Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Nov 17;7:15759. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15835-2

The role of forelimb motor cortex areas in goal directed action in mice

Karin Morandell 1, Daniel Huber 1,
PMCID: PMC5693936  PMID: 29150620

Abstract

Mammalian motor cortex consists of several interconnected subregions thought to play distinct roles in voluntary movements, yet their specific role in decision making and execution is not completely elucidated. Here we used transient optogenetic inactivation of the caudal forelimb area (CFA) and rostral forelimb area (RFA) in mice as they performed a directional joystick task. Based on a vibrotactile cue applied to their forepaw, mice were trained to push or pull a joystick after a delay period. We found that choice and execution are temporally segregated processes. CFA and RFA were both essential during the stimulus delivery for correct choice and during the answer period for motor execution. Fine, distal motor deficits were restricted to CFA inactivation. Surprisingly, during the delay period neither area alone, but only combined inactivation was able to affect choice. Our findings suggest transient and partially distributed neural processing of choice and execution across different subregions of the motor cortex.

Introduction

The ability to manipulate objects is a fundamental characteristic of primate1,2 and rodent behavior3,4 and is highly dependent on frontal cortex circuits. The motor cortex can be subdivided into primary and premotor areas57. Premotor areas are found to be active during movement preparation, whereas the primary motor cortex is more likely to be active during movement execution813. In the context of forelimb-related choice behaviors, decision or motor preparation activity has been found to occur in premotor areas11,12 and has been used to reliably predict the upcoming choice11,1416. Premotor areas are therefore thought to be important for processing the upcoming choice or motor planning, whereas the primary motor cortex is thought to be a main driver during the execution of movement, leading to the idea of a hierarchical organization of the motor cortex.

In rodents, two frontal cortical regions are critically involved in controlling the contralateral forelimb: the rostral forelimb area (RFA) and the caudal forelimb area (CFA). They have been identified by electrical1720 and optogenetic microstimulation21,22, as well as by anatomical tracings2325, yet their respective functional role is still unclear. It has been speculated that they are organized in an analogous way to the primate frontal cortex17,19,23,2628 with CFA as the primary motor cortex and RFA as a premotor area.

However, the idea of a hierarchical premotor-motor organization is frequently challenged5,22,29. The arguments are based on the following observations: (1) Descending corticospinal neurons are not restricted to the primary motor cortex, but can also be found in neighboring premotor and somatosensory areas in primates5 and rodents26. (2) Cortical microstimulation of premotor areas triggers movement execution, including grasping or reaching in primates30,31 and rodents1719,22,27,32,33. (3) The type of movement deficits caused by loss-of-function experiments in the rodent motor cortex have been found to be highly variable34,35, which make unclear to what extent the motor or premotor areas are necessary for movement choice or its execution. For example, lesions, pharmacological inactivation, cooling or optogenetic silencing of rodent motor cortex can halt34 or alter movements of the contralateral forelimb19,3640, or leave movements unaffected35. (4) Finally, neuronal recordings during motor tasks failed to show different CFA and RFA activity in rodents4144, suggesting that the two could be involved in similar neural processes.

To gain a better insight into the role of these two motor cortical areas in mice, we developed a two-choice forelimb joystick task in which the motor action was separated from the sensory stimulation by a delay period. CFA and RFA of trained mice were transiently silenced with optogenetic inactivation during different task phases to study their relation to the preparation and execution of a learned sensory motor transformation. If the rodent motor areas are organized analogously to the primate premotor and primary motor areas, we would expect that silencing RFA should affect choice and that silencing CFA would affect motor execution. In contrast, if rodent motor cortex is organized in a more parallel or redundant fashion where choice and execution of a motor plan are distributed among both areas, then combined silencing should affect these functions. Our results reveal the existence of both types of organizations and suggest that both areas are involved in processing choice and execution in a transient and partially distributed manner.

Results

Head-fixed mice can learn delayed response joystick task

To study goal directed forelimb movements in the head-fixed mouse, we developed a forelimb based delayed response task. Mice were trained to push (“push trials”) or pull (“pull trials”) a miniature joystick in response to a high or low frequency vibration applied to the forepaw (Fig. 1A,B, Movie). Sensation and movement execution were temporally separated by a delay period (Fig. 1B, see Methods). After a baseline period (0.5 to 2 s), a vibrotactile stimulus (10 or 40 Hz) was delivered through the joystick specifying which movement direction would be rewarded (vibration period). After a subsequent delay period (1 s), an auditory “go cue” indicated the mouse to respond (answer period) by moving the joystick. Correct response trials (correct direction of the joystick movement) were rewarded with a drop of sweet water and signaled by an auditory cue. Incorrect responses (incorrect direction of the joystick movement) and aborted trials (licking or movement initiation before the go cue) were not rewarded and were followed by a time-out (Fig. 1B).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A delayed response task to investigate motor cortex function in head-fixed mice. (A) Head-fixed mouse in the automated behavioral setup. A vibrotactile stimulus (Vib.) was delivered through the joystick, indicating to push or to pull. Correct movements were rewarded with a drop of water and signaled by a sound cue. A laser beam was directed via a pair of galvanometric mirrors onto the skull and modulated by an acoustic optical modulator (AOM) and a shutter. Laser light was visually masked by a blue LED (Mask). Two high-speed cameras recorded a side- and top-view of the mouse. A real-time behavioral system controlled and recorded events. (B) Timeline of events during a trial. After a baseline period (0.5–2 s, grey background) during which mice were required to refrain from licking or moving the joystick, a vibration of high or low frequency was applied to the forepaw (1 s, light red background). The vibration was followed by a delay (1 s, orange background). The end of the delay was indicated by an auditory “go cue”. During the answer period (green background), mice were given 1 s to pull (downwards triangles) or push (upwards triangles) the joystick. Correct joystick movements (green triangles) were signalled by an auditory reward cue and a droplet of sweet water. Incorrect (red triangles) and anticipated events (grey markers) resulted in a 1 to 3 s time-out. Optogenetic inactivation was limited to the stimulus, delay or answer period. (C) Example learning session raster plots at different performance levels (beginner, intermediate, expert). In intermediate sessions the delay was gradually increased across trials. The trials are sorted by trial outcome. The vertical colored bar on the right of the raster refers to the outcome of each trial. Colors and symbols as in (B). (D and E) Learning curves showing the discrimination ability d’ (D) and proportion of answered trials (correct and incorrect trials, E) across learning sessions. Black error bars represent the mean ± SEM of 10 animals. Fine grey lines: individual mouse performance. The difference in performance between early (1 to 10) and late sessions (31 to 40) was statistically tested by using a paired t test on animal averages. (F and G) Histograms showing average number of trials completed per learning session (F) and session duration (G) for the 10 trained mice.

During training, task difficulty was progressively increased (Fig. 1C, intermediate session, Methods) and correct movement choice was computed with an adapted version of the sensitivity index d’ (see Methods), measuring the ability to discriminate between trial types. All mice learned to perform the task (d’ >1, Fig. 1D, i.e. >70% correct trials, Fig. 1–1). Mice reached criterion performance in 27 ± 10 (mean ± SEM) learning sessions (Fig. 1D), ranging from 19 to 49 sessions. The proportion of answered trials (correct or incorrect) increased with training and plateaued at 70% after 25 ± 6 (mean ± SEM) sessions (Fig. 1E). Mice performed 498 ± 8 (mean ± SEM) trials in daily sessions lasting 57 ± 0.7 minutes (mean ± SEM, Fig. 1F,G).

Choice and execution are temporally segregated processes

After mice reached criterion performance, we tested if and at what moment CFA and RFA (contralateral to the “active” paw, Methods) are involved in the different aspects of the behavioral task. To perform transcranial optogenetic silencing of the contralateral forelimb motor cortex areas we used transgenic mice expressing channelrhodopsin-2 in cortical inhibitory interneurons (VGAT-ChR2-EYFP)45 in combination with a blue light (473 nm) laser scanning system (Fig. 1A, Methods). The laser light reached the brain through a translucent head cap and the intact skull45. A pseudo-random set of 1/3 of the trials was used for silencing (n = 9 mice, Methods). We first assessed if CFA and RFA are involved in the task by simultaneously inactivating both areas (Fig. 2A,B,D, Methods). When we optogenetically silenced these areas during the vibration or the delay period, mice made significantly more incorrect joystick movements compared to control trials (Fig. 2A,B, vibration period: d’Vib = 0.5 ± 0.2, mean ± SEM, p = 0.005, delay period: d’Del = 0.9 ± 0.2, p = 0.025, n = 5), but the probability of producing a motor response was not affected (Fig. 2D). In contrast, simultaneous inactivation during the answer period significantly impaired motor execution, reducing the number of responded trials to 38.6 ± 6% (Fig. 2D, mean ± SEM, pAns = 0.004, n = 5). In the remaining inactivated trials, in which mice overcame the inactivation effect, mice performed shorter joystick movements (Figs 21A,B, Fig. 2–2A,B) and at a slower speed (Figs 21C,D, Fig. 2–2C,D) without affecting movement onset (Figs 21E,F, Fig. 2–2E,F). Interestingly they produced, on average, the correct motor responses (Fig. 2B, d’Ans = 1.4 ± 0.4 compared to d’Off = 2.1 ± 0.2, p = 0.285). Taken together, these results suggest a double dissociation during different phases of motor tasks: before the “go cue”, motor cortex is necessary for choosing or preparing the appropriate motor plan (without affecting the drive for the upcoming motor output), while after the “go cue”, cortical drive is important for executing the movement (but no more for selecting appropriate motor plan).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Forelimb motor cortex is necessary for choosing and executing joystick movements. (A) Example inactivation session raster sorted by trial outcome and inactivation condition (top, control trials; bottom, inactivated trials). The abscissa represents time from stimulus onset. Blue rectangles represent silencing period. In this example, CFA and RFA were silenced simultaneously. Markers (triangles for joystick events and dots for lick events) and vertical bars’ color code refers to the trial outcome (same than Figs 1 and 2). Note that silencing motor cortex early (vibration and delay period) changed mice’ joystick direction choice, while inactivation during the answer period didn’t affects execution of joystick movements. (B to E) Comparison of performance in control trials with performance in different inactivation conditions. Simultaneous inactivation of RFA and CFA (B,D) decreased the discrimination ability (B) and proportion of answered trials (D) while silencing other cortical areas (C and E, e.g. the barrel cortex (BCx), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the posterior parietal cortex (PPC)) did not change the performance compared to control trials (C,E). (B,D) Black error bars represent the mean ± SEM of 5 animals. Individual performances of mice: fine grey lines. (C,E) Colored error bars represent the mean ± SEM performance for control trials and silenced trials (purple, ACC; blue, BCx; magenta, PPC). For tests of significance for this and all other figures see Methods and Table 1.

Is this effect specific to forelimb related motor areas or does the perturbation of neighboring cortical activity cause similar effects? We silenced three neighboring cortical areas: the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the barrel cortex (BCx). In rodents, ACC has been involved in effort based decision making46,47 and inactivation of ACC could affect the behavior if different values are associated with both actions. PPC plays a role in sensory guided choice15,4850. Finally, the barrel cortex was chosen as a neighboring control area because it is a major sensory input system in rodents and disturbing its activity could act as a salient distractor. We found that when silencing ACC, PPC or BCx, neither d’ (Fig. 2C, see Table 1 for p values) nor the proportion of answered trials (Fig. 2E, see Table 1 for p values) were affected. This suggests that the neuronal activity in neighboring areas such as the ACC, PPC or BCx only plays a minor role during our behavioral task. Since RFA is also partially overlapping with a region termed ALM (anterior lateral motor cortex) which has been shown to control directional licking behavior and tongue movements22,45,51,52, we also analyzed if the inactivation affected licking dynamics in our task. We found that the timing of the licks and the licking rate (see Methods) were not affected by the inactivation of RFA nor CFA (Figs 31), confirming the results of others42. These results confirm that that the frontal cortex areas CFA and RFA play an important role in the execution and preparation of movements, compared to neighboring cortical areas.

Table 1.

p values for Fig. 2C,E. PA, Proportion of answered trials.

Inactivated region Behavioral measure Figure p values N test post hoc
Vibration period Delay period Answer period
ACC d’ 2C p > 0.999 p > 0.999 no data 3 Kruskal-Wallis Tukey-Kramer
PA 2E p = 0.988 p = 0.988
PPC d’ 2C p = 0.952 p = 0.988
PA 2E p = 0.988 p = 0.822
BCx d’ 2C p > 0.999 p = 0.972 p = 0.933 9
PA 2E p = 0.999 p = 0.999 p = 0.983

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The specific role of CFA and RFA depends on the task period. Comparison between performance in control trials (e.g. Off, grey) and trials silenced on single areas at different periods: vibration period (red, A and D), delay period (orange, B and E) and answer period (green, C and F). Black error bars represent the mean ± SEM of the discrimination ability (A to C) and proportion of answered trials (D to F). Fine grey lines: individual mouse performance. Only combined inactivation (CFA and RFA) results in a significant decrease in performance in the delay period.

The effects of single area inactivations

To dissect the roles of RFA and CFA individually, we inactivated them separately during all three phases of the task. Similarly to the combined inactivation, silencing either CFA or RFA during the vibration period led to significantly less correct responses (Fig. 3A, p(CFA)< 0.001, p(RFA) = 0.009, n = 9) while the proportion of answered trials was unchanged compared to control conditions (Fig. 3D, p(CFA) = 0.389, p(RFA) = 0.272, n = 9). Activity in CFA and RFA is therefore necessary during the vibration period for choosing the correct answer.

During the delay period, individual inactivation of CFA or RFA was not sufficient to affect choice (Fig. 3A, p(CFA) = 0.130, p(RFA) = 0.181, p(CFA + RFA) = 0.025, n = 9), in contrast to the combined inactivation which significantly decreased d’ (Figs 2B and 3B). Subsequent motor execution was not affected by single or combined inactivation during the delay period (Fig. 3E, PA(Off) = 84 ± 2.6%, PA(CFA) = 86.1 ± 2.8%, PA(RFA) = 83.5 ± 2.3%, PA(CFA + RFA) = 86.4 ± 1.7%; mean ± SEM, p(CFA) = 0.342, p(RFA) = 0.766, p(CFA + RFA) = 0.894, n = 9). Together, these results confirm that both CFA and RFA are involved in processing choice before the “go cue”, yet in different ways: while during the vibration period, either area is necessary for correct choice outcome, only combined inactivation affects choice during the delay period.

Finally, we analyzed how inactivating CFA or RFA during the answer period affected choice and movement execution. We found that silencing either CFA or RFA significantly decreased the proportion of answered trials (Fig. 3F, CFA: 83.6 ± 2.8% to 58.2 ± 6.9%, p(CFA) = 0.003, RFA: 66.0 ± 6.3%, p(RFA) = 0.026, n = 9), yet the faction of correct choices remained the same (Fig. 3C). Taken together, after the “go cue” CFA and RFA therefore seem to be both involved in initiating or controlling the movement of the forepaw, while choice can no longer be affected.

However, indirect measurements such as the response rate or joystick trajectories might not be sufficient to capture fine differences between CFA and RFA inactivation. In fact, deficits in dexterity or digit closure have been observed after cortical lesions in rodents4 and primates2. We therefore carried out detailed analysis of the forepaw movement using videography. We found that paw movements were prevented in 20.3 ± 5.0% in CFA inactivated trials and to 8.9 ± 3.6% in RFA inactivated trials compared to only 5.0 ± 1.5% in control trials. In the remaining trials (in which the paw still moved) subtle, yet significant changes in the forepaw trajectory and closure were specific to CFA inactivation (Fig. 4), suggesting a distinct role of CFA in the control of more distal forelimb movements.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

CFA inactivation causes fine motor deficits. (A to D) Profile views of head-fixed mice illustrating fine movement impairments: digit trajectories (A), paw “slips” (B), paw drops (C) and digit extension (D). A, Digit trajectories from the go cue to the end of the answer period. The paw position was quantified by tracking the vertical position of digits relative to the baseline position of the joystick. (B) Paw “slips” were scored if two digits laterally slid off the joystick, as illustrated on the zoomed paw bottom image. (C) Example paw drop. Paw drops were counted when the paw dropped below the handle of the joystick. (D) Example digit extension (paw opened and digits spread). (E to H) Population data of changes in paw closure in control (Off) and inactivated trials (CFA or RFA during the answer period, n = 5 mice). Black error bars represent the mean ± SEM of the measured movement impairment. Fine grey lines: values of individual mice. Note that CFA inactivation is sufficient to cause paw closure deficits while RFA is not.

Taken together, our results suggest first, that there is a switch in neuronal processing at the moment of the “go cue”, where correct choice can only be affected by manipulating neuronal activity before, and motor execution only after the beginning of the answer period (Figs 2, 3 and 5). Second, CFA and RFA are both involved in movement execution (Fig. 3F), yet fine, distal motor deficits are specific to CFA inactivation (Figs 4 and 5). Finally, correct choice (Figs 3 and 5) and many coarse aspects of movement kinematics (Figs 2–1, 2–2 and 5) are only affected by combined inactivation, indicating that there might exist considerable functional redundancy across CFA and RFA.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Summary of CFA and RFA inactivation. (A) Temporal distribution of putative cortical functions. D-prime was chosen as a measure of discrimination ability describing the process through which the presentation of a sensory stimulus ultimately leads to one of two movements, while coarse and fine motor execution were estimated by the proportion of answered trials and the proportion of paw “slips”, respectively. (B) Inactivation results summary matrix. Columns correspond to inactivation periods (vibration, delay and answer) and rows to inactivation locations (simultaneous CFA and RFA, CFA alone and RFA alone inactivation). The color code is explained in panel C. Note that discrimination ability and execution are sequentially affected. The ability to choose or plan the appropriate movement is mostly impaired when inactivating during the sensation and delay period (pink) while execution was only affected when inactivation was triggered during the movement (green). Also note that discrimination was most significantly impaired when CFA and RFA were silenced simultaneously during the delay period while inactivating the different forelimb areas individually showed similar non-significant weaker effects. Finally, the fine digit movements were altered by CFA inactivation only (% “slips” but also see Fig. 4) while overall joystick movement execution was not differentially affected by CFA or RFA inactivation (see Figs 21 and 3F). (C) Color code used in (B). Axes represent p values for comparison between control and inactivated trials. The y axis represents the discrimination ability (d’ p value from Fig. 3) and the x axis represents the p values for coarse and fine motor deficits measured by the proportion of answered trials (PA p value from Fig. 3) and the proportion of “slips” (p value from Fig. 4F), respectively. Axes are in a logarithmic scale.

Discussion

A novel forelimb task for mice

Here we studied the role of different frontal cortical areas in a novel delayed joystick movement task for head-fixed mice. Using forepaw movements instead of licking15,45,51,53 as motor output, has several advantages. Although licking is a motor modality that facilitates training, it complicates the interpretation of neuronal data because it is performed instinctively by mice and is concomitant to reward consumption. Furthermore, to what extent insights gained from licking circuits can be extended to understanding the selection and execution of other goal directed actions, such as forelimb movements is unclear. Therefore, forelimb specific tasks for head-fixed rodents involving a manipulandum or a “joystick” are desirable and have started recently to emerge39,42,44,54,55. The most closely related behavioral paradigms to our study consist of rodents moving a lever spontaneously or upon a cue presentation to obtain a reward39,42,44,54,55. In these tasks, rodents are not required to select one of two actions, limiting the possibility of studying different motor plans.

Inspired by primate work11,13,5658, we also separated stimulus from action with a delay period, thus allowing to independently investigate functions such as choice (or movement planning), execution and reward consumption. In our study, we have used “choice” as an umbrella-term (quantified by d’) to refer to the process through which the presentation of a sensory cue ultimately leads to the execution of one of two possible movements to obtain a reward (see Discussion in ref.59). An important aspect of the task was that the time required to be learned was similar, if not shorter, to comparable tasks in primates9,13. Also, our task enables mice to perform a high number of trials per session (498 ± 8 total trials per session, Fig. 1F), which is desirable for reliably interpreting behavioral results59. The symmetric, two-choice task design also makes the performance less sensitive to fluctuations in the engagement of the subjects59. Finally, head-fixation allows precise control of the environment facilitating the behavioral readout of fine movement deficits (Fig. 4) or the observation of subtle postural adjustments (see Discussion15,60) and makes the task suitable for neurophysiology experiments (e.g. two-photon imaging or electrophysiological recordings).

Inactivation strategy and effects

Studies comparing the relative contribution of RFA and CFA to forelimb movements are still rare and use different loss-of-function strategies19,34,37,61. Here, we used optogenetic inactivation to precisely and transiently target different areas of the dorsal cortex in a non invasive manner. The optimal silencing protocol should completely silence the targeted area without affecting the surrounding areas. However, CFA and RFA differ in thickness and size33,62 and incomplete silencing could non significantly affect behavior. Our silencing approach is therefore a trade-off between spatial resolution and significance of behavioral effects. We chose 9 mW as inactivation power of all single areas (CFA, RFA, PPC, ACC, BCx) to affect similar amount of cortical tissue and ensure sufficient coverage of CFA to obtain a significant behavioral effect (Figs 13D–F). Silencing single areas with lower power (Figs 13E a 3 mW, p = 0.223) and silencing of areas surrounding the putative location of CFA (Figs 13F, p = 0.15) did not lead to significant effects. The lower efficacy in affecting behavior with moderate power might be due to the remaining active neurons of the CFA network (Figs 13C). For the combined inactivation of CFA and RFA, the laser was switched between the two sites (see Methods). The laser power was set to 3 mW per site to achieve higher spatial specificity and ensure minimal overlap between CFA and RFA inactivation areas. Using this approach, we found that even at 3 mW per site, the combined inactivation of RFA and CFA significantly affected behavior. In contrast, three times more power did not affect the behavioral performance consistently during the delay period (Fig. 3B). In general, combined silencing effects at lower power were in most cases stronger than single site inactivation (Table 2). This could imply that the specific combination of CFA and RFA (even at reduced laser power), and not just the extended inactivation are sufficient to produce the behavioral effects.

Table 2.

Comparison between double and single area inactivation. p values of the difference in differences analysis are also reported directly on figures.

Figure # Measure Inactivation period Region 1 Region 2 p value
2-1B movement amplitude answer CFA + RFA CFA 0.054
2-1B movement amplitude answer CFA + RFA RFA 0.049
2-1D movement speed answer CFA + RFA CFA 0.043
2-1D movement speed answer CFA + RFA RFA 0.022
3A d-prime vibration CFA + RFA CFA 0.280
3A d-prime vibration CFA + RFA RFA 0.098
3B d-prime delay CFA + RFA CFA 0.197
3B d-prime delay CFA + RFA RFA 0.128
3F percent answered answer CFA + RFA CFA 0.039
3F percent answered answer CFA + RFA RFA 0.006
3-3A percent correct vibration CFA + RFA CFA 0.451
3-3A percent correct vibration CFA + RFA RFA 0.134
3-3B percent correct delay CFA + RFA CFA 0.158
3-3B percent correct delay CFA + RFA RFA 0.100

Distinct roles of motor cortical areas

The role of the motor cortex in the context of controlling goal directed limb movements is still debated, partly because several levels of deficits have been described in inactivation studies. Movements were found to be unaffected35,40 after cortical lesion and altered4,19,37,39,40 or completely halted34,51,63 upon motor cortex inactivation. This diversity of effects might be due to differences in task structure, loss-of-function methods or brain regions. Furthermore, neuronal activity recorded in CFA and RFA in behaving rodents41,42,44 encoded similar information, although CFA neurons seem to discharge after RFA neurons in a phasic short burst during forelimb tasks64. In our study, we have combined a novel task with a rapid and spatially restricted inactivation method for a finer dissection of frontal cortex function. We found that CFA and RFA (Fig. 2B,D) were involved in the completion of the task, in contrast to surrounding areas (Fig. 2C,E). During the answer period, inactivation of either area significantly disrupted motor execution, whereas choice was not affected (Figs 2D and 3C,F). Vice-versa, choice was only significantly affected during vibration and delay period (Fig. 2A–C). This implies that there is a change in function, switching from processing choice to motor execution at the time of the “go cue”. Such temporal specificity across the different phases of the behavioral task is difficult to reveal with other inactivation methods.

The effects of CFA or RFA inactivation on choice during the vibration period (Fig. 3A) also illustrate the rapid emergence of a motor plan during stimulus presentation. These results confirm observations from recent loss-of-function studies in delayed discrimination tasks15,65,66 where the choice of upcoming movements were altered when inactivation occurred during the stimulus presentation. However, our findings differ from a whisker based pole localization task, where choice was not affected during the sensory period inactivation (using lower laser power)45. The somatosensory forelimb area (SFA) and CFA are partially overlapping (Figs 13A and F33,42,) and strongly interconnected67. The inactivation of SFA might therefore account for the increase in wrong choices during the sensory period. Nevertheless, the existence of trial type specific preparatory activity in CFA during the stimulus presentation suggests that CFA might participate to the discrimination between trial types68. On the other hand, inactivating RFA, which is more distant from the sensory representation, still significantly affected choice thereby highlighting the important role of motor areas in choosing the correct action.

Based on the CFA-RFA connectivity pattern2325,27,28 and intracortical microstimulation studies1720,27,33 CFA might be considered as primary motor area and RFA as a premotor area (however also see refs19,34,44). Indeed we found that fine movement execution deficits were specific to CFA inactivation (Fig. 4), confirming CFA lesion studies performed in rats4,36,38 and primary motor cortex in primates1,69. This result is in accordance with a rat cortical lesion study in which reaching skills and their recovery were more strongly impaired by CFA lesions than RFA, suggesting that CFA is more critical for skilled forelimb behavior61. However, movement initiation or coarse motor deficits were observed across both areas (Fig. 3). Whether these changes were caused by loss of motor control, loss of muscle tone40,44 or the change of sensory feedback (due to the close proximity to the sensory forelimb area, see Figs 13F), will have to be addressed in future experiments.

Whereas our results reveal an important role of CFA and RFA in sensory processing and motor control of the forelimb, other neighboring cortical areas, such as PPC15,4850 or ACC46,47 have also been found to be involved in action selection in rodents. Our results do not provide evidence that either PPC or ACC play a major role in this task. Actually, recent studies suggest that the PPC network might be specifically involved in visual information processing during decision making49,50, while not necessarily recruited for maintaining the choice during a delay15,70.

Distributed processing across motor areas

Our experiments also revealed differences in effects between the silencing of individual and combined areas (Table 2). For example, during the answer period behavioral performance (Fig. 3F) and specific movement kinematics (Figs 21B,D) were less strongly affected by individual area inactivation than combined inactivation, even at lower laser powers. Similarly, during the delay period, discrimination performance was robust to single CFA or RFA inactivation (Fig. 3B). The non significant or significantly lower effects during single location inactivation could be due to compensatory responses of other brain areas or a redundant role of CFA and RFA. Functional compensations have been described in several species. Studies in primates and rodents show that after a stroke in “primary” areas the partial recovery of the grasping ability can be lost after a transient pharmacological inactivation of premotor areas2,69,7173. Furthermore, studies in primates and rodents show extensive reorganization of premotor areas after lesions or strokes in primary and secondary areas18,61,71,72,74. This plasticity also correlates with the degree of rehabilitation18,63 and it is suggested to be crucial for functional recovery of movements2,61,71,7476. However, these compensation mechanisms take place over longer time scales. Determining whether fast compensatory mechanisms take place upon inactivation necessitate simultaneous silencing and neuronal recording experiments across multiple areas. Recently, Li et al. elegantly used this strategy and revealed interhemispheric compensation between anterior lateral motor cortices (ALM)77 in mice. Similar to the ALM areas, CFA and RFA are densely and directly interconnected23,24,67. In analogy, the weaker behavioral changes during single area inactivation might reflect compensation or redundancy between CFA and RFA.

Finally, transient manipulation of cortical circuits can potentially disrupt the activity of downstream or connected areas (e.g. basal ganglia or other cortical areas) needed to perform this task. Such “off-target” effects78 or diaschisis can only be identified by combining silencing with simultaneous neuronal recordings, allowing monitoring local effects as well as activity changes in downstream circuits.

In conclusion, we demonstrated that CFA and RFA are both involved in motor execution and selecting the appropriate motor plans. Individual inactivation of each area at different moments of the task suggest that both areas are recruited early during the sensory period participating in the sensory discrimination or leading to motor plan selection. During the delay period the behavior is relatively robust to single area inactivation, suggesting a distributed function among the motor system. Finally, during the execution phase both regions are necessary, while CFA has a specific role in fine movement control. Simultaneously recording and manipulating neuronal activity in forelimb areas in behaving animals will be necessary to determine if and how compensatory mechanisms can explain the observed behaviors.

Methods

Animals

Experiments were performed on 12 VGAT-ChR2-eYFP mice (B6.Cg-Tg(Slc32a1-COP4*H134R/EYFP)8Gfng/J, Jackson Laboratory, http://www.jax.org, RRID IMSR _JAX:014548) (see details in Tables 3 and 4). The animals were bred in hemizygous colonies and used for the experiments between 2 and 9 months of age. Mice from the three different litters were used. They were housed in single cages containing a cardboard house and bedding material, in a normal light cycle room that was dark from 7:00 P.M. to 7 A.M. Water reward based training and experiments occurred during the light phase. To motivate mice to engage in and learn the behavioral task, they were limited to 1 ml/day of water for ≥10 days before training79. On training days, mice received up to 1 mL of water or sweetened water (10% sucrose) during the experimental session. The consumed water volume was determined by weighing the mice before and after a session. If mice had gained less than 1 ml, they were complemented more than an hour later. Food was available ad libitum. The weight and health (posture, appearance of the fur and motor activity) were monitored daily. If mice did not maintain a sufficient body weight (below 80% of their original weight), additional water was provided. All procedures were carried out in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Geneva and with permission of the cantonal authorities.

Table 3.

Details about mice used for behavior and optogenetic experiments.

Behavior mice Mean ± SEM
1116 1117 1119 1120 1130 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580
Weight before water restriction (g) 24 23.5 23.8 25.1 25 27.8 28.6 26 28.7 26 25.8 ± 0.6
# of learning sessions 57 46 62 48 53 47 46 47 47 47 50.0 ± 1.7
# of photoinactivation sessions 32 55 8 56 46 35 42 33 34 40 38.1 ± 4.3

Table 4.

Mice appearing in this paper.

Figures Figure # Behavior mice Electrophys. mice
1116 1117 1119 1120 1130 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580 5227 5228
Learning raster 1C X
Learning parameters 1D-G X X X X X X X X X X
Silencing raster 2A X
Performance is affected by CFA and RFA inactivation 2B + 2D X X X X X
Performance is not affected by inactivation of BCx 2C + E X X X X X X X X X X
Performance is not affected by inactivation of ACC and PPC 2C + E X X X
Motor cortex function is temporally segregated but spatially redundant 3 X X X X X X X X X X
CFA inactivation causes fine motor deficits 4 X X X X X
Summary of CFA and RFA inactivation 5 X X X X X X X X X X
Transcranial optogenetic silencing spatial properties 1-1 X X X X X X X
Detailed analysis of movement dynamics 2-1 X X X X X
Distribution of movement kinematics 2-2 X X X X X
Motor cortex inactivation effects are restricted to the silenced trial 2-3 X X X X X X X X X X
Licking is not affected by forelimb motor cortex inactivation 3-1 X X X X X X X X X X
Movies X

Surgical procedure for transcranial optogenetic cortical silencing

For the purpose of head fixation and optical access, mice were implanted with a small titanium head post and the skull was covered with a transparent cap45. Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (3% and 1.5% by volume in O2, for anesthesia induction and maintenance respectively; Isoflurane Attane, Provet AG) and mounted in a stereotaxic device equipped with a custom made heating pad to maintain the body temperature. Eyes were covered with Vaseline. Prior to the surgery, mice received anti-inflammatory (2.5 mg per kg body weight dexamethasone intramuscular), analgesic (0.1 mg/kg buprenorphine intramuscular, 5 mg/kg Carprofen subcutaneous) and local anesthetic drugs (1% lidocaine subcutaneous under the scalp). After removing the skin above the skull, a thin layer of cyanoacrylate glue was applied to the dried skull. The head post was cemented to the interparietal bone and the area over the skull was covered with a thin layer of transparent dental acrylic (Lang Dental). After curing, the dental acrylic was polished and covered with a layer of cyanoacrylate glue to smooth the surface and thereby render it transparent.

Behavioral setup/Apparatus

To precisely control and record behavioral events, we used an automated behavioral control system (Fig. 1A)53. The behavioral setup was enclosed in a custom-made light and sound proof box. Mice were placed inside a horizontal aluminum tube (32 mm internal diameter) and head-fixed by the titanium head-bar with custom clamps located in front of the tube. The tube was cut to allow mice to freely move their forelimbs. For body support, mice placed their left forelimb on a static paw rest and their right forelimb on a movable joystick. The joystick was custom made from a 1.3 mm diameter rod connected to a rotary encoder (Miniature Absolute Magnetic Shaft Encoder MA3 12 bit, www.usdigital.com). Two pairs of repulsive magnets (S-04-1.5-N, supermagnete.ch) provided the spring load to the joystick. The vibrotactile stimuli (10 Hz or 40 Hz sinusoidal) were delivered to the base of the joystick using a speaker coil. The joystick position from the rotary encoder was decoded with a microcontroller (Arduino Due, www.arduino.cc) and recorded with an analog/digital acquisition board (acquisition rate 5 kHz, PCI-MIO-16E-4, National Instrument). The real-time control system was connected through Ethernet to a MATLAB (Mathworks, RRID:SCR _001622) user interface computer (more details in53). An electrical lickport was placed in front of the snout and licks were detected as binary events80. Liquid reward delivery was controlled by a solenoid valve (The Lee Company). High-speed video (60 frames per second) was acquired through USB cameras (Firefly MV USB Monochrome Camera, Point Grey) using custom MATLAB routines. Potential sounds produced by the vibrotactile stimuli were masked by white noise delivered during stimulus presentation. A “go” cue (“beep” sound) to signal the movement period onset and a “reward” cue (“click” sound) to signal a correct behavioral response were delivered by using a buzzer (5 kHz, 80 ms) and a speaker (single 20 ms square pulse), respectively.

Behavior

Task structure

The trial structure was designed to separate stimulus presentation from action by a delay period (Fig. 1B). Two trial types were possible (“pull” and “push” trials). The trials started by a 0.5 to 2 s long baseline period in which mice were required not to move the joystick. The trial onset was indicated by turning on a blue mask LED (470 nm). A vibrotactile stimulus (10 or 40 Hz) was delivered through the joystick to the right forepaw for 1 second followed by a delay period of 1 second. The end of the delay period was indicated by an auditory “go cue” (buzzer 80 ms, 5 kHz) and informed the mice that they could pull or push the joystick to report their choice with their right paw. Answer period was 1 second long. If mice moved the joystick to the correct direction, a “click” sound notified the mice that a ∼4 uL sweet water droplet (10% sucrose) was available upon licking to restrict licking to reward periods. If mice moved before the go cue or licked before moving the joystick, the blue mask LED turned off, the mice were not rewarded and received a 2 to 3 s time-out before the next trial was started. The time-out was reinitialized if additional movements (joystick or licks) were detected. In order to prevent choice bias observed in similar behavioral paradigms45,81, the trials were chosen based on a fixed set of rules during training and inactivation experiments. The sequence of trial types was random except in the following two situations. First, if 3 consecutive trials were of the same type, the next trial type was switched. Second, if mice did not respond correctly to one trial type (more than 4–6 out of 9 trials), that trial type was repeated. This procedure resulted in a minimal bias towards the end of the training (−0.36 ± 4.82%, mean ± SEM). The bias was computed as percent correct in push trials minus percent correct in pull trials.

Behavioral training procedure

Mice were trained to learn the behavioral task progressively. The training started after 5 to 7 short daily handling sessions (5 to 10 minutes). The first 3 to 5 training sessions consisted in a head-fixed pre-training period designed to (1) habituate mice to the behavioral setup, (2) teach mice that water is delivered by the lickport, (3) associate the “click” sound (reward cue) with reward availability upon lick, (4) discriminate between the reward cue and the stimulus and (5) prevent mice to lick except during reward periods. The joystick was immobilized for this part of the training. The pre-training was structured as a Go/No-Go task. In No-Go trials, one of the two vibrotactile stimuli was delivered and mice were required to withhold licking behavior. Correct No Go trials increased the probability of Go trials. In Go trials, the reward cue sound would be concomitant with the delivery of a ∼10 uL % sucrose water drop (“learning click trials”), or a 1.5 s period in which the mouse could be rewarded only if it licked upon the reward cue (“testing click trials”). The proportion of “learning click trials” was decreased progressively as performance improved. After this training phase, mice associated the reward cue with reward availability upon lick and showed moderated spontaneous licking. In the next phase, mice were required to associate the vibrotactile stimuli with a joystick movement (pulling or pushing). The vibrotactile stimulus was followed by a 50 ms delay period, after which an auditory go cue signalled the end of the withholding period and that mice were allowed to pull or push the joystick. The duration of the delay period was progressively increased within the training session (by 50 ms at each correct trial and up to 1 s) and across learning sessions (initial delay duration was the mean delay of the previous session). Similarly, the required joystick movement amplitude to receive a reward was increased progressively (1 mm to 10 mm) within and across learning sessions. The final required movement amplitude for push or pull trials was 10 mm and sub-threshold movements were ignored. Premature (before the go cue) joystick movements or licking (before the reward cue) resulted in a 1 to 3 s time-out before the next trial. During the learning period, the trial type sequence was automatically chosen to correct for potential response bias.

Transcranial optogenetic cortical silencing

Inactivation trials were randomly intermingled with control unperturbed trials with a probability of 30 to 50%. Light from a 473 nm laser (DHOM, DHL-100A) was controlled by an acousto-optical modulator (AOM; MTS110-A3-VIS, Quanta Tech) and a shutter (Vincent Associates) (Fig. 1A). The AOM provided analog control of laser intensity. Light exiting the AOM was injected into a single mode optical fiber (3.0 μm @ 480 nm; P1-405B-FC-2; Thorlabs) and a 2D galvanometer scanning system (“scanner”, GVSM002, Thorlabs) via a lens system (PAF-X-11-PC-A and PAF-X-5-A; Thorlabs). The mirrors of the scanner, the shutter and the AOM were controlled by the real-time behavioral control system45. Light from the scanner passed through a 5x beam expander and was focused onto the skull surface with a f = 200 mm lens (AC508-200-A, Thorlabs). The laser had a Gaussian profile with a beam diameter of approximately 300 μm. Laser power was checked on a daily basis using a handheld power meter (Lasercheck, Coherent). To inactivate motor cortical areas on the left hemisphere (contralateral to the right, active paw), the laser beam was directed to the target coordinates by the scanning mirrors and the power was modulated by an AOM. To perform simultaneous CFA and RFA inactivation, the beam was displaced between the two targets at a 40 Hz frequency and the laser was blanked with the AOM in between the targets. To deprive mice from visual cues originating from the inactivation laser, a “masking” light was turned on at the beginning of every trial using a 470 nm LED (Luxeon Star) close to the eyes of the mice and controlled by a driver circuit (Luxdrive, Buckpuck 3021). The masking light82 began 0.5 to 2 s before the stimulus period and lasted 5 s. The light was turned off simultaneously to the initiation of the time-out.

Stereotaxic coordinates

The coordinates for optogenetic silencing were based on published studies using intracortical tracers injection24,67, motor maps generated by intracortical microstimulation experiments22,42: CFA (ML = 1.8, AP = 0.8), RFA (ML = 0.9, AP = 2.7), BCx (ML = 3.5, AP = −1.5), ACC (ML = 0.15, AP = 0), PPC (ML = 1.7, AP = −2, in millimeter, ML: mediolateral, AP: anteroposterior, relative to bregma, contralateral hemisphere). To ensure that the stereotaxic coordinates from the literature were reliable, we used the location of the somatosensory representation of the forelimb (SFA) as a second reference point in addition to bregma. The coordinate of SFA were detected by performing intrinsic signal imaging through the transparent skull cap (see Methods82, data not shown).

Electrophysiological recordings for laser power calibration

To evaluate the spatial and functional properties of the transcranial optogenetic cortical silencing technique, we recorded neuronal activity in CFA while probing the surrounding areas with the 470 nm laser beam. Extracellular spikes were monitored using silicon probes (Neuronexus, A1 × 32–10 mm-50–413) under awake, non-behaving conditions in 2 mice. Action potentials were detected, amplified, multiplexed, digitized and recorded at 20 kHz with Open Ephys hardware and software (http://open-ephys.org). Mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane and a 1 mm craniotomy (2 mm lateral and 1 mm anterior to bregma) was performed to target CFA. Before lowering it into the brain, the silicon probe was covered with DiI (Thermofisher, V22885) to mark the location of the recording sites and was inserted with a 30° angle in the coronal plane. After the silicon probe was in place for 30 minutes, electrophysiological recording started and the laser illumination was triggered every 7 s for 1 s at different preset distances from the silicon probe. Optogenetic inactivation was achieved by the transmission of the laser light through the skull and translucent dental cement, like in the behavioral experiments. The power was varied randomly (1, 2, 3.5, 5 and 9 mW) at every location. The probed distances were 0, 0.5, 1, 1.5 and 2 mm from the recording location. The 2 mm distance condition corresponded to the location of RFA as illustrated in Fig. 11A. In total, we obtained 12.2 ± 0.9 repetitions per location and laser intensity for 32 units.

Extracellular recordings were bandpass filtered (300–3000 Hz). Spike detection and cluster classification was performed offline by using Wave _Clus (https://github.com/csn-le/wave_clus). We extracted 32 units from 2 animals. Units were then classified according to the response to laser illumination as enhanced (putative VGAT + , interneurons) or silenced units (putative VGAT-, pyramidal neurons). Movement and other artifacts detected as simultaneous electrical deflections on all the electrodes were used to exclude trials from the analysis. Effect of laser illumination of ongoing neuronal activity was quantified as “normalized firing rate” by dividing the spike rate during the first second of laser illumination to the firing rate of the preceding second. The recorded neuronal signals and unit classification were visually inspected for each trial.

Laser power calibration

Based on the electrophysiological calibration experiment (Figs 13A) and pilot behavioural data (Figs 13D–F) we chose 9 mW per site as default power for all single area inactivation experiments (CFA, RFA, ACC, PPC, BCx). At 9 mW, the average spiking activity dropped to 20.3 ± 5.3% compared to baseline, while affecting most of the surface area considered as CFA or RFA (Figs 13C)33,42, yet still allowing spatial specificity (Figs 13F). For combined inactivation experiments, the silencing power was 3 mW per site (6 mW in total) (see Discussion).

Behavioral analysis

Joystick movement kinematics

To track the paw movement on the joystick, the rotary encoder signals were recorded at 5 kHz. Trajectory signals were filtered with a 4th-order Butterworth filter with a low pass cut-off frequency of 12 Hz and offset corrected by subtracting the average pre-stimulus period (baseline). Joystick trajectories were analyzed in a semi-automated manner using a custom MATLAB script and joystick kinematics were computed: amplitude (Figs 41A), movement speed (Figs 41C) and movement onset (Figs 41E) as illustrated.

Tracking and scoring of forelimb movements

To quantify fine paw movement impairments not captured by the joystick trajectory, in a subset of experiments (N = 5) we recorded high-speed videos (60 fps) and manually scored the forelimb movements during the execution of the task (Fig. 4). First, we tracked the position of the tip of the digit to reconstruct the paw trajectory in case mice released the joystick. We focused on three conditions: control trials, RFA and CFA inactivation during the answer period. In addition to paw trajectories, we observed other behavioral deficits that were classified and scored as follows: (1) paw “slip”, when more than two digits slipped laterally off the joystick (Fig. 4B,F); (2) paw “drop”, when the paw dropped below the handle of the joystick (Fig. 4C,G); (3) digit extension, when the paw was opened and digits spread (Fig. 4D,H). Scoring was done blind to trial type.

Performance analysis and statistics

Trials were classified according to their outcome: “correct” (correct supra-threshold joystick movement), “incorrect” (opposite supra-threshold joystick movement), “no movement detected” during the answer period and “anticipated lick” (licking before moving the joystick). Trials with premature events (threshold crossing prior to the go cue) were excluded from the analysis for the inactivation experiments. In a subset of sessions mice did not answer during the initial period of the session and mice typically stopped answering after satiation53,79. In order to analyse a homogeneous performance (without periods of inactivity due to external factors), periods at the beginning (up to the 5th correct trial) and at the end (last 5 correct trials) of the behavioral sessions were excluded from the analysis.

We quantified the discrimination ability using d-prime (d’, sensitivity index) rather than percent correct to take potential bias into account59. d’ was quantified as following: d’ = z (correct pull rate) + z (correct push rate), where z is the normal inverse of the cumulative distribution function. Only correct and incorrect trials were included to compute d’. Positive d’ values (maximum d’ = 4.65) resulted from a majority of correct pull and push trials; negative d’ (minimum d’ = −4.65) corresponds to a majority of incorrect trials while null values mean chance level. To exclude infinity values from the formula, null and equal to one correct rates were rounded to 0.001 and 0.999, respectively. The fraction of answered trials included correct and incorrect trials (Fig. 1C). Learning sessions with changes in performance due to rig modifications were excluded from the analysis. Behavioral changes caused by cortical inactivation were evaluated by comparing control trial and inactivated trial performances. A Kruskal-Wallis test was used to compare performance in control and different inactivation conditions after testing for normality and homoscedasticity using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and the Bartlett test, respectively. The Fisher’s least significant difference test was used as post-hoc unless stated otherwise. P values are reported on the figures or in Table 1. Difference in differences analysis (DID)83 using a multiple linear regression analysis was performed to evaluate the significance of the differential effect between combined (CFA + RFA) and single inactivation conditions (CFA or RFA). Normality was assessed using the Shapiro Wilk test and heteroscedasticity was corrected by using a robust regression84. All results are reported as mean ± SEM.

Electronic supplementary material

Movie (11.6MB, mov)

Acknowledgements

We thank G. Galiñanes, A. Sideris, E. Rouiller, K. Svoboda, and M. Prsa for comments on the manuscript and all members of the Huber lab for stimulating discussions. This research was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation, European Research Council, New York Stem Cell Foundation. D.H. is a New York Stem Cell Foundation-Robertson Investigator.

Author Contributions

K.M. and D.H. conceived the experiments and wrote the manuscript. K.M. conducted the experiments and analyzed the data.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-017-15835-2.

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Rouiller EM, et al. Dexterity in adult monkeys following early lesion of the motor cortical hand area: the role of cortex adjacent to the lesion. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1998;10:729–740. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00075.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Murata Y, et al. Temporal plasticity involved in recovery from manual dexterity deficit after motor cortex lesion in macaque monkeys. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:84–95. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1737-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Whishaw IQ. Did a change in sensory control of skilled movements stimulate the evolution of the primate frontal cortex? Behav. Brain Res. 2003;146:31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Alaverdashvili M, Whishaw IQ. Motor cortex stroke impairs individual digit movement in skilled reaching by the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:311–322. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06315.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dum RP, Strick PL. Motor areas in the frontal lobe of the primate. Physiol. Behav. 2002;77:677–682. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00929-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Graziano M. The organization of behavioral repertoire in motor cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;29:105–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Rizzolatti G, Cattaneo L, Fabbri-Destro M, Rozzi S. Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding. Physiol. Rev. 2014;94:655–706. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00009.2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Weinrich M, Wise SP, Mauritz KH. A neurophysiological study of the premotor cortex in the rhesus monkey. Brain. 1984;107(Pt 2):385–414. doi: 10.1093/brain/107.2.385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Riehle A, Requin J. Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. J. Neurophysiol. 1989;61:534–549. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.3.534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hocherman S, Wise SP. Effects of hand movement path on motor cortical activity in awake, behaving rhesus monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 1991;83:285–302. doi: 10.1007/BF00231153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Romo R, Hernández A, Zainos A. Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision in ventral premotor cortex. Neuron. 2004;41:165–173. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00817-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cisek P, Kalaska JF. Neural correlates of reaching decisions in dorsal premotor cortex: specification of multiple direction choices and final selection of action. Neuron. 2005;45:801–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Churchland MM, Shenoy KV. Delay of movement caused by disruption of cortical preparatory activity. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:348–359. doi: 10.1152/jn.00808.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;30:535–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Goard, M. J., Pho, G. N., Woodson, J. & Sur, M. Distinct roles of visual, parietal, and frontal motor cortices in memory-guided sensorimotor decisions. Elife5. 10.7554/eLife.13764 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 16.Barthas F, Kwan AC. Secondary motor cortex: Where ‘sensory’ meets ‘motor’ in the rodent frontal cortex. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40:181–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Neafsey EJ, et al. The organization of the rat motor cortex: a microstimulation mapping study. Brain Res. 1986;396:77–96. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(86)90011-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ramanathan D, Conner JM, Tuszynski MH. A form of motor cortical plasticity that correlates with recovery of function after brain injury. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:11370–11375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601065103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brown AR, Teskey GC. Motor cortex is functionally organized as a set of spatially distinct representations for complex movements. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:13574–13585. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2500-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Liang F, Rouiller EM, Wiesendanger M. Modulation of sustained electromyographic activity by single intracortical microstimuli: comparison of two forelimb motor cortical areas of the rat. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 1993;10:51–61. doi: 10.3109/08990229309028823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Silasi G, Boyd JD, LeDue J, Murphy T. Improved methods for chronic light-based motor mapping in mice: automated movement tracking with accelerometers, and chronic EEG recording in a bilateral thin-skull preparation. Front. Neural Circuits. 2013;7:123. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hira R, Terada S-I, Kondo M, Matsuzaki M. Distinct functional modules for discrete and rhythmic forelimb movements in the mouse motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:13311–13322. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2731-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rouiller EM, Moret V, Liang F. Comparison of the connectional properties of the two forelimb areas of the rat sensorimotor cortex: support for the presence of a premotor or supplementary motor cortical area. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 1993;10:269–289. doi: 10.3109/08990229309028837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hira R, et al. In vivo optogenetic tracing of functional corticocortical connections between motor forelimb areas. Front. Neural Circuits. 2013;7:55. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mohammed H, Jain N. Ipsilateral cortical inputs to the rostral and caudal motor areas in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 2016;524:3104–3123. doi: 10.1002/cne.24011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wise SP, Murray EA, Coulter JD. Somatotopic organization of corticospinal and corticotrigeminal neurons in the rat. Neuroscience. 1979;4:65–78. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(79)90218-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Deffeyes JE, Touvykine B, Quessy S, Dancause N. Interactions between rostral and caudal cortical motor areas in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 2015;113:3893–3904. doi: 10.1152/jn.00760.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Mohammed H, Jain N. Two whisker motor areas in the rat cortex: evidence from thalamocortical connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 2014;522:528–545. doi: 10.1002/cne.23424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Graziano MSA, Taylor CSR, Moore T. Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex. Neuron. 2002;34:841–851. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00698-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gharbawie OA, Stepniewska I, Qi H, Kaas JH. Multiple parietal-frontal pathways mediate grasping in macaque monkeys. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:11660–11677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1777-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Overduin SA, d’Avella A, Carmena JM, Bizzi E. Microstimulation activates a handful of muscle synergies. Neuron. 2012;76:1071–1077. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kleim JA, Barbay S, Nudo RJ. Functional reorganization of the rat motor cortex following motor skill learning. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;80:3321–3325. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.6.3321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Tennant KA, et al. The organization of the forelimb representation of the C57BL/6 mouse motor cortex as defined by intracortical microstimulation and cytoarchitecture. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21:865–876. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Guo J-Z, et al. Cortex commands the performance of skilled movement. Elife. 2015;4:e10774. doi: 10.7554/eLife.10774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kawai R, et al. Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron. 2015;86:800–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Castro AJ. The effects of cortical ablations on digital usage in the rat. Brain Res. 1972;37:173–185. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90665-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Barth TM, Jones TA, Schallert T. Functional subdivisions of the rat somatic sensorimotor cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 1990;39:73–95. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90122-U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Gorny BP, Pellis VC. The impairments in reaching and the movements of compensation in rats with motor cortex lesions: an endpoint, videorecording, and movement notation analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 1991;42:77–91. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80042-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Peters AJ, Chen SX, Komiyama T. Emergence of reproducible spatiotemporal activity during motor learning. Nature. 2014;510:263–267. doi: 10.1038/nature13235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Miri A, et al. Behaviorally selective engagement of Short-Latency effector pathways by motor cortex. Neuron. 2017;95:683–696.e11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hyland B. Neural activity related to reaching and grasping in rostral and caudal regions of rat motor cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 1998;94:255–269. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00157-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hira R, et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of functional clusters of neurons in the mouse motor cortex during a voluntary movement. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:1377–1390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2550-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Saiki A, et al. Different modulation of common motor information in rat primary and secondary motor cortices. PLoS One. 2014;9:e98662. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kimura R, Saiki A, Fujiwara-Tsukamoto Y, Sakai Y, Isomura Y. Large-scale analysis reveals populational contributions of cortical spike rate and synchrony to behavioral functions. J. Physiol. 2016 doi: 10.1113/JP272794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Guo ZV, et al. Flow of cortical activity underlying a tactile decision in mice. Neuron. 2014;81:179–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sul JH, Kim H, Huh N, Lee D, Jung MW. Distinct roles of rodent orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex in decision making. Neuron. 2010;66:449–460. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hillman KL, Bilkey DK. Neural encoding of competitive effort in the anterior cingulate cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:1290–1297. doi: 10.1038/nn.3187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Harvey CD, Coen P, Tank DW. Choice-specific sequences in parietal cortex during a virtual-navigation decision task. Nature. 2012;484:62–68. doi: 10.1038/nature10918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Erlich, J. C., Brunton, B. W., Duan, C. A., Hanks, T. D. & Brody, C. D. Distinct effects of prefrontal and parietal cortex inactivations on an accumulation of evidence task in the rat. Elife4, 10.7554/eLife.05457 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 50.Licata, A. M. et al. Posterior parietal cortex guides visual decisions in rats (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 51.Komiyama T, et al. Learning-related fine-scale specificity imaged in motor cortex circuits of behaving mice. Nature. 2010;464:1182–1186. doi: 10.1038/nature08897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Chen T-W, Li N, Daie K, Svoboda K. A map of anticipatory activity in mouse motor cortex. Neuron. 2017;94:866–879.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.O’Connor DH, et al. Vibrissa-based object localization in head-fixed mice. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:1947–1967. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3762-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Isomura Y, Harukuni R, Takekawa T, Aizawa H, Fukai T. Microcircuitry coordination of cortical motor information in self-initiation of voluntary movements. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1586–1593. doi: 10.1038/nn.2431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Chen SX, Kim AN, Peters AJ, Komiyama T. Subtype-specific plasticity of inhibitory circuits in motor cortex during motor learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1109–1115. doi: 10.1038/nn.4049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Tanji J, Evarts EV. Anticipatory activity of motor cortex neurons in relation to direction of an intended movement. J. Neurophysiol. 1976;39:1062–1068. doi: 10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kurata K, Hoffman DS. Differential effects of muscimol microinjection into dorsal and ventral aspects of the premotor cortex of monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 1994;71:1151–1164. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.3.1151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kalaska JF, Crammond DJ. Deciding not to GO: neuronal correlates of response selection in a GO/NOGO task in primate premotor and parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 1995;5:410–428. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.5.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Carandini M, Churchland AK. Probing perceptual decisions in rodents. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:824–831. doi: 10.1038/nn.3410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Weinrich M, Wise SP. The premotor cortex of the monkey. J. Neurosci. 1982;2:1329–1345. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-09-01329.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Gharbawie OA, Karl JM, Whishaw IQ. Recovery of skilled reaching following motor cortex stroke: do residual corticofugal fibers mediate compensatory recovery? Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007;26:3309–3327. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05874.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Atlas, D. M. B. Allen brain atlas. http://www.brain-map.org/ (2006).
  • 63.Wang L, Conner JM, Nagahara AH, Tuszynski MH. Rehabilitation drives enhancement of neuronal structure in functionally relevant neuronal subsets. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:2750–2755. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514682113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Donoghue JP. Contrasting properties of neurons in two parts of the primary motor cortex of the awake rat. Brain Res. 1985;333:173–177. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90141-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kopec CD, Erlich JC, Brunton BW, Deisseroth K, Brody CD. Cortical and subcortical contributions to Short-Term memory for orienting movements. Neuron. 2015;88:367–377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Allen WE, et al. Global representations of Goal-Directed behavior in distinct cell types of mouse neocortex. Neuron. 2017;94:891–907.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Zingg B, et al. Neural networks of the mouse neocortex. Cell. 2014;156:1096–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Storozhuk VM, Brácha V, Brozek G, Bures J. Unit activity of motor cortex during acoustically signalled reaching in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 1984;12:317–326. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(84)90157-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Hoogewoud F, et al. Comparison of functional recovery of manual dexterity after unilateral spinal cord lesion or motor cortex lesion in adult macaque monkeys. Front. Neurol. 2013;4:101. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Katz LN, Yates JL, Pillow JW, Huk AC. Dissociated functional significance of decision-related activity in the primate dorsal stream. Nature. 2016;535:285–288. doi: 10.1038/nature18617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Liu Y, Rouiller EM. Mechanisms of recovery of dexterity following unilateral lesion of the sensorimotor cortex in adult monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 1999;128:149–159. doi: 10.1007/s002210050830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Biernaskie J, Szymanska A, Windle V, Corbett D. Bi-hemispheric contribution to functional motor recovery of the affected forelimb following focal ischemic brain injury in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005;21:989–999. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03899.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Fridman EA, et al. Reorganization of the human ipsilesional premotor cortex after stroke. Brain. 2004;127:747–758. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Touvykine B, et al. The effect of lesion size on the organization of the ipsilesional and contralesional motor cortex. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2016;30:280–292. doi: 10.1177/1545968315585356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Dancause N, et al. Extensive cortical rewiring after brain injury. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:10167–10179. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3256-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Hosp JA, Luft AR. Cortical plasticity during motor learning and recovery after ischemic stroke. Neural Plast. 2011;2011:871296. doi: 10.1155/2011/871296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Li N, Daie K, Svoboda K, Druckmann S. Robust neuronal dynamics in premotor cortex during motor planning. Nature. 2016;532:459–464. doi: 10.1038/nature17643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Otchy TM, et al. Acute off-target effects of neural circuit manipulations. Nature. 2015;528:358–363. doi: 10.1038/nature16442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Guo ZV, et al. Procedures for behavioral experiments in head-fixed mice. PLoS One. 2014;9:e88678. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Slotnick B. A simple 2-transistor touch or lick detector circuit. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2009;91:253–255. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2009.91-253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Huber D, et al. Multiple dynamic representations in the motor cortex during sensorimotor learning. Nature. 2012;484:473–478. doi: 10.1038/nature11039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Prsa M, Galiñanes GL, Huber D. Rapid integration of artificial sensory feedback during operant conditioning of motor cortex neurons. Neuron. 2017;93:929–939.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Card D, Krueger AB. Minimum wages and employment: A case study of the Fast-Food industry in new jersey and pennsylvania. Am. Econ. Rev. 1994;84:772–793. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Holland PW, Welsch RE. Robust regression using iteratively reweighted least-squares. Communications in Statistics - Theory and Methods. 1977;6:813–827. doi: 10.1080/03610927708827533. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Movie (11.6MB, mov)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES