Abstract
Much of the research on the relationships between religious participation and health comes from the United States. Studies in other geographic regions or cultural contexts is more sparse. Evidence presented by Ahrenfelt et al., and that from other research studies, is reviewed concerning the associations between religion and health within Europe and world-wide. The evidence within Europe suggests protective associations between various forms of religious participation and lower depression, lower mortality, and better self-rated health. Methodological challenges in such research are reviewed, and discussion is given as to whether a person-culture-fit explanation suffices to account for the existing data and to what other mechanisms might be operative.
Introduction
Epidemiologic research on religion and health has reached a certain state of maturity [1–3]. The evidence base on the topic has grown considerably, the study designs and methodology in this area of research have notably improved, and the number of religious-spiritual exposures that have been examined and the variety of health and well-being outcomes for which rigorous evidence is now available has dramatically increased [3–5]. However, the current research literature is still subject to a number of limitations. One of these limitations is that the vast majority of the research has been carried out in the United States. Much less is known about its applicability in other countries, cultures, or contexts. Do the associations persist in more secular countries? Do the associations vary across religious groups? Much less evidence is available on these questions. The paper by Ahrenfeldt et al. [6] makes a considerable contribution to the literature in this regard by examining some of these associations, using rigorous designs and methodology, in various countries in Europe. The paper does not contribute much to our understanding of these associations for non-Christian religious groups, but it does at least expand the geographic scope of large rigorous research studies for a number of exposures and outcomes to include Europe.
Religion and Health in Europe
The Ahrenfeldt et al. paper [6] has a number of strengths. Most religion-health analyses have been with data from the United States; theirs is conducted with data from Europe. Most religion-health analyses are cross-sectional [1]; Ahrenfeldt et al. have 5 waves of longitudinal data. Most religion-health analyses that do have longitudinal data only have measures of service attendance; their study has several measures including prayer, and having received a religious education, in addition to standard measures on participation in religious organizations. In addition to examining each of these exposures separately they also examined two distinct patterns of religiosity: first, those who were religiously educated, still participated in a religious organization, and also prayed (what they called the “more religious”) and second, those who prayed only and neither participated in religious organizations nor were religiously educated (the “less religious”); they compared both of these groups to the non-religious. They hypothesized that for the former group, religious participation would have protective associations with health; but with the latter group they hypothesized adverse associations with health, with prayer among the “less religious” arising, perhaps at least sometimes, from a state of crisis. Ahrenfeldt et al. also examined a number of outcomes, including limitations in activities of daily living, self-rated health, long-term pain or health problems, and depression. Their study was further strengthened by the use of multiple waves of data and therefore the ability to control for baseline outcomes in examining associations between religion and health to attempt to rule out reverse causation e.g. that only those who are healthy are able to attend religious services [5]. By making use of the multiple waves of longitudinal data, examining associations with a number of mental and physical health outcomes, and using a variety of religiosity measures, the paper makes a notable contribution to the literature.
Many of the patterns that have been uncovered in prior research are manifest here also. Specifically, first, of the three religiosity measures examined separately, participation in a religious organization (religious service attendance) had the most consistent associations with health. In their primary analyses (Table 2, Model 3), participation in religious organizations had point estimates for all associations in a protective direction, with more substantial evidence specifically for protective associations on for the global activity limitation index (OR=0.86, 95%CI:0.75,0.98) and on depression (OR=0.80, 95%CI:0.69,0.93). Second, there was little evidence that prayer, considered on its own, was associated with the various health outcomes, which likewise matches prior literature suggesting much weaker or null associations between private practice religious-spiritual measures and health [7,8].
However, in addition to confirming, within Europe, some of the same patterns of association found in the United States, the paper also made a number of further novel contributions. Ahrenfeldt et al. examined associations with having received a religious education, an exposure that has been relatively unexamined in prior research, and found this was longitudinally associated with a lower risk of poor self rated health (OR=0.81, 95%CI:0.70,0.93) and a lower risk of long-term health problems (OR=0.84, 95%CI:0.74,0.95). Ahrenfeldt et al. furthermore looked at patterns of more integrated religiosity with consistent organizational religious involvement, and religious education, and prayer (the “more religious”) and found that this group, compared to others, longitudinally had fewer activities of daily living (ADL) limitations (OR=0.76, 95%CI:0.58,0.99) and lower risk of depression (OR=0.77, 95%CI:0.64,0.92). Ahrenfeldt et al. further hypothesized that those who only prayed, without religious organizational involvement or religious education (a pattern they referred to sometimes as “less religious” and sometimes as “crisis religiosity”), would subsequently have worse outcomes and indeed found some evidence of this at least for depression (OR=1.46, 95%CI:1.15,1.86). An interesting open question, commented upon, but left unresolved by their data, was whether such “crisis religiosity” is simply a form of reverse causation with those in worse health or more problematic settings more likely to pray, or whether there might be mechanisms whereby prayer itself, detached from organizational involvement or religious education, might in fact itself causally lead to worse outcomes. Such poorer outcomes could conceivably occur, as a causal consequence of such prayer if, for example, such prayer led to increased rumination, or unjustified hope for relief, or perhaps avoiding medical care. Further research on such questions would be of interest.
Methodological Challenges
The Ahrenfeldt et al. study attempted to address a number of challenging methodological problems. One of the strengths of the study was their use of multiple waves of data. This allowed for adjustment of baseline outcomes in order to try to rule out reverse causation [5]. This is especially important in research examining religion and health since it may be the case that only those who are relatively healthy can attend services. Indeed, there is evidence from longitudinal studies that those who become depressed are subsequently more likely to stop attending services [9–11], and likewise evidence that those with new physical disabilities are at least in the short run less likely to attend [12]. This is problematic for research on religion and health, because, without control for baseline outcomes, one might find a protective association simply because those who are unhealthy cease attending. Longitudinal data is needed to try to rule out this possibility and it is precisely that which Ahrenfeldt et al. [6] attempted to do by using multiple waves of data.
Another methodological challenge encountered by Ahrenfeldt et al. concerns appropriate statistical adjustment for multiple testing. Their study considered a number of different outcomes including activity limitations (three different assessments), self-rated health, long-term pain or health problems, and depression; their study also examined a number of different religious exposures. Considered separately, a number of their estimates suggested evidence for an effect of religious participation on various health outcomes, as judged by a 95% confidence interval for this estimate bounded some way away from the null. However, after correction for multiple testing, these associations did not, in general, pass a p=0.05 threshold. That threshold itself is of course somewhat arbitrary [13–16], but does at least indicate some of the difficulties of assessing evidence under multiple testing. The standard Bonferroni correction is often very conservative, and Ahrenfeldt et al. here instead used the somewhat preferable Holm-Bonferroni method, but still the associations did not generally pass this threshold. Often, in practice, associations are evaluated with a single exposure and single outcome, one at a time per paper, and no adjustment is made, so effectively studies that examine multiple outcomes and/or exposures are essentially penalized in comparison to what emerges in the research literature when such associations, examined one at a time, are evaluated.
Better methods and metrics are needed for handling multiple testing in the presence of correlated outcomes. In Table 2, Ahrenfeldt et al.’s estimates that for the four comparisons for which they hypothesize a protective association (religious organization, religious education, more religious vs. others, and more religious vs. less religious), across the six outcomes, 22 of the 24 associations are in a protective direction, 1 null, and one in a slightly adverse (OR=1.01) direction. While many of the confidence intervals themselves contain the null, the confidence intervals themselves and the multiple testing procedures do not pick up on this sort of nuance. Again, the development of better procedures for evaluating associations with multiple comparisons with numerous correlated outcomes, and/or exposures, would be an important development. Evaluating numerous outcomes at once, as in the Ahrenfeldt et al. paper [6], is both a more efficient use of research resources, and is better able to assess the public health importance of specific exposures since their effects on numerous outcomes are considered simultaneously. Such “outcome-wide epidemiology” [17] may often be preferable to current standard practices of the slow accumulation of evidence from single exposure-outcome relationships. But if epidemiology moves in the direction of such outcome-wide analyses, better approaches to handle multiple testing problems will be needed in the evaluation of evidence.
Ahrenfeldt et al. used individual level longitudinal data in their analyses. Such data is strongly preferable to cross-sectional data for the reasons discussed above. Moreover, such analyses should be sharply distinguished from analyses using group-averaged data, as it is then impossible to control for individual-level confounding, and in the absence of individual level data, even control for country-level variables is not sufficient to adjust for bias for country-level confounding of those variables [18,19]. Such methodological problems are sometimes collectively referred to as the problem of the “ecologic fallacy” – one cannot draw conclusions about causation on individual level outcomes with group-averaged data. Unfortunately, use of such group-averaged data is also common in the religion and health literature. This is problematic. For example, although religious service attendance tends to be associated with greater life satisfaction at the individual level across number cultural contexts [1,3,20,21], there are some reports that more religious countries have lower average levels of life satisfaction [20,22,23]. However, these latter analyses are almost certainly confounded by a country’s economic development [20]. Indeed a study by Diener et al. [20] that uses both individual and societal religiosity, and controls for individual and societal difficult circumstances, suggests that both individual and societal religiosity are associated with greater subjective well-being, though the analysis is still with cross-sectional, not longitudinal, data. In any case, data (ideally longitudinal) on both individual service attendance as well as country aggregates of individual attendance or religiosity, are needed to examine the contextual effects of country-wide religious service attendance on health.
Broader Cultural Contexts
One outcome that Ahrenfeldt et al. did not examine, apparently because of unavailability of linked data across countries, was mortality. With research in the United States, this is arguably the outcome for which the evidence is most substantial [1–3,7,24–27]. However, even in Europe, at least two moderately large longitudinal mortality studies with religious service attendance as the exposure have been carried out, suggesting a protective association between service attendance and mortality in Finland [28] and in Denmark [29], two countries that are relatively low in religiosity [20]. Had mortality data been available to Ahrenfeldt et al. in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe, this could have expanded the evidence base considerably, given the nine countries included in the study. Mortality is also one outcome for which longitudinal associations with service attendance have been found for other non-Christian religious groups, including predominantly Jewish [30] and Taoist/Buddhist [31] populations.
The number of countries included in the study also allowed Ahrenfeldt et al. to examine associations by region of Europe which they categorized as Northern (Denmark and Sweden), Southern (Italy and Spain) and Western (Austria, Belgium, Germany, Switzerland, Netherlands). There has been some indication in the research literature on religion and health, at least from mortality studies [32] that protective associations between religion and health may be more substantial in regions that are more religious. One might then expect the association to be weakest or least protective in Northern Europe, but from the interaction analyses in Table 3 from their paper, it is not at all clear that this is so. Of course, such an analysis is also potentially cofounded by affiliation, with Northern Europe predominantly Protestant, Southern Europe predominantly Catholic and Western Europe mixed. This issue may, however, not be especially problematic insofar as there is relatively little evidence that associations between religious service attendance and health vary by Catholic versus Protestant affiliation, except for the outcome of suicide [33–35]. In any case, it does not seem that the analyses of Ahrenfeldt et al. add much evidence either for or against the hypothesis that associations with health are stronger in areas that are themselves more religious. It may also be that limited sample size is an issue here with the need for much larger samples, and a greater number of countries, to more conclusively examine evidence for the hypothesis.
If we expand the perspective from Europe to other regions of the world as well, the existing analyses with individual do indeed suggest some evidence that the associations between religion and health may be stronger in more religious regions [32,36]. Such evidence comes from cross-cultural analyses with individual level cross-sectional data on self-rated health [32,36]; similar associations with cross-sectional data have been reported for subjective well-being [20]. Unfortunately, longitudinal data suggesting somewhat similar patterns for mortality is only available in the United States [26].
Person-Culture Fit?
Of course, even if the hypothesis that the associations are more protective in those regions which are more religious holds true, the fact that the association is in a protective direction at all still requires some explanation. It is sometimes postulated that if the protective associations are greater in those regions with greater levels of religious service attendance then the protective associations may entirely arise from person-culture fit [32]. Very likely person-culture-fit does play some role in the protective associations, but the culture-fit hypothesis only explains why the associations are even more protective in some regions than others. It does not explain why the associations are almost universally protective to begin with. One study [32] examining 59 countries, albeit with cross-sectional data on self-rated health, found that, after covariate control, there was evidence for an adverse association between self-rated health and religiosity in only two countries (Albania and Maldova), whereas there was evidence for a protective associations in 20 countries. Again, one must be cautious here because of the use of cross-sectional data, but the consistency itself of this, and, more importantly, of the many individual-level longitudinal mortality studies [1–3,7,24–30] needs to be explained. Person-culture-fit may eventually help explain the varying magnitude of the association, but it is not sufficient to explain the direction. As a contrast, we would not expect an exposure like smoking to become protective in areas in which smoking is particularly prevalent because of person-culture-fit, and we certainly would never think it to be protective nearly universally. Again, the direction and consistency of the protective association between service attendance and health itself needs to be explained.
Other Mechanisms for Religion and Health
A number of explanations have been offered for the associations between religious participation and health. For mortality, evidence from longitudinal mediation analysis [37] suggests that better social support, reduced smoking, greater optimism, and less depression may all be important mechanisms [27]. Other proposed mechanisms have included greater meaning and purpose in life and greater self-control [1,3]. While social support does seem to explain some of the associations between attendance and mortality, it does not seem to explain the majority of the effect [3,25,27]. Again other mechanisms seem to be present. Current evidence from longitudinal studies suggests that service attendance is subsequently associated not only with all-cause mortality but also with less smoking, alcohol abuse, and drug use [1,38]; less depression and lower suicide rates [1,3,10,34,35]; greater meaning and purpose [39]; great optimism [27]; greater happiness and life-satisfaction [21]; lower crime [40,41]; greater social support and greater likelihood of developing a more extensive social and friendship network [21,25,27] as well as lower likelihood of divorce [25,42,43]. The mechanisms by which religious participation affects health may thus be numerous and quite diverse, and current evidence thus perhaps suggests that it may be the small contribution of many different pathways, rather than the substantial contribution of any specific one, that supplies religious service attendance with its powerful effects on health. Religious community thus appears to be a major pathway to human well-being [44]. The study of Ahrenfeldt et al. provides evidence that this is so not only in the United States, but also, for some outcomes at least, in Europe also. How these associations play out in populations that are not predominantly Christian and in cultural contexts beyond Europe and the Western world remains an important open question. The study of Ahrenfeldt et al. has contributed substantially to the research literature for Europe; much more in other contexts remains to be done.
Acknowledgments
Funding: John Templeton Foundation Grant 52125 and National Institutes of Health Grant R56 ES017876.
References
- 1.Koenig HG, King DE, Carson VB. Handbook of Religion and Health. 2. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Idler EL, editor. Religion as a Social Determinant of Public Health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 3.VanderWeele TJ. Religion and health: a synthesis. In: Peteet JR, Balboni MJ, editors. Spirituality and Religion within the Culture of Medicine: From Evidence to Practice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2017. pp. 357–401. [Google Scholar]
- 4.VanderWeele TJ. Religious communities and human flourishing. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2017 doi: 10.1177/0963721417721526. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.VanderWeele TJ, Jackson JW, Li S. Causal inference and time-varying exposures: A case study of religion and mental health. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2016;51:1457–1466. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1281-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ahrenfeldt LJ, Möller S, Andersen-Ranberg K, Vitved AR, Lindahl-Jacobsen R, Hvidt NC. Religiousness and health in Europe. European Journal of Epidemiology. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0296-1. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Musick MA, House JS, Williams DR. Attendance at religious services and mortality in a national sample. J Health Soc Behav. 2004;45(2):198–213. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.VanderWeele TJ, Yu J, Cozier YC, Wise L, Argentieri MA, Rosenberg L, Palmer JR, Shields AE. Religious service attendance, prayer, religious coping, and religious-spiritual identity as predictors of all-cause mortality in the Black Womens Health Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2017;185:515–522. doi: 10.1093/aje/kww179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Maselko J, Hayward RD, Hanlon A, Buka S, Meador K. Religious service attendance and major depression: a case of reverse causality? Am J Epidemiol. 2012;175(6):576–83. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwr349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Li S, Okereke OI, Chang SC, Kawachi I, VanderWeele TJ. Religious service attendance and depression among women - a prospective cohort study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2016;50:876–884. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9813-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.VanderWeele TJ. Re: Religious service attendance and major depression: a case of reverse causality? American Journal of Epidemiology. 2013;177:275–276. doi: 10.1093/aje/kws415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Idler EL, Kasl SV. Religion among Disabled and Nondisabled Elderly Persons II: Attendance at Religious Services as a Predictor of the Course of Disability. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences. 1997;52B:S306–S316. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52b.6.s306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wasserstein Ronald L, Lazar Nicole A. The ASA’s Statement on p-Values: Context, Process, and Purpose. The American Statistician. 2016;70(2):129–133. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Greenland S, Senn SJ, Rothman KJ, Carlin JB, Poole C, Goodman SN, Altman DG. Statistical Tests, P-values, Confidence Intervals, and Power: A Guide to Misinterpretations. European Journal of Epidemiology. 31(4):337–350. doi: 10.1007/s10654-016-0149-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Stang A, Poole C, Kuss O. The Ongoing Tyranny of Statistical Significance Testing in Biomedical Research. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;25:225–230. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9440-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.VanderWeele TJ. Re: The ongoing tyranny of statistical significance testing in biomedical research. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;25:843. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9507-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.VanderWeele TJ. Outcome-wide epidemiology. Epidemiology. 2017;28:399–402. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern Epidemiology. 3. Chapter 25 Lippincott; Philadelphia: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wakefield J. Ecologic studies revisited. Annu Rev Public Health. 2008;29:75–90. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Diener E1, Tay L, Myers DG. The religion paradox: if religion makes people happy, why are so many dropping out? J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011 Dec;101(6):1278–1290. doi: 10.1037/a0024402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lim C, Putnam RD. Religion, social networks, and life satisfaction. American Sociological Review. 2010;75:914–933. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Zuckerman P. Atheism, secularity, and well-being: how the findings of social science counter negative stereotypes and assumptions. Sociology Compass. 2009;3(6):949–971. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Zuckerman P. Society Without God: What the Least Religious Nations can Tell us About Contentment. New York, NY: New York University Press; [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hummer RA, Rogers RG, Nam CB, Ellison CG. Religious involvement and US adult mortality. Demography. 1999;36(2):273–285. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Strawbridge WJ, Cohen RD, Shema SJ, Kaplan GA. Frequent attendance at religious services and mortality over 28 years. American Journal of Public Health. 1997;87(6):957–961. doi: 10.2105/ajph.87.6.957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Chida Y, Steptoe A, Powell LH. Religiosity/spirituality and mortality. A systematic quantitative review. Psychother Psychosom. 2009;78(2):81–90. doi: 10.1159/000190791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Li S, Stamfer M, Williams DR, VanderWeele TJ. Association between religious service attendance and mortality among women. JAMA Internal Medicine. 2016;176(6):777–785. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.1615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Teinonen T, Vahlberg T, Isoaho R, Kivela SL. Religious attendance and 12-year survival in older persons. Age and Ageing. 2005;34(4):406–409. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afi107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.la Cour P, Avlund K, Schultz-Larsen K. Religion and survival in a secular region. A twenty year follow-up of 734 Danish adults born in 1914. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62(1):157–164. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Litwin H. What really matters in the social network-mortality association? A multivariate examination among older Jewish-Israelis. European Journal of Ageing. 2007;4(2):71–82. doi: 10.1007/s10433-007-0048-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Yeager D, Glei DA, Au M, Lin H-S, Sloan RP, Weinstein M. Religious involvement and health outcomes among older persons in Taiwan. Social Science and Medicine. 2006;63(8):2228–2241. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Stavrova O. Religion, self-rated health, and mortality: whether religiosity delays death depends on the cultural context. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2015;6(8):911–922. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Durkheim E. Suicide: A Study in Sociology. New York: Free Press; 1897. 1951. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Spoerri A, Zwahlen M, Bopp M, Gutzwiller F, Egger M Swiss National Cohort Study. Religion and assisted and non-assisted suicide in Switzerland: National Cohort Study. Int J Epidemiol. 2010;39(6):1486–94. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.VanderWeele TJ, Li S, Tsai A, Kawachi I. Association between religious service attendance and lower suicide among U.S. women. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:845–851. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.1243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hayward RD, Elliott M. Cross-national analysis of the influence of cultural norms and government restrictions on the relationship between religion and well-being. Review of Religious Research. 2014;56:23–43. [Google Scholar]
- 37.VanderWeele TJ. Explanation in Causal Inference: Methods for Mediation and Interaction. New York: Oxford University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 38.VanderWeele TJ. Religious community in health, health behaviors, and well-being. In: Kivimaki M, Batty GD, Kawachi I, Steptoe A, editors. Handbook of Psychosocial Epidemiology. Routledge; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Krause N, Hayward RD. Religion, meaning in life, and change in physical functioning during late adulthood. Journal of Adult Development. 2012;19:158–169. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Johnson BR, Jang SJ, Larson DB, Li SD. Does adolescent religious commitment matter?: a reexamination of the effects of religiosity on delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2001;38:22–44. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Johnson BR. More God, Less Crime: Why Faith Matters and How it Could Matter More. Templeton Press; West Conshohocken, PA: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Li S, Kubzansky LD, VanderWeele TJ. Religious service attendance, divorce, and remarriage among U.S. women. Social Science Research Network; 2016. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2891385 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2891385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wilcox WB, Wolfinger NH. Soul Mates: Religion, Sex, Love, and Marriage among African Americans and Latinos. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.VanderWeele TJ. On the promotion of human flourishing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2017;31:8148–8156. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1702996114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]