and part of the supplementary motor area.
These brain-region specific effects mainly resemble the patterns observed in the sleep-wake contrast. Together with the fact that we did not observe differences in mean grey matter CBF between N3 and post-sleep wakefulness (in contrast to13; see section “Mean GM CBF Values” and the SI Discussion), this could imply a remaining influence of NREM sleep on post-sleep wakefulness. As we did not restrict post-sleep wake to a period after prolonged continuous consciousness of a certain duration (as Braun and colleagues did), our results could thus be attributed to sleep inertia. This would be in line with a study by Balkin et al.44 that reported a distinct time sequence of ‘awakening’ for specific brain regions. In detail, these authors observed a fast ‘recovery’ of CBF values during wake after sleep to values observed during pre-sleep wake in the brainstem and thalamus but showed that reestablishment of pre-sleep wake CBF took longer in anterior cortex regions. In particular, the caudate region associated with lower CBF during post-sleep wakefulness compared to pre-sleep wake is in favor of such an interpretation: Although this caudate region showed also lower CBF during N3 compared to pre-sleep wake, the spatial extent was larger compared to the contrast of pre-sleep and post-sleep wake. This would be in line with the report of Balkin et al.44 that CBF in these areas returns faster to pre-sleep wake levels than e.g. prefrontal cortical areas.
Mean GM CBF Values
To assess more global changes of CBF in the different vigilance stages, we compared mean GM and whole brain CBF values between them. Mean GM CBF values differed significantly between vigilance stages (Fig. S1). This is in line with previous reports13. However, the changes observed in mean GM CBF values with respect to vigilance stages were not completely in accordance to those observed by Braun et al.13. However, the levels of CBF during pre-sleep wakefulness in this paper were comparable to those found in a recent ASL study45. Several differences between the two studies might have contributed to the divergent findings. These issues are discussed in detail in the SI Discussion.
Correlation of CBF and SWA
We observed significant clusters of correlations between CBF and SWA in distinct brain areas (Fig. 5). When uncorrected for multiple comparisons, these clusters became larger and revealed additional related brain areas (e.g., anterior and medial portions of the cingulate, occipital cortex etc.; Fig. 5). For the first time, we could demonstrate that a temporal relationship between CBF and SWA during continuous NREM sleep exists in individuals, thereby establishing a dynamic link between metabolic (CBF) and electrophysiological (sleep intensity) measures. This extends the findings of PET studies that correlated CBF and SWA values measured at distinct time points during different sleep stages pooled across subjects15,16.
These areas with the positive correlations (higher levels of CBF associated with higher SWA) overlapped with areas shown to have higher CBF values during N2/N3 sleep than during pre-sleep W. As discussed above, this might indicate e.g. either dream-like mentation processes during sleep or constitute a form of remaining basic level of stimulus processing during sleep, possibly due to the underlying unique state of reversible unconsciousness.
Brain areas associated with negative correlations (lower levels of CBF associated with higher SWA) on the other hand overlapped in anterior cingulate and basal ganglia regions with areas displaying lower CBF values during N2 sleep (and additionally thalamus regions during N3 sleep) compared to pre-sleep W. Some of the overlaps of these two analyses might be inherent to the fact, that SWA is highest during NREM sleep, and tends to increase from N2 to N3 sleep. However, the nature of these analyses (distinct, categorized, visually scored 20-s stages grouped together with potentially very different underlying levels of SWA (see CBF, hypnogram and SWA in SI-Fig. 2), averaged per subject and contrasted with pre-sleep wake versus the correlation of each subjects’ continuous temporal evolution of CBF and SWA throughout ongoing NREM sleep; SI-Fig. 2) is quite different. Further, NREM sleep is not exclusively characterized by slow waves and SWA in stages N2/N3 but also consists of more complex frequency compositions (see e.g. the spectrogram in SI-Fig. 2) including features such as K-complexes and sleep spindles.
Therefore, although the scoring of NREM sleep and SWA are not completely orthogonal, we are confident that the correlation analysis furthers our understanding of brain regions involved in sleep-wake regulation and slow wave generation. This is also illustrated in SI-Fig. S2 that shows while SWA and NREM sleep stages share some properties, the correlation analysis provides additional information about dynamical changes occurring within sleep that cannot be assessed by the static contrasting of average CBF in a specific sleep stage with pre-sleep wake.
The negative correlations between CBF and SWA in a small thalamic cluster are in line with the results presented by Hofle et al.16. As mentioned, some of these areas display metabolic dysfunctions in patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome/ in the vegetative state36–38 but not in patients emerging from minimally conscious state38. Therefore, these areas might not only contribute to sleep-wake regulation but could further be related to the subsiding and restoration of consciousness.
Additionally, a small cluster in the hippocampus was associated with significant negative correlations of CBF and SWA. The hippocampus is part of the memory consolidation system and it has been proposed that sleep might amongst others serve a function in the scope of memory processes32,46,47. A study employing intracranial EEG recordings in epilepsy patients linked increases in hippocampal EEG power in the high delta range (2.1–4 Hz) after training and performance improvement in a hippocampus-dependent spatial navigation task the next day48. Both slow waves28 and spindles49 have been shown to occur locally in the hippocampus during sleep. As mentioned above, slow wave generation is due to a burst-mode of firing alternating with periods of silence that is hypothesized to be less energy demanding than tonic firing. As this process seems also to take place in the hippocampus, this could be the reason for the observed negative correlation between hippocampal CBF and SWA.
Conclusions
In their entirety, our data corroborate the involvement of thalamic, basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex areas in sleep-wake regulation and strengthen the notion of their association with different states of consciousness. Further, we were able to demonstrate that specific brain areas (namely the posterior cingulate) showed an increase of CBF during NREM sleep compared to wake. By combining comparisons of mean CBF across vigilance stages and a temporal correlation analysis of continuous CBF with SWA (a marker of sleep intensity), we corroborated that these brain areas do not only change CBF in sleep with respect to a baseline (usually pre-sleep wake) but that there is a close relationship between specific temporal neuronal patterns (as assessed by SWA) and the corresponding CBF. Additionally, we showed that CBF during immediate post-sleep wake mainly resembles NREM sleep-like features, with the exception of the caudate region which seemed to ‘recover’ to pre-sleep wake values faster the other brain areas, in line with its presumed role in awakening and arousal.
Material and Methods
Participants
The study was approved by the ethical committee of the Canton of Zurich and was conducted according to the Helsinki declaration. Participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the study and were remunerated for their participation.
Twenty-four healthy male participants were selected for the study after a thorough screening process excluding excessive daytime sleepiness (defined as values >10, assessed by the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS50; range: 0–24, 0: lowest possible level of daytime sleepiness, above 10: excessive daytime sleepiness), and neurological or psychiatric disorders (based on subjective reports). Participants also had to be compatible to the magnetic resonance (MR) safety regulations as well as in the normal body mass index (BMI) range (World Health Organization (WHO) normal range: 18.5–24.99 kg/m2; http://apps.who.int/bmi/index.jsp?introPage=intro_3.html). Participants further had to report regular sleep-wake patterns involving approximately 8 h of sleep per night (Table S1). All participants were moderate caffeine (≤3 cups/day) and alcohol (≤7 alcoholic drinks/week) consumers, and were not on medications.
Participants underwent a screening night in the sleep laboratory and only those who reached at least 80 % sleep efficiency and could sleep on their back without sleep fragmentation were included in the study. Demographics and behavioral measures of the participants that were included in the subsequent analyses (n = 19, exclusion criteria provided below) are provided in Table S1.
Study Protocol
Three days prior to the first experimental night, participants had to adhere to regularly scheduled bedtimes and time in bed of 8 hours, compliance to this was determined based on both individual sleep diaries and wrist actigraphy recordings (Actiwatch; Cambridge Neurotechnology, Cambridge, UK).
Participants spent the night prior to the scanner recordings in the sleep laboratory. They went to bed at 23:30 and high density (hd) EEG recordings were performed. They had a sleep opportunity of 4 h, and were awakened at 03:30 in the morning. They had to stay awake until the next evening, when the scanning session took place. From 03:30 to 07:00, they had to stay in the laboratory and were under constant supervision. Compliance throughout the day was monitored with wrist actigraphy.
At the scanner site, participants underwent a resting state EEG recording outside of scanner, two resting state recordings (simultaneous EEG/fMRI-BOLD- and EEG/fMRI-ASL measurements, respectively) and performed a modified Sternberg working memory task51. The working memory data will not be assessed in this publication. Afterwards, at ≈23:30, participants were instructed to sleep and to signal if they either were not able to go back to sleep after awakening or if they wanted to abort the experiment completely. If participants only slept for a short duration (≈2 h), after a short break (≈0.5 h) they were asked whether they would be ready to sleep in the scanner again. After completion of the sleep scanning sessions, the EEG cap was removed and anatomical as well as diffusion weighted tensor images were recorded.
fMRI Data Acquisition and Preprocessing
The simultaneous EEG/fMRI-ASL sleep recordings were performed with a 3T Philips Achieva whole-body system (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) with a 32-element receive-only head coil (Philips SENSE head coil 32-elements). ASL images were obtained with 2D EPI readouts with a pseudo-continuous labeling scheme with the following parameters: 72 volumes, a 80 × 79 matrix, a slice thickness of 7 mm with no gap, 3 × 3 × 7 mm3 voxels, 20 slices, repetition time/ echo time /label time/post label delay (TR/TE/τ/PLD) of 4400/20/1650/1525 ms, a flip angle of 90°, a field of view of 240 mm, a labeling offset of 2 cm from the bottom imaging slice (corresponding roughly to 9 cm offset from the anterior commissure-posterior commissure (AC-PC) line) and background suppression. Duration of the recordings varied depending on participants’ sleep duration (see above) but was set maximally to 1600 scan volumes (i.e. ≈4 h).
To quantify CBF52, an equilibrium magnetization volume (M0) was acquired right before the fMRI-ASL recordings, with the same parameters as for the fMRI-ASL sequence described above, except that a longer TR of 10000 ms and no labeling was applied. ASL slices were oriented along the AC-PC line such that the whole cortex was recorded. Therefore, the cerebellum was not fully covered with the selected slices.
The fMRI-ASL data were first realigned in SPM8 http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/. ASL data were quantified to CBF images (MATLAB version R2012b, The MathWorks Inc., Natick, Massachusetts). The approach is based on the simple subtraction method as specified in Aguirre et al.53. This method has been described to reliably minimize BOLD contamination in resting state recordings54. CBF images were then co-registered, normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (and resliced to 3 × 3 × 3 mm3 isomorphic voxels), masked (with participant specific brain masks) and smoothed (6 mm full-width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian Kernel) with SPM8.
Individual grey matter (GM) masks were obtained by segmentation of the anatomical images in SPM8. For the GM images, participants’ GM masks were normalized to MNI space. Participants’ pre-processed mean CBF images of each vigilance stage were masked with the individual GM mask.
EEG Data Acquisition and Preprocessing
The concurrent EEG was acquired with an MR compatible amplifier and electrodes (BrainAmp, BrainCap and BrainAmp ExG MR devices and electrodes; Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). The data were sampled at a 5 kHz and synchronized with the scanner clock. We recorded 66 channels (60 EEG channels, 2 electrooculogram (EOG) channels, 1 electromyogram (EMG) channel, 3 electrocardiogram (ECG) channels). The analog filter settings were as follows: low-pass at 250 Hz, high-pass at 0.0159 Hz (time constant 10 s).
For the further EEG analyses, scanner gradient artifacts were removed by a fast Fourier transform (FFT) approach (adapted from a method used in electron crystallography, collaboration with R. Dürr, in house MATLAB script) and down-sampled to 500 Hz. Data were inspected for movement artifacts and corrected with a standard cardioballistogram (CB) artifact template correction implemented in BrainVision Analyzer 2 (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany; Allen et al., 1998).
Furthermore, the EEG data were filtered (band-pass filter: high-pass cutoff at 0.1 Hz, low-pass cutoff at 49 Hz, additional notch filter at 50 Hz) and an independent component analysis (ICA) derived artifact filter was applied. For most participants (n = 15), the ICA filter was constructed from those independent components (ICs) that were manually selected as artifacts of an ICA performed on combined data sets of resting state ASL recordings (≈10 min, free of movement artifacts) and the ‘clean’ EEG from the resting state outside of the scanner (≈8 min, free of movement artifacts). These resting state data had been obtained prior to the sleep data during rested wake in the same subjects at the same scanner. For the remaining four participants, the ICA filters were constructed from the manual selection of ICs derived from an ICA of ≈4 min of resting state EEG outside of the scanner and ≈4 min of the sleep recordings right at the beginning (free of movement artifacts). For the pre-sleep wake EEG data, the aforementioned filter created from the resting state ASL recordings and the ‘clean’ EEG was used.
Based on the cleaned EEG recordings, sleep was visually scored (derivations C3A2, F3A2, O1A2; A2 approximated by closest electrode in the EEG cap) in 20-s epochs according to standard guidelines (wake, NREM sleep stages 1 (N1), N2, N355,56).
Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria for Participant Data and Analyses Details
To be able to assess CBF across the whole span of vigilance stages, only participants who reached NREM sleep stage N3 according to standard scoring criteria55 were included in the analyses (n = 19). We indicate also if fewer subjects contributed to an analysis due to insufficient number of scans of a particular vigilance stage.
Mean CBF across Vigilance Stages
To assess the mean CBF values per vigilance state (wake or sleep), we averaged across all scans (minimum number of 7 scans, i.e. ≈1 min) of the respective artifact free sleep scans. Movements related to scans were identified based on the frame-wise displacement (FD) data. Scans with an FD values greater than 0.5 were excluded57 (details see below). The resulting mean maps were then averaged per participant, to reveal a single mean CBF value per participant per stage. Wake before sleep (pre-sleep W), NREM stages N1, N2, N3, and wake after sleep (post-sleep W) were compared. Wake before sleep was calculated from those scans of the resting state recording prior to the sleep recording.
Correlation of CBF and SWA
When two scanning sessions had been recorded in a participant, only one of them was included in the analyses (typically, the session where NREM sleep stage N3 had been reached and if this was the case in both sessions, the one with less artifacts was selected). Sleep duration differed between participants (170.3 ± 59.9 min in total; mean ± standard deviation (SD), range from 63.7 to 234.3 min).We selected NREM sleep recordings, that comprised at least NREM sleep stages N2 and N3 and as little movement artifacts as possible, and were at least 1 hour long (see Table S1).
Statistical Analyses
Mean CBF across Vigilance Stages
Mean global CBF values (grey matter, GM) of each participant and vigilance stage (pre-sleep W, N1, N2, N3, post-sleep W) were analyzed with a mixed model repeated measures ANOVA implemented in SAS (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, North Carolina). Post-hoc comparisons were based on the Tukey-Kramer test.
Mean CBF images of each participant and vigilance stage entered a second level SPM factorial design with a one-way within-subject ANOVA. Participants contributed only with those conditions, i.e. stages, for which they had enough artifact free scans (≥7). In detail, 14 participants contributed to the condition ‘pre-sleep W’, 18 to ‘N1’, 19 to ‘N2’ as well as ‘N3’ and 14 to ‘post-sleep W’ (Table S2). The following contrasts were assessed: pre-sleep W – post-sleep W, pre-sleep W - N1/N2/N3, pre-sleep W - N1, pre-sleep W - N2, pre-sleep W - N3, and the inverse of each contrast. We only report contrasts pre-sleep W – post-sleep W, pre-sleep W – N2, pre-sleep W N3 and the respective inversions.
Correlation of CBF and SWA
EEG power density spectra were calculated from the sleep EEG recordings using an FFT routine (4-s epochs, Hanning window), averaging 2 4-s epochs centered at the mid-point of the ASL label and control scans (i.e., 4.4 s after start of label, 8.8-s intervals). The resulting SWA (power in the 1–4.5 Hz range of EEG derivation C3A2; A2 approximated by closest electrode in the EEG cap) time course was smoothed with a moving average across 21 8.8-s intervals (Fig. S2).
Likewise, the CBF time course of every voxel in the brain was smoothed with a moving average across 21 scans (8.8-s intervals). Based on an approach of Power et al.57, we excluded all scans that showed an FD values greater than 0.5. The FD value was derived by a combination of all six movement parameters obtained from the realignment of the functional images.
Artifact–corrected CBF time courses of all voxels in the brain and the corresponding SWA time course were entered in a partial correlation analysis, with nuisance regressors of no interest. These nuisance regressors were the FD values determined by the motion parameters of the realignment step of the functional images, as well as 16 white matter and 16 cerebrospinal fluid regressors derived with the CONN functional connectivity toolbox58. These parameters have been shown to give good estimates of physiological noise contributions due to pulsation and breathing artifacts. These regressors were also smoothed with a moving average filter of 21 intervals.
The resulting partial correlation coefficient images of each participant were Fisher’s z-transformed. These z-transformed images were entered in a second level SPM factorial design with a one-sample T-test (two-tailed). Positive and negative contrasts, i.e. positive and negative correlations of SWA and CBF were assessed.
Figure 4 illustrates the mean correlation (n = 19) between CBF and SWA. Averaging was performed on participants’ z-transformed correlation images and then backtransformed for display.
Data availability
The ethical approval granted to the authors by the ethical committee of the Canton of Zurich (Switzerland) does not allow the publication of the raw data online. If readers would like to reanalyze the data set (for different purposes), additional ethical approval (on a individual user and purpose basis) will be required. The authors would be happy to support additional ethical approval applications from researchers for access to this data set.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation grant CRSII3_136249. We thank Drs. Kay Jann, Roland Dürr, Thomas Rusterholz for technical support and Drs. Mara Kottlow, Lars Michels and Leila Tarokh for fruitful discussions. We also would like to thank Angela Aeschbach, Claudia Aschmann, Daniela Buser, Angela Escobar, Lukas Fürer, Jolanda Müller, Johanna Scherer, Nina Schumacher, Michelle Steinemann, Sarah Untersander and Katharina Wellstein for help with the data acquisition.
Author Contributions
L.T. and P.A. designed the experiment. L.T. and X.O. performed the experiment with technical advice and assistance from R.O.T., A.F., R.L and P.S. L.T. and P.A. analyzed the data with support from T.K. L.T. and P.A. wrote the manuscript. All authors approved the final version.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-017-12890-7.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Berticat, C. et al. Excessive daytime sleepiness and antipathogen drug consumption in the elderly: a test of the immune theory of sleep. Scientific Reports6 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 2.Bryant PA, Trinder J, Curtis N. Sick and tired: Does sleep have a vital role in the immune system? Nat Rev Immunol. 2004;4:457–467. doi: 10.1038/nri1369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ibarra-Coronado, E. G. et al. The Bidirectional Relationship between Sleep and Immunity against Infections. Journal of Immunology Research2015 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 4.Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN, Dement WC. Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science. 1966;152:604–619. doi: 10.1126/science.152.3722.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lesku JA, Roth TC, 2nd, Amlaner CJ, Lima SL. A phylogenetic analysis of sleep architecture in mammals: the integration of anatomy, physiology, and ecology. Am Nat. 2006;168:441–453. doi: 10.1086/506973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Siegel JM. Sleep viewed as a state of adaptive inactivity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:747–753. doi: 10.1038/nrn2697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.de Vivo L, et al. Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle. Science. 2017;355:507–510. doi: 10.1126/science.aah5982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and Synaptic Homeostasis: A Hypothesis. Brain Res Bull. 2003;62:143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep Function and Synaptic Homeostasis. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2006;10:49–62. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Borbély AA, Baumann F, Brandeis D, Strauch I, Lehmann D. Sleep Deprivation: Effect on Sleep Stages and EEG Power Density in Man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1981;51:483–493. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(81)90225-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Brunner, D. P., Dijk, D.-J. & Tobler, I. & Borbély, A. A. Effect of partial sleep deprivation on sleep stages and EEG power spectra: evidence for non-REM and REM sleep homeostasis. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol75, 492–499 (1990). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 12.Achermann, P. & Borbély, A. A. in Principles and Practices of Sleep Medicine (eds Kryger, M., Roth, T. & Dement, W.) 377–387 (Elsevier, Philadelphia, 2017).
- 13.Braun AR, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep–wake cycle An H2(15)O PET study. Brain. 1997;120:1173–1197. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.7.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Czisch M, et al. Functional MRI during sleep: BOLD signal decreases and their electrophysiological correlates. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;20:566–574. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dang-Vu TT, et al. Cerebral Correlates of Delta Waves during non-REM Sleep Revisited. Neuroimage. 2005;28:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hofle N, et al. Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Changes as a Function of Delta and Spindle Activity during Slow Wave Sleep in Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:4800–4808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-12-04800.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Maquet P, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of human slow wave sleep. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:2807–2812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-08-02807.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Maquet P, et al. Functional Neuroanatomy of Human Rapid-eye-movement Sleep and Dreaming. Nature. 1996;383:163–166. doi: 10.1038/383163a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Maquet P. Funtional Neuroimaging of Normal Human Sleep by Positron Emission Tomography. Journal of Sleep Research. 2000;9:207–231. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Nofzinger EA, et al. Human regional cerebral glucose metabolism during non-rapid eye movement sleep in relation to waking. Brain. 2002;125:1105–1115. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Massimini M, Huber R, Ferrarelli F, Hill S, Tononi G. The Sleep Slow Oscillation as a Traveling Wave. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:6862–6870. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1318-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Murphy M, et al. Source Modeling Sleep Slow Waves. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:1608–1613. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807933106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Steriade M. The Corticothalamic System in Sleep. Front Biosci. 2003;8:d878–899. doi: 10.2741/1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Timofeev, I. & Bazhenov, M. In Trends in Chronobiology Research (ed. Columbus, F.) 1–47 (Nova Science Publishers, Inc., 2005).
- 25.McCormick DA, Bal T. Sleep and arousal: thalamocortical mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1997;20:185–215. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vyazovskiy VV, Harris KD. Sleep and the single neuron: the role of global slow oscillations in individual cell rest. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:443–451. doi: 10.1038/nrn3494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Vyazovskiy VV, et al. Cortical Firing and Sleep Homeostasis. Neuron. 2009;63:865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Nir Y, et al. Regional slow waves and spindles in human sleep. Neuron. 2011;70:153–169. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dang-Vu TT, et al. Spontaneous neural activity during human slow wave sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:15160–15165. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801819105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Aslan S, et al. Estimation of Labeling Efficiency in Pseudocontinuous Arterial Spin Labeling. Magn Reson Med. 2010;63:765–771. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Borogovac A, Asllani I. Arterial Spin Labeling (ASL) fMRI: Advantages, Theoretical Constrains and Experimental Challenges in Neurosciences. International Journal of Biomedical Imaging. 2012;2012:818456. doi: 10.1155/2012/818456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Abel T, Havekes R, Saletin JM, Walker MP. Sleep, plasticity and memory from molecules to whole-brain networks. Current Biology. 2013;23:R774–788. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hamandi K, et al. BOLD and perfusion changes during epileptic generalised spike wave activity. Neuroimage. 2008;39:608–618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Loring, D. W. et al. Now you see it, now you don't: statistical and methodological considerations in fMRI. Epilepsy & Behavior3 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Boveroux P, et al. Breakdown of within- and between-network Resting State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Connectivity during Propofol-induced Loss of Consciousness. Anesthesiology. 2010;113:1038–1053. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181f697f5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Laureys S. The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:556–559. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Soddu A, et al. Correlation between resting state fMRI total neuronal activity and PET metabolism in healthy controls and patients with disorders of consciousness. Brain and Behavior. 2016;6:e00424. doi: 10.1002/brb3.424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Thibaut A, et al. Metabolic activity in external and internal awareness networks in severely brain-damaged patients. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine. 2012;44:487–494. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lazarus M, Chen JF, Urade Y, Huang ZL. Role of the basal ganglia in the control of sleep and wakefulness. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2013;23:780–785. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Luppi, P. H. & Fort, P. (eds Nofzinger, E., Maquet, P. & Thorpy, M. J.) 8–14 (Cambridge University Press, 2013).
- 41.Bassetti, C. L. (eds Kryger, M. H., Roth, T. & Dement, W. C.) 993–1015 (Elsevier Saunders, 2011).
- 42.Villablanca, J. R., Marcus, R. J. & Olmstead, C. E. Effects of caudate nuclei or frontal cortex ablations in cats. II. Sleep-wakefulness, EEG, and motor activity. Experimental Neurology53 (1976). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 43.He BJ, Raichle ME. The fMRI signal, slow cortical potentials and consciousness. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13:302–309. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Balkin, T. J. et al. The process of awakening: a PET study of regional brain activity patterns mediating the re-establishment of alertness and consciousness. Brain125 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 45.Ghisleni, C. et al. Effects of Steroid Hormones on Sex Differences in Cerebral Perfusion. PloS One (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 46.Rasch B, Born J. About Sleep's Role in Memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93:681–766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Walker MP. Sleep, memory and emotion. Progress in Brain Research. 2010;185:49–68. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00004-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Moroni F, et al. Hippocampal Slow EEG Frequencies During NREM Sleep are Involved in Spatial Memory Consolidation in Humans. Hippocampus. 2014;24:1157–1168. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sarasso S, et al. Hippocampal sleep spindles preceding neocortical sleep onset in humans. Neuroimage. 2014;86:425–432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Johns MW. A New Method for Measuring Daytime. Sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–545. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Michels, L. et al. Simultaneous EEG-fMRI during a working memory task: modulations in low and high frequency bands. PloS One5, e10298 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 52.Deibler AR, et al. Arterial Spin-Labeling in Routine Clinical Practice, Part 1: Technique and Artifacts. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2008;29:1228–1234. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Aguirre GK, Detre JA, Zarahn E, Alsop DC. Experimental Design and the Relative Sensitivity of BOLD and Perfusion fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002;15:488–500. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Liu TT, Wong EC. A signal processing model for arterial spin labeling functional MRI. Neuroimage. 2005;24:207–215. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson Jr, A. L. & Quan, S. F. (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 1 Westbrook Corporate Center, Suite 920, Westchester, IL 60154, USA, 2007).
- 56.Rechtschaffen, A. & Kales, A. (U. S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Public Health Service - National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Blindness, Neurological Information Network, Bethesda, Maryland 20014, 1968).
- 57.Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectivity. 2012;2:125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The ethical approval granted to the authors by the ethical committee of the Canton of Zurich (Switzerland) does not allow the publication of the raw data online. If readers would like to reanalyze the data set (for different purposes), additional ethical approval (on a individual user and purpose basis) will be required. The authors would be happy to support additional ethical approval applications from researchers for access to this data set.