Skip to main content
Neuropsychopharmacology logoLink to Neuropsychopharmacology
. 2017 Sep 13;43(3):655–664. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.166

Effect of Citalopram on Emotion Processing in Humans: A Combined 5-HT1A [11C]CUMI-101 PET and Functional MRI Study

Sudhakar Selvaraj 1,2,9,*, Chris Walker 1,9, Danilo Arnone 3,4, Bo Cao 1, Paul Faulkner 5, Philip J Cowen 6, Jonathan P Roiser 7,9, Oliver Howes 2,4,8,9
PMCID: PMC5693328  EMSID: EMS73625  PMID: 28776580

Abstract

A subset of patients started on a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) initially experience increased anxiety, which can lead to early discontinuation before therapeutic effects are manifest. The neural basis of this early SSRI effect is not known. Presynaptic dorsal raphe neuron (DRN) 5-HT1A receptors are known to have a critical role in affect processing. Thus we investigated the effect of acute citalopram on emotional processing and the relationship between DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability and amygdala reactivity. Thirteen (mean age 48±9 years) healthy male subjects received either a saline or citalopram infusion intravenously (10 mg over 30 min) on separate occasions in a single-blind, random order, crossover design. On each occasion, participants underwent a block design face-emotion processing task during fMRI known to activate the amygdala. Ten subjects also completed a positron emission tomography (PET) scan to quantify DRN 5-HT1A availability using [11C]CUMI-101. Citalopram infusion when compared with saline resulted in a significantly increased bilateral amygdala responses to fearful vs neutral faces (left p=0.025; right p=0.038 FWE-corrected). DRN [11C]CUMI-101 availability significantly positively correlated with the effect of citalopram on the left amygdala response to fearful faces (Z=2.51, p=0.027) and right amygdala response to happy faces (Z=2.33, p=0.032). Our findings indicate that the initial effect of SSRI treatment is to alter processing of aversive stimuli and that this is linked to DRN 5-HT1A receptors in line with evidence that 5-HT1A receptors have a role in mediating emotional processing.

Introduction

Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) are the most commonly prescribed medications for anxiety and depressive disorders worldwide (Olfson and Marcus, 2009). However, despite being used to treat anxiety disorders, a subgroup of patients experience an initial increase in anxiety after initiation of SSRI treatment (Gollan et al, 2012; Sinclair et al, 2009). Although this generally ameliorates over a few weeks, it can be clinically problematic as these patients with high anxiety are less likely to reach remission (Gollan et al, 2012). The neural basis of this early effect of SSRIs on anxiety and subsequent heterogeneity in treatment response is not known.

Serotonin, or 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT), is thought to be critical for affect regulation in the brain (Dayan and Huys, 2008), and SSRIs are thought to act primarily by altering 5-HT function. The administration of single doses of citalopram, a commonly used SSRI, in healthy human subjects is associated with enhanced startle responses and fear recognition (Browning et al, 2007; Burghardt et al, 2004; Grillon et al, 2007) and altered serotonin release (Selvaraj et al, 2012b). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have revealed that depressed patients have exaggerated amygdala reactivity as measured using blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) responses when presented with emotions of negative valence (fearful or sad faces), and 8 weeks of SSRI treatment attenuates this to ‘normalize’ the amygdala responses (Sheline et al, 2001). Bigos et al (2008) using a double-blind balanced crossover study design found that citalopram 20 mg infusion compared with saline in healthy male participants (N=8) caused concentration-dependent increases in human amygdala reactivity to aversive facial stimuli (Bigos et al, 2008). However, in contrast, Del-Ben et al (2005) used a covert (aversive) face emotion recognition task and found attenuated amygdala response to fear after a 7.5 mg citalopram infusion compared with saline in male volunteers (N=12).

5-HT1A receptors are a key regulator of brain 5-HT activity through inhibitory autoreceptors located presynaptically on 5-HT dorsal raphe neurons (DRNs), as well as on postsynaptic neurons in projection sites (Barnes and Sharp, 1999). Activation of the DRN 5-HT1A receptors causes hyperpolarization and reduces 5-HT neuronal firing, which results in decreased 5-HT release from the 5-HT nerve terminals in the synapses. Acute SSRI administration increases 5-HT by blocking 5-HTT, which then activates raphe 5-HT1A autoreceptor and thus reducing neuronal firing. Raphe 5-HT1A activation causes internalization, which immediately returns to baseline level (Riad et al, 2001), and this phenomenon is not observed in postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (Riad et al, 2001). 5-HT1A receptors have been consistently shown to modulate anxiety-related behavior in animal models. Specifically, 5-HT1A receptor knockout mice exhibit increased fear-related behavior (Ramboz et al, 1998; Richardson-Jones et al, 2011) and an altered fear response (Gross et al, 2000). A common functional variation (C(-1019)G) in the human 5-HT1A gene (HTR1A) is associated with increased 5-HT1A autoreceptor expression and decreased threat-related amygdala reactivity (Fakra et al, 2009). Finally, psychotropic drugs such as buspirone and vilazodone with 5-HT1A receptor-binding properties have been found to be clinically useful for anxiety symptoms (Akimova et al, 2009; Gommoll et al, 2015; Sramek et al, 1999).

An inverse relationship between 5-HT1A receptor binding in the dorsal raphe and amygdala reactivity has been reported in healthy human subjects (Fisher et al, 2006). In a combined fMRI and positron emission tomography (PET) imaging study using the 5-HT1A receptor tracer [11C]-CUMI-101, we similarly found DRN 5-HT1A receptor binding to be inversely related to amygdala BOLD responses to fear vs neutral faces (Selvaraj et al, 2014). The above findings suggest that DRN 5-HT1A receptors may have a critical role in regulating amygdala reactivity during aversive emotion processing.

Citalopram is one of the most selective SSRI compared with fluoxetine, paroxetine, sertraline, or fluvoxamine and has high affinity to serotonin transporter (5-HTT) without any significant affinity for other serotonergic (5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, or 5-HT2A/C), adrenergic, cholinergic, or other neurotransmitters and (Hyttel, 1994) a single administration of citalopram 1 mg/kg in rodents increases 5-HT levels in the raphe but not in the frontal cortex. A 10 mg/kg increases 5-HT release to 400% in the raphe but only 170% in the frontal cortex (Invernizzi et al, 1992). This dose-dependent and differential regional effect of SSRI on 5-HT release is consistent with 5-HT1A-mediated negative feedback mechanism (Chaput et al, 1986; Gartside et al, 1995; Riad et al, 2001). Thus SSRI induced 5-HT release in 5-HT neuronal projection regions could be a balance of SSRIs’ ability to block 5-HTT at local neuronal terminals and to decrease DRN neuronal firing (Fuller, 1994; Gartside et al, 1995; Hjorth and Auerbach, 1996; Richardson-Jones et al, 2011). Interestingly, mice selectively engineered to express lower 5-HT1A autoreceptor levels compared with those with higher DRN 5-HT1A autoreceptor levels had increased raphe firing rate, greater 5-HT release in fronto-limbic regions, and produced robust response to SSRI in reducing the aversive behavior (Richardson-Jones et al, 2010). In addition to 5-HT1A autoreceptor-mediated negative feedback, postsynaptic 5-HT1A heteroreceptor and 5-HT1B mediate the inhibitory actions and 5-HT2A mediates the excitatory actions of 5-HT on target neurons in the prefrontal and limbic cortices along with other 5-HT receptors such as 5-HT3, 5-HT4, and 5-HT7 and also regulate 5-HT neuronal firing and release through postsynaptic feedback (Sharp et al, 2007).

Citalopram is the only SSRI available in intravenous form and is relatively well tolerated by volunteers in clinical studies (Attenburrow et al, 2001). Intravenous citalopram 10 mg has been successfully used as a probe to study brain serotonin function in clinical studies (Attenburrow et al, 2001; Bhagwagar et al, 2004). In addition, we have used intravenous citalopram in PET imaging studies to characterize the specificity of serotonin transporter radioligand [11C]DASB occupancy (Hinz et al, 2008) and to study serotonin displacement (Selvaraj et al, 2012b).

In the present study, we aimed to investigate the effect of acute citalopram infusion on the neural processing of aversive emotional stimuli and to determine its relationship with DRN 5-HT1A receptors as measured with [11C]-CUMI-101 in healthy human subjects. We hypothesized that intravenous citalopram would increase amygdala reactivity to fear vs neutral faces. It is not known how the DRN 5-HT1A is related to the effect of acute citalopram on emotion processing. Based on our work and other studies (Richardson-Jones et al, 2010; Richardson-Jones et al, 2011; Selvaraj et al, 2014; Selvaraj et al, 2012b), we hypothesized that subjects with higher DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability would show a greater increase in amygdala response to emotional facial expressions following intravenous citalopram infusion.

Materials and methods

A total of 13 healthy male participants took part in the citalopram and saline infusion fMRI study. All participants had undergone Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV Disorders (Spitzer et al, 2004) screening interview administered by study investigators to ascertain past and current psychiatric and medical history. Inclusion criteria were male and female subjects, aged 35−65 years, in good physical health, and capable of giving informed consent. Exclusion criteria were contraindication to PET scanning (pregnancy or breast feeding was an absolute contraindication), current or past history of major psychiatric disorder, present or recent (previous 3 months) use of psychotropic medication, current significant illicit substance/alcohol misuse or current significant other co-morbidity, and no MRI contraindications. Electrocardiogram was carried out before the infusion to rule out any prolonged corrected QT interval. All the subjects had urine drug screen on all scan days to check for illegal drug use. All subjects also completed validated subjective scales to assess mood and anxiety and also a visual analog scale (VAS) to quantify side effects, if any. The subjects were paid a small honorarium for taking part in the study. The study was approved by the local research ethics committee. The PET and fMRI scans were carried out at the MRC London Institute of Medical Sciences, Hammersmith Hospital, London, UK.

Research Design

Thirteen subjects first took part in a PET scan experiment in which healthy subjects received either a placebo (saline) or citalopram infusion before a [11C]CUMI PET scan to index 5-HT1A receptor availability (Supplementary Figure 1). The results of this experiment are described in our previous publication (Selvaraj et al, 2012b). Subjects then went on to participate in the new fMRI experiment reported here. Of the 13 subjects who took part in the PET experiment, 3 dropped out, leaving 10 subjects who completed the fMRI component as well and we recruited an additional 3 new subjects who only participated in the fMRI component. We used the data from the 5-HT1A [11C]CUMI PET placebo (saline) scan as an index of baseline DRN 5-HT1A availability (Supplementary Figure 1).

In this new fMRI experiment, all participants received either saline or an intravenous infusion of 10 mg citalopram over 30 min in a single-blinded (participants), random order crossover design on alternate days. About 15–30 min after the end of infusion, the subjects underwent the fMRI emotion processing task (Selvaraj et al, 2012a). Blood samples were collected for citalopram levels at (t=0) and after the infusion (t=45 min). Mood was assessed before and after each scans using VAS to ascertain subjective affective responses on emotions (including anxiety, sadness, happiness, anger, and irritability) across sessions. VAS scale was divided into a 10-point scale for each emotion. There was a gap of at least 1 week between the two scans (mean and SD was 30 (42.9) days).

Measurement of Neural Response to Emotional Stimuli

fMRI data acquisition

MRI was performed on 3 T scanner (3 T Intera Philips Medical Systems (Best, The Netherlands) to acquire T2*-weighted transverse echoplanar images (EPI). A total of 132 whole-brain EPI volumes were collected with 44 slices acquired in an even–odd interleave in a descending direction (TR=3 s; TE=30 ms; slice thickness=3.25 mm; 2.19 × 2.19 mm2 in-plane resolution; phase encoding direction=anterior→posterior; field of view=280 mm2; matrix size 128 × 128). Real-time reconstruction, z-shimming correction, and a slice tilt of −30° to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line were used to minimize orbitofrontal and temporal signal dropout as a result of magnetic field inhomogeneities due to air tissue susceptibility differences in these regions (Weiskopf et al, 2006; Weiskopf et al, 2007). A whole-brain 3D-MPRAGE scan was acquired (TR=9.6 ms, TE=4.5 ms, flip angle=8°, slice thickness=1.2 mm, 0.94 × 0.94 mm2 in-plane resolution, 150 slices) after the EPI scans.

fMRI task

A well-characterized incidental facial emotional processing task was employed as described in our previous studies (O'Nions et al, 2011; Selvaraj et al, 2014). Subjects were shown a series of faces on a projector screen and asked to respond by classifying if each face was male or female. Emotional faces representing a single emotion (ie, happy, fearful, or neutral) were presented in 16 s blocks of eight faces, with a total of 12 blocks (4 per emotion). Subjects were instructed to fixate on a cross during a 16 s rest period between stimulus blocks.

Measurement of 5-HT1A Receptor Availability

PET scan acquisition

All PET scans were performed on the GE Discovery RX PET/CT scanner with a PET axial field of view of 15.7 cm and 47 reconstructed transaxial image planes. [11C]CUMI-101 is a selective 5-HT1A radioligand with high signal-to-noise ratio in the brain. CUMI-101 has higher affinity (Ki=0.15 nM) and better selectivity for 5-HT1A receptor than 5-HT1A agonist 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin (8-OH-DPAT) (Kumar et al, 2013; Kumar et al, 2007). It was initially developed as 5-HT1A partial agonist ligand with specific binding to high affinity 5-HT1A receptors and thus to be more sensitive to study 5-HT release than older antagonist radiotracers, such as [11C]WAY-100635 (Milak et al, 2011). However, the exact nature of [11C]CUMI-101 intrinsic activity as 5-HT1A receptor agonist or antagonist is not clear (Hendry et al, 2011; Kumar et al, 2013; Shrestha et al, 2014). [11C]CUMI-101 was administered via injection into an antecubital vein as a smooth bolus over 30 s. The dynamic PET scan was acquired over 90 min in (simultaneous) frame and list mode (Selvaraj et al, 2012a).

Graphical analysis of reversible radioligand binding together with the metabolite-corrected plasma input function was used to quantify the binding potential BPND in regions of interest (ROIs; Selvaraj et al, 2012b). The specific binding was quantified as BPND (37) where: BPND=(VT target region−VT reference region)/VT reference region. VT is the volume of distribution (ml/cm3) defined as the ratio of the tracer concentration in the region to the metabolite-corrected plasma concentration at equilibrium (Innis et al, 2007).

fMRI analysis

The fMRI preprocessing and analysis were carried out in FSL using FEAT (Smith et al, 2004) and mirrored the analyses reported in our previous study (Selvaraj et al, 2014). Functional MRI data for individual runs were high pass filtered at 0.0078 Hz and motion corrected using a 6 degree of freedom (DOF) rigid body transformation (MCFLIRT). Finally, data were smoothed with an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel prior to a two-stage standard space transformation. fMRI data and individual high-resolution T1 images were registered to a 2 mm MNI standard template using a 12 DOF linear transformation (FLIRT) followed by nonlinear warping of T1 images to standard space (FNIRT). Both linear and nonlinear transformations were concatenated and applied to first-level, native space statistical images before higher-level analyses.

Task regressors for happy, fearful, and neutral face blocks were modeled using a double-gamma function convolved with a 16 s square wave. Motion parameter estimates were included in the model to account for residual motion artifacts. All regressors were temporally filtered to match fMRI data preprocessing parameters (Hallquist et al, 2013). Time series data were prewhitened (FILM) prior to modeling. As per Selvaraj et al (2014), three contrasts were calculated at this level to compare: (1) faces vs baseline; (2) fearful vs neutral faces; and (3) happy vs neutral faces. At the second level of analysis, each first-level contrast was submitted to a fixed-effect analysis, which computed a contrast estimate for each subject comparing citalopram vs placebo. Second-level contrast estimates were submitted for a final mixed-effect analysis using FSL’s FLAME2 tool, which employs Bayesian estimation of mean contrast estimates to determine the group-level effect of citalopram on face (average within contrasts 1, 2, and 3) and valence processing (contrast 2 vs contrast 3).

To constrain the number of simultaneous tests, we defined two spherical ROIs, in the left and right amygdala, by setting a 6 mm radius around the MNI coordinates (x=±21, y=−6, z=−15) adapted from our previous study (O'Nions et al, 2011; Selvaraj et al, 2014) using the same paradigm. This ensured an unbiased ROI definition. Voxelwise corrections for family-wise error inflation were applied using Gaussian random field (GRF) theory-based height thresholding of Z-statistical maps at p<0.05 (corrected). The values reported in the text represent the mean lower-level contrast estimates for citalopram and placebo extracted from voxels showing a significant citalopram vs placebo difference and thus represents a potential selection bias (Kriegeskorte et al, 2009). Therefore, mean condition and contrast estimates across the independently defined spherical amygdala ROIs are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Condition and Contrast Parameter Estimates to Face Stimuli During Placebo (PBO) and Citalopram (CITA) Infusions in the Amygdala.
  Left amygdala, M (SD)
Right amygdala, M (SD)
  PBO CITA PBO CITA
Condition        
 Neutral faces 17.98 (39.60) 13.89 (32.69) 15.73 (27.56) 20.08 (38.40)
 Fearful faces 20.26 (35.23) 30.92 (30.59) 15.06 (41.77) 22.23 (32.38)
 Happy faces 7.57 (37.08) 30.53 (53.00) 11.79 (35.13) 19.07 (22.35)
         
Contrast (level 1)
 All vs baseline 50.61 (85.23) 77.72 (82.18) 46.79 (98.94) 66.56 (76.02)
 Fearful vs neutral 1.46 (25.78) 16.97 (37.33) −1.88 (23.92) 3.11 (36.42)
 Happy vs neutral −12.14 (54.84) 16.79 (60.70) −5.40 (30.81) −1.54 (29.46)

PET data analysis

Subjects’ structural MRIs were segmented (into gray/white matter/cerebrospinal fluid) using the segmentation tool in SPM (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and were re-sliced (1 × 1 × 1 mm3) and co-registered to the corresponding subject’s denoised, head movement-corrected, and summed PET image using SPM5. Amygdala, postsynaptic cortical regions, and cerebellum were defined using a probabilistic brain atlas template (Hammers et al, 2003). The atlas was spatially normalized to the coregistered individual MRI scans with deformation parameters obtained from the normalization to the standard MNI T1 template in SPM. The normalized brain atlas was resliced to the individual’s PET space and fused with the individual gray matter map to obtain a gray matter template for the amygdala and postsynaptic cortical regions. These were then used to sample the dynamic PET to obtain the regional time–activity courses. The presynaptic DRN was manually defined as a fixed-size region (215 mm3) in the midbrain area on the summed PET images of each individual (Bose et al, 2011a; Selvaraj et al, 2014; Selvaraj et al, 2012b). Finally, cerebellar gray matter was used as the reference region (Selvaraj et al, 2012b). See Selvaraj et al (2012b) for further details of the [11C]CUMI PET analysis.

Multi-modal PET-MR analysis

The third-level analysis described in the fMRI analysis above was repeated for the 10 subjects who had also completed the PET imaging protocol. To estimate the relationship between fMRI changes associated with citalopram and DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability, the [11C]CUMI-101-binding potential (BND) values from the DRN were included as a continuous predictor variable in the analysis. Extracted data represent lower-level contrast estimates summarizing mean parameter estimate differences as in the fMRI-only analysis. Correlation coefficients were computed between DRN [11C]CUMI-101 BPND and the citalopram vs placebo contrast estimates for the independent amygdala ROI (rAMYG) to display general strength and direction of association. Extracted Z-statistics represent the mean of significant Z-scores with corresponding GRF theory-based p-values (Beckmann et al, 2003; Jenkinson et al, 2002; Woolrich et al, 2001). Owing to the possible presence of an outlier (ie, DRN [11C]CUMI-101 BND>2.4 SD of mean; q.v., Figure 2b and c), these analyses were repeated with FEAT’s outlier deweighting tool. Statistical results were identical. Furthermore, our review of the specific outlier case found that [11C]CUMI-101 availability in other brain regions (eg, amygdala) and the first-level fMRI results were comparable to other participants’ PET/first-level results. We deemed the case to exhibit a real physiological effect and the final reported statistics reflect its inclusion.

Results

Thirteen male subjects (mean (SD) age=48±9 years) completed the emotion processing task on both days. Study participants generally tolerated intravenous citalopram well with either no or minimal self-limiting adverse effects, which included mild nausea, hot flush, lightheadedness, and tiredness. One subject reported mild nausea after saline (placebo). None of the subjects stopped the scan procedures or dropped out of study during the study day. There were no significant differences in behavioral measures between sessions on subjective VAS affective state measures (paired t-tests, all ps>0.1), especially no significant change in anxiety measures. The mean serum citalopram concentration at 45 min after the start of infusion is 757.45 μg/l (SD 802.75). There was no significant correlation was observed between serum citalopram concentration and citalopram effect on amygdala reactivity to fearful faces (p>0.1).

Effect of Citalopram on Amygdala Reactivity to Aversive Faces

For the all faces vs baseline contrast, citalopram infusions resulted in a significantly increased BOLD response bilaterally in the amygdala with a larger cluster in the left hemisphere: right amygdala (k=2, peak MNI coordinates: (x=16, y=−10, z=−18); MPBO=12.31, SDPBO=110.48; MCITA=82.32, SDCITA=106.10), Z=2.46, p=0.039 (FWE-corrected); left amygdala (k=11, peak MNI coordinates: (x=−20, y=−4, z=−12); MPBO=33.30, SDPBO=94.71; MCITA=75.06, SDCITA=81.33), Z=2.33, p=0.046 (FWE-corrected) (Figure 1a and b). See Table 1 for individual condition and contrast parameter estimates across the spherical amygdala ROIs.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Functional imaging reveals bilateral patterns of increased face-dependent activation with citalopram infusion. (a) Intravenous citalopram (CITA) significantly increased activation bilaterally for the all faces vs rest (blue) and fearful vs neutral face (red) contrasts when compared with placebo (PBO). Trend-level activation increases were also observed in the right amygdala for the happy vs neutral face contrast (yellow). (b; cyan) Mean parameter estimates (±SEM) extracted across the voxels with significant differences in the second-level contrasts (ie, CITA–PBO). Within each hemisphere, the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal elicited by faces increased after CITA infusions compared with PBO (striped bars indicate PBO estimates). (c; red) A similar pattern of activity was observed in the left hemisphere for the fearful vs neutral face contrasts as well; however, (d; yellow) the right hemisphere effects for fearful vs neutral and happy vs neutral faces are less clear owing to a task-related deactivation in the PBO condition. A full color version of this figure is available at the Neuropsychopharmacology journal online.

To test the hypothesis that intravenous citalopram would specifically increase the response to fearful faces, we repeated the citalopram contrast analysis for the fearful vs neutral faces contrast estimates and again identified increased bilateral amygdala activation associated with citalopram. Statistical differences were identified in both the right amygdala (k=14, peak MNI coordinates: (x=24, y=−6, z=−20); MPBO=−7.31, SDPBO=24.15, MCITA=13.74, SDCITA=25.65), Z=2.59, p=0.025 (FWE-corrected), and the left amygdala (k=18, peak MNI coordinates: (x=−22, y=−6, z=−18); MPBO=4.25, SDPBO=26.64, MCITA=23.43, SDCITA=50.89), Z=2.29, p=0.038 (FWE-corrected) (Figure 1a and c). There were no significant differences for the happy vs neutral faces contrast in the left amygdala after citalopram, though a significant increase was observed in the right amygdala (k=3, peak MNI coordinates: (x=24, y=−4, z=−16); MPBO=−13.70, SDPBO=31.01, MCITA=9.07, SDCITA=16.27), Z=2.12, p=0.042 (FWE-corrected) (Figure 1a and d). Despite qualitative hemispheric differences between the fearful and happy face response, a direct comparison only demonstrated a trend-level effect of greater fearful face response modulation by citalopram compared with the happy face response in the left amygdala (k=2, peak MNI coordinates: (x=−20, y=−10, z=−12); MFEARFUL=3.50, SDFEARFUL=24.73, MHAPPY=−5.24, SDHAPPY=18.42), Z=2.02, p=0.062.

There was no significant correlation observed between serum citalopram concentration and citalopram effect on amygdala reactivity to fearful faces (p>0.1).

Relationship between Citalopram Induced Changes in Amygdala Reactivity and DRN 5-HT1A Receptor

Of the total of 13 subjects, 10 completed the [11C]CUMI-101 PET imaging protocol as well. 5-HT1A receptor BPND values from the DRN (mean=1.63, SD=0.34) were included as a continuous predictor in the citalopram contrast model, and analyses were repeated within this subset to identify associations within the amygdala. There was a significant positive association between DRN 5-HT1A and the all faces vs baseline citalopram contrast in the right amygdala, (k=1, peak MNI coordinates: (x=16, y=−8, z=−12); MPBO=36.57, SDPBO=67.94, MCITA=33.07, SDCITA=56.66), rAMYG(10)=0.05, Z=2.59, p=0.032, (FWE-corrected).

A significant positive association was identified within the left amygdala for the fearful faces vs neutral faces contrast. The citalopram vs placebo difference in the response to fearful faces vs neutral faces was larger in participants with greater DRN [11C]CUMI-101 binding (k=11, peak MNI coordinates: (x=−16, y=−10, z=−14); MPBO=0.74, SDPBO=38.78, MCITA=5.29, SDCITA=35.19), rAMYG(10)=0.38, Z=2.51, p=0.027 (FWE-corrected) (Figure 2a, c and e). There was a smaller positive association between DRN [11C]CUMI-101 binding and the citalopram vs placebo difference in response to fearful vs neutral faces in the right amygdala (k=1, peak MNI coordinates: (x=24, y=−10, z=−14); MPBO=4.51, SDPBO=27.91, MCITA=−8.51, SDCITA=48.77), rAMYG(10)=0.06, Z=2.16, p=0.042, FWE-corrected.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) 5-HT1A availability was positively associated with the degree of modulation induced by citalopram infusion in the amygdala. (a) PET [11C]CUMI-101-binding estimates demonstrated a functionally lateralized positive association with activity in the left amygdala between the response to fearful faces and DRN 5-HT1A availability (red) and the right amygdala showing an association with happy faces (yellow). (b and c) Citalopram (CITA) minus placebo (PBO) differences show that individuals with greater DRN 5-HT1A availability have reduced citalopram-induced modulation of the amygdala to happy (yellow) and fearful (red) faces in the right and left hemispheres, respectively. (d and e) Same as panels (b and c) but with citalopram and placebo data presented separately. Whereas individuals with less DRN 5-HT1A availability show heightened amygdala responses during placebo infusions (cyan), the association largely disappears with citalopram (magenta), suggesting that individual differences in the response of the amygdala to emotional content is associated with differences in DRN 5-HT1A availability. A full color version of this figure is available at the Neuropsychopharmacology journal online.

For the happy vs neutral face contrasts, DRN values demonstrated positive associations in the right amygdala (k=6, peak MNI coordinates: (x=24, y=−10, z=−14); MPBO=-2.85, SDPBO=28.63, MCITA=−12.60, SDCITA=40.37), rAMYG(10)=0.21, Z=2.33, p=0.032, (FWE-corrected) (Figure 2a, b and d), and again, a smaller effect in the left amygdala (k=1, peak MNI coordinates: (x=−16, y=−10, z=−14); MPBO=−13.53, SDPBO=60.41, MCITA=−7.38, SDCITA=32.36), rAMYG(10)=0.34, Z=2.29, p=0.033, (FWE-corrected). DRN [11C]CUMI-101-binding associations did not differ significantly between fearful and happy faces. Collectively, this pattern of results suggests that greater [11C]CUMI-101-binding potential in the DRN is positively associated with the degree of increase in BOLD signaling to emotional faces induced by citalopram infusion. However, the specificity of the amygdala response to particular valences is still unclear.

Discussion

In this multimodal brain imaging study, we report the effect of acute intravenous citalopram on amygdala reactivity and its relationship with DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability (as indexed by [11C]CUMI-101 PET) in healthy human male subjects. The main findings of this study are that: (1) acutely citalopram increased the BOLD response bilaterally in the amygdala to fearful faces and in the right amygdala to happy faces with a trend-level left amygdala selectivity to fearful vs happy faces; and (2) DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability is positively associated with the degree of increase in amygdala BOLD response to emotional faces (both fearful and happy vs neutral) induced by citalopram infusion. The current findings, when combined with other findings (Fisher et al, 2006; Richardson-Jones et al, 2010; Richardson-Jones et al, 2011; Selvaraj et al, 2014), support the critical role of presynaptic DRN 5-HT1A receptors in regulating emotion processing.

Intravenous citalopram increased amygdala BOLD response bilaterally for fearful vs neutral faces. This finding is consistent with our a priori hypothesis regarding amygdala reactivity to SSRI and in agreement with Bigos et al (2008) but not with Del-Ben et al (2005). The differences could be due to differences in the nature of emotion task paradigms of using covert or explicit emotion faces task. Curiously, voxels demonstrating significant increases in BOLD response were not positively active for the placebo in the fearful and happy faces vs neutral contrasts (Figure 1c and d). One possibility for this result is that our analytical approach highlighted those voxels that were found to be maximally different between the two sessions rather than which voxels are maximally activated by the task per se. However, parameter estimates extracted from the condition regressors presented in Table 1 demonstrate consistent positive activation across all conditions, suggesting that the amygdala was sensitive to faces but not uniquely sensitive to emotional faces during the placebo visit. Thus only during the citalopram infusion were amygdala responses sensitive to affect. Another possibility is that the voxels responsive to emotional faces during the placebo visit were already active at ceiling and the voxels identified in the present analysis represent an increase in the spatial extent of activation (ie, similar peak but wider spread). Future investigations may consider varying the valence of affective stimuli to better address this question of state-based reactivity vs activation span.

Our data provide evidence that variations in DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability are related to the SSRI effect on emotion processing. However, the mechanistic pathway of this relationship cannot be determined from the correlations we report. Our finding is, however, consistent with preclinical research on the role of presynaptic DRN 5-HT1A receptors. Preclinical studies show that stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors decreases the firing rate of 5-HT neurons (Sprouse and Aghajanian, 1987) and 5-HT release (Bosker et al, 1997; Bosker et al, 2001). Mice selectively expressing high DRN 5-HT1A autoreceptors compared with low DRN 5-HT1A autoreceptors have decreased 5-HT cell firing and therefore reduced 5-HT tone in the projection sites such as the amygdala (Richardson-Jones et al, 2011). Acute SSRI-induced increases in extracellular raphe 5-HT also activates 5-HT1A autoreceptors, thereby decreases 5-HT firing and release in projection sites (Auerbach et al, 1995; Fuller, 1994; Gartside et al, 1995; Haddjeri et al, 2004; Romero and Artigas, 1997). Thus high DRN 5-HT1A may be associated with low 5-HT in amygdala. Based on our findings, we speculate that individuals with high DRN 5-HT1A autoreceptors are more sensitive to the autoreceptor activation and thus show greater amygdala response when given acute dose of SSRI. However, acute citalopram treatment also increases 5-HT levels in the amygdala in rodent models (Bosker et al, 2001) and elevated 5-HT in amygdala increases fear learning and acquisition (Bocchio et al, 2016; Deakin and Graeff, 1991). Furthermore, the net effect of synaptic 5-HT at projection sites may be influenced by regional variations in 5-HT transporters (Bose et al, 2011b). Finally, the modulatory effect of 5-HT on anxiety response depends upon a balance of excitatory (5-HT2A) and inhibitory (5-HT1A and 5-HT1B) 5-HT signaling on cortical pyramidal and interneurons in the prefrontal and amygdala circuitry (Albert et al, 2014; Fisher et al, 2011). Therefore, multiple mechanisms may be involved in SSRI-induced amygdala response.

When taken together, these findings indicate that combined knowledge of 5-HTT and 5-HT1A autoreceptor density may have predictive value in understanding antidepressant response. Based on our preliminary findings in healthy volunteers, we speculate that patients with high DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability would be predicted to have more severe anxiety responses to SSRIs. Further research in patients with major depression might help clarify the contribution of this mechanism to the increase in anxiety levels reported by some patients after initiation of antidepressant treatment.

This study has a number of limitations; first, we only studied male participants. Future studies should assess whether gender has an effect on the relationship between DRN 5-HT1A receptor and amygdala reactivity as some studies have reported associations between DRN 5-HT1A autoreceptor availability and sex (Parsey et al, 2002). Second, although N=13 is comparable to the size of similar studies (Bigos et al, 2008; Del-Ben et al, 2005), the sample size for the combined PET/fMRI experiment does not permit investigation of additional potential moderators (eg, 5-HTT genetic polymorphisms) on the influence of the 5-HT1A receptors on amygdala reactivity. This will have to be addressed in future larger studies. Furthermore, analysis of smaller sample sizes, as reported here, is known to provide inflated estimates of effect sizes (Button et al, 2013). As such, we have presented means and SDs from voxels showing significant differences and from our a priori amygdala ROI as a whole. A substantially larger sample (eg, N>78 for the fearful vs neutral face citalopram contrasts; cf., fmripower.org; (Mumford and Nichols, 2008) will be required to establish well-powered effect size estimates. Therefore, we put forward our own results with caution to be interpreted as indicating the presence of an association rather than a specific magnitude of effect. Third, correlation between DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability and amygdala BOLD response to emotional faces after citalopram does not prove causality. Further experimental research studies will be needed to study the direct role of 5-DRN HT1A in emotion processing in humans. Finally, the average interval between PET and fMRI data acquisition was 125 (SD 7.7) weeks (2.4 years). Although the PET and fMRI scans were performed at different time points, several lines of evidence indicate that the binding potential measures in this study would be stable over the timescale of the experiment. A previous 5-HT1A [11C]CUMI-101 study reported high test–retest reliability for raphe measurements, with an intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.8 (Milak et al, 2010), indicating that it can be reliably measured. A selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonist ligand (18F-MPPF) study that collected test–retest scans with a mean delay 27 weeks between scans in healthy volunteers reported high reliability of dorsal raphe-binding potential measurements (ICC 0.78) (Costes et al, 2007). This result suggests that 5-HT1A PET measures from the dorsal raphe are reliable over time, albeit using a different tracer (18F-MPPF). In addition, PET studies report no significant decline of 5-HT1A availability (as indexed by [11C]WAY-100635) in presynaptic or postsynaptic regions over time with age (age range of 24–53 years) in a large cohort (N=61; Rabiner et al, 2002), suggesting that ageing does not significantly affect 5-HT1A availability. Thus brain 5-HT1A receptor availability in vivo using PET provides stable measure of 5-HT1A binding and the time interval between acquisitions of the PET/MRI scan data is less likely to have influenced the results. Nevertheless, we cannot exclude variation over time, although this would, if anything, be expected to increase noise and weaken the results.

Conclusion

An acute intravenous administration of citalopram increased amygdala reactivity to aversive emotion, and this was positively associated with DRN 5-HT1A receptor availability. Our findings indicate that the initial effect of SSRI treatment is to alter processing of aversive stimuli and that this is linked to DRN 5-HT1A receptors in line with evidence that 5-HT1A receptors have a role in mediating emotional processing.

Funding and disclosure

This study was supported by an Academy of Medical Sciences, UK clinical lecturer starter grant to SS (grant number: AMS-SGCL6). This study was funded by a Medical Research Council (UK) grant to OH (grant number: MC-A656-5QD30); Maudsley Charity (no. 666), Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, and Wellcome Trust (no. 094849/Z/10/Z) grants to OH; and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. PF was supported by an MRC studentship. DA was supported by the Academy of Medical Sciences, UK (grant number: AMS SGCL8) and has received travel grants from Jansenn-Cilag and Servier. PJC has been a member of advisory boards of Servier and Lundbeck and has been a paid lecturer for Servier and Lundbeck. JPR has been a member of a media advisory board for Lundbeck and consults for Cambridge Cognition Ltd. Intravenous citalopram was kindly provided by Lundbeck, UK. OH has received investigator-initiated research funding from and/or participated in advisory/speaker meetings organized by Astra-Zeneca, Autifony, BMS, Eli Lilly, Heptares, Janssen, Lundbeck, Lyden-Delta, Otsuka, Servier, Sunovion, Rand, and Roche. The other authors declare no conflict of interest. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, or the Department of Health.

Acknowledgments

We thank the staff at Hammersmith Imanet (Andrew Blyth, Hope McDevitt, Andreanna Williams, Safiye Osman, and Noora Ali) and Robert Steiner MRI unit at Hammersmith Hospital for the technical expertise they provided.

Footnotes

Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Neuropsychopharmacology website (http://www.nature.com/npp)

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Figure 1

References

  1. Akimova E, Lanzenberger R, Kasper S (2009). The serotonin-1A receptor in anxiety disorders. Biol Psychiatry 66: 627–635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Albert PR, Vahid-Ansari F, Luckhart C (2014). Serotonin-prefrontal cortical circuitry in anxiety and depression phenotypes: pivotal role of pre- and post-synaptic 5-HT1A receptor expression. Front Behav Neurosci 8: 199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Attenburrow MJ, Mitter PR, Whale R, Terao T, Cowen PJ (2001). Low-dose citalopram as a 5-HT neuroendocrine probe. Psychopharmacology 155: 323–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Auerbach SB, Lundberg JF, Hjorth S (1995). Differential inhibition of serotonin release by 5-HT and NA reuptake blockers after systemic administration. Neuropharmacology 34: 89–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barnes NM, Sharp T (1999). A review of central 5-HT receptors and their function. Neuropharmacology 38: 1083–1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM (2003). General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI. Neuroimage 20: 1052–1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bhagwagar Z, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM, Harmer CJ (2004). Normalization of enhanced fear recognition by acute SSRI treatment in subjects with a previous history of depression. Am J Psychiatry 161: 166–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bigos KL, Pollock BG, Aizenstein HJ, Fisher PM, Bies RR, Hariri AR (2008). Acute 5-HT reuptake blockade potentiates human amygdala reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 3221–3225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bocchio M, McHugh SB, Bannerman DM, Sharp T, Capogna M (2016). Serotonin, amygdala and fear: assembling the puzzle. Front Neural Circuits 10: 24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bose SK, Mehta MA, Selvaraj S, Howes OD, Hinz R, Rabiner EA et al (2011. a). Presynaptic 5-HT1A is related to 5-HTT receptor density in the human brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 2258–2265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bose SK, Mehta MA, Selvaraj S, Howes OD, Hinz R, Rabiner EA et al (2011. b). Presynaptic 5-HT1A is related to 5-HTT receptor density in the human brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 2258–2265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bosker F, Vrinten D, Klompmakers A, Westenberg H (1997). The effects of a 5-HT1A receptor agonist and antagonist on the 5-hydroxytryptamine release in the central nucleus of the amygdala: a microdialysis study with flesinoxan and WAY 100635. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 355: 347–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bosker FJ, Cremers TI, Jongsma ME, Westerink BH, Wikström HV, den Boer JA (2001). Acute and chronic effects of citalopram on postsynaptic 5-hydroxytryptamine(1A) receptor-mediated feedback: a microdialysis study in the amygdala. J Neurochem 76: 1645–1653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Browning M, Reid C, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM, Harmer CJ (2007). A single dose of citalopram increases fear recognition in healthy subjects. J Psychopharmacol 21: 684–690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Burghardt NS, Sullivan GM, McEwen BS, Gorman JM, LeDoux JE (2004). The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram increases fear after acute treatment but reduces fear with chronic treatment: a comparison with tianeptine. Biol Psychiatry 55: 1171–1178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES et al (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14: 365–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chaput Y, de Montigny C, Blier P (1986). Effects of a selective 5-HT reuptake blocker, citalopram, on the sensitivity of 5-HT autoreceptors: electrophysiological studies in the rat brain. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 333: 342–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Costes N, Zimmer L, Reilhac A, Lavenne F, Ryvlin P, Le Bars D (2007). Test-retest reproducibility of 18F-MPPF PET in healthy humans: a reliability study. J Nucl Med 48: 1279–1288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dayan P, Huys QJ (2008). Serotonin, inhibition, and negative mood. PLoS Comput Biol 4: e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Deakin JF, Graeff FG (1991). 5-HT and mechanisms of defence. J Psychopharmacol 5: 305–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Del-Ben CM, Deakin JF, McKie S, Delvai NA, Williams SR, Elliott R et al (2005). The effect of citalopram pretreatment on neuronal responses to neuropsychological tasks in normal volunteers: an FMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 1724–1734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fakra E, Hyde LW, Gorka A, Fisher PM, Munoz KE, Kimak M et al (2009). Effects of HTR1A C(-1019)G on amygdala reactivity and trait anxiety. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 33–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fisher PM, Meltzer CC, Ziolko SK, Price JC, Moses-Kolko EL, Berga SL et al (2006). Capacity for 5-HT1A-mediated autoregulation predicts amygdala reactivity. Nat Neurosci 9: 1362–1363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fisher PM, Price JC, Meltzer CC, Moses-Kolko EL, Becker C, Berga SL et al (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex serotonin 1A and 2A receptor binding interacts to predict threat-related amygdala reactivity. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 1: 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fuller RW (1994). Uptake inhibitors increase extracellular serotonin concentration measured by brain microdialysis. Life Sci 55: 163–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gartside SE, Umbers V, Hajos M, Sharp T (1995). Interaction between a selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonist and an SSRI in vivo: effects on 5-HT cell firing and extracellular 5-HT. Br J Pharmacol 115: 1064–1070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gollan JK, Fava M, Kurian B, Wisniewski SR, Rush AJ, Daly E et al (2012). What are the clinical implications of new onset or worsening anxiety during the first two weeks of SSRI treatment for depression? Depress Anxiety 29: 94–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gommoll C, Forero G, Mathews M, Nunez R, Tang X, Durgam S et al (2015). Vilazodone in patients with generalized anxiety disorder: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, flexible-dose study. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 30: 297–306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Grillon C, Levenson J, Pine DS (2007). A single dose of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram exacerbates anxiety in humans: a fear-potentiated startle study. Neuropsychopharmacology 32: 225–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gross C, Santarelli L, Brunner D, Zhuang X, Hen R (2000). Altered fear circuits in 5-HT(1A) receptor KO mice. Biol Psychiatry 48: 1157–1163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Haddjeri N, Lavoie N, Blier P (2004). Electrophysiological evidence for the tonic activation of 5-HT(1A) autoreceptors in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 1800–1806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hallquist MN, Hwang K, Luna B (2013). The nuisance of nuisance regression: spectral misspecification in a common approach to resting-state fMRI preprocessing reintroduces noise and obscures functional connectivity. Neuroimage 82: 208–225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hammers A, Allom R, Koepp MJ, Free SL, Myers R, Lemieux L et al (2003). Three-dimensional maximum probability atlas of the human brain, with particular reference to the temporal lobe. Hum Brain Mapp 19: 224–247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hendry N, Christie I, Rabiner EA, Laruelle M, Watson J (2011). In vitro assessment of the agonist properties of the novel 5-HT1A receptor ligand, CUMI-101 (MMP), in rat brain tissue. Nucl Med Biol 38: 273–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hinz R, Selvaraj S, Murthy NV, Bhagwagar Z, Taylor M, Cowen PJ et al (2008). Effects of citalopram infusion on the serotonin transporter binding of [11C]DASB in healthy controls. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 28: 1478–1490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hjorth S, Auerbach SB (1996). 5-HT1A autoreceptors and the mode of action of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI). Behav Brain Res 73: 281–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hyttel J (1994). Pharmacological characterization of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). Int Clin Psychopharmacol 9(Suppl 1): 19–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Innis RB, Cunningham VJ, Delforge J, Fujita M, Gjedde A, Gunn RN et al (2007). Consensus nomenclature for in vivo imaging of reversibly binding radioligands. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 27: 1533–1539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Invernizzi R, Belli S, Samanin R (1992). Citalopram's ability to increase the extracellular concentrations of serotonin in the dorsal raphe prevents the drug's effect in the frontal cortex. Brain Res 584: 322–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17: 825–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS, Baker CI (2009). Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat Neurosci 12: 535–540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kumar JS, Parsey RV, Kassir SA, Majo VJ, Milak MS, Prabhakaran J et al (2013). Autoradiographic evaluation of [3H]CUMI-101, a novel, selective 5-HT1AR ligand in human and baboon brain. Brain Res 1507: 11–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kumar JS, Prabhakaran J, Majo VJ, Milak MS, Hsiung SC, Tamir H et al (2007). Synthesis and in vivo evaluation of a novel 5-HT1A receptor agonist radioligand [O-methyl- 11C]2-(4-(4-(2-methoxyphenyl)piperazin-1-yl)butyl)-4-methyl-1,2,4-triazine -3,5(2H,4H)dione in nonhuman primates. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 34: 1050–1060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Milak MS, DeLorenzo C, Zanderigo F, Prabhakaran J, Kumar JS, Majo VJ et al (2010). In vivo quantification of human serotonin 1A receptor using 11C-CUMI-101, an agonist PET radiotracer. J Nucl Med 51: 1892–1900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Milak MS, Severance AJ, Prabhakaran J, Kumar JS, Majo VJ, Ogden RT et al (2011). In vivo serotonin-sensitive binding of [11C]CUMI-101: a serotonin 1A receptor agonist positron emission tomography radiotracer. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 31: 243–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mumford JA, Nichols TE (2008). Power calculation for group fMRI studies accounting for arbitrary design and temporal autocorrelation. Neuroimage 39: 261–268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. O'Nions EJ, Dolan RJ, Roiser JP (2011). Serotonin transporter genotype modulates subgenual response to fearful faces using an incidental task. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 3681–3693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Olfson M, Marcus SC (2009). National patterns in antidepressant medication treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 848–856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Parsey RV, Oquendo MA, Simpson NR, Ogden RT, Van Heertum R, Arango V et al (2002). Effects of sex, age, and aggressive traits in man on brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential measured by PET using [C-11]WAY-100635. Brain Res 954: 173–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Rabiner EA, Messa C, Sargent PA, Husted-Kjaer K, Montgomery A, Lawrence AD et al (2002). A database of [(11)C]WAY-100635 binding to 5-HT(1A) receptors in normal male volunteers: normative data and relationship to methodological, demographic, physiological, and behavioral variables. Neuroimage 15: 620–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ramboz S, Oosting R, Amara DA, Kung HF, Blier P, Mendelsohn M et al (1998). Serotonin receptor 1A knockout: an animal model of anxiety-related disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 14476–14481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Riad M, Watkins KC, Doucet E, Hamon M, Descarries L (2001). Agonist-induced internalization of serotonin-1a receptors in the dorsal raphe nucleus (autoreceptors) but not hippocampus (heteroreceptors). J Neurosci 21: 8378–8386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Richardson-Jones JW, Craige CP, Guiard BP, Stephen A, Metzger KL, Kung HF et al (2010). 5-HT1A autoreceptor levels determine vulnerability to stress and response to antidepressants. Neuron 65: 40–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Richardson-Jones JW, Craige CP, Nguyen TH, Kung HF, Gardier AM, Dranovsky A et al (2011). Serotonin-1A autoreceptors are necessary and sufficient for the normal formation of circuits underlying innate anxiety. J Neurosci 31: 6008–6018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Romero L, Artigas F (1997). Preferential potentiation of the effects of serotonin uptake inhibitors by 5-HT1A receptor antagonists in the dorsal raphe pathway: role of somatodendritic autoreceptors. Journal of neurochemistry 68: 2593–2603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Selvaraj S, Mouchlianitis E, Faulkner P, Turkheimer F, Cowen PJ, Roiser JP et al (2014). Presynaptic serotoninergic regulation of emotional processing: a multimodal brain imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 78: 563–571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Selvaraj S, Turkheimer F, Rosso L, Faulkner P, Mouchlianitis E, Roiser JP et al (2012. a). Measuring endogenous changes in serotonergic neurotransmission in humans: a [11C]CUMI-101 PET challenge study. Mol Psychiatry 17: 1254–1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Selvaraj S, Turkheimer F, Rosso L, Faulkner P, Mouchlianitis E, Roiser JP et al (2012. b). Measuring endogenous changes in serotonergic neurotransmission in humans: a [(11)C]CUMI-101 PET challenge study. Mol Psychiatry 17: 1254–1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sharp T, Boothman L, Raley J, Quérée P (2007). Important messages in the 'post': recent discoveries in 5-HT neurone feedback control. Trends Pharmacol Sci 28: 629–636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sheline YI, Barch DM, Donnelly JM, Ollinger JM, Snyder AZ, Mintun MA (2001). Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study. Biol Psychiatry 50: 651–658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Shrestha SS, Liow JS, Lu S, Jenko K, Gladding RL, Svenningsson P et al (2014). (11)C-CUMI-101, a PET radioligand, behaves as a serotonin 1A receptor antagonist and also binds to α(1) adrenoceptors in brain. J Nucl Med 55: 141–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sinclair LI, Christmas DM, Hood SD, Potokar JP, Robertson A, Isaac A et al (2009). Antidepressant-induced jitteriness/anxiety syndrome: systematic review. Br J Psychiatry 194: 483–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H et al (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl 1): S208–S219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Gibbon M, First MB (2004) Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID-I/P). American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC, USA. [Google Scholar]
  65. Sprouse JS, Aghajanian GK (1987). Electrophysiological responses of serotoninergic dorsal raphe neurons to 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B agonists. Synapse 1: 3–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sramek JJ, Hong WW, Hamid S, Nape B, Cutler NR (1999). Meta-analysis of the safety and tolerability of two dose regimens of buspirone in patients with persistent anxiety. Depress Anxiety 9: 131–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Weiskopf N, Hutton C, Josephs O, Deichmann R (2006). Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: a whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T. Neuroimage 33: 493–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Weiskopf N, Hutton C, Josephs O, Turner R, Deichmann R (2007). Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex: compensation of susceptibility-induced gradients in the readout direction. MAGMA 20: 39–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Woolrich MW, Ripley BD, Brady M, Smith SM (2001). Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. Neuroimage 14: 1370–1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Figure 1

Articles from Neuropsychopharmacology are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES