Deciding between different choices: neurocognitive factors of decision-making and response variability
Perceptual decision-making tasks usually require subjects to recognize stimulus categories and select between different response alternatives. For example, in Go/No-go tasks, one has to respond to target stimuli and withhold responding to non-target stimuli. Accomplishing even just a single trial of such a task needs a complex sequence of functions (most of them executive) consisting, for example, in motor readiness, sustained attention, sensory processing, inhibitory control, conflict monitoring, stimulus-response mapping, context updating and, if any, error detection and awareness. In this context, the motor response reflects the behavioral outcome of the fast and proper interaction of the above-mentioned processes, and the response consistency (or variability) is often adopted as index of executive functioning.
Nowadays, one challenge of the cognitive neuroscience is to understand how executive functions allow to make decisions. In fact, understanding decisional processes, and reasons of decision failure, would be helpful to clarify the executive dysfunctions of clinical conditions such as obsessive compulsive disorders, impulsivity, and addictions (typically intended as a failure of inhibition; Chamberlain et al., 2005; Crews and Boettiger, 2009; Álvarez-Moya et al., 2011), as well as success in real-life tasks (e.g., car driving; Bunce et al., 2012) and goal-directed behaviors (e.g., complying with diet schedules; Jahanshahi et al., 2015). In this context, the response variability reflects a behavioral index of efficiency of frontal cognitive control (Bellgrove et al., 2004), and this association was suggested since the first half of the twentieth century, when Head (1926, p. 145) reported that “an inconsistent response is one of the most striking consequences of lesions to the cerebral cortex.” More recently, consistent literature indicated response variability as an indirect index of top down control (Tamm et al., 2012), executive functioning (Swick et al., 2013), neurological (Segalowitz et al., 1997; Hultsch et al., 2000), and psychiatric conditions (Barkley et al., 1992; Vinogradov et al., 1998; Leth-Steensen et al., 2000), and frontal lobes integrity (Bunce et al., 2007; Walhovd and Fjell, 2007; Lövdén et al., 2013). The frontal cortex is in fact considered as the main region supporting the executive functions and behavioral variability (Stuss et al., 2003), as revealed by the poor response consistency and accuracy of frontal patients performing a decision-making task (Arnot, 1952; Stuss et al., 1999, 2003; Picton et al., 2007).
Even though it is evident the relationship between executive functioning and performance variability at group level (e.g., in the comparison between high- and low-level athletes in sport; Vestberg et al., 2012), it is less known the mediating role of response variability at intra-individual level. Also, it is still not clear the mediating role of PFC activity in the intra-individual variability because of contrasting results of neuroimaging studies: in fact, two studies reported a greater dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) activation associated with high intra-individual variability (Bellgrove et al., 2004; Simmonds et al., 2007), while Weissman et al. (2006) reported reduced pre-stimulus activity of the right DLPFC in the less consistent trials. In other words, depending on the main findings, neuroimaging literature interpreted the high individual variability as the consequence of the greater need of top-down executive control (enhanced PFC activation), or in terms of lapses in attention (reduced PFC activation).
ERPs and executive functions: state of the art and main limitations
Identification of neurophysiological correlates of executive functioning requires to investigate different cognitive abilities, which in part depend on the experimental paradigm: for example, in Stroop or sustained attention tasks (Demeter and Woldorff, 2016), voluntary selective attention would be more stressed than Go/No-go tasks in which accumulation of sensory evidence would be determinant, or oddball tasks where decision making in effected by expectancy, or stop-signal tasks in which the so-called “reactive inhibition” is often required (for a review see Jahanshahi et al., 2015). It is also relevant to note that decisional processes work in a narrow temporal window, such as the time needed to perform a single trial in a speeded decision-making task. This constraint requires the researchers to adopt a technique with adequate temporal resolution to carry out their own investigations: this means that neuroimaging studies may not be the most suitable to investigate the fast temporal succession of the decisional processes. Moreover, as also suggested by Bogacz et al. (2010), the duration of the decision processes can affect the amplitude of the BOLD signal, therefore functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) findings should be interpreted with caution when studying the decision-making. In other terms, it could be possible that the long time needed to make a decision (leading to large response variability) explains the greater PFC activation reported by some studies (Bellgrove et al., 2004; Simmonds et al., 2007). At the opposite, even if less informative on the anatomical source of the observed activities, the electroencephalography (EEG), and especially the event-related potentials (ERPs) technique, is particularly appropriate to catch the fast succession of the decisional brain's events. However, most of the ERP literature in this field focused on post-response activities like the central-parietal P3 (Segalowitz et al., 1997; Saville et al., 2011), and the error-negativity (Ne) and error positivity (Pe) in case of error commission (Falkenstein et al., 1991, 1995, 1996). Similarly, when focused on the pre-movement activities, electrophysiological studies mainly observed the frontal-medial modulation of the N2 component (Bokura et al., 2001; van Boxtel et al., 2001; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003), whose functional role is still debated (Perri et al., 2015b; Di Russo et al., 2017). In other terms, the main limitation of ERP literature was the lack of a solid background on the contribution of the executive functions of the frontal cortex in the decisional processes. In fact, except for the motor preparation activities of the frontal areas, as reflected by the Bereitschaftspotential (BP; e.g., Shibasaki and Hallett, 2006) and the lateralized readiness potential (LRP; e.g., Rinkenauer et al., 2004), only the very recent literature started to report the ERP correlates of the PFC in the executive functioning and variability (for a review see Di Russo et al., 2017).
Looking into the frontal lobes: emerging evidence on the ERP correlates of executive functions
When performing a decision-making task, the contribution of the PFC executive functions is manifested mainly through cognitive processes like top-down attentional control, maintenance of information in the short-term memory, ability to ignore distractors and focus on relevant features, and inhibition of the wrong schema and selection of the appropriate one. Since even one of these processes would be able to affect the inter- and intra-individual variability of the performance, they should be dissociated and investigated separately.
Recent ERP studies described different pre-movement activities within the frontal cortex emerging both before and after the stimulus appearance in decisional tasks: as reviewed by Di Russo et al. (2017), there is a growing body of evidence defining the mediating role of these components in the variability of executive functions. It was shown that before the appearance of a stimulus, that is the preparation stage, at frontopolar sites is possible to detect the so called prefrontal negativity (pN) component, as shown in Figure 1A and in the left side of Figure 1C, together with the more posterior BP (e.g., Di Russo et al., 2013). The bilaterally-distributed pN was described as the electrophysiological correlate of the inferior frontal gyrus (iFg) activity (Di Russo et al., 2016; Sulpizio et al., 2017), especially involved in the proactive inhibitory (Perri et al., 2016; Bianco et al., 2017a,b,c) and top-down attentional control (Perri et al., 2014a, 2015a, 2017). Another ERP component is the dorsolateral pN (DLpN), that on the right hemisphere was associated to modulation of baseline levels in the accuracy system (i.e., the larger the right DLpN, the poorer the accuracy performance; Perri et al., 2014b; Lucci et al., 2016). The mediating role of the pN component in intra-individual variability emerged through studies that showed how enhancement of this activity predisposes subjects to effective inhibitory control (Perri et al., 2016) and, at the opposite, that its reduction predicts poor attentional control leading to response omission (Perri et al., 2017). A prefrontal activity compatible with the pN was described by West and Alain (2000) adopting a Stroop task: findings are consistent in revealing that momentary lapses of attention are associated with a pre-stimulus change in the ERP activity over the frontal regions. Moreover, it was shown that also inter-individual variability of performance was associated with the individual level of pN activity: in other words, the more consistent performers are marked by larger pN activity than the less consistent ones (Perri et al., 2015a). If the pN component can be described as a sort of readiness activity (a cognitive disposition in performing the task), there are also executive functions that work in the information processing stage, that is after the stimulus onset. In this regard, the ERP literature identified a complex of three components that were labeled as prefrontal N1 (pN1), prefrontal P1 (pP1), and prefrontal P2 (pP2): they were respectively associated with the sensory-awareness, the sensory-motor integration and the stimulus-response mapping process (for a review see Di Russo et al., 2017). The main generator of the prefrontal ERPs was localized in the bilateral anterior insula (Di Russo et al., 2016; Sulpizio et al., 2017), and these components are typically detected 80–400 ms after the stimulus appearance; however, while the pN1 and pP1 components reflect top-down perceptual processing of any stimulus to be processed (even in a passive-perception task), the later pP2 can be detected only in presence of decisional requests, that is the need to classify the information by matching it with the relative response, or stimulus-response mapping. Figure 1B shows the ERP waveforms of the pN1, pP1 and Pp2 together with the well-known N2 and P3. Figure 1C (right) shows the pP2 scalp topography concomitant to the N2. It is noteworthy that the pP2 component was also labeled as Go-P2 (Gajewski and Falkenstein, 2013), anterior P2 (P2a; Potts et al., 1996), frontal selection positivity (FSP; Kenemans et al., 1993), and frontal P3 (P3f; Makeig et al., 1999), and in all cases larger amplitudes were reported for target than non-target trials, regardless of task and response modality (finger movement, speech, silent count). Further, modulation of this component were repeatedly associated with groups difference in decisional speed (Perri et al., 2014b; Bianco et al., 2017c), such as with accuracy variability at both inter-individual (Di Russo et al., 2013; Perri et al., 2014b), and intra-individual level (Perri et al., 2015b, 2017).
Figure 1.
(A) Pre-stimulus ERP waveforms of the pN and BP components over medial prefrontal (Fpz) and frontal sites (Cz), respectively. (B) Post-stimulus pN1, pP1 and pP2 ERP components over Fpz; N2 and P3 components at Cz. (C) Scalp topography of the pN and BP (left), and of the pP2 and N2 (right). Data derived from Di Russo et al. (2016).
Concluding, there is a growing literature revealing the utility of the ERPs in the study of the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. In fact, even if with a less spatial resolution of neuroimaging scans, recent ERP literature has proven to be able in overcoming some concerns such as the source localization and the presence of artifacts in the anterior sites, that in the past may have limited the EEG investigation of the frontal executive functioning. At the opposite, the background reviewed here on the functional roles and generators of prefrontal ERPs suggest the latter as a promising tool to foster a new-way approach in the neurocognitive study of the executive functions. In fact, the extensive review of Di Russo et al. (2017) revealed the potential role of the prefrontal ERPs in identifying the cognitive factors mediating the variability of performance within subjects and between groups. Also, since it was shown that prefrontal ERPs are affected by neurological disease and susceptible of modifications as effect of rehabilitation (Di Russo et al., 2013) and sport training (Bianco et al., 2017b,c), future studies may clarify which cognitive factors could operate on them. Similarly, investigation of the prefrontal ERPs in clinical populations would be useful to shed new light on the strength relationship between prefrontal lesions and executive functions (for a review see Alvarez and Emory, 2006).
Author contributions
RP: conception and writing of the work; FD: contribution to writing and conception of the work, critical revision of the text; RP and FD: approved the final version of the manuscript.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
- Alvarez J. A., Emory E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 16, 17–42. 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnot R. (1952). A theory of frontal lobe function. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 67:487. 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1952.02320160071008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkley R. A., Grodzinsky G., DuPaul G. J. (1992). Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: a review and research report. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 20, 163–188. 10.1007/BF00916547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellgrove M. A., Hester R., Garavan H. (2004). The functional neuroanatomical correlates of response variability: evidence from a response inhibition task. Neuropsychologia 42, 1910–1916. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bianco V., Berchicci M., Perri R. L., Quinzi F., Di Russo F. (2017a). Exercise-related cognitive effects on sensory-motor control in athletes and drummers compared to non-athletes and other musicians. Neuroscience 360, 39–47. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bianco V., Berchicci M., Perri R. L., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. (2017b). The proactive self-control of actions: time-course of underlying brain activities. Neuroimage 156, 388–393 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bianco V., Di Russo F., Perri R. L., Berchicci M. (2017c). Different proactive and reactive action control in fencers' and boxers' brain. Neuroscience 343, 260–268. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bogacz R., Wagenmakers E. J., Forstmann B. U., Nieuwenhuis S. (2010). The neural basis of the speed-accuracy tradeoff. Trends Neurosci. 33, 10–16. 10.1016/j.tins.2009.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bokura H., Yamaguchi S., Kobayashi S. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 2224–2232. 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00691-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bunce D., Anstey K. J., Christensen H., Dear K., Wen W., Sachdev P. (2007). White matter hyperintensities and within-person variability in community-dwelling adults aged 60–64 years. Neuropsychologia 45, 2009–2015. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bunce D., Young M. S., Blane A., Khugputh P. (2012). Age and inconsistency in driving performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 49, 293–299. 10.1016/j.aap.2012.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain S. R., Blackwell A. D., Fineberg N. A., Robbins T. W., Sahakian B. J. (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 399–419. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crews F. T., Boettiger C. A. (2009). Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93, 237–247. 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demeter E., Woldorff M. G. (2016). Transient distraction and attentional control during a sustained selective attention task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 935–947. 10.1162/jocn_a_00949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Russo F., Berchicci M., Bozzacchi C., Perri R. L., Pitzalis S., Spinelli D. (2017). Beyond the “Bereitschaftspotential:” action preparation behind cognitive functions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 57–81. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Russo F., Berchicci M., Perri R. L., Ripani F. R., Ripani M. (2013). A passive exoskeleton can push your life up: application on multiple sclerosis patients. PLoS ONE 8:e77348 10.1371/journal.pone.0077348 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Russo F., Lucci G., Sulpizio V., Berchicci M., Spinelli D., Pitzalis S., et al. (2016). Spatiotemporal brain mapping during preparation, perception, and action. Neuroimage 126, 1–14. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falkenstein M., Hohnsbein J., Hoormann J. (1995). Event-related potential correlates of errors in reaction tasks. Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl. 44, 287–296. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falkenstein M., Hohnsbein J., Hoormann J. (1996). Differential processing of motor errors, in Recent Advance Event-Related Brain Potential Research, eds Ogura C., Koga Y., Shimokochi M. (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science; ), 579–585. [Google Scholar]
- Falkenstein M., Hohnsbein J., Hoormann J., Blanke L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophysiol. 78, 447–455. 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90062-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gajewski P. D., Falkenstein M. (2013). Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/No-go tasks. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 87, 273–278. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Head H. (1926). Aphasia and Kindred Disorders of Speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hultsch D. F., MacDonald S. W., Hunter M. A., Levy-Bencheton J., Strauss E. (2000). Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in older adults: comparison of adults with mild dementia, adults with arthritis, and healthy adults. Neuropsychology 14:588. 10.1037/0894-4105.14.4.588 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jahanshahi M., Obeso I., Rothwell J. C., Obeso J. A. (2015). A fronto-striato-subthalamic-pallidal network for goal-directed and habitual inhibition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16:719. 10.1038/nrn4038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenemans J. L., Kok A., Smulders F. T. Y. (1993). Event-related potentials to conjunctions of spatial frequency and orientation as a function of stimulus parameters and response requirements. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 88, 51–63. 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90028-N [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leth-Steensen C., King Elbaz Z., Douglas V. I. (2000). Mean response times, variability, and skew in the responding of ADHD children: a response time distributional approach. Acta Psychol. 104, 167–190. 10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00019-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lövdén M., Schmiedek F., Kennedy K. M., Rodrigue K. M., Lindenberger U., Raz N. (2013). Does variability in cognitive performance correlate with frontal brain volume? Neuroimage 64, 209–215. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucci G., Berchicci M., Perri R. L., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. (2016). Effect of target probability on pre-stimulus brain activity. Neuroscience 322, 121–128. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makeig S., Westerfield M., Jung T. P., Covington J., Townsend J., Sejnowski T. J., et al. (1999). Functionally independent components of the late positive event-related potential during visual spatial attention. J. Neurosci. 19, 2665–2680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Álvarez-Moya E. M., Ochoa C., Jiménez-Murcia S., Aymamí M. N., Gómez-Pea M., Fernández-Aranda F., et al. (2011). Effect of executive functioning, decision-making and self-reported impulsivity on the treatment outcome of pathologic gambling. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 36:165. 10.1503/jpn.090095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuis S., Yeung N., Van Den Wildenberg W., Ridderinkhof K. R. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a Go/No-go task: effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 17–26. 10.3758/CABN.3.1.17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perri R. L., Berchicci M., Lucci G., Cimmino R. L., Bello A., Di Russo F. (2014a). Getting ready for an emotion: specific premotor brain activities for self-administered emotional pictures. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:197. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perri R. L., Berchicci M., Lucci G., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. (2015a). The premotor role of the prefrontal cortex in response consistency. Neuropsychology 29:767. 10.1037/neu0000168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perri R. L., Berchicci M., Lucci G., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. (2015b). Why do we make mistakes? neurocognitive processes during the preparation-perception-action cycle and error-detection. Neuroimage 113, 320–328. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perri R. L., Berchicci M., Lucci G., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. (2016). How the brain prevents a second error in a perceptual decision making task. Sci. Rep. 6:32058. 10.1038/srep32058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perri R. L., Berchicci M., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. (2014b). Individual differences in response speed and accuracy are associated to specific brain activities of two interacting systems. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:251. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perri R. L., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. (2017). Missing the target: the neural processing underlying the omission error. Brain Topogr. 30, 352–363. 10.1007/s10548-017-0545-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picton T. W., Stuss D. T., Alexander M. P., Shallice T., Binns M. A., Gillingham S. (2007). Effects of focal frontal lesions on response inhibition. Cereb. Cortex 17, 826–838. 10.1093/cercor/bhk031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potts G. F., Liotti M., Tucker D. M., Posner M. I. (1996). Frontal and inferior temporal cortical activity in visual target detection: evidence from high spatially sampled event-related potentials. Brain Topogr. 9, 3–14. 10.1007/BF01191637 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rinkenauer G., Osman A., Ulrich R., Müller-Gethmann H., Mattes S. (2004). On the locus of speed-accuracy trade-off in reaction time: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133:261. 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saville C. W., Dean R. O., Daley D., Intriligator J., Boehm S., Feige B., et al. (2011). Electrocortical correlates of intra-subject variability in reaction times: average and single-trial analyses. Biol. Psychol. 87, 74–83. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segalowitz S. J., Dywan J., Unsal A. (1997). Attentional factors in response time variability after traumatic brain injury: an ERP study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 3, 95–107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibasaki H., Hallett M. (2006). What is the Bereitschaftspotential?. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 2341–2356. 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.04.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmonds D. J., Fotedar S. G., Suskauer S. J., Pekar J. J., Denckla M. B., Mostofsky S. H. (2007). Functional brain correlates of response time variability in children. Neuropsychologia 45, 2147–2157. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuss D. T., Murphy K. J., Binns M. A. (1999). The frontal lobes and performance variability: evidence from reaction time. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 5:123. [Google Scholar]
- Stuss D. T., Murphy K. J., Binns M. A., Alexander M. P. (2003). Staying on the job: the frontal lobes control individual performance variability. Brain 126, 2363–2380. 10.1093/brain/awg237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sulpizio V., Lucci G., Berchicci M., Galati G., Pitzalis S., Di Russo F. (2017). Hemispheric asymmetries in the transition from action preparation to execution. Neuroimage 148, 390–402. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swick D., Honzel N., Larsen J., Ashley V. (2013). Increased response variability as a marker of executive dysfunction in veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychologia 51, 3033–3040. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamm L., Narad M. E., Antonini T. N., O'Brien K. M., Hawk L. W., Jr., Epstein J. N. (2012). Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics 9, 500–508. 10.1007/s13311-012-0138-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Boxtel G. J., van der Molen M. W., Jennings J. R., Brunia C. H. (2001). A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm. Biol. Psychol. 58, 229–262. 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00117-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vestberg T., Gustafson R., Maurex L., Ingvar M., Petrovic P. (2012). Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players. PLoS ONE 7:e34731. 10.1371/journal.pone.0034731 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinogradov S., Poole J. H., Willis-Shore J., Ober B. A., Shenaut G. K. (1998). Slower and more variable reaction times in schizophrenia: what do they signify? Schizophr. Res. 32, 183–190. 10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00043-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walhovd K. B., Fjell A. M. (2007). White matter volume predicts reaction time instability. Neuropsychologia 45, 2277–2284. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman D. H., Roberts K. C., Visscher K. M., Woldorff M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 971–978. 10.1038/nn1727 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West R., Alain C. (2000). Evidence for the transient nature of a neural system supporting goal-directed action. Cereb. Cortex 10, 748–752. 10.1093/cercor/10.8.748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]