Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Pers. 2017 Jun 21;86(3):380–396. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12322

Too much of a good thing? Exploring the inverted-U relationship between self-control and happiness

Christopher W Wiese 1, Louis Tay 2, Angela L Duckworth 3, Sidney D'Mello 4, Lauren Kuykendall 5, Wilhelm Hofmann 6, Roy F Baumeister 7, Kathleen D Vohs 8
PMCID: PMC5677575  NIHMSID: NIHMS874358  PMID: 28480971

Abstract

Objective

Can having too much self-control make people unhappy? Researchers have increasingly questioned the unilateral goodness of self-control and proposed that it is beneficial only up to a certain point, after which it becomes detrimental. The little empirical research on the issue shows mixed results. Hence, we tested whether a curvilinear relationship between self-control and subjective well-being exists.

Method

We used multiple metrics (questionnaires, behavioral ratings), sources (self-report, other-report), and methods (cross-sectional measurement, day-reconstruction method, experience sampling method) across six studies (Ntotal = 5,318).

Results

We found that self-control positively predicted subjective well-being (cognitive and affective), but there was little evidence for an inverted U-shaped curve. The results held after statistically controlling for demographics and other psychological confounds.

Conclusion

Our main finding is that self-control enhances subjective well-being with little to no apparent downside of too much self-control.

Keywords: happiness, well-being, self-control, curvilinear, self-regulation


Can too much self-control make you unhappy? The literature suggests different answers. One perspective argues that there is no downside to self-control since people with more tend to be happier and view their lives as being highly satisfying (Hofmann, Luhmann, Fisher, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2013). Another perspective holds that some self-control is beneficial, but there could be costs to having too much — namely in the form of reduced subjective well-being (SWB) or affective and cognitive evaluations of one's life (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Self-control, defined as the ability to control short-term impulses and desires in conflict with long-term goals (Hofmann, Baumesiter, Förster, & Vohs, 2011), could entail frequent and sometimes unnecessary regulation of emotions, thoughts, and behaviors, resulting in a life marked by rigidity and blandness, thereby lowering SWB (Grant & Schwartz, 2011).

Among different virtues, self-control has been recognized as a “master virtue” which makes all other virtues possible (Baumeister & Exline, 1999). At the same time, the development of self-control is a central concern of schools (Diamond & Lee, 2011) and, consequently, interventions have been designed to improve self-control under the assumption that there is no downside. However, these interventions may be harmful if self-control is ranged to problematic levels. Despite the importance of self-control (Duckworth & Kern, 2011), SWB (Diener, et al., 1999), and competing viewpoints on their relationship, there is scant research on the topic. Hence, we tested whether happiness declines at high levels of self-control.

Two theoretical perspectives on self-control and SWB

Psychologists widely agree that self-control promotes SWB. There are many mechanisms through which self-control fosters SWB. Someone with high self-control may feel a flush of success by routinely setting goals, making progress toward them, and ultimately accomplishing their objectives. Similarly, when faced with a choice between the immediate and delayed reward, individuals can experience positive emotions by simply anticipating what it will feel like when they eventually reach a distal goal (MacLeod, Coates, & Hetherton, 2008). Further, self-control aids in making progress toward goals, which leads to positive emotions (Bagozzi, Baumgartner, & Pieters, 1998). Those with higher levels of self-control also employ better strategies that facilitate goal progress and accomplishment (Duckworth, Grant, Loew, Oettingen, & Gollwitzer, 2011), which leads to longer-term happiness (Diener, et al., 1999).

Although most researchers agree that self-control is generally positive, they do not agree on whether someone can have too much self-control. One perspective, which argues that self-control has a functional relationship with SWB, states that SWB monotonically increases as self-control increases. This reasoning makes sense through an evolutionary lens—people exercise self-control in order to increase their chances of survival, which is inextricably tied to well-being. It is hypothesized that the prefrontal cortex (the part of the brain most responsible for self-control) developed when humans needed to restrain impulsive instincts to improve survival (and well-being) of their present and future self (Barkley, 2001; Dunbar, 2003). Behaviors that improved the chances of survival were rewarded with positive feelings, whereas negative feelings were the result of behaviors that decreased the likelihood of survival (Grinde, 2005). It is not surprising that self-regulation benefits a wide variety of life outcomes such as health (Tsukayama, Toomey, Faith, & Duckworth, 2010), relationships (Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004), and work/scholastic outcomes (Duckworth & Seligman, 2005), all of which in turn can promote SWB. If exercising self-control only occurs when it is beneficial for well-being, it is unlikely that we would observe downturns in well-being at high levels of self-control.

Conversely, positive antecedents can eventually turn negative if taken too far. This is known as the “too much of a good thing” effect (Pierce & Aguinis, 2013), which questions the unilateral goodness of self-control. Ideas of a “dark side” of self-control run deep in psychology, beginning as early as Freud's ideas of anal retentiveness, which refers to individuals with a strong compulsion for control. More recent research suggests something similar—overregulating cognitions, emotions, and behaviors can harm positive interpersonal relationships (Letzring, Block, & Funder, 2005), which will likely have detrimental consequences for well-being given the importance of social relations for SWB (Tay & Diener, 2011). Individuals with excessive self-control may have obsessive-compulsive tendencies for rigidity and inhibition, which may hinder social relationships (Letzring et al., 2005). In line with this, researchers have found the expected curvilinear pattern in closely related constructs. For example, abnormally high levels of conscientiousness predict obsessive-compulsive behaviors and less psychological well-being (Carter, Guan, Maples, Williamson, & Miller, 2015). Similarly, anorexia, which can be regarded as overregulation of eating (Halse, Honey, & Boughtwood, 2007), is associated with lower SWB (Kitsantas, Gillgan, & Kamata, 2003).

Another line of research argues that goal-setting may not always be beneficial (Ordóñez, Schweitzer, Galinsky, & Bazerman, 2009). Goal-setting is a primary mechanism through which self-control produces positive life outcomes such as SWB (Cheung, Gillebaart, Kroese & de Ridder, 2014; Hofmann et al., 2014). There are personal and psychological trade-offs when setting and investing in goals. Individuals with high self-control may focus exclusively on the accomplishment of their personal goals, potentially to the detriment of their personal happiness (McGregor & Little, 1998). Furthermore, ignoring this trade-off may lead to excessive worrying and anxiety (Pomerantz, Saxon, & Oishi, 2000). By consistently refraining from immediate gratification and instead focusing on one's goals, one never fully reaps the fruits of one's labor, thereby negatively impacting SWB.

Past Studies

Scant research has investigated the curvilinear relationship between self-control and well-being. To our knowledge, the three studies that directly test for a curvilinear relationship have had limited success in finding supporting evidence. In a study of young adults, Tangney et al., (2004) found that people with more self-control were less depressed, anxious, paranoid, and had less obsessive/compulsive tendencies. Further, Finkenauer, Engels, and Baumeister (2005) found that adolescents with more self-control were less depressed and less stressed. Neither of these studies found significant curvilinear effects. In contrast, Situ, Li, and Dou (2015) examined the relationship between self-control and emotional well-being (e.g., depression, anxiety) and found significant quadratic effects across three different samples (adolescents, young adults, employees). However, people with high self-control did not experience more emotional problems. Instead, the results reflected a pattern of diminishing returns where self-control improved emotional well-being up to a point, beyond which it had no effect.

It is important to note that none of these studies measured SWB directly. In each of the studies, well-being was conceptualized as maladaptive attitudes or behaviors (e.g., depression; obsessive-compulsion). Research has shown that these are related to SWB but are conceptually distinct (e.g., Brown, Chorpita, & Barlow, 1998; Watson, Clark, & Carey, 1988). Furthermore, these studies only evaluated affective/emotional components of well-being, and it is important to further evaluate the curvilinear relationship between self-control and cognitive aspects of SWB (i.e., life satisfaction). Methodologically, past research has been conducted using cross-sectional assessments with self-reported data. Although SWB is often assessed through self-report questionnaires, it is beneficial to use multiple measures (e.g., informant reports, behavioral measures) and research designs (e.g., day reconstruction, experience sampling) to examine this issue.

Current Investigation

The current investigation uses new and existing data to test our hypotheses. Previous publications have used data from Study 1 (e.g., Park, Tsukayama, Goodwin, Patrick, & Duckworth, 2016), Study 2 (e.g., Tsukayama, Duckworth, & Kim, 2013), Study 3 (e.g., Galla et al., 2014) and Study 6 (e.g., Hofmann et al., 2012; Hofmann, Luhmann, Fisher, Vohs, Baumeister, 2014). The current research questions and analyses do not overlap with previous reports from these data sets.

Through six studies, the present paper directly examines how self-control relates to different components of SWB while also expanding on the past methodological approaches. Although we varied the methodological techniques across studies, we consistently measured SWB using Diener and colleagues' (1999) tripartite conceptualization (positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction). Also, although self-control manifests differently across contexts and age ranges, our measures were centered on the idea that self-control represents the tendency to control short-term impulses that conflict with long-term goals (Hofmann, et al., 2011). We also took precaution to control for potentially confounding variables, such as demographics, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and neuroticism when they were available. Because self-control is a facet of conscientiousness (Eisenberg, Duckworth, Spinrad, & Valiente, 2014), it was not included as a control due to shared variance.

We adopted a similar analytic strategy across studies by conducting hierarchical linear regression analyses with a base model (control variables), self-control model (base model with self-control measure), and a quadratic model (self-control model with the addition of a quadratic term). Additionally, we took extra analytic steps in examining the inverted-U effect. Because individuals use an ideal point response process (i.e., it assumes a non-monotonic relation between the trait and observed score) for self-reports of constructs such as self-control and SWB (Tay, Drasgow, Rounds, & Williams, 2009; Tay & Drasgow, 2012; Tay & Kuykendall, 2016) and recent research suggesting that ideal-point response models may more accurately detect curvilinear relationships (Carter et al., 2014), we also examine whether ideal point scoring (compared to typical factor scoring) yields different results. Furthermore, due to the multiple comparisons conducted, we applied Bonferroni corrections in each study to reduce the likelihood that significant results are due to chance (Abdi, 2007). That is, we divided traditional significance values (i.e., .05, .01) by the total number of analyses conducted in each study.

The first three studies were conducted on similar samples (5th through 12th graders) using similar measures (self-reports of SWB, self- and teacher- reports of students' self-control). Given the similarity of the measures, we also conducted an integrative data analysis (Curran & Hussong, 2009) and these studies are discussed both individually and collectively. In order to address the issue of reference bias (i.e., the use of different standards when endorsing items based on context; Duckworth & Yeager, 2015) associated with questionnaires, we added a behavioral self-control task (D'Mello, Galla, & Duckworth, 2017) known to be immune to these effects (O'Brien et al., in preparation) in Study 4.

Study 5 tested the predictions using college undergraduates. Because there can be systematic biases associated with self-report measures of SWB, this study used the Day Reconstruction Method (DRM; Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwartz, & Stone, 2004) that evokes specific contexts to gather reports of episodic affect. Last, Study 6 used an experience sampling method (ESM) on a sample of community adults. Our use of diverse measures and samples allowed for more robust tests of the competing hypotheses.

Study 1

Study 1 used middle-school students to test for a curvilinear effect between self-control and SWB. Students completed a measure of self-control that taps their ability to control their impulses in academic and interpersonal contexts. Additionally, teachers rated students' self-control in these two contexts. Students also reported their SWB through reports of positive and negative affect as well as a rating of their current life satisfaction.

Method

Participants

Participants were 1,539 5th- through 8th-grade students (mean age = 11.65, SD = 1.30; 52.4% female) from seven schools in the United States. The sample included African-American (32%), Caucasian (17%), Hispanic (43%), Asian (5%), Native American (1%) and multi-racial (2%) individuals.

Measures

Self-Control

Students completed the Domain Specific Impulsivity Scale for Children (Tsukayama, Duckworth, & Kim, 2013). The measure required students to rate their self-control behaviors at school (α = .73) with 4 items (e.g., I paid attention and resisted distractions) and during interpersonal interactions (α = .78) with 4 items (e.g., I remained calm even when criticized or otherwise provoked) on 7-point Likert scales (1 = Almost Never, 7 = Almost Always).

Teachers (N = 134) were presented with the same self-control items as the students; however, they asked to provide an overall evaluation of each student's school and interpersonal self-control on 7-point Likert scales (1 = Almost Never, 7 = Almost Always). On average, teachers rated 3.5 students and inter-rater reliability was moderate for both school (rwg = .49) and interpersonal (rwg = .40) self-control. Student and teacher ratings were also moderately correlated (r = .44, p < .01 for school; r = .46, p < .01 for interpersonal).

Subjective Well-Being

Students reported how often they feel six positive feelings (e.g., happy, relaxed, excited) and four negative feelings (e.g., sad, worried, angry) to assess positive (α = .83) and negative (α = .68) affect (1 = Never, 5 = Always). As an indicator of life satisfaction, students answered the question “Overall, how satisfied are you with your life?” (1= Extremely Unsatisfied, 7 = Extremely Satisfied).

Statistical Controls

We controlled for gender, the school the student attended, and ethnicity. We also controlled for student reported extraversion (α = .66), agreeableness (α = .80), openness (α = .74), and neuroticism (α = .82) using 4 selected items from the BFI-44 (John & Srivastava, 1999) for each construct, which was measured concurrently with the self-control and SWB ratings.

Results

Table 1 reports results from the regression analyses. We applied Bonferroni corrections to the 12 regression analyses. Significance values were divided by 12, resulting in significance threshold of .004 (for α at .05) and .0008 (for α at .01). Both self-reported and teacher reported ratings of self-control significantly predicted all three components of SWB, with the exception that teacher ratings of school self-control did not predict negative affect (r = -.05, p > .004). There was no evidence for curvilinear effects.

Table 1. Standardized regression coefficients of Self-Control variables on SWB from Study 1 (n = 1539).

Positive Affect Negative Affect Life Satisfaction



Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report






βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2

SCS .30** .31** .13** .11** -.18** -.18** -.05 -.05 .27** .24** .11** .11**
SCS2 .01 -.05 -.01 .00 -.06 .00
R2 .26** .26** .20** .20** .23** .23** .21** .21** .21** .21** .17** .17**
ΔR2 .07** .00 .01** .00 .03** .00 .00 .00 .06** .00 .01** .00
SCI .30** .32** .11** .11** -.23** -.23** -.10* -.10* .22** .20** .10** .11**
SCI2 .04 .00 .00 -.01 -.05 .02
R2 .25** .25** .20** .20** .25** .25** .22** .22** .19** .19** .17** .17**
ΔR2 .07** .00 .01** .00 .04** .00 .01* .00 .03** .00 .01** .00

Note. SCS = Self-Control School; SCI = Self-Control Interpersonal; Self-Control variables were added to the regression equation after controls (gender, school, ethnicity, extraversion, agreeableness, openness, and neuroticism)); ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

Bonferroni corrected p-values:

*

p < .004;

**

p < .0008

We then applied an ideal point model scoring approach (as recommended by Carter et al., 2014) to all multi-item measures (student-reported self-control, positive and negative affect) through the GGUM2004 software (Roberts, Fang, Cui, & Wang, 2006). This approach cannot be applied to single item measures (teacher ratings of student self-control, and self-reports of life satisfaction). Ten of the aforementioned analyses were re-run using the ideal-point model scores in lieu of mean estimates. The two exceptions were the relationship between teacher reported self-control (both school and interpersonal) and life satisfaction as all three relied on a single item measure. The results 10 remaining analyses mirrored the earlier results in that there was evidence of a linear relationship between self-control and SWB, but no evidence of a quadratic effect.

Study 2

Study 1 found a linear relationship between self-control and SWB on 11 of 12 tests, indicating a consistent association. The more that students possessed self-control (as rated my themselves and their teachers), the more they experienced positive affect, negative affect, and were satisfied with their lives. There was no indication of a curvilinear effect. Study 2 aimed to replicate these effects using slightly different measurements to ensure that the effects of Study 1 were not dependent on specific measures.

Method

Participants

Participants were 667 6th- through 8th- grade students (52.8% female) enrolled in three schools in the United States. On average, students in these grades range between 11 and 14 years old. The sample included African-American (26%), Caucasian (24%), Hispanic (45%), Asian (3%), and multi-racial (2%) students.

Measures

Self-Control

Using the same scale as in Study 1, students self-rated their school (α = .63) and interpersonal (α = .72) self-control with four items each. Unlike Study 1, teachers were asked to report student's self-control using the same 8-item measure as the students. Also, although teachers did rate several students, each student was only rated once. We found sufficient internal consistency reliability for teacher ratings of both school (α = .91) and interpersonal (α = .89) self-control. Student and teacher ratings were moderately correlated for both school (r = .24, p < .01) and interpersonal (r = .37, p < .01) self-control.

Subjective Well-Being

Using the Positive and Negative Affect Scale for Children (PANAS-C: Laurent et al., 1999), students rated 15 positive and 15 negative emotions (e.g., delighted, active, afraid) on 5-point Likert scales (1 = Very slightly or not at all, 5 = Extremely). Both positive (α = .87) and negative affect (α = .88) were internally consistent. We assessed life satisfaction using the 5-item Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). Students were asked to rate their current life satisfaction (e.g., In most ways my life is close to my ideal, The conditions of my life are excellent) on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Disagree strongly, 5 = Agree strongly), which yielded and good reliability (α = .80).

Statistical Controls

Our analyses controlled for gender, school, and ethnicity. We also controlled for student reported extraversion (8 items; α = .66), agreeableness (9 items; α = .68), openness (10 items; α = .70), and neuroticism (8 items; α = .70) measured via the BFI-44 (John & Srivastava, 1999).

Results

We conducted 12 regression analyses and used Bonferroni corrections for significance thresholds (.004 for α = .05; .0008 for α = .01). Results (Table 2) produced only two significant main effects: students' self-reports of school self-control and life satisfaction (β = .13, p < .0008) and teacher ratings of students' school self-control and positive affect (β = .14, p < .0008).

Table 2. Standardized regression coefficients of Self-Control variables on SWB from Study 2 (n = 667).

Positive Affect Negative Affect Life Satisfaction



Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report






βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2

SCS .05 .06 .14** .19** -.09 -.09 -.00 -.09 .13** .14* .07 .11
SCS2 .01 .07 .01 .13* .01 .06
R2 .32** .32** .34** .34** .35** .35** .35** .36** .26** .26** .25** .25**
ΔR2 .00 .00 .02** .00 .01 .00 .00 .01* .01** .00 .00 .00
SCI .03 .02 .03 .17 -.09 -.11 -.04 .01 .11 .20** .02 .02
SCI2 -.01 .17 .05 -.06 -.14* .00
R2 .32** .32** .32** .33** .35** .35** .35** .35** .25** .26** .25** .25**
ΔR2 .00 .00 .00 .01 .01 .00 .00 .00 .01 .01* .00 .00

Note. SCS = Self-Control School; SCI = Self-Control Interpersonal; Self-Control variables were added to the regression equation after controlling for gender, school, ethnicity, extraversion, agreeableness, openness, and neuroticism; ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

Bonferroni corrected p-values:

*

p < .004;

**

p < .0008

Additionally, student ratings of interpersonal self-control showed a significant quadratic effect on life satisfaction (β = -.14, p < .004); likewise, teacher ratings of school self-control had a significant quadratic effect on negative affect (β = .13, p < .004). However, the shape of the curves did not reflect the too much of a good thing effect (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Study 2 curvilinear relations for: a) student reported interpersonal self-control and life satisfaction and b) teacher reported school self-control on negative affect.

Figure 1

We also used an ideal-point modeling scoring approach on all self-control (i.e., both student and teacher reports) and SWB (i.e., positive affect, negative affect, life satisfaction). These results replicated the linear effects and there was no evidence of a too much of a good thing effect.

Study 3

Although Study 1 found linear association between self-control and SWB, the evidence in for the linear effect was less strong in Study 2. Further, there was no evidence that more self-control would result in worse SWB. Study 3 was conducted to test the competing hypotheses again, and thus was another replication attempt.

Method

Participants

Participants were 1,386 12th-grade students (mean age = 17.98, SD = .55; 51.1% female) from three United States schools. The ethnic breakdown of the sample was African-American (31%), Caucasian (36%), Hispanic (11%), Asian (20%), and multi-racial (2%).

Measures

Self-Control

Study 3 used the school (4-items; α = .68) and interpersonal (4-items; α = .72). self-control scales from Study 1 (Tsukayama, et al., 2013). Teacher ratings for self-control were gathered in the same manner as Study 1. Two teachers for each student answered one item tapping the student's self-control at school (rwg = .53) and one item tapping student's interpersonal self-control (rwg = .61). We averaged the scores across teachers. Correlations between student and teacher ratings of self-control were r = .22, p < .01 for school self-control and r = .19, p < .01 for interpersonal self-control.

Subjective Well-Being

We also measured SWB similarly to Studies 1 and 2. Participants responded to 5 positive items (e.g., happy, related, excited) and 5 negative items (e.g., sad, worried, angry) to assess positive (α = .79) and negative (α = .74) affect respectively. The students answered one Life Satisfaction question, “Overall, how satisfied are you with your life,” on a 7-point Likert scale (1= Extremely Unsatisfied, 7 = Extremely Satisfied).

Statistical Controls

Analyses also controlled for gender, school, ethnicity, extraversion (α = .75), agreeableness (α = .68), openness (α = .69), and neuroticism (α = .78). The latter four were measured using 4 selected items (16 total) from the BFI-44 (John & Srivastava, 1999).

Results

Bonferroni corrections on 12 regression analyses resulted in significance thresholds of.004 (for α at .05) and .0008 (for α at .01). Regression results (Table 3) demonstrated that, while student ratings of school self-control predicted all three components of SWB, student ratings of interpersonal self-control only predicted negative affect (β = -.10, p < .0008). None of the teacher ratings of student's self-control significantly predicted SWB. Most important, adding a quadratic term to the models did not account for additional variance in any of the SWB measures. We conducted ideal point scoring of self-reported self-control as well as positive and negative affect. These models did not reveal any significant inverted-U effects.

Table 3. Standardized regression coefficients of Self-Control variables on SWB from Study 3 (n = 1386).

Positive Affect Negative Affect Life Satisfaction



Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report






βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2

SCS .15** .15** .00 .00 -.12** -.11** -.03 -.04 .18** .19** .03 .04
SCS2 .03 .00 .04 -.02 .03 .03
R2 .35** .35** .34** .34** .49** .49** .47** .47** .19** .19** .16** .16**
ΔR2 .02** .00 .00 .00 .01** .00 .00 .00 .03** .00 .00 .00
SCI .05 .04 -.02 -.02 -.10** -.09** .03 .02 .06 .05 -.02 .03
SCI2 -.03 -.01 .02 -.02 -.03 .07
R2 .34** .34** .34** .34** .48** .48** .47** .47** .16** .16** .16** .16**
ΔR2 .00 .00 .00 .00 .01** .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00

Note. SCS = Self-Control School; SCI = Self-Control Interpersonal; Self-Control variables were added to the regression equation after controls (gender, school, ethnicity, extraversion, agreeableness, openness, and neuroticism)); ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

Bonferroni corrected p-values:

*

p < .004;

**

p < .0008

Study 1-3 Discussion

Studies 1-3 found indications of a linear relationship between self-control and SWB — but no quadratic effects. In order to make stronger claims, we conducted an Integrative Data Analysis (IDA; Curran & Hussong, 2009). IDA allows for pooling data across different samples, which can increase the power to detect relationships beyond that of an individual study. We harmonized the measures (e.g., transforming all items to be on the same scale), integrated the data sets, and created scale means of each construct using observed item scores (as recommended by Bainter & Curran, 2015; Curran & Hussong, 2009). We then assigned each student a corresponding Study ID and conducted hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) modeling with Study ID as a Level 2 random effect.

The results from the IDA are reported in Table 4 using traditional significance values. There were clear, consistent linear effects between students' self-reported self-control scores and SWB after controlling for demographic and psychographic variables (as reported in Studies 1-3). Additionally, some teacher reports of students' self-control significantly predicted SWB. Most importantly, there was no evidence of a significant inverted-U shape effect. That is, neither teacher ratings nor student self-ratings indicated that students with very high self-control are less happy than others. The one significant quadratic effect between teacher reported school self-control and negative affect was not in the expected direction (Figure 2).

Table 4. Effect size estimates of Self-Control variables on SWB from Study 1-3 using Integrative Data Analysis.

Positive Affect Negative Affect Life Satisfaction



Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report






βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2

SCS .14* .15** .08* .09* -.11** -.11** -.03 -.05 .17** .17** .07* .08*
SCS2 .01 .01 .01 -.02* -.01 .01
R2a .36** .36** .31** .31** .42** .42** .40** .40** .22** .22** .19** .19**
ΔR2a .06 .00 .01 .00 .03 .00 .01 .00 .03 .00 .00 .00
SCI .11* .11* .03 .05 -.12** -.13** -.01 -.02 .11** .11** .04 .05
SCI2 .01 .02 .00 .00 .00 .01
R2a .32** .32** .30** .30** .41** .41** .40** .40** .20** .20** .19** .19**
ΔR2a .02 .00 .00 .00 .02 .00 .01 .00 .01 .00 .00 .00

Note. Values are unstandardized parameter estimates. SCS = Self-Control School; SCI = Self-Control Interpersonal; Self-Control variables were added to the regression equation after controlling for gender, ethnicity, extraversion, agreeableness, openness, and neuroticism; ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

a

R2 is a conditional R2 which explains the proportion of variance explained by random and fixed factors

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01

Figure 2. IDA curvilinear relations for teacher reported school self-control on negative affect.

Figure 2

Study 4

Studies 1-3 found no evidence of a downturn in SWB at high levels of trait self-control. One possible objection is that self- or informer-reports of self-control are biased. Study 4 therefore used a behavioral measure of self-control called the Academic Diligence Task (ADT). The ADT presents participants with the ongoing choice between working toward academic goals (e.g., practicing math or spelling) and doing fun, rewarding activities (e.g., playing a video game or watching YouTube videos). This measure aptly captures one possible route by which self-control could reduce SWB, because high scores require foregoing pleasures for the sake of work. If scoring very high on the ADT indicates a joyless, duty-bound approach to life, it might well lead to lower SWB.

Method

Participants

Participants were 1,280 9th-grade students (mean age = 14.89, SD = .47, 50.4% female) from eight schools in the United States. The sample was comprised of African-American (45%), Caucasian (26%), Hispanic (16%), Asian (12%), and multi-racial (1%) students. A sub-sample of students completed the Academic Diligence Task (n = 300), a behavioral measure of self-control.

Measures

Self-Control

Students completed the same items for school and interpersonal self-control as Study 1 (Tsukayama, et al., 2013), plus an additional item for each domain (i.e., 5 items total per domain). Both school (α = .77) and interpersonal (α = .79) self-control demonstrated good reliability.

Teacher ratings of student self-control were gathered in the same manner as Study 1. On average, the 59 teachers rated approximately 91 students each on both school and interpersonal self-control. We calculated rwg for both school (.75) and interpersonal (.70) self-control and averaged ratings to create overall scores for each.

Academic Diligence Task

The ADT is a web-based computerized task designed to mirror real-world situations where a student must make the difficult decision of completing an easy but tedious skill-building task (i.e., single-digit subtraction for the math domain; spelling for the verbal domain; and navigation for the spatial domain) while forgoing entertaining distractions (e.g., viewing music videos, movie trailers, sports highlights, or playing Tetris). After explaining the importance of the skill-building task, students interact (across three, 3-minute blocks) with a split-screen interface that provides them the choice to either complete the skill-building activity or engage with the distractors. The dependent variable is the percent of time spent on the skill-building activity (time on task) and how many skill-building tasks they answered correctly (productivity).

Subjective Well-Being

Subjective well-being was measured with three indices: positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction. Positive and negative affect was measured (1 = Never; 5 = Always) using four and six items (Diener et al., 2009). Participants indicated how often in the past month they felt good, happy, joyful, and satisfied (α = 82). Negative affect was measured using six items Diener et al., 2009). Participants were asked how often they felt bad, sad, afraid, angry, worried, and stressed (α = .79). Life satisfaction was measured with a single item (How satisfied or unsatisfied were you with your life?) on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly unsatisfied; 6 = Strongly satisfied).

Statistical Controls

We used gender, school, and ethnicity as controls.

Results

The Bonferroni corrections for the 18 regression analyses resulted in significance targets of .003 (for α at .05) and .0006 (for α at .01). Parallel to the IDA conducted on Studies 1-3, both self-report self-control measures significantly predicted positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction (Table 5). Teacher reports of student self-control predicted positive affect and life satisfaction, but not negative affect. Neither metric of self-control from the Academic Diligence Task (productivity, time on tasks) predicted SWB (Table 6).

Table 5. Standardized regression coefficients of Self-Control (self/teacher reports) variables on SWB from Study 4 (n = 1280).

Positive Affect Negative Affect Life Satisfaction



Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report Self Report Teacher Report






βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2

SCS .27** .27** .12** .11** -.21** -.20** -.08 -.08 .19** .19** .12** .12**
SCS2 .01 -.03 .04 .03 .02 .01
R2 .11** .11** .05** .05** .15** .15** .12** .12** .09** .09** .07** .07**
ΔR2 .07** .00 .01** .00 .04** .00 .01 .00 .03** .00 .01** .00
SCI .30** .29** .10* .09* -.25** -.25** -.06 -.06 .21** .22** .09* .10*
SCI2 -.04 -.04 -.01 .01 .02 .03
R2 .12** .12** .05** .05** .17** .17** .11** .11** .10** .10** .06** .06**
ΔR2 .08** .00 .01* .00 .05** .00 .00 .00 .04** .00 .01 .00

Note. SCS = Self-Control School; SCI = Self-Control Interpersonal; Self-Control variables were added to the regression equation after controls (gender, school, and ethnicity); ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

Bonferroni corrected p-values:

*

p < .003;

**

p < .0006

Table 6. Standardized regression coefficients of Self-Control (ADT) variables on SWB from Study 4 (n = 300).

Positive Affect Negative Affect Life Satisfaction



βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2
Productivity .00 .04 -.02 -.04 .13 .17
Productivity2 -.08 .05 -.08
R2 .07 .07 .14** .14** .09 .10
ΔR2 .00 .00 .00 .00 .01 .01
Task Time -.02 -.01 -.01 -.01 .12 .12
Task Time2 -.16 .03 -.07
R2 .07 .09 .14** .14** .09 .09
ΔR2 .00 .02 .00 .00 .01 .00

Note. ADT = Academic Diligence Task; self-control variables (i.e., productivity and task time) were added to the regression equation after controlling for gender, school, and ethnicity; ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

Bonferroni corrected p-values:

*

p < .003;

**

p < .0006

Most central to our paper, the addition of the squared term to test the curvilinear effects between self-control and SWB revealed no significant effects. Re-running the models after using the ideal-point modeling scoring approach on self-reported self-control ratings as well as positive and negative affect revealed no significant curvilinear effects.

Discussion

As in Studies 1-3, Study 4 provided strong support that perceptions of self-control were linearly related to SWB with no downturn. When we assessed self-control using other methods, such as teacher ratings and the behavioral task, there was less evidence of a relationship between self-control and SWB. More importantly, we did not find the inverted-U effect, which could conceal the lack of a positive relationship.

The behavioral measure of self-control likewise failed to show any sign of a curvilinear relationship to SWB. However, it also failed to find the linear relationship that has been robust across self-report measures. The lack of a positive relationship suggests two possibilities. First, the task may be too specific, and doing well on the specific task may not generalize to broader life domains to affect SWB. Second, the task may demonstrate that behavioral measures of self-control are not related to SWB. This would imply that there is some degree of global positivity bias—individuals who view themselves as having greater self-control also view themselves as happy. Study 5 was designed to tease these apart.

Study 5

The failure of the behavioral measure in Study 4 to yield results comparable to those of the self-report measures raises the possibility of a global positivity bias in aggregate self-reports. Study 5 aimed to minimize that problem by using the Day Reconstruction Method (DRM) (Kahneman, et al., 2004), which has people list activities during different segments of their day. We had them rate their SWB during each event.

Method

Participants

We tested 320 college undergraduates (mean age = 19.33, SD = 1.38; 48% female). The sample was comprised of African-American (4%), Caucasian (72%), Hispanic (3%), Asian (17%), Native American (1%) and multi-racial (3%) individuals.

Measures

Self-Control

We used four measures to assess self-control. Participants completed the 36-item Self-Control Scale (Tangney, et al., 2004), the 30-item Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (BIS-11; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995), the 12-item Delay of Gratification Scale (Ray & Najman, 1986), and the 10-item Academic Delay of Gratification Scale (Bembenutty & Karabenick, 1998). Each measure demonstrated acceptable internal consistency (α = .97, .83, .74, .69, respectively).

Subjective Well-Being

We measured the affective components of SWB in two ways. First, participants completed the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) before engaging in the DRM. The PANAS presented 10 mood states of each valence (positive affect, α = .88; negative affect, α = .89). Second, we calculated positive and negative affect using the DRM (Kahneman, et al., 2004). Using this method, participants split the previous day into three parts: morning, afternoon, and evening. Within these three parts, they listed as many events as they could think of, and the time the event began and ended. For each event, they rated on a scale from 0 to 10 how much they felt each of the 14 positive affect states (e.g., excited, serene, active, proud) and 16 negative affect states (e.g., upset, guilty, bored). These affective responses were subsequently weighted by how long the event lasted to create a single score for positive and negative affect.

Controls

We controlled for age, gender, and ethnicity.

Study 5 Results

Bonferroni corrections stipulated significance thresholds of .003 (for α= .05) and .0006 (for α= .01) for the 16 regression analyses. For PANAS measures, self-control indices significantly predicted both positive and negative affect (Table 7), with the exception that delay of gratification did not significantly predict self-reported positive affect (β = .15, p > .003). Furthermore, all self-control measures significantly predicted negative affect using the DRM, but they did not predict positive affect.

Table 7. Standardized regression coefficients of Self-Control variables on SWB from Study 5 (n = 320).

Positive Affect Negative Affect


Self Report DRM Self Report DRM




βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2 βStep 1 βStep 2

SC .34** .33** .11 .11 -.48** -.47** -.37** -.37**
SC2 .06 .04 -.06 .03
R2 .16** .17** .08* .08* .32** .33** .23** .23**
ΔR2 .11** .01 .01 .00 .22** .01 .13** .00
IMP -.25** -.24** -.09 -.09 .32** .32** .28** .28**
IMP2 .02 -.03 -.02 -.03
R2 .11** .11** .08* .08* .20** .20** .18** .18**
ΔR2 .06** .00 .01 .00 .10** .00 .08** .00
DOG .15 .15 .11 .11 -.26** -.28** -.24** -.25**
DOG2 .04 .01 -.10 -.05
R2 .07 .07 .08* .08* .17** .18** .16** .16**
ΔR2 .02 .00 .01 .00 .07** .01 .06** .00
ADOG .31** .31** .09 .09 -.26** -.26** -.24** -.24**
ADOG2 .11 .06 -.06 .03
R2 .14** .15** .08* .08* .17** .17** .15** .15**
ΔR2 .09** .01 .01 .00 .07** .00 .05** .00

Note. DRM = Day Reconstruction Method; SC = Self-Control; IMP = Impulsivity; DOG = Delay of Gratification; ADOG = Academic Delay of Gratification; Self-Control variables were added to the regression equation after controlling for gender, age, and ethnicity; ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

Bonferroni corrected p-values:

*

p < .003;

**

p < .0006

Crucially, no significant curvilinear effects were found in these analyses and after estimating self-reported positive and negative affect (via PANAS) and all four measures of self-control using ideal-point modeling scoring.

Discussion

The DRM was employed to overcome potential biases in global assessments of self-control and SWB. Nonetheless, and consistent with Studies 1-4, Study 5 found no evidence for a curvilinear relationship between self-control and SWB.

The linear positive relationship between self-control and SWB was once again found in Study 5, especially with self-report measures. The four self-control scales predicted both traditional self-reported and DRM negative affect, but not DRM positive affect. Although DRM positive affect was significantly correlated with self-reported positive affect (r = .34, p < .01), it was not significantly correlated with any of the self-control measures. These findings suggest that self-control may not produce positive affect in the moment, whereas engaging in self-control may reduce negative feelings.

Still, the main finding is that there was no sign that high levels of self-control bring a downturn in SWB. Study 5 ruled out the alternative interpretation that the linear relationship between self-reported SWB and self-control reflects a positivity bias in one-shot aggregate self-report measures, because it still emerged with the DRM.

Study 6

Studies 1-5 tested our hypothesis across a developmental span of 5th grade through young adults. Study 6 further extended the investigation to a community sample of adults. We analyzed data from the Everyday Temptations Study (Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster, & Vohs, 2012), which applied an experience-sampling method (ESM) to capture self-control episodes in daily life. The use of ESM rules out the possible influence of lay theories that may be present in global judgments (in Studies 1-3). In particular, traditional self-report survey data are dependent on reconstructive judgments, which may be influenced by existing lay theories of the universal ‘goodness’ of self-control. This may result an artificial linear effect between self-control and happiness and mask the true underlying inverted-U effects. Because momentary assessments are focused on specific events, they would be less prone to these biases (Hektner, et al., 2007). Hence this approach offered our best hope for finding evidence for an inverted-U relationship between self-control and SWB.

Method

Participants

As described in Hofmann et al. (2012), the sample consisted of 205 adults (66% female) from Würzburg, Germany. Participants ranged from 18 to 55 years old (M = 25.24, SD = 6.32). Participants were given €20 with an additional incentive of movies passes (€15) if they completed 80% of the signals as well as entrance into raffle for one of two portable music players (iPod Touch).

Procedure

Participants were provided with Blackberry personal data assistants (PDAs) for seven consecutive days (for a detailed overview of the procedure, see Hofmann et al., 2012). Each day they received seven signals to the PDA and completed an experience-sampling protocol designed to assess whether they were experiencing any desires and whether they used self-control to resist their desires.

Measures

Self-Control

Self-control was measured both at the person and event level. Before completing the ESM part of the study, participants completed the 13-item version of the Trait Self-Control Scale (Tangney et al., 2004, α = .87). Additionally, event-level self-control was measured by having participants rate how successful they were at resisting a given desire using a 6-point Likert scale. Importantly, in order to receive this question, participants needed to have indicated that they experienced a temptation within the previous half an hour and that they had tried to resist it. Unlike previous investigation using these data, our sample excluded events when individuals gave into their temptations.

Subjective Well-Being

SWB was also measured at both the person and event level. Prior to the experience sampling portion of the study, participants completed the five-item Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985, α = .80). The event-level indication of SWB was a single item concerning their momentary affective well-being on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = very bad, 7 = very good).

Controls

We controlled for several demographic variables (age, gender, nationality) and used a German adaptation of the brief ten-item personality measure (Gosling, Rentfrow, and Swann, 2003) to measure extraversion (α = .63), neuroticism (α = .75), agreeableness (α = .14), and openness (α = .54).

Results

We used three approaches to examine the data. With the person-level data, we investigated the potential curvilinear relationship between trait self-control and life satisfaction. Results from the hierarchical linear regression analysis demonstrated a significant linear effect (β = .38, p < .01), but not a significant curvilinear effect (β = .06, p > .05). The ideal-point scoring replicated the results with respect to a linear but no inverted-U effect.

Given that the event-level data was nested within individuals, hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) assessed the relation between both momentary self-control (i.e., self-control success; SCSij) and self-control success aggregated to the person level (i.e., SCS¯j), where j represents the person and i represents the event. To disentangle momentary effects from aggregated effects, we conducted group-mean centering of momentary self-control (i.e., an individual's momentary self-control score minus the average of the same individual's momentary self-control scores; Enders & Tofighi, 2007). The quadratic scores were calculated from the group-mean centered self-control success variable (i.e., [SCSijSCS¯j]2) and the aggregated self-control variable (i.e., SCS¯j2).

Level 1:

SWBij=β0j+β1j(SCSijSCS¯j)+β2j([SCSijSCS¯j]2)+rij (1)

Level 2:

β0j=γ00j+γ01(SCS¯j)+γ02(SCS¯j2)+μ0j (2)
β1j=γ10j+μ1j (3)
β2j=γ20j+μ2j (4)

Additional controls in equation (2) including gender, age, and personality (extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism, openness) were also included but not displayed in the equations for simplicity.

We used the lme4 (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015) in R to run these models. Due to the multilevel nature of the data, we estimated a conditional R, which explains the proportion of variance explained by both random and fixed factors. Results from these analyses are presented in Table 8. Aggregated self-control success significantly predicted average momentary SWB (at Level 2 of the model); however, the quadratic term was not significant. Additionally, we found a significant linear relationship between momentary self-control success and momentary SWB (at Level 1) as well as a significant quadratic effect, however it was not in the shape of an inverted-U relationship (Figure 3).

Table 8. Multilevel Models for testing the effects of self-control successes on momentary affective well-being from Study 6.

Momentary Affective Well-Being

Fixed Effects Step 1 Step 2

Level 2 SCS¯j01) .08 .13**
SCS¯j202) .04
Level 1 (SCSijSCS¯j)10) .07** .11**
[SCSijSCS¯j]220) .03**
Variance Components
Intercept Variance .34 .34
SCS Slope Variance .02 .02
R2a .24** .24**
ΔR2a .05 .00

Note. n = 205 subjects (Level 2), n = 3192 events (Level 1), SCS = Self-Control Successes; self-control successes variables ( SCS¯j; (SCSijSCS¯j)) were added to the model after controlling for gender, age, nationality and personality; ΔR2 denotes self-control variables over and above controls (and self-control main effects for squared terms)

a

R2 the conditional R2, which explains the proportion of variance explained by random and fixed factors

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01

Figure 3. Study 6 curvilinear relations for self-control successes and momentary affective well-being.

Figure 3

We also used multiple regression analysis to test whether aggregated self-control success at the person level predicted participants' scores on the Satisfaction with Life Scale as an alternative measure of SWB. Again, the linear term was significant, β = .21, p < .01, while the quadratic term was not, β = -.07, p > .05. Last, a bootstrapping mediation analysis (Preacher & Hayes, 2004) established that aggregated self-control success partially mediated the above relationship between dispositional self-control (TSC) and SWB as measured with the Satisfaction With Life Scale, as indicated by a reliable indirect effect (β = .03; 95% confidence interval > 0).

Discussion

Study 6 used ESM to overcome potential biases associated with self-report data. Further, this study extends the previous studies by using a sample of adults. We found consistent evidence of a linear effect of self-control on SWB with person-level (i.e., trait self-control and aggregated self-control successes) and event-level (i.e., self-control success) data. But once again there was no evidence of an inverted-U relation.

Supplementary Re-Analysis

Conducting Bonferroni corrections may be too conservative and increases the likelihood of Type II errors (Perneger, 1998). To address this possibility, we re-ran all of our analyses without any corrections to the traditional significance criteria of α = .05. However, even when using this criterion, only two significant quadratic terms resembled the expected pattern. Using the uncorrected .05, Study 1 yielded a significant quadratic effect between self-reported SCS and life satisfaction (β = -.06, p < .05). Happiness increased with school self-control up to a point and then leveled off. It was never detrimental, and so this analysis failed to show too much of a good thing — merely enough of a good thing, consistent with the notion of diminishing returns from continuing to exercise ever higher levels self-discipline in schoolwork. The other significant quadratic effect was found in Study 4 with the relationship between time on the ADT and positive affect (β = -.11, p < .05). This effect is the most representative of the “inverted U” shape (Figure 4), as participants who spent more time on task began to report less positive affect after a certain point. We are reluctant to place much evidence on this isolated finding, not least because the ADT generally yielded little, and this finding could also suggest that refusing all the pleasures on offer during the ADT might have primed the view of the self as not having fun. Moreover, in terms of effect size, the quadratic terms in both of these cases accounted for 1% or less of the incremental variance in SWB. In general, there does not seem to be strong evidence of inverted-U effects for SWB even when not controlling for multiple comparisons.

Figure 4. Study 4 curvilinear relations for time on task and positive affect.

Figure 4

General Discussion

This investigation answered the call of Grant and Schwartz (2011) to investigate “the inverted U” in positive psychology contexts. Although those authors suggested that such studies may identify cases when you can have too much of a good thing, our results suggest that too much self-control is not one of these, at least with regard to SWB. Across six studies employing multiple samples, methods, and measures, we found no support for the inverted U effect of self-control on SWB. These results echo prior investigations that have studied other aspects of well-being (e.g., depression, stress, social relationships; Finkenauer et al., 2005; Tangney et al., 2004) and lend support to the functional perspective of self-control. Apparently, the more self-control people have, the happier they are, at least within the bounds tested in these studies.

From an evolutionary perspective, natural selection favored self-control insofar as it improved survival and reproduction — and the same for subjective feelings of pleasure. Self-control helps individuals succeed and thrive, and these successes bring happiness. Hence they should be (and are, in our data) positively correlated. Too much self-control would only reduce SWB if it led to fewer positive outcomes. The human mind differs from most other animals in its ability to project into the future, and so people can modify current behavior to bring later benefits. Although self-control often involves foregoing immediate pleasure, these sacrifices may be rewarded in the long run. Our findings suggest that such benefits outweigh the loss of in-the-moment pleasures for an overall higher SWB.

The goal of the present investigation was to test the potential curvilinear effects of self-control conceptualized as a virtue. Often, high levels of self-control are intuitively associated with concepts such as dysfunctional perfectionism (i.e., rigid adherence to unreachable standards) or obsessive-compulsive tendencies. However, we believe these represent the inappropriate application of self-regulation (e.g., regulation when not faced with temptation) and may actually be indicative of less self-control.

Crucially, the present study was one of the first to investigate the inverted-U relationship between self-control and SWB using diverse measures and methods. Despite many variations in measures and procedures, we found very consistent patterns across the six studies: SWB does not appear to decline as self-control increases, even at the highest levels. There are several implications of these findings.

First, given that greater self-control is associated with higher SWB (rather than lower), our results provide preliminary support for promoting self-control among school-aged children. This is because the potential downturn in happiness could present a dilemma for those designing self-control interventions as they would have to balance the trade-off between the improved academic success (Tangney, et al., 2004) and decreases in a student's well-being. However, our results suggest that self-control interventions could improve not only academic success but also SWB.

Second, as the monotonic trend between self-control and SWB holds in a general adult population (Study 6), it may also be worthwhile for organizational policy makers to look into the development of worker self-control. As self-control tendencies are generally linked to better performance (e.g., Steel, Brothen, & Wambach, 2001), self-control interventions may have the added effect of improving worker SWB.

Third, as self-control is frequently regarded as the “master virtue” that underlies other virtues (Baumeister & Exline, 1999) these findings may generalize to other types of positive character traits and virtues in that their growth and development may be associated with greater SWB with little decrements at high levels. The possibility that increases in specific virtues likewise lead to higher overall happiness is a promising avenue for further work.

Possible Limitations and Future Directions

Although the current undertaking of six studies has been significant, there are also limitations. For one, the current studies do not disentangle the intensity and frequency of the affective components of SWB, which may provide more nuance to the findings. For example, high levels of self-control frequency may not have noticeable downturns in the intensity of positive feelings immediately, but the frequency in which one experiences positive feelings from self-control actions may lessen over time. This may also have a bearing on whether further assessing intensity or frequency components of affective SWB will reveal inverted-U effects. Future research could address this intriguing question, although it is also necessary to determine how best to parse and measure self-control in terms of intensity and frequency in such an effort.

In our studies, we found that while self-reported self-control ratings consistently predicted SWB, teacher-reported self-control ratings were not as consistent. Although an outsider's perspective might provide less biased assessments of self-control, it is also possible that such assessments may be deficient in some respects. This is because successfully exercising self-control may not result in behaviors that are transparent to others, and teacher reports are often informed by failures in self-control. For example, a student may need to successfully exercise self-control several times during class in order to pay prolonged attention. The teacher cannot recognize these successful instances of self-control, but is more likely to take notice when the student fails to pay attention. This limitation can be rectified in future research by utilizing behavioral measures of self-control.

Although our studies focused on SWB, the concept of well-being is multifaceted (Su, Diener, & Tay, 2014). Future research should examine whether the same conclusion holds for other aspects of well-being, such as psychological well-being (Ryff, 1999). In her conceptualization, Ryff proposes six dimensions of psychological well-being (autonomy, self-acceptance, positive relations with others, environmental mastery, purpose in life, and personal growth) which are representative of a fulfilling life. In order to fulfill these needs, individuals will need to exert a significant amount of self-control, potentially to the point where the fulfillment of one need may come at the detriment of another. Furthermore, the self-control tendencies needed to achieve these dimensions may be contradictory with one another. For instance, it may be possible that high levels of self-control tendencies needed to facilitate feelings of mastery or personal accomplishment may sacrifice close social relationships (Letzring, et al., 2005). In this case, there may be some trade-offs between self-control and well-being leading to observable inverted-U effects.

Conclusion

Self-control lies at the center of current public policy debates (Moffitt et al., 2011). There are wide-scale programs being designed to help improve self-control among the masses as it has been shown to lead to several beneficial outcomes (de Ridder et al., 2012). Yet, some remain concerned that too much self-control can have detrimental consequences (Grant & Schwartz, 2011). The present investigation did not find evidence suggesting detrimental consequences with respect to SWB. Instead, the more self-control people have, the happier they will be. There may be no such thing as too much self-control – at least for happiness.

Acknowledgments

Funding: The author(s) disclose receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Preparation of this manuscript was supported by the National Institute on Aging (Grants K01-AG033182-02 and R24- AG048081-01), Character Lab, the Gates Foundation, the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, the Spencer Foundation, the Templeton Foundation, the National Science Foundation (DRL 1235958 and IIS 1523091), and the German Science Foundation (grants HO 4175/3-1 and HO 4175/4-1). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies.

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Contributor Information

Christopher W. Wiese, Purdue University

Louis Tay, Purdue University.

Angela L. Duckworth, University of Pennsylvania

Sidney D'Mello, University of Notre Dame.

Lauren Kuykendall, George Mason University.

Wilhelm Hofmann, University of Cologne.

Roy F. Baumeister, Florida State University

Kathleen D. Vohs, University of Minnesota

References

  1. Abdi H. The Bonferroni and Šidák corrections for multiple comparisons. In: Salkind N, editor. Encycopedia of Measurement and Statistics. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2007. pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bagozzi RP, Baumgartner H, Pieters R. Goal-directed emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 1998;12:1–26. http://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379754. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bainter S, Curran P. Advantages of integrative data analysis for developmental research. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2015;16:1–10. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.871721. http://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2013.871721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barkley RA. The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review. 2001;11:1–29. doi: 10.1023/a:1009085417776. http://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009085417776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67:1–48. http://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [Google Scholar]
  6. Baumeister RF, Exline JJ. Virtue, personality, and social relations: Self-control as the moral muscle. Journal of Personality. 1999;67:1165–1194. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00086. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bembenutty H, Karabenick SA. Academic delay of gratification. Learning and Individual Differences. 1998;10:329–346. http://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80126-5. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brown TA, Chorpita BF, Barlow DH. Structural relationships among dimensions of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders and dimensions of negative affect, positive affect, and autonomic arousal. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107:179–192. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.2.179. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.107.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carter NT, Dalal DK, Boyce AS, O'Connell MS, Kung MC, Delgado KM. Uncovering curvilinear relationships between conscientiousness and job performance: How theoretically appropriate measurement makes an empirical difference. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2014;99:564–586. doi: 10.1037/a0034688. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0034688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carter NT, Guan L, Maples JL, Williamson RL, Miller JD. The downsides of extreme conscientiousness for psychological well-being: The role of obsessive compulsive tendencies. Journal of Personality. 2015:510–522. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12177. http://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12177. [DOI] [PubMed]
  11. Cheung T, Gillebaart M, Kroese F. Why are people with high self-control happier? The effect of trait self-control on happiness as mediated by regulatory focus. Multiple Values Selected. 2014:1–6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00722. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00722/abstract. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  12. Curran P, Hussong A. Integrative data analysis: The simultaneous analysis of multiple data sets. Psychological Methods. 2009;14:81–100. doi: 10.1037/a0015914. http:///doi.org/10.1037/a0015914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. D'Mello SK, Galla BM, Duckworth AL. Domain-specificity of performance measures of academic diligence. Paper to be presented at the 2017 annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association; San Antonio, TX. 2017. [Google Scholar]
  14. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333:959–964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1985;49:71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125:276–302. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276. [Google Scholar]
  17. Diener E, Wirtz D, Biswas-Diener R, Tov W, Kim-Prieto C, Choi DW, Oishi S. Assessing well-being. In: Diener E, editor. Assessing well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener. Vol. 39. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2009. pp. 247–266. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-2354-4. [Google Scholar]
  18. Duckworth AL, Kern ML. A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality. 2011;45:259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Duckworth AL, Seligman MEP. Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science. 2005;16:939–944. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Duckworth AL, Yeager DS. Measurement matters: Assessing personal qualities other than cognitive ability for educational purposes. Educational Researcher. 2015;44:237–251. doi: 10.3102/0013189X15584327. http://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X15584327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Duckworth AL, Grant H, Loew B, Oettingen G, Gollwitzer PM. Self-regulation strategies improve self-discipline in adolescents: benefits of mental contrasting and implementation intentions. Educational Psychology. 2011;31:17–26. http://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2010.506003. [Google Scholar]
  22. Dunbar RIM. The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary Perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology. 2003;32:163–181. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093158. [Google Scholar]
  23. Eisenberg N, Duckworth A, Spinrad T, Valiente C. Conscientiousness: Origins in childhood? Developmental Psychology. 2014;50:1331–1349. doi: 10.1037/a0030977. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0030977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Enders C, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods. 2007;12:121–138. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121. http://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Finkenauer C, Engels R, Baumeister R. Parenting behaviour and adolescent behavioural and emotional problems: The role of self-control. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2005;29:58–69. http://doi.org/10.1080/01650250444000333. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in personality. 2003;37:504–528. [Google Scholar]
  27. Grant AM, Schwartz B. Too much of a good thing: The challenge and opportunity of the inverted U. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6:61–76. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393523. http://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Grinde B. Darwinian happiness: Can the evolutionary perspective on well-being help us improve society? World Futures. 2005;61:317–329. http://doi.org/10.1080/026040290500598. [Google Scholar]
  29. Halse C, Honey A, Boughtwood D. The paradox of virtue: (re)thinking deviance, anorexia and schooling. Gender and Education. 2007;19:219–235. http://doi.org/10.1080/09540250601166068. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hofmann W, Baumeister R, Förster G, Vohs K. Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2012;102:1318–1335. doi: 10.1037/a0026545. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hofmann W, Luhmann M, Fisher RR, Vohs KD, Baumeister RF. Yes, but are they happy? Effects of trait self-control on affective well-being and life satisfaction. Journal of Personality. 2013;82:265–277. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12050. http://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. John OP, Srivastava S. The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In: Pervin LA, John OP, editors. Handbook of personality theory and research. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 102–138. [Google Scholar]
  33. Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade DA, Schwarz N, Stone AA. A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Scientometrics. 2004;306:1776–1780. doi: 10.1126/science.1103572. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Laurent J, Catanzaro SJ, Joiner TE, Jr, Rudolph KD, Potter KI, Lambert S, et al. A measure of positive and negative affect for children: scale development and preliminary validation. Psychological Assessment. 1999;11:326–338. http://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.11.3.326. [Google Scholar]
  35. Letzring TD, Block J, Funder DC. Ego-control and ego-resiliency: Generalization of self-report scales based on personality descriptions from acquaintances, clinicians, and the self. Journal of Research in Personality. 2005;39:395–422. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.06.003. [Google Scholar]
  36. MacLeod AK, Coates E, Hetherton J. Increasing well-being through teaching goal-setting and planning skills: results of a brief intervention. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2007;9:185–196. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-007-9057-2. [Google Scholar]
  37. McGregor I, Little BR. Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: on doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:494–512. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.2.494. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. O'Brien J, Galla B, D'Mello SK, Duckworth A, Yeager D. Direct evidence that “frame of reference bias” can undermine the validity of cross-context comparisons of competency perceptions (in preparation) [Google Scholar]
  39. Ordóñez LD, Schweitzer ME, Galinsky AD, Bazerman MH. Goals gone wild: the systematic side effects of overprescribing goal setting. Academy of Management Perspectives. 2009;23:6–16. http://doi.org/10.5465/AMP.2009.37007999. [Google Scholar]
  40. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. http://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Perneger TV. What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. The BMJ. 1998;316:1236–1238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236. http://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pierce JR, Aguinis H. The too-much-of-a-good-thing effect in management. Journal of Management. 2013;39:313–338. http://doi.org/10.1177/0149206311410060. [Google Scholar]
  43. Pomerantz EM, Saxon JL, Oishi S. The psychological trade-offs of goal investment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:617–630. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.617. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ray JJ, Najman JM. The generalizability of deferment of gratification. The Journal of Social Psychology. 1986;126:117–119. http://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1986.9713578. [Google Scholar]
  46. Roberts JS, Fang HR, Cui W, Wang Y. GGUM2004: A Windows-based program to estimate parameters in the generalized graded unfolding model. Applied Psychological Measurement 2006 [Google Scholar]
  47. Situ QM, Li JB, Dou K. Reexamining the linear and U-shaped relationships between self-control and emotional and behavioural problems. Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 2015 n/a–n/a. http://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12118.
  48. Steel P, Brothen T, Wambach C. Procrastination and personality, performance, and mood. Personality and individual differences. 2001;30:95–106. [Google Scholar]
  49. Su R, Tay L, Diener E. The development and validation of the Comprehensive Inventory of Thriving (CIT) and the Brief Inventory of Thriving (BIT) Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2014;6:251–279. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tay L, Diener E. Needs and subjective well-being around the world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;101:354–365. doi: 10.1037/a0023779. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0023779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tay L, Drasgow F. Theoretical, statistical, and substantive issues in the assessment of construct dimensionality: Accounting for the item response process. Organizational Research Methods. 2012;15:363–384. http://doi.org/10.1177/1094428112439709. [Google Scholar]
  53. Tay L, Drasgow F, Rounds J, Williams B. Fitting measurement models to vocational interest data: Are dominance models ideal? Journal of Applied Psychology. 2009;94:1287–1304. doi: 10.1037/a0015899. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0015899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tay L, Kuykendall L. Why self-reports of happiness and sadness may not necessarily contradict bipolarity: A psychometric review and proposal. Emotion Review. 2016 Advance online publication. http://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916637656.
  55. Tsukayama E, Duckworth AL, Kim B. Domain-specific impulsivity in school-age children. Developmental Science. 2013;16:879–893. doi: 10.1111/desc.12067. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tsukayama E, Toomey SL, Faith MS, Duckworth AL. Self-control as a protective factor against overweight status in the transition from childhood to adolescence. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2010;164:631–635. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.97. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Watson D, Clark LA, Carey G. Positive and negative affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1988;97:346–353. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.97.3.346. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.97.3.346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES