Abstract
A range of species exploit anthropogenic food resources in behaviour known as ‘raiding’. Such behavioural flexibility is considered a central component of a species’ ability to cope with human-induced environmental changes. Here, we study the behavioural processes by which raiding male chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) exploit the opportunities and mitigate the risks presented by raiding in the suburbs of Cape Town, South Africa. Ecological sampling and interviews conducted with ‘rangers’ (employed to manage the baboons’ space use) revealed that baboons are at risk of being herded out of urban spaces that contain high-energy anthropogenic food sources. Baboon-attached motion/GPS tracking collars showed that raiding male baboons spent almost all of their time at the urban edge, engaging in short, high-activity forays into the urban space. Moreover, activity levels were increased where the likelihood of deterrence by rangers was greater. Overall, these raiding baboons display a time-activity balance that is drastically altered in comparison to individuals living in more remote regions. We suggest our methods can be used to obtain precise estimates of management impact for this and other species in conflict with people.
Introduction
Foraging strategies are intimately linked to the energy and time budgets of animal species1. For example, large herbivores such as elephants (Loxondota africana) spend their time using low-cost, low nutrient foraging strategies to meet their energetic requirements2, whilst cursorial predators such as cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) rely on short but intense bursts of energy to catch their high nutrient prey3–5. Between these extremes exists a diversity of foraging strategies that balance the investment in time and energy according to species and context6.
With unprecedented human-induced environmental change7, individuals may be able to modify their foraging behaviour (behavioural plasticity8) within their morphological and physiological constraints, and venture into human-modified environments to exploit high energy food resources9. Such ‘raiding’ behaviours can be particularly important for species with long generation times (because human-modified changes occur faster than evolutionary responses via natural selection10). Examples range from bears exploiting bins in North America11 to elephants consuming crops throughout large regions of Africa and Asia12, resulting in human-wildlife conflicts across the globe.
In the Cape Peninsula, South Africa (Fig. 1A), chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) not only raid farms, homes (Fig. 1B) and commercial properties in search of high-energy human derived foods, but also enter cars and even take food directly from people13,14. The severity of baboon raiding behaviour in the Cape Peninsula threatens human health and safety15,16 and poses significant risks to the baboons, with dozens of human-caused deaths each year16. In an attempt to prevent raiding behaviour, Cape Town’s city council employs a wildlife management company that uses baboon ‘rangers’ that ‘herd’ the baboons away from the urban edge using noise and paintball marker guns17. This management strategy reduces the time baboons spend in human-landscapes16, but urban and farm raiding - particularly by lone adult male baboons - still occurs14, resulting in troops staying within the vicinity of raiding spots18. We investigated the foraging strategies employed by raiding male baboons in Cape Town and estimate three key metrics: food resources available (representing energetic input), activity levels (representing energetic output), and management effort (representing risk). We focus on adult males – the most frequent raiders19 – in order to investigate the strategies by which these primates exploit human dominated landscapes and so expand their ecological niche9,20.
We hypothesised that flexibility in behaviour8 would be central to the baboons’ ability to cope with human-induced environmental changes and management strategies imposed upon them17, and tested a series of inter-connected predictions. First, we predicted that compared to ‘natural’ baboon habitats, urban spaces provide higher energy food rewards (prediction 1). Second, we expected that whilst urban spaces might provide greater rewards, baboons would experience increased risk of being deterred by field rangers17 (prediction 2), and thus engage in riskier and energetically costly activities in order to evade field rangers and navigate the complex urban topography (prediction 3). Consequently, if baboons are to exploit the opportunities presented by the urban space, we predicted they would employ behavioural strategies that are consistent with the baboons minimising costs and maximising rewards (prediction 4), resulting in extreme differences in behaviour when compared to (non-raiding) baboon populations (prediction 5).
Methods
Study subjects
One chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) troop with a long history of urban raiding14 was studied from 03/04/2014 to 21/07/2014 on the Cape Peninsula, South Africa (S -34.0349, E 18.4156). The focal troop comprised 10 adult and 3 sub-adult males, 20 adult and sub-adult females and approximately 30 juveniles. Our period of observation was relatively short but representative of a time of year when raiding was frequent in the population21 and when food availability in the natural environment was high21. We opportunistically collected data (ad libitum) on the occurrence of raids into urban spaces.
Study area and baboon management
The study area was categorised as one of five broad habitat categories (fynbos, trees, meadows, vineyards and urban areas) based on researcher knowledge of the site and images from Google Earth (accessed 06/06/2014). Urban areas included private residential suburbs, vineyards and commercial properties (e.g., restaurants and wine producing plants). Fynbos was the indigenous vegetation within the Table Mountain National Park. The City of Cape Town and a local farm employed baboon ‘rangers’ to actively deter baboons from the vineyards and urban areas. Rangers monitored the troop, following their movements to keep them in sight, and used noise (shouts and whistles), physical presence and paintball guns to deter the baboons17. Two alternating teams of five field rangers managed the baboons on a daily basis from approximately 7am to 5 pm, with each team working four days on, four days off.
Baboon tracking collars
Male baboons are known to be the most frequent raiders19,22, and 8 adult males within our focal troop were cage-trapped following the Baboon Technical Team’s approved protocol23 before being sedated by a certified veterinarian and fitted with a custom-built tracking collar (see supplemental information). Collars weighed less than 2.5% of the body mass of the baboons and were approved for use by Swansea University Ethics Committee (Swansea University IP-1314-5). Two collars failed to record data, decreasing the sample size to 6 individuals (see supplemental information for details of collar failure and subsequent improvements to this collar design).
Collars were equipped with sensors recording GPS every 5 minutes and acceleration in 3 axes at 40 Hz. Acceleration was recorded continuously at 40 Hz for 5 individuals and at 20 Hz for one individual (M2). GPS data were recorded from 7:30 am to 6:00 pm for a mean ± Standard Error (SE) of 10.3 ± 3.0 days, and a mean ± SE of 102 ± 5 GPS fixes per day, for each collared baboon. This resulted in 7,572 GPS fixes in total from which 7,428 fixes were associated with activity (acceleration) data. Any GPS fixes recorded before sunrise or after sunset according to the South African Astronomical Observatory were excluded, resulting in 6,325 fixes. Any pair of successive fixes more than 1 km apart were assumed to be errors, and removed, resulting in 6,274 fixes. The GPS receiver calculates standalone horizontal position to a quoted accuracy of 2.5 m, though, in practice, this depends upon satellites available and how the collar was positioned on the baboon at any time point, as well as the immediate environment surrounding the collared individual. Ad-hoc checks of the data where baboons crossed known landmarks indicate positional accuracy of < 10 m for our GPS data. GPS fixes were converted to Universal Transverse Mercator coordinate system before use in spatial analyses (below). Supplemental Table S1 summarises data collected by each male’s collar.
Risks and returns
A map of the study region was divided into 150 m2 grid cells (n = 200). Due to the presence of a water dam, we removed one 150 m2 cell mostly covered by this feature, bringing the total number of cells to 199. We then estimated the risk of deterrence by rangers and food rewards for each cell.
Risk of deterrence by rangers was estimated by interviews with rangers (n = 11) employed to manage the troop. Interviews were carried out by GF and CK-S following the completion of the baboon data collection (see below) so as to not influence these data. Interviews were anonymous and conducted with the consent of the rangers and their employers. Rangers were provided with a map of the study area (Supplemental Fig. S1) and asked to colour in areas where, in their opinion, the baboons were allowed to be: at any time (green; score 0), some of the time over the course of a day (orange; score 1), or never, being consistently chased away (red; score 2). This provided us with 11 different maps and we used these data to derive a composite map created by summing all field rangers’ scores within each of the 150 m2 grid cells to provide an indication of the likelihood of deterrence. These data have previously been presented by Fehlmann et al.24. Note that using summed scores does not necessarily reflect ‘agreement’ between rangers (which can also impact on the troop’s space use), but agreement is high for urban environments nonetheless24. Further details of how the baboons respond to ranger management at a troop level can be found in Fehlmann et al.24.
To quantify potential energy returns, we surveyed baboons’ diets during focal observations of adult males (30 minute direct focal observations, see ‘Baboon activity’ below). For each food item, we defined the species (for vegetation), the part consumed, and estimated the volume of a baboon bite (a mouthful of this specific item; hereafter referred to as a baboon bite). In order to get an estimation of the weight of each food item consumed per baboon bite, we collected food material mimicking baboons’ foraging behaviour and weighed it. We then used data from the literature to estimate the energy content of one baboon bite based on its weight (Supplemental Table S2). When data on food energy content were not available, we estimated it by averaging the energy composition of similar food items (Supplemental Table S2). This gave us an estimated energy content for any given food item that was independent of absolute size, but meaningful with respect to a foraging baboon.
In order to map these potential energy returns, we surveyed the vegetation in 7.0 ± 3.5 random quadrats of 1 m2 (n = 1906 quadrats) for each of the 150 m2 grid cells. In each quadrat, we reported the presence and absence of each food type and estimated the number of baboon bites. We then averaged the energy per bite of all food items present in each quadrat to provide us with an estimation of the potential energy reward (in kcal) that a baboon would gain per bite of food in a given 150 m2 cell. For cells falling in urban habitats, we didn’t conduct analyses as quadrats but instead assessed the energy availability for each residence within a cell; recording presence or absence of food types. Whilst this approach provides a particularly coarse measure of available energy, we assume that it provides qualitatively accurate results for the broad scale habitat-level differences we investigate here.
Baboon activity
To estimate baboon activity levels we used the Vector of the Dynamic Body Acceleration (VeDBA) calculated from acceleration data25. Raw acceleration data were first decomposed into static and dynamic components, using a running mean of two seconds to estimate the static acceleration26. Static acceleration provides information on the orientation of the device with respect to gravity, and the dynamic component reflects how much the animal is moving in each of the three dimensions (anterior-posterior, lateral and vertical). VeDBA is the square root of the sum of the squared dynamic acceleration values (measured in each of the three axes; x, y, z):
VeDBA therefore combines the dynamic part of the acceleration signal across three axes, providing a single measure of body motion and hence activity. We took the mean VeDBA around each GPS fix to be representative of the average level of activity at a given GPS location. A sensitivity test was conducted to assess the effect of changing the period over which acceleration data were averaged around GPS points. This showed a plateau at 60 s (Supplemental Fig. S2). We therefore computed the mean average VeDBA for 30 seconds before and after each GPS fix, and this information was used as an indication of the intensity of activity being performed by a collared baboon at any recorded time point.
To compare focal baboon activity budgets to those published in the literature, we conducted 30 minute direct focal observations of each male (n = 311; mean ± sd per baboon = 51.8 ± 0.7). At each minute we recorded the baboons’ instantaneous behaviour (classified as resting, grooming, foraging or travelling). Focal identity and times were randomly selected over the study period.
Statistical tests
All statistical analyses were conducted in R version 3.1.127 with α set at 0.05 and are reported in the supplementary “Source code”. Data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository at http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.10b93. To test for differences in energy availability and VeDBA according to habitat type (prediction 1), we conducted a Kruskal-Wallis test. To investigate relationships between the risk of deterrence by rangers and habitat type (prediction 2), we conducted Mantel tests whilst controlling for spatial autocorrelation (R package ‘vegan’28). We used a Kruskal Wallis test to see whether VeDBA (activity levels) changed according to habitat type and used a Linear Mixed Model, LMM (R package nlme29), to investigate if VeDBA changed as a function of risk of deterrence by rangers (prediction 3). For our LMM we log-transformed our response variable (VeDBA) to meet model assumptions and included management strategy score (0–22) as a fixed effect, whilst also including an exponential spatial correlation structure by including a nugget effect in the model. We fitted baboon identity as a random intercept to allow individuals to differ in their baseline VeDBA (this improved model fit according to AIC criterion (R package nlme29)). Homogeneity and homoscedasticity of residuals were checked to ensure model assumptions were met. To investigate whether baboons were minimising costs and maximising rewards (prediction 4), we calculated time spent in different habitat types and computed the distance of each GPS fix to the nearest urban edge (R package ‘spdep’30,31). To compare focal baboon activity budgets to those published in the literature (prediction 5), a Kruskal-Wallis test was used.
Results
Rewards
Estimated food resource quality differed by habitat type (Kruskal Wallis: X2 = 51.7, df = 4, p < 0.001, n = 199, Fig. 1C,D), with 90% of the food resources sampled in the urban space yielding a potential energy intake ~10 times greater per bite than the natural environment (Fig. 2), in support of our first prediction.
Risks
Rangers were more likely to deter baboons in urban spaces (Partial Mantel test: management strategy vs. urbanisation, R = 0.66, p < 0.001, n = 199, Fig. 1C,E), which we assumed to translate to increased risk, supporting our second prediction. Baboons also showed significantly higher activity levels (VeDBA) in urban locations (Kruskal Wallis: VeDBA vs. habitat type, X2 = 576.34, df = 4, p < 0.001, n = 6274; Fig. 1F) and specifically where ranger management strategy was reported to be strictest (Linear Mixed Model, estimate = −0.077, t-value = −7.91, p-value < 0.001, n = 6274, Fig. 3). This contributed to VeDBA scores that were 4 times greater in urban habitats than in other habitats. Management effort also appeared to be 27 times greater in this environment (Fig. 1G), making urban spaces more costly for baboons to utilise than other habitats in their range (Fig. 1G), supporting our third prediction.
Baboon raiding strategy
We recorded 105 raids in 109 days during the study period (Supplemental Table 3) and identified a further 49 raids from collar data (between 5 and 17 urban incursions per baboon) over 35 days (60 days of GPS recordings across all males). Despite the high number of raids, the collared baboons spent just 1.8 ± 0.7% (mean ± SE; range: 0.5 to 5.2%) of their time in the urban space, but stayed in close vicinity (median [1st and 3rd quartile]; 300 m [149 m, 501 m] to urban space, Fig. 4A), mainly engaging in low-activity behaviours (mean ± sd VeDBA = 0.08 ± 0.09) before engaging in brief (mean ± SE = 11.0 ± 1.5 minutes), high-activity (mean ± sd VeDBA = 0.19 ± 0.11) forays into the urban space to raid (Fig. 4B).
The collared males spent just 9.5% ± 0.8% of their overall time budget foraging (Fig. 4C). This was significantly less than baboon troops elsewhere in Africa (prediction 5), which spend between 20.3% and 59.3% of their time feeding13,19,32 (Kruskal-Wallis rank test, X2 = 13.5, p < 0.001, Fig. 2C).
Discussion
Throughout baboon species’ distribution, raiding individuals have shorter daily path lengths18,33, smaller home ranges18, invest less time in foraging19, and have greater reproductive success19 than non-raiding baboons. The apparent success of this strategy suggests there are substantial benefits to raiding despite significant associated risks16,34,35. However, no study has yet examined the behavioural processes by which raiding baboons (or any other raiding animal) exploit the opportunities and mitigate the risks to survive and thrive in a human-changed environment. Our study of raiding male baboons in Cape Town, which are subjected to intense management efforts to reduce conflict with people, has provided an estimation of the risks and rewards associated with baboon raiding.
We found that urban spaces afford access to high energy food resources, but these resources occur in areas where field rangers are likely to deter baboons. We expect that this threat of deterrence, in conjunction with complex urban topography, contributes to the higher baboon activity levels recorded in urban spaces when compared to other habitats in their range. Given that there is also potential for injury or death as a result of human-baboon conflicts15,16 (one study animal was killed by a dog while raiding during the study period), we suggest that the risks associated with raiding are high.
We have previously shown that the area bordering the urban space is (i) where inter-individual variation in field ranger management strategy is highest (i.e. sometimes they deter, sometimes they do not) and (ii) close to refuges24. Thus, baboons stay at the urban edge, engaging in low-activity behaviours near refuges, where risks of herding are variable24, and then engage in brief, high-activity forays into the urban space to raid when the opportunities arise. This last point is important; whilst baboons are more likely to be herded away from certain areas within their range, inconsistency in ranger responses24 and the urban topography mean that “once in a while” baboons get through, or are let through, to the urban space to raid (spending 1.8% of their time in urban space). Getting into the urban space to raid, even infrequently, is incentive enough to persist with raiding attempts: the baboons spend almost all of their time close to the urban space (the majority of GPS points are located within 500 m of the urban edge) and take opportunities to exploit urban resources, with 49 raids in 35 days.
Based on our findings, the management strategies in the study period appeared to be contributing to increased costs of moving through urban spaces. However, baboon activity levels in urban spaces, whilst high, were not maximal, as VeDBA values recorded for running were 1.6 times greater than the maximum VeDBA recorded for urban areas (Supplemental Fig. S3). Furthermore, even if ranger effort to deter baboons was theoretically 27 times higher in the urban areas (Fig. 3), this only translated to a four-fold increase in the raiding costs experienced by baboons. Whilst this may have been due to an exaggeration of real effort by baboon field rangers during the interviews, it is likely that rangers could not increase their effort in urban areas sufficiently, since baboons were not operating at maximum activity when raiding.
Future research and management decisions should consider complementary or additive approaches to keep the baboons out of urban areas. Bins were the most raided item/location during this study (Supplemental Table S3) and so more time and resources could be devoted to ensuring these are managed more effectively, as suggested by Kaplan36. Improved waste management is frequently used in countries where human-wildlife conflicts exist with particularly dangerous species such as bears37,38 and has proven important in mitigating conflict in urban areas39. There is also potential for decreasing baboon raiding efficiency through increasing the travel time to urban areas40. This could be achieved by either building baboon proof fences in judicious places, which would require the baboon to circumnavigate the obstacle, or by implementing a no-go buffer zone around the urban edge, preventing baboons from adopting a sit, wait, and observe strategy24. Similarly, technological development of ‘virtual fences’ where sounds are played via loud-speakers when baboons approach particular locations, currently being tested in Cape Town41, may provide a novel way of introducing such a buffer zone, but the impacts on other wildlife and reduction in access to natural land for baboons need to be determined first42,43.
Given that our sample is restricted to adult males which are habitual raiders, it would be worthwhile investigating the effect of management strategies according to baboon age-sex classes or even personality types10,44,45. This may help explain some of the underlying mechanisms linked to the consistent inter-individual differences in raiding propensities according to sex19,22,46, or even between individuals of the same sex22, for a variety of species in conflict with humans. This approach could be particularly important since association networks are known to influence crop-raiding behavior in male African elephants22, and high ranking adult male baboons (but not low ranked males) can influence the foraging decisions and space use of the entire group47–49. Therefore, where certain individuals have a disproportionate influence within their social units (and play “keystone” roles50), it could be more efficient to attempt to manage these individuals.
In summary, we have used data acquired from raiding male baboons fitted with high-resolution (GPS and motion sensor) tracking collars to show that exploitation of the human-modified environment is costly. We found baboons engaged in brief, high-activity forays into the urban space, where high energy resources are located. Consequently, the raiding male baboons in the Cape Peninsula exhibit a time-activity balance that is drastically altered in comparison to individuals in populations living in more remote regions18,19,32; switching from a time-expensive low-risk foraging strategy, to brief and risky high-activity forays into the urban space. Our results also present unequivocal evidence of an extreme behavioural flexibility. Such behavioural flexibility has long been considered a central component of a species ability to cope with human-induced environmental changes10, but explicit quantification of its occurrence and the associated trade-offs has been lacking in wild animal populations.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We thank the Baboon Technical Team for authorisation to conduct our research on Cape Peninsula baboons. We are grateful to Mark Holton and Phil Hopkins at Swansea University and Graeme McPhillips at University College London for their assistance in the design and manufacture of the collars. For their cooperation and assistance in the field, we thank vets Hamish Currie and Dorothy Breed, Ines Fürtbauer, Human Wildlife Solutions and their rangers who participated in the interviews, and owners and staff of the two Farms on which the baboons ranged. We also thank Jack Beresford and Charlotte Rees-Roderick for their help conducting the vegetation surveys and Terry Trinder-Smith for identifying vegetation samples. This work was supported by grants (awarded to A.J.K.) from Swansea University’s College of Science, the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour (A.S.A.B.), British Council Researcher Links, and the Natural Environment Research Council (NE/H016600/3), in addition to NRF incentive funding provided to JO’R. G.F. was supported by a Swansea University PhD Scholarship. Thank you to two anonymous Referees and Handling Editor Andrew MacIntosh for their comments and suggestions which greatly improved this manuscript.
Author Contributions
A.J.K. and G.F. conceived the study and designed the experimental protocols with input from C.K.-S. G.F. led the fieldwork and performed analyses with support from A.J.K., E.L.C.S. and A.L. G.F., C.K.-S. collected data, S.H. provided technical support, and JO’R supported fieldwork. All authors discussed and interpreted the data. G.F. and A.J.K. wrote the manuscript with input from all co-authors.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-017-14871-2.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Huey RB, Pianka ER. Ecological consequences of foraging mode. Ecology. 1981;62:991–999. doi: 10.2307/1936998. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wall J, Douglas-Hamilton I, Vollrath F. Elephants avoid costly mountaineering. Curr. Biol. 2006;16:R527–R529. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wilson AM, et al. Locomotion dynamics of hunting in wild cheetahs. Nature. 2013;498:185–189. doi: 10.1038/nature12295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Williams TM, et al. Instantaneous energetics of puma kills reveal advantage of felid sneak attacks. Science. 2014;346:81–85. doi: 10.1126/science.1254885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gorman ML, Mills MG, Raath JP, Speakman JR. High hunting costs make African wild dogs vulnerable to kleptoparasitism by hyaenas. Nature. 1998;391:479–481. doi: 10.1038/35131. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stephens, D. W. & Krebs, J. R. Foraging theory: monographs in behavior and ecology. Foraging Theory: Monographs in Behavior and Ecology (1986).
- 7.Vitousek PM, Mooney HA, Lubchenco J, Melillo JM. Human domination of Earth’s ecosystems. Science. 1997;277:494–499. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5325.494. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Snell-Rood EC. An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural plasticity. Anim. Behav. 2013;85:1004–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sol D, Lapiedra O, González-Lagos C. Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Anim. Behav. 2013;85:1101–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sih A. Understanding variation in behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change: a conceptual overview. Anim. Behav. 2013;85:1077–1088. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mazur R, Seher V. Socially learned foraging behaviour in wild black bears, Ursus americanus. Anim. Behav. 2008;75:1503–1508. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.10.027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Webber CE, Sereivathana T, Maltby MP, Lee PC. Elephant crop-raiding and human-elephant conflict in Cambodia: Crop selection and seasonal timings of raids. ORYX. 2011;45:243–251. doi: 10.1017/S0030605310000335. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hoffman TS, O’Riain MJ. The spatial ecology of chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) in a human-modified environment. Int. J. Primatol. 2010;32:308–328. doi: 10.1007/s10764-010-9467-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Richardson, P. HWS monthly report. Available at: www.capenature.co.za/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/Baboon-Management-June-2014-HWS-Report.pdf (Accessed: 2nd January 2015) (2014).
- 15.Drewe JA, O’Riain MJ, Beamish E, Currie H, Parsons S. Survey of infections transmissible between baboons and humans, Cape Town, South Africa. Emerging Infect. Dis. 2012;18:298–301. doi: 10.3201/eid1802.111309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Beamish EK, O’Riain MJ. The effects of permanent injury on the behavior and diet of commensal chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) in the Cape Peninsula, South Africa. Int. J. Primatol. 2014;35:1004–1020. doi: 10.1007/s10764-014-9779-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cape Nature. Protocol and Standard Operational Procedure (SOP) for using paintball markers as baboon deterrents within the Cape Peninsula (2012).
- 18.Hoffman TS, O’Riain MJ. Troop size and human-modified habitat affect the ranging patterns of a chacma baboon population in the cape peninsula, South Africa. Am. J. Primatol. 2012;74:853–863. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Strum SC. The development of primate raiding: implications for management and conservation. Int. J. Primatol. 2010;31:133–156. doi: 10.1007/s10764-009-9387-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Layman CA, Quattrochi JP, Peyer CM, Allgeier JE. Niche width collapse in a resilient top predator following ecosystem fragmentation. Ecol. Lett. 2007;10:937–944. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2007.01087.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.van Doorn Ac, O’Riain M. j. & Swedell, L. The effects of extreme seasonality of climate and day length on the activity budget and diet of semi-commensal chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) in the Cape Peninsula of South Africa. Am. J. Primatol. 2010;72:104–112. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chiyo PI, Moss CJ, Alberts SC. The influence of life history milestones and association networks on crop-raiding behavior in male African elephants. PLoS One. 2012;7:e31382. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hoffman, T. S. The spatial ecology of chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) in the Cape Peninsula, South Africa: Towards improved management and conservation strategies. (University of Cape Town, 2011).
- 24.Fehlmann G, O’Riain MJ, Kerr-Smith C, King AJ. Adaptive space use by baboons (Papio ursinus) in response to management interventions in a human-changed landscape. Anim. Conserv. 2017;20:101–109. doi: 10.1111/acv.12293. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Qasem L, et al. Tri-axial dynamic acceleration as a proxy for animal energy expenditure; Should we be summing values or calculating the vector? PLoS One. 2012;7:e31187. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Shepard ELC, et al. Derivation of body motion via appropriate smoothing of acceleration data. Aquat. Biol. 2009;4:235–241. doi: 10.3354/ab00104. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2014).
- 28.Oksanen, J. et al. Vegan: Community Ecology Package. (2014).
- 29.Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D. & R Core Team. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. (2017).
- 30.Bivand R, Hauke J, Kossowski T. Computing the Jacobian in Gaussian spatial autoregressive models: An illustrated comparison of available methods. Geogr. Anal. 2013;45:150–179. doi: 10.1111/gean.12008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bivand R, Piras G. Comparing Implementations of Estimation Methods for Spatial Econometrics. J Stat. Softw. 2015;63:1–36. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Altmann J, Muruthi P. Differences in daily life between semiprovisioned and wild-feeding baboons. Am. J. Primatol. 1988;15:213–221. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350150304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Johnson, C., Piel, A. K., Forman, D., Stewart, F. A. & King, A. J. The ecological determinants of baboon troop movements at local and continental scales. Mov. Ecol. 3 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 34.Campbell-Smith G, Simanjorang HVP, Leader-Williams N, Linkie M. Local attitudes and perceptions toward crop-raiding by orangutans (Pongo abelii) and other nonhuman primates in Northern Sumatra, Indonesia. Am. J. Primatol. 2010;72:866–876. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Murray MH, Clair CCS. Individual flexibility in nocturnal activity reduces risk of road mortality for an urban carnivore. Behav. Ecol. 2015;26:1520–1527. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kaplan, B. In pursuit of a panacea: mitigating human-baboon conflict in the Cape Peninsula, South Africa. (Ph. D. Thesis, Department of Zoology, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, 2013).
- 37.Lewis DL, et al. Foraging ecology of black bears in urban environments: guidance for human-bear conflict mitigation. Ecosphere. 2015;6:1–18. doi: 10.1890/ES15-00137.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Merkle JA, Krausman PR, Booth MM. Behavioral and attitudinal change of residents exposed to human–bear interactions. Ursus. 2011;22:74–83. doi: 10.2192/URSUS-D-10-00010.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hopkins JB, Koch PL, Ferguson JM, Kalinowski ST. The changing anthropogenic diets of American black bears over the past century in Yosemite National Park. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2014;12:107–114. doi: 10.1890/130276. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Charnov EL. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor. Popul. Biol. 1976;9:129–136. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Richardson, P. et al. Remotely operated virtual fences: a successful new approach to baboon management. hwsolutions.co.za Available at: http://hwsolutions.co.za/wp-content/uploads/2016/11/2016_11_02-Virtual-Fence-PO_CC-V9.pdf (Accessed: 23rd February 201 7) (2016).
- 42.Freilich JE, Emlen JM, Duda JJ, Freeman DC, Cafaro PJ. Ecological effects of ranching: A six-point critique. BioScience. 2003;53:759–765. doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2003)053[0759:EEORAS]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Holderegger R, Di Giulio M. The genetic effects of roads: A review of empirical evidence. Basic Appl. Ecol. 2010;11:522–531. doi: 10.1016/j.baae.2010.06.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Merrick MJ, Koprowski JL. Should we consider individual behavior differences in applied wildlife conservation studies? Biol. Conserv. 2017;209:34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2017.01.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Seyfarth RM, Silk JB, Cheney DL. Variation in personality and fitness in wild female baboons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:16980–16985. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210780109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Merkle JA, Robinson HS, Krausman PR, Alaback P. Food availability and foraging near human developments by black bears. J. Mammal. 2013;94:378–385. doi: 10.1644/12-MAMM-A-002.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kaplan BS, O’Riain MJ, Eeden Rvan, King AJ. A low-cost manipulation of food resources reduces spatial overlap between baboons (Papio ursinus) and humans in conflict. Int. J. Primatol. 2011;32:1397–1412. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9541-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.King AJ, Douglas CMS, Huchard E, Isaac NJB, Cowlishaw G. Dominance and affiliation mediate despotism in a social primate. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:1833–1838. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.King AJ, Sueur C. Where next? Group coordination and collective decision making by primates. Int. J. Primatol. 2011;32:1245–1267. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9526-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Modlmeier AP, Keiser CN, Watters JV, Sih A, Pruitt JN. The keystone individual concept: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Anim. Behav. 2014;89:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.