Abstract
Camouflage, and in particular background-matching, is one of the most common anti-predator strategies observed in nature. Animals can improve their match to the colour/pattern of their surroundings through background selection, and/or by plastic colour change. Colour change can occur rapidly (a few seconds), or it may be slow, taking hours to days. Many studies have explored the cues and mechanisms behind rapid colour change, but there is a considerable lack of information about slow colour change in the context of predation: the cues that initiate it, and the range of phenotypes that are produced. Here we show that peppered moth (Biston betularia) larvae respond to colour and luminance of the twigs they rest on, and exhibit a continuous reaction norm of phenotypes. When presented with a heterogeneous environment of mixed twig colours, individual larvae specialise crypsis towards one colour rather than developing an intermediate colour. Flexible colour change in this species has likely evolved in association with wind dispersal and polyphagy, which result in caterpillars settling and feeding in a diverse range of visual environments. This is the first example of visually induced slow colour change in Lepidoptera that has been objectively quantified and measured from the visual perspective of natural predators.
Keywords: Colour change, Camouflage, Reaction norm, Biston betularia, Masquerade, Polyphenism, Predator-prey interactions
Introduction
Some of the most diverse and visually striking phenotypes seen in nature are those of camouflaged animals (Stevens & Merilaita, 2009). Background matching, or crypsis, is a common anti-predator strategy that has provided a test-bed for the theory of evolution through natural selection (Wallace, 1879; Wallace, 1889). Crypsis is selected for by visual predators such as birds (Merilaita, Lyytinen & Mappes, 2001), whereby prey that match the colour/pattern of the surrounding backgrounds survive for longer than non-matching prey (Endler, 1981; Merilaita, Scott-Samuel & Cuthill, 2017). In heterogeneous habitats, comprised of visually contrasting patches, or a gradient from one habitat type to another (Fig. 1), optimising crypsis to all of the background components presents a challenge (Merilaita, Tuomi & Jormalainen, 1999). One solution to this problem is a genetic polymorphism, which can produce two or more morphs that are specialised to different patch types (Merilaita, Lyytinen & Mappes, 2001; Surmacki, Ozarowska-Nowicka & Rosin, 2013). However, a species with a genetically fixed phenotype is restricted to camouflage on one background, or limited camouflage across varied patch colours (Fig. 1A). Therefore, in environments that change appearance across small temporal and spatial scales, detrimental phenotype-environment mismatching can occur (Cook et al., 2012; Farkas et al., 2015). In this case, selection may favour phenotypic plasticity, enabling individuals to actively change their appearance to utilise different habitat patches without compromising camouflage (Fig. 1B; Stevens, 2016). An example of plasticity is colour change, which is a topic of current research interest and can be used to study the adaptive value and the physiology of camouflage (Duarte, Flores & Stevens, 2017).
Rapid colour change (<2 h), as reported in fish, cephalopods, and amphibians has been widely studied (Allen et al., 2015; Buresch et al., 2011; Hanlon et al., 2009), and much is known about how chromatophores produce rapid changes in colour and pattern in these systems (Kingston et al., 2015; Mathger & Hanlon, 2007). Comparatively slower colour changes (days to months) occur in some arthropod and fish species (Llandres et al., 2013; Ryer et al., 2008). In many of these systems we still do not know whether slow colour change is adaptive, nor do we know the precise cues or biochemical processes involved. A number of potential cues have been proposed, with dietary and visual cues receiving most attention (Duarte, Flores & Stevens, 2017; Stevens & Merilaita, 2009).
One example of a diet-induced phenotypic switch, or polyphenism, is seen in the larval stage of the moth Nemoria Arizona, which resembles inedible objects in its environment (Greene, 1989). In the spring the larvae resemble oak catkins, and in the summer they look like the branches of oak. This form of visual resemblance to inanimate objects is referred to as masquerade (Skelhorn, Rowland & Ruxton, 2010). Masquerade enables prey to avoid attack because predators misclassify these prey, rather than failing to detect them (Skelhorn et al., 2010b). The larvae of the peppered moth (Biston betularia) also masquerade as the twigs of their foodplant and change colour to match them (Noor, Parnell & Grant, 2008; Poulton, 1892). These brown and green colour morphs occur in response to the background colour on which the larvae rest (Noor, Parnell & Grant, 2008; Poulton, 1892). Changing appearance in response to background cues in the environment may be beneficial for animals that masquerade, as masquerade is often associated with polyphagy (Higginson et al., 2012). Visually hunting predators, like birds, heavily predate caterpillars that do not display warning colours (Lichter-Marck et al., 2015), and twig-mimicking caterpillars that do not match the twigs they rest on are also more likely to be predated (Skelhorn & Ruxton, 2010). Therefore, the ability to change colour could enhance masquerade in the wider range of environments these prey are likely to encounter, and consequently reduce their foraging restrictions (Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed, 2004).
It is important to determine the exact cues eliciting colour change, as these cues initiate the colour change cascade (Duarte, Flores & Stevens, 2017), and can therefore provide information on the evolution of adaptive colour and the mechanisms of colour production (Cuthill et al., 2017). Visual stimuli exist in two forms: achromatic (luminance), and chromatic (hue/chroma). Responses to achromatic stimuli (luminance) have been reported in sand fleas, geckos, toads, and flatfish (Polo-Cavia et al., 2016; Ryer et al., 2008; Stevens et al., 2015; Vroonen et al., 2012). Tree frogs (Hyla japonica) adjust their body colour and luminance, to maximise camouflage against visually heterogeneous backgrounds, although the response to achromatic stimuli was stronger (Choi & Jang, 2014; Kang, Kim & Jang, 2016). Many of these studies propose that colour change in these animals is induced by visual cues, but the visual pathways were not explicitly studied, and additional cues such as temperature or texture were often not controlled (Lin, Lin & Huang, 2009; Polo-Cavia et al., 2016; Yamasaki, Shimizu & Fujisaki, 2009).
To address this topic, we conducted a series of experiments to explore the type of visual cues that elicit colour change in B. betularia. The colour change in B. betularia has previously been described as a polyphenism: a switch of phenotype (Noor, Parnell & Grant, 2008). However, in the only study so far to investigate this behaviour, Noor, Parnell & Grant (2008) only provided two discrete stimuli: green vs. brown, and measured colour subjectively from a human perspective. The larvae of B. betularia are polyphagous and wind dispersed at first instar (Noor, Parnell & Grant, 2008; Tietz, 1972). The wide variety of twig colours between and within host plant species (Edmonds, 2010) presents a highly heterogeneous resting background. Therefore, it may be beneficial for individuals to change appearance on a continuous scale over time (Fig. 1D), known as a reaction norm (Woltereck, 1909). Colour reaction norms have been reported in squid, geckos, and anurans (Kang, Kim & Jang, 2016; Mathger & Hanlon, 2007; Vroonen et al., 2012), and are commonly induced by visual stimuli aquired by the animal about its environment. Reaction norms have not yet been investigated in lepidopteran larvae in this context.
We used calibrated stimuli in order to investigate the adaptive significance of colour change in B. betularia (Stevens & Merilaita, 2009). We manipulated luminance (brightness) and colour, and evaluated the degree to which B. betularia caterpillars are able to respond to intermediate strength cues (i.e., discrete polyphenism vs. reaction norm). We also measured the response to heterogeneous twig colour environments. For the purpose of these experiments, ‘colour’ encompasses hue and chroma. Hue is defined as the direction of the colour vector, and chroma as how different a colour is from achromatic white/black (Stoddard & Prum, 2008). ‘Luminance’ is defined as achromatic intensity, or perceived brightness (Stevens, Lown & Denton, 2014; Stoddard & Prum, 2008). We modelled colour using the avian visual system which allows a more direct adaptive interpretation of larval colour change in B. betularia, compared to using human vision. We tested the following predictions: (1) larvae respond to both colour and luminance; (2) larvae produce intermediate phenotypes in response to changing colour and/or luminance on a continuous scale (i.e., a reaction norm rather than a polyphenism, as suggested by Noor, Parnell & Grant (2008)); (3) when faced with a heterogeneous background, larvae adopt an intermediate colour reflecting the relative proportion of twig colours.
Materials & Methods
Experimental animals and rearing
All larvae for the various experiments were the F1 offspring from crosses between wild-caught or captively reared adults. Larvae typically undergo five moults, resulting in six instars until pupation (Noor, Parnell & Grant, 2008). In the first instar, larvae appear countershaded, and in subsequent instars the larvae develop colours that appear to match the twigs of the different host plants on which they may rest. The adults occur as a series of more or less discrete morphs differing in the degree of melanism: typica, insularia and carbonaria (Cook & Muggleton, 2003). As previous observations suggested no effect of adult morph on larval colour, or vice versa, some families used in these experiments were segregating for adult melanism alleles, whilst others were fixed for the typica allele (Table S1). For experiments requiring more larvae than one cross could provide, larvae from multiple crosses were split across treatments to minimise any family effects (Table S1). Larvae were initially reared from eggs on intact goat willow (Salix caprea) branches with leaves until second instar. Groups of 25 caterpillars from the same family (full siblings) were then introduced into transparent plastic boxes measuring 279 × 159 × 102 mm (length × width × depth), containing an irregular lattice of twenty 12 cm-long (ten 3 mm and ten 5 mm diameter) painted softwood dowels. The dowels were held in place with a chicken wire mesh frame painted the same colour as the dowels (Fig. 2). All paints used for dowels were from the Dulux Matte range (Table S1). To facilitate cleaning, the base of each box was lined with a plain blue C-Fold 1-ply paper towel and larvae were fed on stripped, stalkless leaves of goat willow (Salix caprea), which was replenished so that the larvae had a constant supply of food. Boxes were regularly cleaned and hands and equipment were washed in dilute bleach (10%) between handling of boxes to reduce risk of disease transmission. Experiments were conducted in a Sanyo Versatile Environment Test Chamber (MLR-351), with light intensity set at 15,000 lx during the day. Boxes were arranged two on each shelf, 20 cm apart, leaving a 60 cm height space with a shelf between boxes.
Colour and luminance quantification
Spectrophotometric analysis
Reflectance measurements of larvae and dowels were taken using an Ocean optics USB2000 spectrophotometer, with specimens illuminated at 45° to normal by a DH1000 balanced halogen deuterium light source. The measuring spot diameter was 3 mm, with spectra recorded at 0.34 nm intervals from 300 to 700 nm and measured relative to a WS-1 reflectance standard. Spectrophotometry data were visualised using Overture (v.1.0.1). Reflectance spectra were reduced to 1 nm intervals within the 300–700 nm range using customised code (provided by I Cuthill).
Firstly, to determine differences in ‘colour’ between larvae and the dowels from the colour experiment, the predicted photon catches of cone types (longwave LW, mediumwave MW, shortwave SW, ultraviolet UV, and double dorsal DD) of a blue tit, Cyanistes caeruleus, were modelled for each spectrum in tetrahedral colour space following the Vorobyev & Osorio (1998) model, using a program written in MATLAB (provided by I. C. Cuthill). We used the blue tit to represent the avian visual system because there is good visual data available and this species is likely a natural predator of B. betularia larvae. Cone stimulation values were converted to Cartesian coordinates and plotted in a tetrahedral space using a MATLAB program (Stoddard & Prum, 2008), such that each cone is represented by an axis. This colour space is useful because if a colour stimulates only one cone type, then its coordinates lie at the appropriate tip of the tetrahedron, and when all four cone types are equally stimulated the point lies at the origin. To provide a simpler measure of colour, we calculated greenness as the ratios between the cone catch values of the mediumwave and longwave photoreceptors [MW/(MW + LW)], which represent opponent mechanisms, following Arenas & Stevens (2017). For the achromatic dowel experiment we created a stimulus that increased in luminance in the absence of ‘colour’ (black to white), therefore we did not model response to colour, only luminance. We analysed only the blue tit double dorsal cone catch, as these cones mediate luminance vision (Campenhausen & Kirschfeld, 1998; Osorio & Vorobyev, 2005).
We modelled the ease with which an avian predator might discriminate between dowels and larvae using just noticeable differences (JND; see Vorobyev & Osorio, 1998 for equations). For chromatic contrasts, we used spectral sensitivities of the blue tit using relative cone ratios of SW = 0.7111; MW = 0.9926; LW = 1.0 and UV = 0.3704 (Hart et al., 2000), with a Weber fraction of 0.05 and idealized irradiance (D65). To model luminance JNDs, we used blue tit double dorsal (DD) cones. JND <1.00 indicate that two stimuli are indiscriminable; stimuli differing by 1–3 JND units are only discriminable under good viewing conditions; and stimuli showing values above this should be distinguishable with increasing ease (Stevens et al., 2015).
Photographic analysis
Colour/luminance analysis on larvae from the heterogeneous dowel experiments was performed using calibrated photographs, as the spectrophotometer was not available when these experiments were conducted. Photographs of individual larvae were normalised to a standardised grey background (18%) and linearised to 32-bit files using the Image Calibration and Analysis Toolbox (Troscianko & Stevens, 2015) in ImageJ (v.1.49p). RGB values were extracted from processed images using ImageJ (Schneider, Rasband & Eliceiri, 2012), from an average of six dorso-lateral measurements per larva: one from each side of the 3rd thoracic, and 2nd and 6th abdominal segments. An average measure of percentage greenness was then calculated across the six measurements using G/(R + G + B) * 100 from RGB ratios. Although objective, these measurements were not modelled using an avian visual system.
Experimental treatments
A total of four experiments were conducted to test three main hypotheses concerning the nature of the environmental cue and the phenotypic response (Table 1).
Table 1. Summary of experiments and hypotheses.
Experiment | Twig environment | Hypothesis |
---|---|---|
i | Contrasting colour | 1a. Larvae respond to differences in twig colour |
ii | Luminance gradient | 1b. Larvae respond to differences in twig luminance 2a. Larvae can produce intermediate responses to twig luminance |
iii | Colour and luminance gradient | 2b. Larvae can produce intermediate responses to twig colour and luminance |
iv | Heterogeneous environment | 3. Larvae generalise across twig colours |
(i) Colour treatments
Dowels were painted either isoluminant green or isoluminant brown (Fig. 2: IG, IB) to create two treatments that differed in overall colour (colour JND: 21.2) and greenness (Two sample t-test, t9.99 = − 16.86, P < 0.0001), but not luminance (luminance JND: 1.8, Two sample t-test, t5.86 = 0.37603, P = 0.7201). Five replicate boxes were used for each treatment (Table S1). Larvae were reared on a 12:12 hour day: night cycle, at 24 °C in the day and 18 °C at night. Once larvae had reached final instar, six reflectance measurements per larva were taken with a spectrophotometer, three from each lateral surface, on the 3rd thoracic, and 2nd and 6th abdominal segments. These segments were chosen to obtain repeated measurements of the main body colour, excluding any prominent markings.
(ii) Luminance gradient
Five luminance treatments were created using painted dowels (Fig. 2: B1, BW1, BW2, BW3, Wh) increasing in luminance (ANOVA, F4 = 8,415, P < 0.0001) from near-black to white (Fig. S1A, Table S1), and approximately equal in colour. Paint was matched to the grey standards from a Gretag Macbeth colour chart using a Gretag Macbeth colour scanner at a UK hardware store (B&Q). Three replicate boxes per treatment were used (Table S1). Larvae were reared on a 15:9 hour day: night cycle at 21 °C in the day and 19 °C at night. Once larvae had reached final instar, four reflectance measurements were taken with a spectrophotometer from the dorsal surface of each caterpillar, on the 3rd thoracic segment, and the 2nd, 4th and 6th abdominal segments.
(iii) Colour and luminance gradient
We mixed brown (Br) and green (Gr) paint in three different ratios to give a total of five treatments that ranged from brown to green (Table S1 , Fig. 2: Br, BG1, BG2, BG3, Gr). These treatments differed in greenness (Fig. S1D; ANOVA, F4 = 1,378, P < 0.0001) and luminance (Fig. S1C; ANOVA, F4 = 82.68, P < 0.0001), although greenness of BG1, BG2 and BG3 was less than expected based on the proportion of Gr paint in the mixture. Three replicate boxes per treatment were used. Larvae were reared on a 12:12 hour day: night cycle, at 24 °C in the day and 18 °C at night. Once larvae had reached final instar, six reflectance measurements per larva were taken as for colour treatments.
(iv) Heterogeneous dowel environment
Five treatments were created using only two colours of dowel, brown and green (Fig. 2: Br, Gr), but in different ratios: 100% brown, 70 brown: 30 green, 50 brown: 50: green, 30 brown: 70 green, and 100% green (Table S1). Larvae were reared on a 15:9 hour day: night cycle at 21 °C in the day and 19 °C at night. Once final instar was reached, the dorsal surface of each larva was photographed on a standard grey card background using a Nikon D80 digital camera, 60 mm macro lens with the following settings: 1/60 s (shutter speed), 16 (F), 400 (ISO), cloudy (white balance), 2× Nikon Speedlight SB-400 External Flash.
Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.1.0 (R Core Team, 2014). Responses to contrasting colour cues were compared using a linear mixed model in the lme4 package in R (Bates et al., 2015), with replicate nested within treatment. Luminance gradient and greenness gradient response means for each of the five treatment levels were compared using a one-way ANOVA. Polynomial models from orders 1–4 were fitted to the luminance and greenness correlations to explore the relationship between environmental gradient and larval response. All polynomial models are presented as fitted in Fig. S2. R2 value, visual fit, and examination of plotted residuals were used to determine the best model for each correlation. ANOVA was performed to look for significant differences in fit between models. The ‘greenness’ response of larvae reared under different degrees of dowel-colour heterogeneity was analysed by comparing means using a one-way ANOVA. Homogeneity of variance between treatment medians was explored using Levene’s test.
Results
Response to colour (isoluminant dowels)
Larvae presented with the isoluminant green (IG) treatment were significantly greener than those in the isoluminant brown (IB) treatment (Fig. 3A; F11, 125 = 33.69, P < 0.0001). In colour space, the larvae resembled their own treatment colour more closely than the alternative treatment colour (Fig. 3B), and green and brown larvae were discriminable to a bird (colour JND: 11.3). The average response was consistent across replicates within treatments (Fig. 3C), but the discrepancy between larval and dowel greenness was greater for larvae reared on green dowels (colour JND: 9.9), than those reared on brown dowels (colour JND: 8.3). Colour change took approximately 14–21 days to complete, depending on the individual. Although colour change is not restricted to the final instar, to the human eye, noticeable change did not occur until 4th instar (Edmonds, 2010).
Response to luminance gradient
Larvae responded to dowel luminance (F4 = 148.2, P < 0.0001), ranging from very pale on white dowels to very dark on black dowels, with intermediate degrees of luminance on grey dowels (Fig. 4A). The relationship between larvae and dowel luminance was significantly cubic (F3, 261 = 156.3, P < 0.0001, R2 = 0.64): relatively steep at the extremes and shallow at intermediate luminance (Fig. 4B). This shape was due to smaller luminance differences between larvae from intermediate treatments (BW1 vs. BW2: luminance JND: 1.2; BW2 vs. BW3 luminance JND 3.4). The differences between larvae from the two extremes of the gradient (black and white) vs. intermediate were larger (Bl vs. BW1, luminance JND: 17.4; and Wh vs. BW3, luminance JND: 13.2).
When comparing larvae to their corresponding dowels, larvae from higher luminance treatments (BW2, BW3 and Wh) were most different from their dowels (luminance JNDs: 35.9, 43.4 and 35.8, respectively). Black (Bl) and dark grey (BW1) treatments showed comparatively lower JNDs between larvae and dowels (luminance JNDs: 28.9 and 20.0, respectively).
Response to colour and luminance gradient
B. betularia larvae adjusted both greenness (F4 = 120.6, P < 0.0001) and luminance (F4 = 82.68, P < 0.0001) in response to dowel stimuli (Fig. 5A), showing a significant positive quadratic correlation between larvae and dowel greenness (Fig. 5B; F2, 277 = 225.6, R2 = 0.62, P < 0.0001). Two of the intermediate brown-green treatments (BG1 and BG2) were very close in greenness (Fig. 5B), and discrimination between them was low (colour JND: 4.61, luminance JND: 2.9). Larvae from these treatments followed this pattern closely, with identical greenness of 0.45 (Fig. 5B) and low discrimination values (colour JND: 2.4, luminance JND: 2.9). The colour discrepancy between larvae and dowels from the brown treatment (Br) was smaller (colour JND: 5.8) than for the green (Gr) treatment (colour JND: 14.1).
Response to heterogeneous colour environments
As the proportion of green dowels relative to brown dowels in each enclosure increased, the average greenness of B. betularia larvae in each enclosure also increased (Fig. 6; F4 = 16.2, P < 0.0001). Between-individual variance in larval greenness was significantly higher in the three heterogeneous than in the two homogeneous colour environments (Levene’s test, F4 = 16.558, P < 0.0001). This result still held when the most variable treatment was removed (Levene’s test, F3 = 8.3093, P < 0.0001). The apparent change in the average level of greenness in mixed treatments (Fig. 6) arose predominantly from changes to the ratio of ‘green’: ‘brown’ individuals, which was roughly in line with the dowel ratios, rather than every larva taking on an intermediate colour (Fig. S3).
Discussion
Biston betularia caterpillars changed colour to match the twigs upon which they rested, across all four experiments. Our results support the prediction that larvae would be able to respond to both colour and luminance (hypotheses 1a and 1b in Table 1). Larvae reared on green and brown dowels changed colour to match those dowels, and larvae reared on black and white dowels changed their luminance. Many other species can adjust luminance to enhance their camouflage from predators, such as flounders, sand fleas, and toads (Fairchild & Howell, 2004; Polo-Cavia et al., 2016; Stevens et al., 2015). Frogs, gobies and cephalopods can rapidly adjust colour in response to visual backgrounds using chromatophores (Hanlon et al., 2009; Kang, Kim & Jang, 2016; Mathger & Hanlon, 2007; Stevens, Lown & Denton, 2014). To our knowledge, our results are the first to show that lepidopteran larvae respond to both colour and luminance, and are likely to use dermal pigments as opposed to chromatophores to change their appearance. We also found that when B. betularia larvae were presented with colour and luminance gradients, the larvae produced intermediate phenotypes, on a continuous scale, to approximately match each background. This supports our second prediction (hypotheses 2a and 2b, Table 1). Intermediate phenotypes have been reported in amphibians, fish, and a number of benthic invertebrates, but the cues and mechanisms controlling these phenotypes have not been rigorously explored (De Bruyn & Gosselin, 2014; Kang, Kim & Jang, 2016; Lin, Lin & Huang, 2009; Skold, Aspengren & Wallin, 2013). Contrary to our third hypothesis (Table 1), larvae did show an increase in average greenness across the treatments with a heterogeneous background. However, this was largely due to an increasing proportion of green individuals compared with brown individuals, rather than every larva becoming greener. Our four experiments provide the first conclusive evidence of intermediate colour change in lepidopteran larvae in response to visually graded or heterogeneous cues. Our results extend our understanding of the type of visual cues that B. betularia larvae use for colour change, and the range of colours they can produce.
Visual control of colour change is well known in animals that exhibit rapid colour change, such as flatfish (Kelman, Tiptus & Osorio, 2006) and cephalopods (Mathger & Hanlon, 2007; Ramirez & Oakley, 2015), where chromatophores, under direct control from the visual nerve system, are responsible for the rapid colour change (Kingston et al., 2015; Messenger, 2001). Other species show comparatively slower responses to background manipulation. For example, shore crabs respond predominantly to luminance cues over colour to match their background (Stevens, Lown & Wood, 2014), and sand fleas are able to match changes in luminance and colour to avoid predation (Stevens et al., 2015). In Lepidoptera, early experimental evidence indicated that colour change was associated with larvae and pupae sensing their visual environment (Poulton, 1890). Since this pioneering work, the evidence collected in support of visually induced colour change in Lepidoptera has been limited and inconclusive: the experiments in B. betularia larvae (Noor, Parnell & Grant, 2008), and two species of hawkmoth larvae, Smerinthus ocellata and Laothoe populi (Grayson & Edmunds, 1989) did not measure colour objectively from the perspective of an ecologically relevant predator, and other potential cues were not controlled for.
In showing that background colour induces the phenotypic change in B. betularia, our results are in accordance with some of the conclusions drawn by Noor, Parnell & Grant (2008). By keeping dietary and tactile cues constant, we also found that B. betularia larvae use visual cues to change colour. However, our results differ from Noor, Parnell & Grant (2008) in that we have found that the response is a continuous reaction norm, not a polyphenism of only two phenotypes. This may be because the experiments by Noor, Parnell & Grant (2008) did not provide a spectrum of background colours, or because the responses of the larvae in Noor, Parnell & Grant (2008) were measured subjectively by assigning individual caterpillars as “best fits” to one of four colour categories. This necessarily reduces any variability to four levels. We objectively measured the colour of both the stimuli and the larvae from the perspective of avian predators. This information provides us with a better understanding of how the highly polyphagous larvae of B. betularia might avoid predation in a changing environment. The range of colour phenotypes that the larvae are able to produce could enable crypsis on a wide range of host plants, reducing costs of lost foraging opportunity, and explaining the higher probability of polyphagy by masquerading species (Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed, 2004). The background matching ability of B. betularia larvae is also likely to allow them to adapt to the blackening of trees and shrubs caused by atmospheric coal pollution. There is no direct evidence for this in B. betularia, as its larvae are very difficult to collect from the wild and occur at low densities. However, the twig-mimicking caterpillars of another geometrid, Odontopera bidentata, which are normally pale to medium brown, or with green (‘lichen’) patches, were uniformly black in the heavily polluted inner-city areas of 1970’s Manchester (Bishop & Cook, 1980).
The continuous relationship we observed between dowel colour and larval colour is non-linear, whereas the standard reaction norm is generally depicted as a linear relationship (Oomen & Hutchings, 2015). Non-linear reaction norms are common in nature; for example, in response to temperature: life history in butterflies (Brakefield, Kesbeke & Koch, 1998), pigmentation in fruit flies (Rocha, Medeiros & Klaczko, 2009), and morphology in sticklebacks (Ramler et al., 2014). The reason that we observed a non-linear relationship may be because colour change is costly (Polo-Cavia & Gomez-Mestre, 2017). However, the cost of colour change and the trade-off between these and foraging costs has yet to be explored in this species. An alternative explanation is that our stimuli did not surpass the thresholds needed to elicit the cascade from vision to colour (Burtt, 1951).
Vision in lepidopteran larvae has been much less studied than the compound eye of the adult stage (Briscoe & Bernard, 2005; Liu et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2013), but it is assumed that the simple ring of eyes or ocelli provides relatively poor vision (Ichikawa, 1990; Lin, Hwang & Tung, 2002). Our results show that B. betularia larvae can perceive differences in brightness and colour, and support the idea that visually induced plastic colour change in arthropods is mediated through the eyes. There is also growing evidence that camouflage may be partly guided by light-detecting opsin proteins outside the eye (Stevens, 2016). Further exploration of the visual processes and pathways that determine the sensitivity and range of colour change is important for understanding camouflage, and has been highlighted as a key area for future work (Duarte, Flores & Stevens, 2017).
In our experiments, there was variation in the degree of colour and luminance matching of the larvae to the dowels. For example, larvae were able to increase luminance as dowel luminance increased, but were always less bright than their corresponding dowels and in some cases would be detectable by birds. Larvae were also not able to closely match the green (Gr) dowel, and again would be detectable by birds. However, we know that resembling twigs is sufficient for masquerade to reduce predation risk, and a lack of perfect colour/luminance match is less detrimental for masqueraders than for cryptic prey (Skelhorn et al., 2010b). This lack of perfect resemblance could be due to physiological constraints, as the white dowels were highly luminant and the green dowels had a very high greenness score. The colours we used were chosen to test the range of colours that B. betularia larvae were able to match, rather than closely resembling the colour/luminance of twigs that individuals are exposed to in nature.
The physiological mechanism behind the colour change in B. betularia is unknown, though preliminary investigations have revealed that the external colour is achieved by varying pigmentation in three primary layers of epidermal tissue (Fig. S4). Cuticular pigments are responsible for colour patterns and have been described in other lepidopteran larvae (Dahlman, 1969; Goodwin, 1953). It is assumed that sequestering bright white or green pigments through a plant diet may be difficult, but yellow and white colouration is achieved with ommochrome pigments in the crab spider, Misumena vatia (Insausti & Casas, 2008). Material properties may also affect luminance, as different materials reflect different amounts of light, which may be the reason for B. betularia larvae achieving lower luminance than dowels. Another explanation for the larvae not achieving a perfect match to their backgrounds is similar to the ideas on imperfect mimicry (Greene & McDiarmid, 1981; Pekar & Jarab, 2011). Masquerade alone enables larvae like B. betularia to avoid being eaten by birds (Skelhorn et al., 2010a), therefore, if there is weak selection against imperfect mimics, then imperfect colour and pattern may not entirely negate the deceptive effect. Kallima butterflies masquerade as dead leaves, closely matching the shape, texture, and colour of the leaf (Suzuki, Tomita & Sezutsu, 2014). It is not currently known whether the shape (and posture), or colour is more important in remaining inconspicuous to predators, but it is thought that relaxed selection on close colour mimicry may occur because cognitive processes of predators (learned discrimination) are more important than sensory processing for visual detection of prey (Stoddard, 2012). Relaxed selection has been suggested as a precursor to phenotypic plasticity (Hunt et al., 2011), and relaxed colour selection in B. betularia could have contributed to colour plasticity in the larvae. However, more research is needed on this topic to understand the origins of colour plasticity in antipredator defences.
The increased variance among individuals produced by the heterogeneous environment treatment implies that the change in mean colour was mostly due to altered ratios of green and brown larvae, which may also be appreciated by inspection of the individual photographs (Fig. S3). This outcome is in contrast to theoretical models on camouflage in heterogeneous environments, which indicate that animals utilising resources on heterogeneous backgrounds should combine markings from each background, instead of optimising camouflage for a single background type (Merilaita, Tuomi & Jormalainen, 1999). This model was tested empirically with bird predators, and in this case prey with colour compromised between two habitats were predated less than those with matching colouration (Merilaita, Lyytinen & Mappes, 2001). However, in certain situations, such as large separation between patch types, specialisation towards one background type is predicted (Houston, Stevens & Cuthill, 2007), and has been observed in animals with fixed (Merilaita, Lyytinen & Mappes, 2001; Pellissier et al., 2011; Sandoval, 1994) and plastic phenotypes (Magellan & Swartz, 2013; Wente & Phillips, 2003). Although patch types (different coloured twigs) within our experimental environments were in close proximity and within easy range of every larva, individual larvae likely receive the strongest cues from the dowel they are resting on and, as they develop, may increasingly choose to rest on that type, reinforcing the specialisation response. Tracking resting behaviour of individual larvae and modelling this colour response using an avian visual system would allow us to make a more solid conclusion. Specialising crypsis to one colour would be a particular benefit to B. betularia larvae, which feed at night and are almost motionless during daylight hours when visual hunters are active. A sedentary lifestyle may accommodate a slow rate of colour change, as also observed in seahorses (Lin, Lin & Huang, 2009), whereas rapid colour change is required for camouflage success if an animal moves rapidly over spatially heterogeneous environments, as observed in fish and cephalopods (Mathger & Hanlon, 2007; Watson, Siemann & Hanlon, 2014).
Conclusions
We show that larvae of the peppered moth use visual cues to closely match the colour and luminance of their background and that this is a continuous response, or reaction norm. The adult and larval stages of B. betularia show alternative evolutionary routes to crypsis, with colour polymorphism under genetic control in the adult moths and reaction norm in the larvae. Both routes achieve protection against avian predation, and it is likely that these contrasting evolutionary strategies have been influenced by differences in life history traits, such as dispersal, reproduction, and feeding behaviour between adults and larvae, as well as physiology. Our results show a novel response in a species belonging to a group of animals whose camouflage potential has been poorly studied in comparison to other taxonomic groups.
Supplemental Information
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the reviewers, Tim Caro and Jolyon Troscianko, for their constructive feedback on the manuscript.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the Natural Environment Research Council (Grant no. NE/J022993). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare there are no competing interests.
Author Contributions
Amy Eacock conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Hannah M. Rowland and Ilik J. Saccheri conceived and designed the experiments, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Nicola Edmonds conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
Eacock, Amy (2017): RGB raw data. figshare. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5239657.v2
Eacock, Amy (2017): Raw spectrophotometer files for luminance gradient experiment. figshare. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4780519.v3
Eacock, Amy (2017): Raw spectrophotometer data for chroma gradient experiments. figshare. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4780516.v2
The MATLAB code used for analyzing spectrophotometry data in the article has not yet been published but can be made available upon request from Innes Cuthill at i.cuthill@bristol.ac.uk. The analysis can be repeated by using calculations from Vorobyev & Osorio (1998) with an R package such as Pavo (Maia et al., 2013).
References
- Allen et al. (2015).Allen JJ, Akkaynak D, Sugden AU, Hanlon RT. Adaptive body patterning, three-dimensional skin morphology and camouflage measures of the slender filefish Monacanthus tuckeri on a Caribbean coral reef. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2015;116:377–396. doi: 10.1111/bij.12598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arenas & Stevens (2017).Arenas LM, Stevens M. Diversity in warning coloration is easily recognized by avian predators. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2017;30:1288–1302. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates et al. (2015).Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67:1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop & Cook (1980).Bishop JA, Cook LM. Industrial melanism and the urban environment. Advances in Ecological Research. 1980;11:373–404. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2504(08)60270-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brakefield, Kesbeke & Koch (1998).Brakefield PM, Kesbeke F, Koch PB. The regulation of phenotypic plasticity of eyespots in the butterfly Bicyclus anynana. American Naturalist. 1998;152:853–860. doi: 10.1086/286213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briscoe & Bernard (2005).Briscoe AD, Bernard GD. Eyeshine and spectral tuning of long wavelength-sensitive rhodopsins: no evidence for red-sensitive photoreceptors among five Nymphalini butterfly species. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2005;208:687–696. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buresch et al. (2011).Buresch KC, Mathger LM, Allen JJ, Bennice C, Smith N, Schram J, Chiao CC, Chubb C, Hanlon RT. The use of background matching vs. masquerade for camouflage in cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Vision Research. 2011;51:2362–2368. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burtt (1951).Burtt E. The ability of adult grasshoppers to change colour on burnt ground. Proceedings of the Royal Entomological Society London. 1951;26:45–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1951.tb00119.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Campenhausen & Kirschfeld (1998).Campenhausen MV, Kirschfeld K. Spectral sensitivity of the accessory optic system of the pigeon. Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology. 1998;183:1–6. doi: 10.1007/s003590050229. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Choi & Jang (2014).Choi N, Jang Y. Background matching by means of dorsal color change in treefrog populations (Hyla japonica) Journal of Experimental Zoology Part A-Ecological Genetics and Physiology. 2014;321:108–118. doi: 10.1002/jez.1841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook et al. (2012).Cook LM, Grant BS, Saccheri IJ, Mallet J. Selective bird predation on the peppered moth: the last experiment of Michael Majerus. Biology Letters. 2012;8:609–612. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook & Muggleton (2003).Cook LM, Muggleton J. The peppered moth, Biston betularia (Linnaeus, 1758) (Lepidoptera: Geometridae): a matter of names. Entomologist’s Gazette. 2003;54:211–221. [Google Scholar]
- Cuthill et al. (2017).Cuthill IC, Allen WL, Arbuckle K, Caspers B, Chaplin G, Hauber ME, Hill GE, Jablonski NG, Jiggins CD, Kelber A, Mappes J, Marshall J, Merrill R, Osorio D, Prum R, Roberts NW, Roulin A, Rowland HM, Sherratt TN, Skelhorn J, Speed MP, Stevens M, Stoddard MC, Stuart-Fox D, Talas L, Tibbetts E, Caro T. The biology of color. Science. 2017;357:eaan0221. doi: 10.1126/science.aan0221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahlman (1969).Dahlman DL. Cuticular pigments of Tobacco Hornworm (Manduca sexta) larvae-effects of diet and genetic differences. Journal of Insect Physiology. 1969;15:807–814. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(69)90121-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- De Bruyn & Gosselin (2014).De Bruyn RAJ, Gosselin LA. Prevalence of ontogenetic changes in colour brightness among benthic invertebrates and their association with microhabitat shifts. Marine Ecology Progress Series. 2014;498:147–159. doi: 10.3354/meps10626. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Duarte, Flores & Stevens (2017).Duarte RC, Flores AAV, Stevens M. Camouflage through colour change: mechanisms, adaptive value and ecological significance. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2017;372 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0342. Article 20160342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edmonds (2010).Edmonds N. PhD Thesis. 2010. Mechanisms of colour change in larval and adult peppered moths. [Google Scholar]
- Endler (1981).Endler JA. An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1981;16:25–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fairchild & Howell (2004).Fairchild EA, Howell WH. Factors affecting the post-release survival of cultured juvenile Pseudopleuronectes americanus. Journal of Fish Biology. 2004;65:69–87. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-1112.2004.00529.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Farkas et al. (2015).Farkas TE, Hendry AP, Nosil P, Beckerman AP. How maladaptation can structure biodiversity: eco-evolutionary island biogeography. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2015;30:154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2015.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin (1953).Goodwin TW. The pigments in colour phases of the larvae of Plusia gamma L (the Silver Y moth) Biochemical Journal. 1953;55:834–838. doi: 10.1042/bj0550834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grayson & Edmunds (1989).Grayson J, Edmunds M. The causes of color and color-change in caterpillars of the poplar and eyed hawkmoths (Lathoe-populi and Smerinthus-ocellata) Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1989;37:263–279. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1989.tb01904.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Greene (1989).Greene E. A diet-induced developmental polymorphism in a caterpillar. Science. 1989;243:643–646. doi: 10.1126/science.243.4891.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene & McDiarmid (1981).Greene HW, McDiarmid RW. Coral snake mimicry-does it occur. Science. 1981;213:1207–1212. doi: 10.1126/science.213.4513.1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanlon et al. (2009).Hanlon RT, Chiao CC, Mathger LM, Barbosa A, Buresch KC, Chubb C. Cephalopod dynamic camouflage: bridging the continuum between background matching and disruptive coloration. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2009;364:429–437. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart et al. (2000).Hart NS, Partridge JC, Cuthill IC, Bennett ATD. Visual pigments, oil droplets, ocular media and cone photoreceptor distribution in two species of passerine bird: the blue tit (Parus caeruleus L.) and the blackbird (Turdus merula L.) Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2000;186:375–387. doi: 10.1007/s003590050437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higginson et al. (2012).Higginson AD, De Wert L, Rowland HM, Speed MP, Ruxton GD. Masquerade is associated with polyphagy and larval overwintering in Lepidoptera. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2012;106:90–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2012.01850.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Houston, Stevens & Cuthill (2007).Houston AI, Stevens M, Cuthill IC. Animal camouflage: compromise or specialize in a 2 patch-type environment? Behavioral Ecology. 2007;18:769–775. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arm039. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt et al. (2011).Hunt BG, Ometto L, Wurm Y, Shoemaker D, Yi SV, Keller L, Goodisman MAD. Relaxed selection is a precursor to the evolution of phenotypic plasticity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:15936–15941. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104825108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ichikawa (1990).Ichikawa T. Spectral sensitivities of elementary color-coded neurons in butterfly larva. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1990;64:1861–1872. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.6.1861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insausti & Casas (2008).Insausti TC, Casas J. The functional morphology of color changing in a spider: development of ommochrome pigment granules. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2008;211:780–789. doi: 10.1242/jeb.014043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang, Kim & Jang (2016).Kang CK, Kim YE, Jang Y. Colour and pattern change against visually heterogeneous backgrounds in the tree frog Hyla japonica. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0001-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelman, Tiptus & Osorio (2006).Kelman EJ, Tiptus P, Osorio D. Juvenile plaice (Pleuronectes platessa) produce camouflage by flexibly combining two separate patterns. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2006;209:3288–3292. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kingston et al. (2015).Kingston ACN, Kuzirian AM, Hanlon RT, Cronin TW. Visual phototransduction components in cephalopod chromatophores suggest dermal photoreception. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2015;218:1596–1602. doi: 10.1242/jeb.117945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lichter-Marck et al. (2015).Lichter-Marck IH, Wylde M, Aaron E, Oliver JC, Singer MS. The struggle for safety: effectiveness of caterpillar defenses against bird predation. Oikos. 2015;124:525–533. doi: 10.1111/oik.01515. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lin, Hwang & Tung (2002).Lin JT, Hwang PC, Tung LC. Visual organization and spectral sensitivity of larval eyes in the moth Trabala vishnou Lefebur (Lepidoptera:Lasiocampidae) Zoological Studies. 2002;41:366–375. [Google Scholar]
- Lin, Lin & Huang (2009).Lin Q, Lin JD, Huang LM. Effects of substrate color, light intensity and temperature on survival and skin color change of juvenile seahorses, Hippocampus erectus Perry, 1810. Aquaculture. 2009;298:157–161. doi: 10.1016/j.aquaculture.2009.10.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liu et al. (2017).Liu ZX, Wang XY, Lei CL, Zhu F. Sensory genes identification with head transcriptome of the migratory armyworm, Mythimna separata. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llandres et al. (2013).Llandres AL, Figon F, Christidès J, Mandon N, Casas J. Environmental and hormonal factors controlling reversible colour change in crab spiders. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2013;216:3886–3895. doi: 10.1242/jeb.086470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magellan & Swartz (2013).Magellan K, Swartz ER. Crypsis in a heterogeneous environment: relationships between changeable polymorphic colour patterns and behaviour in a galaxiid fish. Freshwater Biology. 2013;58:793–799. doi: 10.1111/fwb.12086. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Maia et al. (2013).Maia R, Eliason CM, Bitton P-P, Doucet SM, Shawkey MD. pavo: an R Package for the analysis, visualization and organization of spectral data. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2013;4(10):609–613. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12069. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mathger & Hanlon (2007).Mathger LM, Hanlon RT. Malleable skin coloration in cephalopods: selective reflectance, transmission and absorbance of light by chromatophores and iridophores. Cell and Tissue Research. 2007;329:179–186. doi: 10.1007/s00441-007-0384-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merilaita, Lyytinen & Mappes (2001).Merilaita S, Lyytinen A, Mappes J. Selection for cryptic coloration in a visually heterogeneous habitat. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2001;268:1925–1929. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merilaita, Scott-Samuel & Cuthill (2017).Merilaita S, Scott-Samuel NE, Cuthill IC. How camouflage works. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2017;372:1–9. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merilaita, Tuomi & Jormalainen (1999).Merilaita S, Tuomi J, Jormalainen V. Optimization of cryptic coloration in heterogeneous habitats. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1999;67:151–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01858.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Messenger (2001).Messenger JB. Cephalopod chromatophores: neurobiology and natural history. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2001;76:473–528. doi: 10.1017/S1464793101005772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noor, Parnell & Grant (2008).Noor MAF, Parnell RS, Grant BS. A reversible color polyphenism in American peppered moth (Biston betularia cognataria) caterpillars. PLOS ONE. 2008;3:e3142. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oomen & Hutchings (2015).Oomen RA, Hutchings JA. Genetic variability in reaction norms in fishes. Environmental Reviews. 2015;23:353–366. doi: 10.1139/er-2014-0077. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Osorio & Vorobyev (2005).Osorio D, Vorobyev M. Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities in terrestrial animals: adaptations for luminance and colour vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2005;272:1745–1752. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pekar & Jarab (2011).Pekar S, Jarab M. Assessment of color and behavioral resemblance to models by inaccurate myrmecomorphic spiders (Araneae) Invertebrate Biology. 2011;130:83–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7410.2010.00217.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pellissier et al. (2011).Pellissier L, Wassef J, Bilat J, Brazzola G, Buri P, Colliard C, Fournier B, Hausser J, Yannic G, Perrin N. Adaptive colour polymorphism of Acrida ungarica H. (Orthoptera: Acrididae) in a spatially heterogeneous environment. Acta Oecologica-International Journal of Ecology. 2011;37:93–98. doi: 10.1016/j.actao.2010.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Polo-Cavia & Gomez-Mestre (2017).Polo-Cavia N, Gomez-Mestre I. Pigmentation plasticity enhances crypsis in larval newts: associated metabolic cost and background choice behaviour. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:39739. doi: 10.1038/srep39739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polo-Cavia et al. (2016).Polo-Cavia N, Oliveira JM, Villa AJR, Marquez R. Background colour matching in a wild population of Alytes obstetricans. Amphibia-Reptilia. 2016;37:253–260. doi: 10.1163/15685381-00003050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Poulton (1890).Poulton EB. The colours of animals. D Appleton and Company; New York: 1890. [Google Scholar]
- Poulton (1892).Poulton EB. Further experiments upon the colour-relation between certain lepidopterous larvae, pupae, cocoons, and imagines and their surroundings. Transactions of the Royal Entomological Society of London. 1892;40:293–487. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2311.1892.tb02973.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2014).R Core Team . R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez & Oakley (2015).Ramirez MD, Oakley TH. Eye-independent, light-activated chromatophore expansion (LACE) and expression of phototransduction genes in the skin of Octopus bimaculoides. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2015;218:1513–1520. doi: 10.1242/jeb.110908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramler et al. (2014).Ramler D, Mitteroecker P, Shama LNS, Wegner KM, Ahnelt H. Nonlinear effects of temperature on body form and developmental canalization in the threespine stickleback. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2014;27:497–507. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocha, Medeiros & Klaczko (2009).Rocha F, Medeiros HF, Klaczko LB. The reaction norm for abdominal pigmentation and its curve in Drosophila mediopuncta deoend on the mean phenotypic value. Evolution. 2009;63:280–287. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00503.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed (2004).Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed M. Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. OUP; Oxford: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Ryer et al. (2008).Ryer CH, Lemke JL, Boersma K, Levas S. Adaptive coloration, behavior and predation vulnerability in three juvenile north Pacific flatfishes. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology. 2008;359:62–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jembe.2008.02.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoval (1994).Sandoval CP. The effects of the relative geographic scales of gene flow and selection on morph frequencies in the walking-stick Timema christinae. Evolution. 1994;48:1866–1879. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1994.tb02220.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, Rasband & Eliceiri (2012).Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. NIH image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:671–675. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skelhorn, Rowland & Ruxton (2010).Skelhorn J, Rowland HM, Ruxton GD. The evolution and ecology of masquerade. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2010;99:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2009.01347.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Skelhorn et al. (2010a).Skelhorn J, Rowland HM, Speed MP, De Wert L, Quinn L, Delf J, Ruxton GD. Size-dependent misclassification of masquerading prey. Behavioral Ecology. 2010a;21:1344–1348. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq159. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Skelhorn et al. (2010b).Skelhorn J, Rowland HM, Speed MP, Ruxton GD. Masquerade: camouflage without Crypsis. Science. 2010b;327:51–51. doi: 10.1126/science.1181931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skelhorn & Ruxton (2010).Skelhorn J, Ruxton GD. Mimicking multiple models: polyphenetic masqueraders gain additional benefits from crypsis. Behavioral Ecology. 2010;22:60–65. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq166. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Skold, Aspengren & Wallin (2013).Skold HN, Aspengren S, Wallin M. Rapid color change in fish and amphibians—function, regulation, and emerging applications. Pigment Cell & Melanoma Research. 2013;26:29–38. doi: 10.1111/pcmr.12040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens (2016).Stevens M. Color change, phenotypic plasticity, and camouflage. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2016;4:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2016.00051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens et al. (2015).Stevens M, Broderick AC, Godley BJ, Lown AE, Troscianko J, Weber N, Weber SB. Phenotype—environment matching in sand fleas. Biology Letters. 2015;11 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0494. Article 20150494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens, Lown & Denton (2014).Stevens M, Lown AE, Denton AM. Rockpool gobies change colour for camouflage. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e110325. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens, Lown & Wood (2014).Stevens M, Lown AE, Wood LE. Color change and camouflage in juvenile shore crabs Carcinus maenas. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2014;2 doi: 10.3389/fevo.2014.00014. Article 14. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens & Merilaita (2009).Stevens M, Merilaita S. Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2009;364:423–427. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoddard (2012).Stoddard MC. Mimicry and masquerade from the avian visual perspective. Current Zoology. 2012;58:630–648. doi: 10.1093/czoolo/58.4.630. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stoddard & Prum (2008).Stoddard MC, Prum RO. Evolution of avian plumage color in a tetrahedral color space: a phylogenetic analysis of new world buntings. American Naturalist. 2008;171:755–776. doi: 10.1086/587526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surmacki, Ozarowska-Nowicka & Rosin (2013).Surmacki A, Ozarowska-Nowicka A, Rosin ZM. Color polymorphism in a land snail Cepaea nemoralis (Pulmonata: Helicidae) as viewed by potential avian predators. Naturwissenschaften. 2013;100:533–540. doi: 10.1007/s00114-013-1049-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki, Tomita & Sezutsu (2014).Suzuki TK, Tomita S, Sezutsu H. Gradual and contingent evolutionary emergence of leaf mimicry in butterfly wing patterns. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2014;14:13. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-14-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tietz (1972).Tietz HM. An index to the described life histories, early stages, and hosts of the Macrolepidoptera of the continental United States and Canada. A.C. Allyn; Sarasota: 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Troscianko & Stevens (2015).Troscianko J, Stevens M. Image calibration and analysis toolbox—a free software suite for objectively measuring reflectance, colour and pattern. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2015;6:1320–1331. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorobyev & Osorio (1998).Vorobyev M, Osorio D. Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1998;265:315–358. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vroonen et al. (2012).Vroonen J, Vervust B, Fulgione D, Maselli V, Van Damme R. Physiological colour change in the Moorish gecko, Tarentola mauritanica (Squamata: Gekkonidae): effects of background, light, and temperature. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2012;107:182–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2012.01915.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace (1879).Wallace AR. Colour in nature. Nature. 1879;19:501–505. doi: 10.1038/019501a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace (1889).Wallace AR. Darwinism: an exposition of the theory of natural selection, with some of its applications. Macmillan; London: 1889. [Google Scholar]
- Watson, Siemann & Hanlon (2014).Watson AC, Siemann LA, Hanlon RT. Dynamic camouflage by Nassau groupers Epinephelus striatus on a Caribbean coral reef. Journal of Fish Biology. 2014;85:1634–1649. doi: 10.1111/jfb.12519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wente & Phillips (2003).Wente WH, Phillips JB. Fixed green and brown color morphs and a novel color-changing morph of the Pacific tree frog Hyla regilla. American Naturalist. 2003;162:461–473. doi: 10.1086/378253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woltereck (1909).Woltereck R. Weitere experimentelle Un-tersuchungen uber Artveranderung, speziell uber das Wesen quantitativer Artunterschiede bei Daphniden [Further experimental studies on species modification, specifically on the nature of quantitative species differences in Daphniden] Verhandlungen der Deutschen Zoologischen Gesellschaft. 1909;2:110–117. [Google Scholar]
- Xu et al. (2013).Xu PJ, Lu B, Xiao HJ, Fu XW, Murphy RW, Wu KM. The evolution and expression of the Moth visual opsin family. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamasaki, Shimizu & Fujisaki (2009).Yamasaki A, Shimizu K, Fujisaki K. Effect of host plant part on larval body-color polymorphism in Helicoverpa armigera (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) Annals of the Entomological Society of America. 2009;102:76–84. doi: 10.1603/008.102.0108. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
Eacock, Amy (2017): RGB raw data. figshare. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5239657.v2
Eacock, Amy (2017): Raw spectrophotometer files for luminance gradient experiment. figshare. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4780519.v3
Eacock, Amy (2017): Raw spectrophotometer data for chroma gradient experiments. figshare. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4780516.v2
The MATLAB code used for analyzing spectrophotometry data in the article has not yet been published but can be made available upon request from Innes Cuthill at i.cuthill@bristol.ac.uk. The analysis can be repeated by using calculations from Vorobyev & Osorio (1998) with an R package such as Pavo (Maia et al., 2013).