Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Parkinsons Dis. 2017;7(4):661–667. doi: 10.3233/JPD-171113

Statins and Cognition in Parkinson’s Disease

Benjamin L Deck a,*, Jacqueline Rick a, Sharon X Xie b, Alice Chen-Plotkin a, John E Duda a,e, James F Morley a,e, Lana M Chahine a, Nabila Dahodwala a, John Q Trojanowski c, Daniel Weintraub a,d,e
PMCID: PMC5675567  NIHMSID: NIHMS916682  PMID: 28922167

Abstract

Background

The relationship between statins and cognition in Parkinson’s disease (PD) is poorly understood.

Objectives

Analyses were performed to determine associations between statin use and cross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive performance in PD.

Methods

Neuropsychological tests, medication logs, and ratings of functional abilities were collected from 313 PD participants longitudinally.

Results

At baseline, statin users (SU; N = 129) were older, more likely male, and had shorter PD duration than non-statin users (NSU; N = 184). In Cross-sectional analysis, SU performed better on global cognition, Trails B, semantic fluency, and phonemic fluency tasks. Rate of long-term global cognitive (Dementia Rating Scale-2 and MoCA) decline was significantly less in SU.

Keywords: Apolipoprotein A-I, cognition, hydroxymethylglutaryl-CoA reductase inhibitors, longitudinal studies, Parkinson’s disease, reactive oxygen species

INTRODUCTION

Approximately 80% of Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients develop cognitive impairment [1]. HMG- CoA reductase inhibitors (i.e., statins) modulate cholesterol levels and have emerged as possible disease-modifying drugs for neurodegenerative disorders, perhaps related to their pleiotropic abilities [2]. A possible mechanism is statin-induced increases in HDL levels leading to increased Apolipoprotein A-1 (ApoA1) levels [3, 4]. PD patients have lower ApoA1 than controls or individuals with other neurodegenerative diseases, their age of disease onset is positively correlated with plasma ApoA1, and lower ApoA1 is associated with advanced PD [3, 4].

Another possible explanation is statin-mediated reduction of reactive oxygen species (ROS) may decrease vascular disease burden often comorbid in PD [57]. Support for potential protective effects of statins comes from two epidemiological studies [8, 9]. A US study followed both statin users (SU) and non-statin users (NSU) long-term and found that statin use was significantly associated with decreased PD incidence [8]. A Taiwanese study found that statin therapy continuation, versus discontinuation, was associated with decreased risk of PD, especially in lipophilic SU [9]. However, there are contradictory findings, including statins either raising or having non-significant effects on PD risk [1012].

Some studies in non-PD populations suggest that statin use is associated with a lower risk of dementia [1320], while others report no [2124] or deleterious effects on cognition [2528]. We report analyses from an observational study examining statin use and cognitive abilities in PD patients, hypothesizing that SU would have better cognitive performance.

METHODS

Population

Patients with idiopathic PD (n = 313) based on UK Brain Bank criteria [29] were enrolled from 2006–2015 at the University of Pennsylvania NINDS-funded Udall Center. The cohort was followed prospectively (from 2–6 years), with baseline (BL) and either annual (years 1–4) or biennial (after year 4) neuropsychological testing.

Participants were divided into current SU (hydrophilic or lipophilic) and NSU at their BL based on self-report, excluding patients starting statins after BL. The size of the SU sample decreases over time due to patients discontinuing statin use and variability in length of study participation (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Number of participants assessed longitudinally

Visit SU NSU
Baseline 129 184
V01 99 155
V02 89 125
V03 75 103
V04, V05 67 85
V06, V07 43 53
V08 17 23

Assessments

Motor disease severity was quantified using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) Part III [30] and Hoehn & Yahr (H&Y) scale [31], and depression severity with the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) [32]. Informant impression of activities of daily living was collected by the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study-Activities of Daily Living Inventory (ADCS-ADL) [33] and the Penn Parkinson’s Daily Activities Questionnaire (PDAQ) [34].

Neuropsychological assessments

Administered cognitive tests were: global cognitive abilities (Dementia Rating Scale-2 and Montreal Cognitive Assessment [35, 36]); executive abilities/working memory (Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS) [37], phonemic fluency (FAS) [38], and Trails B [39]); attention (Symbol Digit Modalities Test [40]) and Trails A [39]); memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) [41]); visuospatial function (Judgment of Line Orientation (JOLO) [42] and clock drawing [43]); and language (short Boston Naming Test (BNT)[44] and semantic verbal fluency (animals) [38]).

Consensus cognitive diagnosis

Cognitive status (normal cognition, mild cognitive impairment or dementia) was determined annually or biennially by a consensus of movement disorders specialists or psychiatrist affiliated with the Penn Udall Center as described previously [45].

Modified framingham heart study cardiovascular risk factor scale (mFRF)

The scale [46] weighs vascular risk factors based on age and sex. We modified the scale to include self-identification of high blood pressure as a substitute for measured systolic blood pressure (a score of “3” for women and a score of “2” for men).

Plasma ApoA1 analysis

As previously described [4] plasma was collected in a subset of participants (N = 93). Levels of ApoA1 were measured via enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA; catalog no 0462; Abnova, Taipei, Taiwan) [47]. Duplicate samples were run (<1% of samples had a coefficient of variation of >0.2). ELISA was used to process all samples, on the same day, via the same operators.

Statistical analyses

T-tests were used to compare SU and NSU demographic variables. Significant (p ≤ 0.05) variables associated with statin use were included as co-variates in linear regression and linear mixed-effects model analyses. Linear regression models analyzed group differences in baseline data, while linear mixed-effects models analyzed group differences in rates of long-term cognitive decline. Since our study was exploratory rather than confirmatory, multiple testing was not performed [48].

RESULTS

Participant demographics

There were 129 participants on HMG-CoA reductase inhibitors with the mean duration of statin use prior to baseline visit = 7.26 years (SD = 6.3). At baseline SU were significantly older (t(295) = −2.06; p = 0.04), had shorter duration of illness (t(311) = 1.96; p = 0.05), scored higher on mFRF (denoting greater vascular risk) (t(203.5) = −2.99; p = 0.003), and had lower UPDRS motor scores (t(296.4) = 2.03; p = 0.04) than NSU (n = 184). As higher mFRF scores in SU would not bias results in support of our hypothesis, we did not include it as a co-variate in subsequent analyses. Demographic and clinical variables are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographics

Variables Percentage or Mean, SU (SD) Percentage or Mean, NSU (SD) P-Value
Age 70.5 (7.5) 68.6 (8.6) 0.04*
Sex 72.9% 27.1% Male 0.09
modified-Framingham Heart Study Vascular Score 16.6 (3.6) 15.2 (3.4) 0.003*
UPDRS-motor score 20.2 (9.7) 22.7 (11.6) 0.05*
Geriatric Depression Scale-15 score 2.5 (2.5) 3 (2.9) 0.12
PD Disease duration 6.7 (5.1) 7.9 (5.2) 0.05*
PD age of onset 63.8 (9.1) 60.7 (9.0) 0.004*
Hoehn & Yahr stage 2.2 (.66) 2.3 (.67) 0.63
Activities of Daily Living Inventory (total score) 71.3 (12.7) 69.5 (12.3) 0.34
Number of Participants Follow-up (In Years) 65.00 (30.4) 90.7 (40.0) 0.29
*

Denotes that significance p ≤ 0.05.

Statin classification

Frequency of statin type (hydrophilic or lipophilic) and mean dosage for SU can be found in Table 4.

Table 4.

Statin classification

Hydrophilic Frequency (# participants) Mean dosage in mg (SD)
 Pravastatin 5 30 (11.5)
 Rosuvastatin 12 13.3 (10.3)
Lipophilic
 Lovastatin 5 20.0 (12.2)
 Atorvastatin 44 21.4 (17.3)
 Simvastatin 63 31.8 (19.0)

Baseline neuropsychological assessments

Baseline cognitive scores are listed in Table 2. After controlling for UPDRS motor score, age, and disease duration, SU performed significantly better than NSU on the MoCA (F(4,121) = 11.41; p = 0.04), FAS (F(4, 225) = 5.08; p = 0.02), animal fluency (F(4, 263) = 22.65; p = 0.04), and Trails B (F(4, 80) = 22.12; p = 0.008).

Table 2.

Neuropsychological assessments

Neuropsychological Assessment (N) SU Baseline Mean (SD) NSU Baseline Mean (SD) P-value Annul change (SE) in SU group Annual change (SE) in NSU group P-value for group difference of annual change
MoCA total score (128) 25.52 (3.99) 25.05 (3.45) 0.04* −0.262 (0.064) −0.435 (0.061) 0.01*
DRS-2 total score (313) 135.04 (10.00) 134.00 (11.83) 0.22 −0.732 (0.212) −1.64 (0.203) 0.001*
Attention subscore 35.62 (1.57) 35.47 (2.04) 0.61 −0.070 (0.054) −0.150 (0.048) 0.35
Initiation subscore 34.83 (4.46) 34.20 (5.20) 0.95 −0.458 (0.085) −0.783 (0.082) 0.002*
Construction subscore 5.87 (0.60) 5.72 (0.85) 0.05* −0.065 (0.020) −0.096 (0.019) 0.201
Concept subscore 36.40 (2.99) 36.12 (3.43) 0.48 −0.015 (0.066) −0.189 (0.063) 0.04*
Memory subscore 22.33 (3.32) 22.48 (3.10) 0.95 −0.135 (0.055) −0.326 (0.053) 0.006*
LNS (279) 9.38 (3.17) 9.04 (3.02) 0.14 −0.223 (0.043) −0.328 (0.042) 0.06
FAS (231) 41.96 (15.79) 37.96 (15.38) 0.02* −0.878 (0.164) −0.961 (0.160) 0.69
Animals (269) 17.70 (5.83) 16.72 (5.90) 0.04* −0.461 (0.090) −0.487 (0.088) 0.82
Trails A total time (85) 49.34 (47.37) 45.94 (25.47) 0.21 0.237 (0.577) 0.815 (0.594) 0.27
Trails B total time (85) 101.34 (59.11) 103.94 (63.57) 0.008* 4.05 (1.23) 5.50 (1.23) 0.24
Symbol Digit Modalities Test (85) 37.97 (9.43) 37.44 (11.10) 0.11 −0.714 (0.191) −0.885 (0.186) 0.37
BNT (274) 54.99 (6.06) 55.14 (4.91) 0.97 −0.106 (0.048) −0.0872 (0.049) 0.77
Clock Drawing Scale (291) 5.75 (1.43) 5.32 (1.84) 0.07 −0.084 (0.028) −0.093 (0.028) 0.80
JOLO (271) 22.41 (5.67) 22.05 (5.33) 0.71 −0.106 (0.094) −0.335 (0.097) 0.06
HVLT-R Total (295) 19.77 (6.24) 19.63 (5.84) 0.48 −0.374 (0.088) −0.387 (0.086) 0.91
HVLT-R Delayed Recall 5.93 (3.22) 5.98 (3.39) 0.86 −0.197 (0.053) −0.224 (0.052) 0.69
HVLT-R Retention % 68.73 (29.93) 69.47 (33.09) 0.92 −1.15 (0.638) −1.63 (0.627) 0.54
HVLT-R Recognition Discrimination 8.88 (2.62) 8.82 (2.43) 0.58 −0.097 (0.042) −0.093 (0.041) 0.94
*

Denotes that significance p≤0.05.

Longitudinal neuropsychological assessments

After controlling for baseline age, baseline UPDRS motor score, baseline cognitive assessment scores, and disease duration at baseline, we found that MoCA (F(1, 402.9) = 6.19; p = 0.013) and DRS-2 total scores (F(1, 706.1) = 3.95; p = 0.05), DRS-2 initiation/perseveration (F(1, 817.2) = 9.5; p = 0.002), concept (F(1, 896.2) = 4.3; p = 0.039), and memory (F(1,874.8) = 7.5; p = 0.006) subscores declined at a significantly slower rate over time in SU group compared with NSU group. These results were supported by near-significant findings on LNS and JOLO. Longitudinal neuropsychological scores are listed in Table 2.

Lipophilic statin-users vs. non-statin-users

Upon removal of hydrophilic SU, lipophilic SU performed significantly better on MoCA (F(4,107.3) = 10.94; p = 0.04) at baseline. Longitudinally, lipophilic SU declined at a significantly slower rater than NSU on DRS total score (F(1,684.2) = 5.99; p = 0.02).

ApoA1 analyses

Statin use did not significantly correlate with baseline ApoA1 plasma levels (all p > 0.05) and baseline ApoA1 levels did not significantly correlate with any baseline neuropsychological measures (all p > 0.05).

DISCUSSION

In this examination of the impact of statin use on cognition in PD we observed that SU performed better on assessments of global cognition, verbal fluency, and executive processing in cross-sectional analysis when controlling for important covariates. In addition, in longitudinal analyses rates of cognitive decline were less for SU on global scales and initiation/perseveration, concept, and memory subscores of DRS-2.

Results of our analysis on ApoA1 yielded non-significant results and leads us to believe that statins are not modulating cognition through increasing levels of ApoA1. Thus, our findings suggest that statin use-mediated cognitive reserve occurs via a different mechanism. A possible explanation relates to findings that, on autopsy, PD brains compared to matched controls have significantly larger blood vessel size, with vessel degeneration particularly in substantia nigra, medial frontal gyrus, and caudate nucleus [49]. PD cases were also found to have decreased number of capillaries and more small arterioles/veins, causing significant reduction in the ratio of small to large vessels in these brain regions [49]. Decreases in small blood vessels alter vascular networks and may lead to decreased vascular performance.

Statins may decrease effects of this vascular remodeling by removing ROS, and a reduction in ROS has been shown to delay cell death in the brain in vivo, animal models, and humans [57]. Simvastatin reduces mitochondrial stress in myocytes [50], yet effects on neurons is unknown. Statins may contribute to cognitive reserve in PD by reducing ROS in the medial frontal gyrus/cortex, an area found to exhibit increased vascular burden in PD [49].

In terms of study limitations, SU might have had better cognitive performance due to a healthier lifestyle and less medical comorbidity that could be associated with the decision to take statins. We addressed this by including a mFRF composite score, although this could introduce bias because hypertension was self-reported. We found that SU actually had more vascular risk factors typically associated with cognitive impairment in PD [51], thus making it less likely that there were other, unrelated factors explaining improved cognitive performance in SU. Another limitation is made apparent by a recent study that PD symptoms may worsen with statin use, which could have biased use of statins in our population (10). A further limitation was an inability to provide a proxy biomarker for statin use, e.g. fasting cholesterol levels. Additionally, our study had variable duration of follow-up, which can be addressed in future analyses of this cohort.

An ongoing, prospective, multi-site study known as “PD Stat” is examining use of simvastatin as a disease-modifying drug (https://penctu.psmd.plymouth.ac.uk/pdstat/Default.aspx). This and other studies will determine if statins truly have neuroprotective effects, with impact on both motor and non-motor symptoms.

Acknowledgments

Funding source (National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke) did not provide guidance on design or preparation of study or manuscript.

Footnotes

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

Mr. Deck has no conflicts of interest to report. Dr. Rick has no conflicts of interest to report. Dr. Xie has no conflicts of interest to report. Dr. Chen-Plotkin is supported by the NIH (RO1 NS082265, and UO1 NS082134), the Burroughs Wellcome Fund, the Benaroya Fund, the Alzheimer’s Association//Michael J. Fox Foundation/Weston Brain Institute, and the Pechenik Montague Award Fund. Dr. Duda has no conflicts of interest to report. Dr. Morley receives research funding from the Department of Veteran Affairs and GE Healthcare. Dr. Chahine receives support from the Michael J. Fox Foundation and receives royalties from Wolters Kluwel (for book authorship). Dr. Dahodwala receives grant funding from NIH, Parkinson’s Foundation, Michael J. Fox Foundation, Parkinson’s Council, AbbVie, Biotie and Medtronic. Dr. Trojanowski may accrue revenue in the future on patents submitted by the University of Pennsylvania wherein he is co-Inventor and he received revenue from the sale of Avid to Eli Lily as co-inventor on imaging related patents submitted by the University of Pennsylvania. Dr. Weintraub has received research funding or support from Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research, National Institutes of Health (NINDS), Novartis Pharmaceuticals, Department of Veterans Affairs, Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study, and the International Parkinson and Movement Disorder Society; honoraria for consultancy from Acadia, Biogen, Biotie (Acorda), Bracket, Clintrex LLC, Eisai Inc., Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, Takeda, UCB, and the CHDI Foundation; license fee payments from the University of Pennsylvania for the QUIP and QUIP-RS; royalties from Wolters Kluweland; and fees for legal consultation for lawsuits related to medication prescribing in patients with Parkinson’s disease.

References

  • 1.Hely MA, Reid WGJ, Adena MA, Halliday GM, Morris JGL. The Sydney Multicenter Study of Parkinson’s disease: The inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Mov Disord. 2008;23:837–844. doi: 10.1002/mds.21956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Liao JK, Laufs U. Pleiotropic effects of statins. Ann Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2009;45:89–118. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.45.120403.095748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Swanson CR, Li K, Unger TL, Gallagher MD, Van Deerlin VM, Agarwal P, Leverenz J, Roberts J, Samii A, Gross RG, Hurtig H, Rick J, Weintraub D, Trojanowski JQ, Zabetian C, Chen-Plotkin AS. Lower plasma Apolipoprotein A1 levels are found in Parkinson’s disease and associate with apolipoprotein A1 genotype. Mov Disord. 2014;30:805–812. doi: 10.1002/mds.26022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Swanson CR, Berlyand Y, Xie SX, Alcalay RN, Chahine LM, Chen-Plotkin AS. Plasma apolipoprotein A1 associates with age at onset and motor severity in early Parkinson’s disease patients. Mov Disord. 2015;30:1648–1656. doi: 10.1002/mds.26290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Di Napoli P. Statins and stroke: Evidence for cholesterol-independent effects. Eur Heart J. 2002;23:1908–1921. doi: 10.1053/euhj.2002.3236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tanaka SI, Fukumoto Y, Nochioka K, Minami T, Kudo S, Shiba N, Takai Y, Williams CL, Liao JK, Shimokawa H. Statins exert the pleiotropic effects through small gtp-binding protein dissociation stimulator upregulation with a resultant rac1 degradation. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 2013;33:1591–1600. doi: 10.1161/ATVBAHA.112.300922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wassmann S, Laufs U, Baumer AT, Muller K, Ahlbory K, Linz W, Itter G, Rösen R, Böhm M, Nickenig G. HMG-CoA reductase inhibitors improve endothelial dysfunction in normocholesterolemic hypertension via reduced production of reactive oxygen species. Hypertension. 2001;37:1450–1457. doi: 10.1161/01.hyp.37.6.1450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gao X, Simon K, Schwarzschild M, Ascherio A. Prospective study of statin use and risk of Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2012;69:380–384. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.1060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lee YC, Lin CH, Wu RM, Lin MS, Lin JW, Chang CH, Lai MS. Discontinuation of statin therapy associates with Parkinson disease: A population-based study. Neurology. 2013;81:410–416. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31829d873c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Liu G, Sterling NW, Kong L, Lewis MM, Mailman RB, Chen H, Douglas L, Huang X. Statins may facilitate Parkinson’s disease: Insight gained from a large, national claims database. Mov Disord. 2017;32:913–917. doi: 10.1002/mds.27006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Undela K, Gudala K, Malla S, Bansal D. Statin use and risk of Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis of observational studies. J Neurol. 2013;260:158–165. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6606-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bykov K, Yoshida K, Weisskopf MG, Gagne JJ. Confounding of the association between statins and Parkinson disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2017;26:294–300. doi: 10.1002/pds.4079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wolozin B, Wang SW, Li NC, Lee A, Lee T, Kazis LE. Simvastatin is associated with a reduced incidence of dementia and Parkinson’s disease. BMC Med. 2007;5:20. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-5-20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Jick H, Zornberg G, Jick S, Seshadri S, Drachman D. Statins and the risk of dementia. Lancet. 2000;356:1627–1631. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)03155-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wolozin B, Kellman W, Ruosseau P, Celesia GG, Siegel G. Decreased prevalence of Alzheimer disease associated with 3-hydroxy-3-methyglutaryl coenzyme A reductase inhibitors. Arch Neurol. 2000;57:1439–1443. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.10.1439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wolozin B. Cholesterol, statins and dementia. Curr Opin Lipidol. 2004;15:667–672. doi: 10.1097/00041433-200412000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sparks DL, Sabbagh MN, Connor DJ, Lopez J, Launer LJ, Browne P, Wasser D, Johnson-Traver S, Lochhead J, Ziolwolski C. Atorvastatin for the treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: Preliminary results. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:753–757. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.5.753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rockwood K, Kirkland S. Use of lipid-lowering agents, indication bias, and the risk of dementia in community-dwelling elderly people. Neurology. 2002;59:223–227. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.2.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Yaffe K, Barrett-Connor E, Lin F, Grady D. Serum lipoprotein levels, statin use, and cognitive function in older women. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:378–384. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.3.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wong WB, Lin VW, Boudreau D, Devine EB. Statins in the prevention of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis of observational studies and an assessment of confounding. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2013;22:345–358. doi: 10.1002/pds.3381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McGuinness B, Craig D, Bullock R, Passmore P. Statins for the prevention of dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016:CD003160. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003160.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Heart Protection Study Collaborative Group. MRC/BHF Heart Protection Study of cholesterol lowering with simvastatin in 20,536 high-risk individuals: A randomised placebo-controlled trial. Lancet. 2002;360:7–22. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61125-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Trompet S, van Vliet P, de Craen AJ, Jolles J, Buckley BM, Murphy MB, Ford I, Macfarlane PW, Sattar N, Packard CJ, Stott DJ, Shepherd J, Bollen EL, Blauw GJ, Jukema JW, Westendrop RG. Pravastatin and cognitive function in the elderly. Results of the PROSPER study. J Neurol. 2010;257:85–90. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5271-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rea TD, Breitner JC, Psaty BM, Fitzpatrick AL, Lopez OL, Newman AB, Hazzard WR, Zandi PP, Burke GL, Lyketsos CG, Kuller LH. Statin use and the risk of incident dementia: The Cardiovascular Health Study. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:1047–1051. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.7.1047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wagstaff LR, Mitton MW, Arvik BM, Doraiswamy PM. Statin-associated memory loss: Analysis of 60 case reports and review of the literature. Pharmacotherapy. 2003;23:871–880. doi: 10.1592/phco.23.7.871.32720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Evans MA, Golomb BA. Statin-associated adverse cognitive effects: Survey results from 171 patients. Pharmacotherapy. 2009;29:800–811. doi: 10.1592/phco.29.7.800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Muldoon MF, Barger SD, Ryan CM, Flory JD, Lehoczky JP, Matthews KA, Manuck SB. Effects of lovastatin on cognitive function and psychological well-being. Am J Med. 2000;108:538–546. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9343(00)00353-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Muldoon MF, Ryan CM, Sereika SM, Flory JD, Manuck SB. Randomized trial of the effects of simvastatin on cognitive functioning in hypercholesterolemic adults. Am J Med. 2004;117:823–829. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2004.07.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hughes A, Daniel S, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: A clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Fahn S, Elton R, Members of the UPDRS Development Committee . Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Calne DB, Goldstein M, editors. Recent Developments in Parkinson’s Disease. Vol. 2. Macmillan Health Care Information; Florham Park, NJ: 1987. pp. 153–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hoehn M, Yahr M. Parkinsonism: Onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17:427–442. doi: 10.1212/wnl.17.5.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Meara J, Mitchelmore E, Hobson P. Use of the GDS-15 geriatric depression scale as a screening instrument for depressive symptomatology in patients with Parkinson’s disease and their careers in the community. Age Ageing. 1999;28:35–38. doi: 10.1093/ageing/28.1.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Galasko D, Bennett D, Sano M, Ernesto C, Thomas R, Grundman M, Ferris S. An inventory to assess activities of daily living for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1997;11(Suppl 2):33S–39S. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Brennan L, Siderowf A, Rubright JD, Rick J, Dahodwala N, Duda JE, Hurtig H, Stern M, Xie SX, Rennert L, Karlawish J, Shea JA, Trojanowski JQ, Weintraub D. Development and initial testing of the Penn Parkinson’s Daily Activities Questionnaire. Mov Disord. 2016;31:126–134. doi: 10.1002/mds.26339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale-2. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; Lutz, FL: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nasreddine Z, Phillips N, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wechsler D. WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: Administration and Scoring Manual. Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gladsjo J, Shuman C, Evans J, Peavy G, Miller S, Heaton R. Norms for letter and category fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment. 1999;6:147–178. doi: 10.1177/107319119900600204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Reitan R. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain disease. Percept Mot Skills. 1958;8:271–276. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Smith A. Symbol digit modalities test: Manual. Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles: 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Brandt J, Benedict R. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Benton A, Varney N, Hamsher K. Visuospatial judgment: A clinical test. Arch Neurol. 1978;35:364–367. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hubbard EJ, Santini V, Blankevoort CG, Volkers KM, Barrup MS, Byerly L, Chaisson C, Jefferson AL, Kaplan E, Green RC, Stern RA. Clock drawing performance in cognitively normal elderly. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23:295–327. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. Boston Naming Test. Lea and Febiger; Philadelphia: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pigott K, Rick J, Xie SX, Hurtig H, Chen-Plotkin A, Duda JE, Morley JF, Chahine LM, Dahodwala N, Akhtar RS, Siderowf A, Trojanowski JQ, Weintraub D. Longitudinal study of normal cognition in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2015;85:1276–1282. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.D’Agostino RB, Vasan RS, Pencina MJ, Wolf PA, Cobain M, Massaro JM, Kannel WB. General cardiovascular risk profile for use in primary care: The Framingham heart study. Circulation. 2008;117:743–753. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.699579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Qiang JK, Wong YC, Siderowf A, Hurtig HI, Xie SX, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ, Yearout D, Leverenz J, Montline TJ, Stern M, Mendick S, Jennings D, Zabetian C, Marek K, Chen-Plotkin AS. Plasma apolipoprotein A1 as a biomarker for Parkinson disease. Ann Neurol. 2013;74:119–127. doi: 10.1002/ana.23872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bender R, Lange S. Adjusting for multiple testing – When and how? J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54:343–349. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00314-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Guan J, Pavlovic D, Dalkie N, Waldvogel HJ, O’Carroll SJ, Green CR, Nicholson LFB. Vascular degeneration in parkinsons disease. Brain Pathol. 2013;23:154–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2012.00628.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Mattar EH, Haffor ASA. Effect of dobutamine and hyperoxia on free radicals production in relation to the ultrastructural alterations in the endothelial of myocardial capillary in rats, Rattus norvigicus. Ultrastruct Pathol. 2009;33:209–215. doi: 10.3109/01913120903275626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Malek N, Lawton MA, Swallow DMA, Grosset KA, Marrinan SL, Bajaj N, Barker RA, Burn DJ, Hardy J, Morris HR, Williams NM, Wood N, Ben-Shlomo Y, Grosset DG, PRoBaND Clinical Consortium Vascular disease and vascular risk factors in relation to motor features and cognition in early Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2016;31:1518–1526. doi: 10.1002/mds.26698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES