Abstract
Brain activity during sleep is fairly ubiquitous and the best studied possible function is a role in memory consolidation, including motor memory. One suggested mechanism of how neural activity effects these benefits is through reactivation of neurons in patterns resembling those of the preceding experience. The specific patterns of motor activation replayed during sleep are largely unknown for any system. Brain areas devoted to song production in the songbird brain exhibit spontaneous song-like activity during sleep, but single cell neural recordings did not permit detection of the specific song patterns. We have now discovered that this sleep activation can be detected in the muscles of the vocal organ, thus providing a unique window into song-related brain activity at night. We show that male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) frequently exhibit spontaneous song-like activity during the night, but that the fictive song patterns are highly variable and uncoordinated compared to the highly stereotyped day-time song production. This substantial variability is not consistent with the idea that night-time activity replays day-time experiences for consolidation. Although the function of this frequent activation is unknown, it may represent a mechanism for exploring motor space or serve to generate internal error signals that help maintain the high stereotypy of day-time song. In any case, the described activity supports the emerging insight that brain activity during sleep may serve a variety of functions.
Keywords: Motor replay, Sleep, Song, Motor program, Consolidation
Introduction
Brain activity during sleep is thought to play an important role in consolidation of declarative and procedural memory (e.g., Maquet, 2001; Stickgold, 2005; Diekelmann & Born, 2010; Yang et al., 2014; Tononi & Cirelli, 2014), and one suggested mechanism of how neural activity effects these benefits is through reactivation of neurons in patterns that resemble those of the preceding experience during the active period (Wilson & McNaughton, 1994; Dave & Margoliash, 2000; Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee, 2002; Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee, 2006; Euston, Tatsuno & McNaughton, 2007; Peyrache et al., 2009; Shank & Margoliash, 2009). Specifically, motor replay during sleep can be induced by sensory cues and is thought to consolidate motor memory through sensorimotor re-processing in the relevant brain areas (Derégnaucourt et al., 2005; Margoliash, 2005; Orban et al., 2006; Hupbach et al., 2009). However, the role of sleep on memory formation for a specific newly acquired motor skill is debated (e.g., Korman et al., 2003; Rickard et al., 2008; Brawn, Nusbaum & Margoliash, 2010). Furthermore, alternative patterns of sleep activation have been reported and led to different, debated hypotheses regarding the function of this brain activity (e.g., Dragoi & Tonegawa, 2011; Dragoi & Tonegawa, 2013; Brawn & Margoliash, 2015; Eichenbaum, 2015; Silva, Feng & Foster, 2015; Grosmark & Buzsáki, 2016).
This possibility of multiple functions of sleep activation patterns is also suggested by the occurrence of replay in well-established and stereotyped motor behaviors (Dave & Margoliash, 2000; Margoliash & Schmidt, 2010). Mechanisms for how sleep activation enhances consolidation of recently acquired memories may be different from those involved in stereotyped motor sequences, but this distinction has not been clearly made. In general, our understanding of how night-time motor replay in the brain can benefit motor performance is impeded by the fact that we do not have detailed insight into the nature of the replayed motor patterns.
One of the best postulated examples for motor replay is learned vocal behavior (Giret, Edeline & Del Negro, 2017). In the zebra finch, activity of neurons in sensorimotor motor (HVC) and motor cortical (the robust arcopallial nucleus, RA) areas strongly suggested that they were activated in song-like manner at night during song playback as well as spontaneously (Dave & Margoliash, 2000; Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee, 2002; Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee, 2006). However, constraints on recording from a large number of neurons simultaneously and on maintaining single unit responses over sufficient time spans did not permit detection of which song sequences are replayed at night. Oscine birdsong is a learned behavior, whose control involves coordination of multiple motor systems to generate a highly stereotyped acoustic sequence (Brainard, 2008; Goller & Cooper, 2008; Suthers & Zollinger, 2008; Méndez et al., 2010; Riede & Goller, 2010; Beckers, 2013). Insight into the specific motor patterns of night-time brain activation would therefore be a major advance in our understanding of how activity during sleep might benefit stereotyped motor control of complex behavior. Here we use song-like night-time activation of the muscles controlling the vocal organ of birds as a sufficiently detailed output to allow identification of which song syllable sequence is replayed in the brain during night-time.
Material and Methods
During this study ten adult male zebra finches were used (age >120 days), and all procedures and experiments were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the University of Utah (#16-03014). Zebra finches were housed individually in a 31.8 cm × 22.9 cm × 27.9 cm wire cage with newspaper lining. They were fed a mixture of red and white millet, canary seed, and water ad libitum. This diet was supplemented with peas and corn every other day. Before any surgical procedure was performed, a baseline recording of directed song was obtained using a directional microphone (Audio-Technica 835B; Audio-Technica, Machida, Tokyo, Japan) placed approximately 20 cm away from the bird. The microphone output was amplified using a Brownlee 410 amplifier (200–400×; Brownlee Precision, San Jose, CA, USA) and connected to an analog to digital converter (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA). Recordings were obtained using Avisoft-Recorder (Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany) at 44.1 kHz sample rate.
Zebra finches were accustomed to the procedures three to five days before the surgery by first attaching a leash and backpack to the bird. The backpack is custom-built and consists of a Velcro tab on the back that is secured on the bird with elastic bands around the base of each wing and around the upper part of the thorax. Birds were tethered by a wire, which was fixed on the backpack and connected to a balancing lever arm positioned above the cage to allow the bird to move freely. Once birds resumed singing activity on the tether, surgical procedures were initiated. One hour prior to surgery, birds were deprived of food and water.
Surgery was performed under general anesthesia by administering Ketamine/Xylazine. During surgery, a flexible cannula was inserted into a thoracic air sac. The cannula was sutured and adhered (Vetbond; 3M Animal Care Products, St. Paul, MN, USA) to the rib cage and attached to a piezoresistive pressure transducer (Fujikura FPM-02PG; Fujikura, Tokyo, Japan) mounted on the backpack. Next, the syrinx was accessed by a skin incision in the furcula area and opening of the interclavicular air sac membrane. The tips of custom-built bipolar wire electrodes (California Fine Wire, SS 304; 25 µm) were inserted into the syringeal muscles. Bipolar electrodes were placed in two different muscles during surgery. The electrodes were adhered to the surface of the syringeal muscles with tissue adhesive (Vetbond). Electrode wires were routed subcutaneously to the backpack where the ends were attached to a connector from which larger wires made connection to signal conditioning equipment. Following surgery, the bird was placed in the cage, tethered, and allowed to recover overnight.
In each bird, we recorded from two different muscles, either on the same side or from left and right muscles (Fig. S1; Table S1). We recorded from all four intrinsic muscles, because all are activated during song. In the brown thrasher (Toxostoma rufum), correlations of acoustic and physiological features with EMG activity of different intrinsic muscles revealed the following picture of their respective functions: control of fundamental frequency by the ventral syringeal muscle and gating of airflow by the ventral and dorsal tracheobronchial muscles as abductor and adductor of the labia (the medial portion of the dorsal syringeal muscle was not studied). In zebra finches however, the muscles appear to be much more synergistically activated, such that for example frequency control is more complex (for review see Goller & Riede, 2013). We therefore recorded from all intrinsic syringeal muscles to exclude the possibility that night-time activation is specific for individual muscles or acoustic features. Because zebra finch song is also generated with asymmetrical contributions of the two independent sound generators, we recorded from muscles on both sides of the syrinx (10 recordings on each side, Table S1).
Following surgery, four channels were recorded simultaneously using Avisoft-Recorder, air sac pressure, two EMGs, and audio. EMG channels were band pass filtered (100–3,000 Hz) with a gain of 1,000×–3,000× (440; Brownlee Precision, San Jose, CA, USA). Audio recording was done with the same settings that were used to record pre-surgery song. Prior to any nighttime recording, we obtained multiple examples of complete, directed song from the male during the day by placing a female in a cage next to him. The spectrograms from these songs were compared to those recorded pre-surgery to ensure that no damage was done to the syringeal muscles during surgery.
During the night, all channels were continuously recorded at a sample rate of 8 kHz. Recordings were started and stopped manually. The photoperiod was set from 6:00 AM to 8:00 PM and was controlled by an automatic timer.
Comparison of spontaneous EMG activity to EMG activity that occurred during song was done using custom written software implementing the following procedures. The simultaneous measurement of air sac pressure and muscle activity (EMG) for singing birds was used to segment the latter into syllable-related fragments. We computed the envelopes of the EMG signals, and those smoothed time series were cut at specific time points, chosen in the following way. The minima of the pressure patterns with subatmospheric values of air sac pressure were computed, and then, for each minimum, we looked for the immediately following minimum of the simultaneously measured EMG envelope. Those times were used to cut the EMG envelope in N segments, corresponding to the each syllable of the song. These EMG segments corresponding to syllables are hereafter called syllable templates.
The envelopes of EMG signals were computed by applying a Hilbert transform (impulse response filter length IRFL = 128), followed by a first order integration (τιντ = 0.01), and a Savitsky Golay filter (Nleft = 256, Nright = 256). We applied this procedure to the syllable templates, as well as to the EMG data recorded during the night (n(t)). Then, we computed the correlation between the envelope of the night data n(t) and the envelope of each of the syllable templates, shifted at delays τ between 0 and T − Ttemplate, where T is the duration of the night recording and Ttemplate the duration of the template analyzed. In this way, for each of the templates, we obtained a continuous time series whose largest peaks indicate a high correlation between a fragment of the envelope of the night data, and the syllable template under analysis. In order to identify the largest correlation values, we computed for each bird, all the maxima of the correlation coefficients with all the templates. Then, we fitted a Gaussian G (μ, σ) to the resulting histogram, and defined the threshold value X such that Z = X − μ∕σ = 1.64. With this choice of threshold, every segment of the envelope of the night activity n(t), whose correlation with a template is higher than X, was found to correspond to a segment of the time series in which activity was found (no false positives), and was similar to the template. To test the automated search algorithm, we compared EMG patterns of syllables during song to establish correlation coefficients for matching and non-matching syllables. For matching syllables the mean correlation coefficient and STD was 0.75 ± 0.23 (n = 75) and for comparisons of different syllables of 0.24 ± 0.19 (n = 75). The criterion of 0.8 for identification of EMG patterns that match specific syllables therefore constitutes a conservative approach toward identification of nocturnal EMG patterns. No false positive segments were identified with this approach, although some matching EMG segments may have been discarded with this high threshold. Visual inspection of >500 identified cases was used to further confirm the selectivity of the automated search approach and the choice of 0.8 as threshold correlation value.
To quantitatively assess night-time EMG sequences for syllable sequence, we identified the one syllable which occurred most frequently during song-like activation (SLA) for each individual. Then we quantified the highest correlation score for all other syllables around the identified syllable. To account for timing differences between song and SLA, we searched within 640 ms near the time point of the syllable as expected from day-time song motifs. From these data we compiled histograms of correlation scores for each individual to assess syllable sequences that occur during SLA.
Results
Adult male zebra finches produce a stereotyped song sequence that is composed of a repeated series of acoustically distinct syllables, called motif (Zann, 1996; Franz & Goller, 2002; Williams, 2008; Wood et al., 2013). Each song syllable is generated by a stereotyped and characteristic respiratory pulse, which is coordinated with specific activation patterns of syringeal muscles (Figs. 1A and 1B) (Vicario, 1991a; Goller & Cooper, 2004; Méndez et al., 2010). After recording subsyringeal air sac pressure and electromyograms (EMG) from syringeal muscles in adult male zebra finches during song, we then monitored physiological activity during the night. Syringeal muscles showed spontaneous SLA during the night. Using a search algorithm based on template matching of the EMG patterns for song syllables, we scanned night-time files for activity. The algorithm showed sufficient distinctive power that permitted reliable identification of EMG patterns from different syllables (see ‘Methods’) during day-time song and night-time activity. Even with stringent criteria for the search algorithm, SLA in the syrinx was found remarkably frequently throughout the night, with a range of 110–2,370 (seven birds, over a total of 350 hrs of night-time recording) syllable-like occurrences spread throughout the night-time period (Fig. 2A). However, while SLA occurred in the syrinx, the respiratory system was not simultaneously activated in a song-like fashion and, therefore, no sound was produced. Respiratory rate during syringeal SLA (1.59 ± 0.46 Hz; n = 179; 6 birds) was indistinguishable from normal breathing at night (1.53 ± 0.36 Hz; n = 177; 6 birds; paired t-test, p = 0.59). Exhalation amplitude and duration were not song-like during SLA. Amplitude expressed as normalized relative voltage output during song was 8 ± 0.23 (n = 83 from 5 birds) whereas amplitude during syringeal SLA was 0.9 ± 0.04, n = 100 from 5 birds. Additionally, the duration of expiratory pressure pulses during syringeal SLA was 0.61 ± 0.33 s compared to 0.13 ± 0.07 s during song. The prolonged duration of expiration compared to awake breathing indicates that the birds were asleep (Fig. 2B). Of all the SLA events, we only found a different, more song-like respiratory pattern once in one bird, but the pressure was still not sufficiently elevated to result in phonation (Fig. 2B).
Although complete EMG patterns of the entire motif were occasionally replayed at night (Fig. 1), most of the EMG activity was not a stereotyped repetition of activity during day-time song. To quantify this, we used mean EMG patterns of individual syllables as a template to scan the night-time files for SLA occurrence and then determined how much of the full song motif was replayed. We chose data from five birds with the most distinctive EMG signals for different syllables. First, we used a subset of identified SLA occurrences to categorize 457 SLA events into one of seven categories of activity (Fig. 3). The categories describe the degree of completeness of the motif and capture deviations in timing from a typical song motif. Of these seven categories, on average over 50% of SLA occurrences fell into the single syllable or partial syllables categories. In contrast, replays of complete motifs occurred in only 7% of the total instances of SLA (Fig. 3D, Table S2).
Consistent with previous work (Vicario, 1991a; Franz & Goller, 2002), motor gestures of song production in adult male zebra finches were highly stereotyped (Figs. 4A and 4B). In contrast, night-time SLA was extremely variable when compared to stereotyped EMG activity of the song motif during day-time song (Fig. 4C). Motor replay consisted of single or multiple syllables without a discernible pattern for which particular syllable in the motif preferentially occurred during SLA. The timing between syllable EMG patterns was also more variable during night-time activity when compared to timing during song. The mean duration between syllables during night-time SLA was longer by 3 ms compared to day-time song and was generally more variable (Fig. 3E).
In addition to the variable timing and inconsistent ordering of syllables during SLA, other phenomena occurred in SLA that were never seen in stereotyped song production. EMG patterns of individual syllables showed deviations from the stereotyped pattern of day-time song. Incomplete syllables occurred, with either the first or the last half of the activity pattern missing or only present at a lower EMG amplitude. In other instances, parts of a typical EMG pattern for a syllable were omitted with the rest of the syllable pattern shifted in time, while the other muscle performed the standard song like pattern (Fig. 3C). This caused a mismatch in the SLA pattern between the two syringeal sound generators.
Second and in addition to analyzing this subset, we quantified SLA structure for the entire data set. For each bird, we chose SLA occurrences of the most frequently generated syllable and compared the EMG activity near this syllable to all other templates, allowing for different timing. If a full motif was executed, the other cross correlation coefficients should be near our cutoff threshold of 0.8. Any deviations from a full motif will yield low correlation coefficients for the other syllables. The data show clearly for all birds that the full motif was rarely produced (Fig. 4D), as correlation coefficients for other syllables rarely exceed the 0.8 cutoff criterion. Furthermore, activation patterns for different syllables occurred at very different rates, because correlation coefficients for some syllables remained much lower than those for others. Whereas the mean correlation coefficients for the target syllables ranged from 0.82 to 0.89, those for the other syllables ranged from 0.39 to 0.82. Finally, this analysis reveals differences between individuals in respect to the syllable composition of SLA sequences (Fig. 4D).
Discussion
Here we have shown that night-time SLA does not only occur in the forebrain circuitry (HVC and RA—Dave & Margoliash, 2000; Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee, 2002; Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee, 2006; Area X—Yanagihara & Hessler, 2012), but that this activity also is relayed to the syringeal muscles. Whereas during song production RA initiates activation of both respiratory pre-motor circuits and the syringeal motor neurons (nXIIts) (e.g., Vicario, 1991b; Roberts et al., 2008; Schmidt, McLean & Goller, 2012), during SLA only activation of the syringeal motor system occurs. The specific mechanism for disengaging respiration from the song motor sequence is not known, but inhibition at the level of the respiratory pre-motor nuclei may prevent the song-like activation of the respiratory system and, thus, sound generation.
The functional significance of activating the syringeal muscles during night-time motor replay is unclear. Because song generation involves coordinated activity of respiratory, vocal organ and upper vocal tract motor systems, the activation of only one of them during SLA is unlikely to provide useful peripheral feedback information for consolidation of the central song motor program. The activation of the whole motor circuit including the syringeal motor nucleus (nXIIts) may be necessary for providing benefits to the maintenance of the motor program. Alternatively, the activation of the syrinx may not be serving any function for song program consolidation, but, unlike respiration, may not have been selected against as it produces no externally visible movement and is likely not very energetically costly (Oberweger & Goller, 2001; Franz & Goller, 2003). Whether or not it may provide stimuli needed for the homeostatic maintenance of the superfast syringeal muscles remains unclear (Elemans et al., 2008; Uchida et al., 2010).
Nevertheless, the feed through of central activity to the syrinx provides the first detailed insight into the frequency of occurrence and specific features of motor replay of song-like activity in the brain. Such detailed information is not available for any complex behavior and, thus, enables us to characterize spontaneous motor activity for the first time. SLA occurred throughout the night period and very frequently. Replayed motor gestures for individual syllables are not consistently ordered into the correct motif sequence, are often incomplete, and even coordination of the left and right motor pathways to the two sound generators of the syrinx is not consistently present.
Zebra finch song is generated by two independently controlled sound generators, which typically contribute simultaneously to harmonic stack syllables, but high-frequency syllables are generated only on the right side, while the left side is closed to airflow (Goller & Cooper, 2004; Jensen et al., 2007). Because closure of the labia is an active process, activation of left and right muscles accompanies production of high-frequency syllables. The activation patterns of muscles therefore allow identification of all syllables, irrespective of whether the side of the syrinx contributes to sound production. The fact that some SLA in the muscles shows lack of left–right coordination points toward activation of the left and right forebrain pathways without the synchronization that is typically present during day-time song. The mechanisms for this coordination are not completely understood, but may involve cross connections in medullar, mesencephalic and diencephalic components of the song control circuit (e.g., Schmidt, McLean & Goller, 2012).
These findings from muscle recordings are not inconsistent with spontaneous RA activity during sleep (Dave & Margoliash, 2000), because the firing patterns of individual neurons in RA is such that they spike at specific syllable segments and are silent during the remaining song components. A full assessment of song-like activity during sleep is therefore not possible from these neuronal data. Whereas the spike patterns of spontaneous activity during sleep can be matched to activity during song performance, the absence of spikes and non-matching bursts are difficult to interpret (Dave & Margoliash, 2000; Shank & Margoliash, 2009; Rauske et al., 2010). Syringeal activation therefore provides more information about which song elements are replayed and permits the interpretation that sleep activation is not replay of the stereotyped motor program for song. This conclusion therefore differs from the one derived from ensemble recordings in the hippocampus, where night-time activation of place cells is thought to replay previously experienced sequences fairly faithfully (e.g., Wilson & McNaughton, 1994; Skaggs & McNaughton, 1996), albeit sometimes with different tempos (e.g., Euston, Tatsuno & McNaughton, 2007). In contrast, in a tactile exploration task, rats showed neuronal reverberation, rather than accurate replay, in multiple forebrain areas during slow-wave sleep (e.g., Ribeiro et al., 2004). The motor replay patterns in adult zebra finches occur in another context (stereotyped motor program not recently learned motor behavior) and as such night-time activity constitutes a highly variable motor activation that may be similar to plasticity experienced during vocal learning (Tchernichovski et al., 2001; Derégnaucourt et al., 2005; Shank & Margoliash, 2009; Ölveczky et al., 2011).
During the sensorimotor period of song development, birds “practice” song production and generate error signals in the auditory feedback that are thought to be used for refining imitation of the acquired song model. Interestingly, SLA in HVC occurs less frequently in young birds during the sensorimotor phase than in adult birds (Crandall et al., 2007). In adult birds, however, song production is highly stereotyped, and variation in tempo and acoustic parameters (e.g., frequency) is very small (1–5%; e.g., Franz & Goller, 2002; Cooper & Goller, 2006; Glaze & Troyer, 2006; Crandall et al., 2007; Williams, 2008; Méndez et al., 2010; Wood et al., 2013). Frequent SLA and its high variability relative to the stereotyped motif of day-time song could be related to the maintenance of the motor program.
Conclusions
Although it is currently unknown which function this high variability in SLA may serve, some possibilities are suggested here. Inspired by systems in which “preplay” has been proposed (e.g., Dragoi & Tonegawa, 2011; Dragoi & Tonegawa, 2013), motor circuits may generally explore motor space during sleep. The specific relevance of such activity for a highly stereotyped motor program, as the song production program in zebra finches, is unclear. Another possible mechanism is that variable SLA may enhance the stability of the song motor program by generating internal error signals that contribute to motor stability. An efference copy of HVC activation is thought to be involved in auditory feedback controlled song learning (e.g., Troyer & Doupe, 2000; Prather et al., 2008; Mooney, 2009; Bolhuis, Okanoya & Scharff, 2010; Brainard & Doupe, 2013; Fee, 2014). In adult birds this efference copy could predict motor output, which could internally be compared to the motor representation of stereotyped song. This internal error generation may counteract a possible decay in motor program stability in the absence of error signals (e.g., Criscimagna-Hemminger & Shadmehr, 2008; Kitago et al., 2013; Vaswani & Shadmehr, 2013; Brennan & Smith, 2015) and may facilitate the incorporation of new neurons into the song circuit (e.g., Alvarez-Buylla, Theelen & Nottebohm, 1988; Nottebohm, 2008; Pytte et al., 2012) without jeopardizing the stereotypy of day-time singing. Although merely speculative at this point, this proposed model makes clear, testable predictions and generates a distinction between memory consolidation by repeating stereotyped patterns during sleep (replay) and stabilization of motor programs through internal error generation induced by variability in activation.
Supplemental Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Natalia Verzhbitskiy for help in the laboratory.
Funding Statement
This research was funded by the NIH. Ezequiel Arneodo was supported by a Premio Fundación Bunge y Born postdoctoral fellowship. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare there are no competing interests.
Author Contributions
Brent K. Young and Franz Goller conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Gabriel B. Mindlin analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Ezequiel Arneodo analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Animal Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
The IACUC of the University of Utah approved all procedures (# 16-03014).
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
Goller, Franz (2017): GY63 daytime song. figshare.
https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5406718.v1.
Goller, Franz (2017): GY63 night. figshare.
References
- Alvarez-Buylla, Theelen & Nottebohm (1988).Alvarez-Buylla A, Theelen M, Nottebohm F. Birth of projection neurons in the higher vocal center of the canary forebrain before, during and after song learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1988;85:8722–8726. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.22.8722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckers (2013).Beckers GJL. Peripheral mechanisms of vocalization in birds: a comparison with human speech. In: Bolhuis JL, Everaert M, editors. Birdsong speech and language. The MIT Press; Cambridge: 2013. pp. 399–422. [Google Scholar]
- Bolhuis, Okanoya & Scharff (2010).Bolhuis JJ, Okanoya K, Scharff C. Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:747–759. doi: 10.1038/nrn2931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard (2008).Brainard MS. The anterior forebrain pathway and vocal plasticity. In: Zeigler HP, Marler P, editors. Neuroscience of birdsong. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2008. pp. 240–255. [Google Scholar]
- Brainard & Doupe (2013).Brainard MS, Doupe AJ. Translating birdsong: songbirds as a model for basic and applied medical research. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2013;36:489–517. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brawn & Margoliash (2015).Brawn TP, Margoliash D. A bird’s eye view of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Current Topics in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2015;25:207–237. doi: 10.1007/7854_2014_349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brawn, Nusbaum & Margoliash (2010).Brawn TP, Nusbaum HC, Margoliash D. Sleep-dependent consolidation of auditory discrimination learning in adult starlings. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:609–613. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4237-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan & Smith (2015).Brennan AE, Smith ME. The decay of motor memories is independent of context change detection. PLOS Computational Biology. 2015;11:e1004278. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper & Goller (2006).Cooper BG, Goller F. Physiological insights into the social-context-dependent changes in the rhythm of the song motor program. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;95:3798–3809. doi: 10.1152/jn.01123.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crandall et al. (2007).Crandall SR, Adam M, Kinnischtzke AK, Nick TA. HVC neural sleep activity increases with development and parallels nightly changes in song behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:232–240. doi: 10.1152/jn.00128.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Criscimagna-Hemminger & Shadmehr (2008).Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R. Consolidation patterns of human motor-memory. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:9610–9618. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3071-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave & Margoliash (2000).Dave A, Margoliash D. Song replay during sleep and computational rules for sensorimotor vocal learning. Science. 2000;282:2250–2254. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derégnaucourt et al. (2005).Derégnaucourt S, Mitra PP, Fehér O, Pytte C, Tchernichovski O. How sleep affects the developmental learning of bird song. Nature. 2005;433:710–716. doi: 10.1038/nature03275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diekelmann & Born (2010).Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:114–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dragoi & Tonegawa (2011).Dragoi G, Tonegawa S. Preplay of future place cell sequences by hippocampal cellular assemblies. Nature. 2011;469:397–401. doi: 10.1038/nature09633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dragoi & Tonegawa (2013).Dragoi G, Tonegawa S. Distinct preplay of multiple novel spatial experiences in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110:9100–9105. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306031110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum (2015).Eichenbaum Does the hippocampus preplay memories? Nature Neurosciences. 2015;18:1701–1702. doi: 10.1038/nn.4180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elemans et al. (2008).Elemans CPH, Mead AF, Rome LC, Goller F. Superfast vocal muscles control song production in songbirds. PLOS ONE. 2008;3:e2581. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Euston, Tatsuno & McNaughton (2007).Euston DR, Tatsuno M, McNaughton BL. Fast-forward playback of recent memory sequences in prefrontal cortex during sleep. Science. 2007;318:1147–1150. doi: 10.1126/science.1148979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fee (2014).Fee MS. The role of efference copy in striatal learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2014;25:194–200. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franz & Goller (2002).Franz M, Goller F. Respiratory units of motor production and song imitation in the zebra finch. Journal of Neurobiology. 2002;51:129–141. doi: 10.1002/neu.10043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franz & Goller (2003).Franz M, Goller F. Respiratory patterns and oxygen consumption in singing zebra finches. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2003;206:967–978. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giret, Edeline & Del Negro (2017).Giret N, Edeline J-M, Del Negro C. Neural mechanisms of vocal imitation: the role of sleep replay in shaping mirror neurons. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;77:58–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glaze & Troyer (2006).Glaze CM, Troyer TW. Temporal structure in zebra finch song: implications for motor coding. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:991–1005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3387-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goller & Cooper (2004).Goller F, Cooper BG. Peripheral motor dynamics of song production in the zebra finch. Annals New York Academy of Science. 2004;1016:130–152. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goller & Cooper (2008).Goller F, Cooper BG. Peripheral mechanisms of sensorimotor integration during singing. In: Zeigler HP, Marler P, editors. Neuroscience of birdsong. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2008. pp. 99–114. [Google Scholar]
- Goller & Riede (2013).Goller F, Riede T. Integrative physiology of fundamental frequency control in birds. Journal of Physiology. 2013;107:230–242. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosmark & Buzsáki (2016).Grosmark AD, Buzsáki G. Diversity in neural firing dynamics supports both rigid and learned hippocampal sequences. Science. 2016;351:1440–1443. doi: 10.1126/science.aad1935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee (2002).Hahnloser RHR, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature. 2002;419:65–70. doi: 10.1038/nature00974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser, Kozhevnikov & Fee (2006).Hahnloser RHR, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. Sleep-related neural activity in a premotor and a basal-ganglia pathway of the songbird. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;96:794–812. doi: 10.1152/jn.01064.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hupbach et al. (2009).Hupbach A, Gomez RL, Bootzin RR, Nadel L. Nap-dependent learning in infants. Developmental Science. 2009;12:1007–1012. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen et al. (2007).Jensen KK, Cooper BG, Larsen ON, Goller F. Songbirds use pulse tone register in two voices to generate low frequency sound. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2007;274:2703–2710. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitago et al. (2013).Kitago T, Ryan SL, Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW, Haith AM. Unlearning versus savings in visuomotor adaptation: comparing effects of washout, passage of time, and removal of errors on motor memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:307. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korman et al. (2003).Korman M, Raz N, Flash T, Karni A. Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100:12492–12497. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2035019100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maquet (2001).Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science. 2001;294:1048–1052. doi: 10.1126/science.1062856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margoliash (2005).Margoliash D. Song learning and sleep. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:546–548. doi: 10.1038/nn0505-546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margoliash & Schmidt (2010).Margoliash D, Schmidt MF. Sleep, off-line processing, and vocal learning. Brain & Language. 2010;115:45–58. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Méndez et al. (2010).Méndez JM, Dall’Asén AG, Cooper BG, Goller F. Acquisition of an acoustic template leads to refinement of song motor gestures. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104:984–993. doi: 10.1152/jn.01031.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney (2009).Mooney R. Neurobiology of song learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2009;19:654–660. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm (2008).Nottebohm F. The discovery of replaceable neurons. In: Ziegler HP, Marler P, editors. The neuroscience of birdsong. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2008. pp. 425–448. [Google Scholar]
- Oberweger & Goller (2001).Oberweger K, Goller F. The metabolic cost of birdsong production. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2001;204:3379–3388. doi: 10.1242/jeb.204.19.3379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ölveczky et al. (2011).Ölveczky BP, Otchy TM, Goldberg JH, Aronov D, Fee MS. Changes in the neural control of a complex motor sequence during learning. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:386–397. doi: 10.1152/jn.00018.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orban et al. (2006).Orban P, Rauch G, Balteau E, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Maquet P, Peigneux P. Sleep after special learning promotes covert reorganization of brain activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:7124–7129. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510198103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyrache et al. (2009).Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Battaglia FP. Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:919–926. doi: 10.1038/nn.2337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prather et al. (2008).Prather JF, Peters S, Nowicki S, Mooney R. Precise auditory–vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication. Nature. 2008;451:305–310. doi: 10.1038/nature06492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pytte et al. (2012).Pytte CL, George S, Korman S, David E, Bogdan D, Kirn JR. Adult neurogenesis is associated with the maintenance of a stereotyped, learned motor behavior. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:7052–7057. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5385-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauske et al. (2010).Rauske PL, Chi Z, Dave AS, Margoliash D. Neuronal stability and drift across periods of sleep: premotor activity patterns in a vocal control nucleus of adult zebra finches. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:2783–2794. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3112-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ribeiro et al. (2004).Ribeiro S, Gervasoni D, Soares ES, Zhou Y, Lin S-Ch, Pantoja J, Lavine M, Nicolelis MAL. Long-lasting novelty-induced neuronal reverberation during slow-wave sleep in multiple forebrain areas. PLOS Biology. 2004;2:126–137. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rickard et al. (2008).Rickard TC, Cai DJ, Rieth CA, Jones J, Ard MC. Sleep does not enhance motor sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2008;34:834–842. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riede & Goller (2010).Riede T, Goller F. Peripheral mechanisms for vocal production in birds—differences and similarities to human speech and singing. Brain & Language. 2010;115:69–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts et al. (2008).Roberts TF, Klein ME, Kubke MF, Wild JM, Mooney R. Telencephalic neurons monosynaptically link brainstem and forebrain premotor networks necessary for song. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:3479–3489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0177-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, McLean & Goller (2012).Schmidt MF, McLean J, Goller F. Breathing and vocal control: the respiratory system as both a driver and a target of telencephalic vocal motor circuits in birds. Experimental Physiology. 2012;97:455–461. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2011.058669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shank & Margoliash (2009).Shank SS, Margoliash D. Sleep and sensorimotor integration during early vocal learning in a songbird. Nature. 2009;458:73–77. doi: 10.1038/nature07615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silva, Feng & Foster (2015).Silva D, Feng T, Foster DJ. Trajectory events across hippocampal place cells require previous experience. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1772–1779. doi: 10.1038/nn.4151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skaggs & McNaughton (1996).Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL. Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial experience. Science. 1996;271:1870–1873. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5257.1870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stickgold (2005).Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature. 2005;437(7063):1271–1278. doi: 10.1038/nature04286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suthers & Zollinger (2008).Suthers RA, Zollinger SA. From brain to song: the vocal organ and vocal tract. In: Zeigler HP, Marler P, editors. Neuroscience of birdsong. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2008. pp. 78–98. [Google Scholar]
- Tchernichovski et al. (2001).Tchernichovski O, Mitra PP, Lints T, Nottebohm F. Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science. 2001;291:2564–2569. doi: 10.1126/science.1058522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi & Cirelli (2014).Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron. 2014;81:12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troyer & Doupe (2000).Troyer TW, Doupe AJ. An associational model of birdsong sensorimotor learning I. Efference copy and the learning of song syllables. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;84:1204–1223. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.3.1204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchida et al. (2010).Uchida AM, Meyers RA, Cooper BG, Goller F. Fibre architecture and song activation rates of syringeal muscles are not lateralized in the European starling. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2010;213:1069–1078. doi: 10.1242/jeb.038885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaswani & Shadmehr (2013).Vaswani PA, Shadmehr R. Decay of motor memories in absence of error. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:7700–7709. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0124-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vicario (1991a).Vicario DS. Contributions of syringeal muscles to respiration and vocalization in the zebra finch. Journal of Neurobiology. 1991a;22:63–73. doi: 10.1002/neu.480220107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vicario (1991b).Vicario DS. Organization of zebra finch song control system: functional organization of outputs from nucleus robustus archistriatalis. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1991b;309:486–494. doi: 10.1002/cne.903090405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams (2008).Williams H. Birdsong and singing behavior. In: Zeigler HP, Marler P, editors. Neuroscience of birdsong. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2008. pp. 32–49. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson & McNaughton (1994).Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265:676–679. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood et al. (2013).Wood WE, Osseward PJ, Roseberry TK, Perkel DJ. A daily oscillation in the fundamental frequency and amplitude of harmonic syllables of zebra finch song. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e82327. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yanagihara & Hessler (2012).Yanagihara S, Hessler NA. Phasic basal ganglia activity associated with high-gamma oscillation during sleep in a songbird. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;107:424–432. doi: 10.1152/jn.00790.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang et al. (2014).Yang G, Lai CSW, Cichon J, Ma L, Li W, Gan W-B. Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science. 2014;344:1173–1178. doi: 10.1126/science.1249098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zann (1996).Zann RA. The zebra finch: a synthesis of field and laboratory studies. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1996. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
Goller, Franz (2017): GY63 daytime song. figshare.
https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5406718.v1.
Goller, Franz (2017): GY63 night. figshare.