Abstract
Objectives
The demand for total knee arthroplasty (TKA) is increasing. Differentiating who will derive a clinically meaningful improvement from TKA from others is a key challenge for orthopaedic surgeons. Decision aids can help surgeons select appropriate candidates for surgery, but their uptake has been low. The aim of this study was to explore the barriers and facilitators to decision aid uptake among orthopaedic surgeons.
Design
A qualitative study involving face-to-face interviews. Questions were constructed on the Theoretical Domains Framework to systematically explore barriers and facilitators.
Setting
One tertiary hospital in Australia.
Participants
Twenty orthopaedic surgeons performing TKA.
Outcome measures
Beliefs underlying similar interview responses were identified and grouped together as themes describing relevant barriers and facilitators to uptake of decision aids.
Results
While prioritising their clinical acumen, surgeons believed a decision aid could enhance communication and patient informed consent. Barriers identified included the perception that one’s patient outcomes were already optimal; a perceived lack of non-operative alternatives for the management of end-stage osteoarthritis, concerns about mandatory cut-offs for patient-centred care and concerns about the medicolegal implications of using a decision aid.
Conclusions
Multifaceted implementation interventions are required to ensure that orthopaedic surgeons are ready, willing and able to use a TKA decision aid. Audit/feedback to address current decision-making biases such as overconfidence may enhance readiness to uptake. Policy changes and/or incentives may enhance willingness to uptake. Finally, the design/implementation of effective non-operative treatments may enhance ability to uptake by ensuring that surgeons have the resources they need to carry out decisions.
Keywords: Knee, qualitative research, Total Knee Arthroplasty, decision aids, implementation
Strengths and limitations of this study.
Very few qualitative studies involving orthopaedic surgeons have been published in the literature.
This study took place in one of the largest arthroplasty clinics in Australia. All 20 surgeons performing total knee arthroplasty in this hospital participated in a one-to-one interview.
A theoretical framework was used to systematically explore the barriers and facilitators to uptake of a decision aid by orthopaedic surgeons.
Consistent findings are documented between this single-site study and international surveys of surgeon’ attitudes.
Beliefs and attitudes are distinct from actual behaviour, and therefore the themes elicited in this study do not provide evidence of the actual influences on uptake of a decision aid.
Background
Up to one quarter of total knee arthroplasties (TKA) are performed on inappropriate candidates according to evidence-based criteria1 and a similar proportion experience minimal clinical benefit from surgery.2 The rates of TKA are increasing3; differentiating who will derive a clinically meaningful improvement from TKA from others is a key challenge for orthopaedic surgeons. While the degree of osteoarthritis (OA) severity, pain severity and the impact of pain are key indicators for TKA, surgeons’ consideration of other evidence-based indicators such as psychosocial factors remains varied.4–6 Observations of orthopaedic consultations suggest that other ‘unstated factors’ may also influence clinical judgements such as the surgeons’ beliefs in their own ability to conduct surgery and their ‘instincts’ about the patients’ ability to cope with pain.7
In recent years, there has been a move towards a model of shared decision making in orthopaedics as part of informed consent.8 9 This model implies that surgeons have a duty to inform patients about the benefits and harms of TKA and the likelihood of their occurrence, supporting them to arrive at an informed, shared decision. Evidence suggests that there is often a lack of time during the clinical encounter for patients to consider or discuss all available treatment options and arrive at a decision congruent with their own preferences.10
To help address variations in clinical judgements and promote shared decision making, decision aids may be useful.11 12 Decision aids can be designed to estimate important, patient-specific risks of responding to surgery, based on independent prognostic correlates of post-TKA response such as body mass index, degree of OA severity, preoperative pain, function and mental health.13 Similar aids have been found to predict outcomes in other areas of medicine with superior accuracy to clinical judgements alone.14 15 Decision aids have improved patient knowledge and confidence in decisions16 and have even been found to reduce the rate of surgical procedures.17
Orthopaedic surgeons recognise the need for an aid to support their decision making for TKA and optimise communication with patients.6 18 However, the uptake of decision aids among surgeons has been low.9 A number of factors can influence uptake of a decision aid, and the success of implementation efforts depends on the careful assessment of the barriers to, and facilitators of, uptake.19 The implementation literature advocates the use of theory to ensure the systematic identification of such factors and inform the design of interventions to address them.20 Using theory assists in designing studies that are better able to facilitate behaviour change21 and provides a basis for better understanding the processes underpinning behaviour change.22 To date, studies of decision making among orthopaedic surgeons have been few and atheoretical,6 7 23 and the barriers and facilitators of uptake of decision aids for TKA have not been rigorously explored.9 18
Methods
Aim and design
This theoretically informed qualitative study is the first phase of a wider project seeking to design and implement a decision aid into an Australian orthopaedic clinic setting. The aim of this study was to explore the barriers and facilitators to uptake of a TKA decision aid through structured one-to-one interviews with orthopaedic surgeons.
Participants
All orthopaedic surgeons and registrars performing TKA at one tertiary teaching hospital in Australia were eligible. Eligible candidates received the study details via email from an institutional administration officer and were invited to contact the researchers to arrange an interview at a time and place suitable to them.
Data collection
The Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF)24 was selected as a comprehensive and validated framework for determining barriers and facilitators related to the implementation of best practice and clinicians’ behaviour change. The TDF was developed by implementation scientists to synthesise existing behaviour change theories into a single framework. A six-stage consensus approach resulted in the identification of 12 theoretical ‘domains’ describing possible mediators of behaviour change.24 A subsequent validation study22 revised the original TDF to 14 domains: knowledge, skills, social/professional role and identity, beliefs about capabilities, optimism, beliefs about consequences, reinforcement, intentions, goals, memory, attention and decision processes, environmental context and resources, social influences, emotions and behavioural regulation. The revised TDF, used in this study, has explained implementation problems and informed the development of theory-informed behaviour change interventions.25–27
Interview questions were developed for each domain of the TDF with the advice of content experts in the TDF (SF), orthopaedic surgery (MD) and decision-making processes (AS), and in consultation with the literature.28 Interviews were prefaced by stating that the researchers planned to develop a decision aid, thus the discussion was centred around a hypothetical decision aid rather than a defined one. In the first part of the interview, questions aimed to elicit current decision-making processes and biases. In the second part, questions aimed to identify beliefs and attitudes towards decision aids and factors that may influence decisions to use one. Table 1 presents the interview schedule.
Table 1.
TDF domain | Questions |
Knowledge | Evidence from the literature suggests that up to 22% of patients presenting for TKA will not have a clinically meaningful improvement from surgery.
|
For the purposes of this interview, we are interpreting clinically meaningful improvement as no improvement in pain, function or QOL following surgery.
| |
Beliefs about capabilities |
|
Behavioural regulation |
|
Skills |
|
Beliefs about consequences | Based on a set of evidence-based parameters, decision aids can predict the degree of risk that a patient will not achieve a clinically meaningful improvement from TKA.
|
Intentions, goal |
|
Reinforcement |
|
Environmental context and resources |
|
Decision process |
|
Social/professional role and identity |
|
Social/emotional influences |
|
Optimism |
|
QOL, quality of life; TDF, Theoretical Domains Framework; TKA, total knee arthroplasty.
The interviewer (SB), a female postdoctoral researcher with methodological expertise in qualitative research, had no previous relationship with the participants and no affiliation with the hospital. Data saturation was considered complete when the beliefs and attitudes of all 20 surgeons working in this setting had been elicited. Face-to-face interviews were conducted with 18 participants in a private office; phone interviews were conducted with two participants. Interviews lasted 20–30 min. Nineteen interviews were audio recorded and transcribed verbatim. One participant did not wish the interview to be recorded, therefore hand-written notes were made during the interview. Participant anonymity was ensured at all times. All transcripts were deidentified prior to data analysis. All participants had the opportunity to review the study findings during a presentation at a scheduled surgical meeting. There was consensus agreement with the researchers’ interpretations and no adjustments were made to the study themes.
Analysis
Adopting an implementation approach,26 three stages of data analysis were conducted. In the first stage, two researchers (SB and EN) independently coded interview transcripts by classifying each interview response or utterance into one of the 14 TDF domains. For example, this response to the first question in the interview schedule: ‘I think 22 per cent is the high end. But there are a lot of different papers that all suggest 10, 15, 20 per cent', was classified into the ‘Knowledge’ domain of the TDF. Definitions for each domain were derived from the literature19 and adapted to the study context. Pilot coding was performed in which the two researchers independently coded two transcripts. Intercoder comparisons resulted in the refinement of domain definitions (see online supplementary file). This process was conducted three times, until the two researchers were confident that all relevant interview responses could be clearly coded into one domain. The two researchers then independently coded all 20 transcripts. Disagreements were discussed, and consensus was reached in each instance. Coded responses were uploaded into a qualitative data sorting software (Codesort29) to facilitate further analysis.
bmjopen-2017-018614supp001.pdf (34.8KB, pdf)
In the second stage of analysis, one researcher (SB) generated ‘belief statements’ based on the coded interview responses. For example, from the response: ‘I think 22 per cent is the high end. But there are a lot of different papers that all suggest 10, 15, 20 per cent’ classified in the ‘Knowledge’ domain, we generated the belief statement: ‘I am aware of the literature that up to 20 per cent of patients do not have a clinically meaningful improvement from TKA’. Belief statements were worded such that they could describe similar responses from different participants. Belief statements were reviewed by two further researchers (EN and MD), before being interpreted as a likely ‘facilitator’ or ‘barrier’ to surgeon’s uptake of a decision aid. Continuing the example above, the belief statement: ‘I am aware of the literature that up to 20 per cent of patients do not have a clinically meaningful improvement from TKA’ was interpreted as a facilitator to uptake, in that we considered surgeons would be more likely to use a decision aid if they were aware that a substantial proportion of TKA’s resulted in suboptimal outcomes.
In the third stage of analysis, we identified the domains most likely to influence surgeon’s behaviour (ie, using a decision aid or not). This was determined by: (1) frequency of beliefs across transcripts and (2) the perceived strength of beliefs in influencing behaviour. To illustrate, the belief statement: ‘I think that the percentage of my patients who achieve clinically meaningful improvement is higher than that reported in the literature’ appeared in 17/20 transcripts and was considered to have a strong influence on whether a surgeon would use a decision aid or not, implying that behaviour change (ie, using a decision aid) was unlikely if surgeons considered that their patients’ outcomes were already optimised. ‘Knowledge’ was therefore identified as a relevant domain. Where the researchers considered that beliefs within and between domains represented similar barriers/facilitators, these were grouped into themes. We present frequencies of beliefs (see table 2) to provide the reader with a better understanding of the range of interview responses and to assist us in identifying ‘relevant’ domains of the TDF. However, readers should be cognisant that the absence of a belief in a transcript is not the same as a lack of endorsement.
Table 2.
Relevant TDF domains | Specific belief | Facilitator or barrier | Example quote (participant code) | Frequency out of 20 |
Knowledge | I am aware of the literature that up to 20% of patients do not have a CMI from TKA. | Facilitator | ‘I think 22 per cent is the high end. But there are a lot of different papers that all suggest 10, 15, 20 per cent’. (012) | 19 |
I think that this % is lower in my patients. | Barrier | ‘I don’t count it, but I think around 10 per cent would be saying they aren’t entirely satisfied by surgery’. (016) | 17 | |
Any improvement in pain is still an improvement; it depends how you define ‘meaningful’. | Barrier | ‘If the surgery is done for the right reason, the pain would decrease, the question is whether the decrease would be 10, 50 or 100 per cent depending on whether there are other reasons for the pain. But there would be an improvement’. (025) ‘To me a good result is: they are going to have some intermittent ache in the knee, they’re not going to be able to kneel or squat. Others on some assessment scale might consider that a failure. So you have get those parameters right’. (010) |
7 | |
Behavioural regulation | I am aware that the feedback I get from my patients may be biased. | Facilitator | ‘To please you, patients often say it is doing better than it really is. So I would think my outcomes are better than 20 per cent, but I am aware of the glasses that I see it through as well as what patients might tell me’. (014) | 6 |
I would be interested in feedback on the percentage of my patients who achieve a clinically meaningful improvement. | Facilitator | ‘There’s always a difference between how well you think you are doing and how you are doing. Having formal feedback on patient outcomes gives you the opportunity to change things if you are not doing as well as you want to’. (023) ‘What I would like to know is the patient who overall is unhappy with their results and didn’t get the result they expected’. (010) ‘I would like feedback on the number of patients who are in each category of satisfaction and I would like to see how my personal results are compared to the group’. (016) |
20 | |
Memory, attention and decision processes | Patient expectations are an important consideration in surgical decision making. | Facilitator | ‘If the patients’ expectations are not meeting mine, I won’t do the operation because then the patient isn’t happy and sometimes they have 2/10 pain and they are not happy. And that is silly. So it is about telling the patient what they can expect and after the operation it is about you remember what we said’. (013) | 20 |
The lack of effective non-operative alternatives influences my surgical decision making. | Barrier | ‘You have to be able to say ‘although we don’t think you would benefit from surgery, we’re going to put you in this intense physiotherapy program with dieticians to improve your knee pain. They need to be offered something. The problem is these things are available at an individual component level… but I don’t think there is anything formally put in place that patients can be referred from arthroplasty clinics into these program’. (029) ‘Well if you make an alternative plan and say we are not going to do surgery we are going to lose weight, do some physiotherapy, take pain killers, you send them off and they come back and say they have done all of that. It means you don’t have another option to offer them and those patients often just want an option and if there is an option you can give them it is easier to push them away from surgery’. (016) ‘I think there are limitations on what you can improve with non-operative measures’. (016) |
12 | |
My threshold of acceptable risk for surgery is >80 per cent likelihood of good outcome. | Facilitator | ‘You have got to be 95 per cent and above. I wouldn’t accept anything less than that. I wouldn’t offer the operation. It is too big an operation, to bigger deal, too bigger cost’. (024) | 8 | |
My level of acceptable risk is patient dependent. | Facilitator (of shared decision making) | ‘It is all about risk for reward. When you think about… the person is not unwell, they can safely have an anaesthetic, even risks as high as 50 per cent one in two that the patient will have no benefit, are worth considering… A patient may be so severely impacted that a 1 in 2 shot is worth it…it is totally patient dependent’. (023) ‘I would rather a 10 per cent chance of getting better than sitting in a wheelchair in a lot of pain’. (022) |
11 | |
Beliefs about capabilities | I find it difficult to assess the patient-related factors that can influence TKA outcome. | Facilitator | ‘It is patient factors more than anything else. Because it is easy to look at xrays and say Kellgren-Lawrence scale, 1, 2, 3, 4 for disease severity. There’s not much argument over that. It’s about the patient factors, the psychology and behavioural aspects of it which you want reassurance for’. (016) ‘Obviously I am not very good because 1 in 5 come back with a problem… so no I didn’t know how to identify them pre- operatively. Something is happening from my assessment to the patients’ outcome and I don’t know what the link is’. (024) |
8 |
I am reasonably good at picking the patients who will do well. | Barrier | ‘I think I am reasonably good… I do have a little bit of a gut feeling about patients’. (013) | 12 | |
It can be difficult to say no to patients. | Facilitator | ‘Most of the time if we bring a patient to the case conference it is to get the support of everyone else to say no don’t do it. Because if want to do the operation, you just go ahead and do it. If you don’t want to do it and you want support that is when you take them along’. (016) ‘It is always easier to consent than decline’. (025) |
5 | |
Skills | I mostly rely on my experience when it comes to surgical decision making. | Barrier | ‘You spend all your life looking at patients and assessing them and you start to develop a bit of a gut feeling as to what might be happening. Sometimes you sit in front a patient and think: I know you are telling me this, but I know something else is happening’. (015) ‘I don’t use any formal tools. I use I guess old fashioned clinical acumen is what I would call it…I have been doing this for a while and you develop a way of assessing people’. (028) |
10 |
Social/professional role and identity | Surgery is an art and a science – it is not just about the evidence. | Barrier | ‘The human body is not a scientific machine. Medicine is an art and science and the art isn’t always represented in the research’. (028) ‘I think that medicine is not about numbers, it is about patients. Each patient has their own different pathology and own different personality’. (017) |
10 |
Beliefs/attitudes towards a decision aid | ||||
Intention | I would use a decision aid to support, not replace my decision-making. | Facilitator | ‘I don’t think it would really influence my surgical decision making, I think it would more affirm my decision to not offer a patient an operation’. (029) ‘If I think they are ok and they score badly I will relook at it and say why is that? Am I missing something obvious? But at the end of the day if an aid says one thing and my sniff test says there is something not right, I’m still following my nose’. (010) |
16 |
Beliefs about consequences | I think a decision aid would be a useful objective tool to help me say no to patients. | Facilitator | ‘It would be clinically helpful in the patient cohort who we don’t think will do well from surgery, giving us an evidenced based approach for saying this is the reasons why we don’t think you will benefit from surgery’. (029) ‘I think that the main benefit of an aid would be making the patient understand if I am saying no to the surgery it’s not because I don’t like him or her, it is because there is data written black on white that they are not going to do well…It will not just be my gut feeling. I can give them data and say sorry it is written here. It’s not me it’s the computer. So it backs up what I am saying’. (013) |
9 |
I think an aid would be useful for gaining patient informed consent and shared decision making. | Facilitator | ‘I think that is one of the important things about a decision aid and part of the consent process is that they know what to expect and it is still the patients decision to decide if they want to have surgery or not, but they have to be appropriately informed and have the appropriate expectations to weigh up the risk and benefit’. (019) ‘It comes back down to getting patient consent, as part of that I would incorporate it into my consent form and say preoperatively you have a 50:50 chance and that has been discussed with a validated tool’. (021) |
10 | |
I think a decision aid has the potential to improve the use of resources and save costs. | Facilitator | ‘If you could use a decision aid to triage patients and push them somewhere else, it would be more effective for the patient and there would be cost savings for the hospital and the community’. (016) | 7 | |
A disadvantage of a decision aid is that it may not capture the nuances of the individual patient and some patients may miss out on surgery. | Barrier | ‘There are always reasons why people will fall on one side of the line or the other and the data will show that the tool might predict you will do really well but you happen to fall in that small group who are set to do really well but don’t, similarly the tool might say you will do really badly we better not operate on you but someone took the punt and you turned out really well so there are always those smaller groups and at times it is possible for the tool to miss certain nuances’. (015) | 13 | |
I have concerns about the legal/ethical implications of a decision aid | Barrier | ‘You have to think of the medico-legal implications of a patient having a risk value documented in their notes. If they don’t have a good result and then lawyers look through and say you had this validated tool and you still went ahead, where would we lie medico-legally?’. (024) ‘I guess the ethicists would say you are denying patient-centred care, so that is where there is a potential for a can of worms’. (021) |
8 | |
Environmental context and resources (how the tool might be implemented) | I would not like to see a decision aid with mandatory cut-offs implemented. | –* | ‘I don’t think there are things that can become compulsory in terms of a decision aid as I mentioned because it takes away patient-centred care’. (025) ‘No you can’t make anything compulsory like that. Not in medicine. Medicine is not black and white, it is grey, you can never make anything compulsory because a surgeon will operate according to their experience’. (024) ‘Surgeons wouldn’t care if it was compulsory to use an aid, as long as they didn’t have to do any work. Making it compulsory to follow it would be dangerous. Because we’re all individuals, what you are doing is taking the human experience aspect of the consultation out and then you turn us into proceeduralists that just look at a tick box and operate on someone’. (016) |
17 |
I don’t think surgeons could ever agree on a cut-off level on a decision aid. | –* | ‘A lot of surgeons would say in their hands they will get better results, that is just an inherent bias associated with surgical procedures and surgeons themselves so it would be hard to agree on a level’. (019) ‘Unless you can clearly demonstrate a certain cut-off does better, so until there is almost black and white there will be some shades of grey and surgeons will differ in those shades of grey. And even if there is evidence you will still get surgeons that will reject it. That is just my feeling’. (021) |
17 | |
I could see an electronic or online tool working well in my practice. | Facilitator | ‘I can imagine something working on the phone, just an app. Simple and intuitive so you put in a little info - BMI, age, degree of arthritis etc tick tick tick. And then it gives you the number, bang’. (013) ‘A lot of patients look on my website. You could have a thing on your website saying: ‘sometimes patients with certain problems may not be appropriate for a TKA, this test can give you a rough idea of your likely success rate’. You could put it out there before they even come to see you. ‘Is this operation for you?’ type of thing’. (028) |
6 | |
Time would be a key concern to using a decision aid in my practice. | Barrier | ‘I just couldn’t use a tool that is going to take up more time. There is already so much demands on our time and there is not enough time as it is. So the tool may only take 5 min but then you add 4, 5, 6 patients and that is half an hour extra of your time that you didn’t have’. (022) ‘It is frantic getting patients through and there is always that rush to see all the patients in a really short time and to spend 5–10 minutes to do a questionnaire with a patient… it is hard to justify that. But if there was something validated and it was done as a routine process, the patient came in with a form filled out with a score that would be really nice’. (016) |
6 | |
Reinforcement | Evidence that tool had been widely validated would not convince me to use it. I would need to correlate it with my own clinical decision making. | –* | ‘I never trust evidence because you only have to go to Dr x …even in research, there is a lot of doubtful stuff. You’ve got to be careful about basing something totally on results. I know we have got to be evidenced-based but the evidence may apply to a certain situation in a certain individual at a period in time and there is always variations or exceptions around that. So I would try and correlate them in my own mind and if after a while I am seeing well that person is a bit odd and they are scoring badly on the aid, well ok, this has legs’. (010) ‘I trust [the research] but I want my data no doubt about it because I think I am better… I know lots of faults in techniques or little things that really can comprise outcome. So everyone has a different hand and surgery is very touchy practice…. So I believe what happens around but at the same time I want mine as well because I know what I do differently or I am more careful about’. (013) |
9 |
I would be more likely to trust a tool developed and implemented by my peers. | Facilitator | ‘If a decision aid is implemented and I see my colleagues implementing it and it is working in their hands then possibly that would convince me’. (024) ‘I think people are mistrustful of things that come out of other institutions but I would trust that a study from [the Department] would be a rigorous design. Where people are invested in something, they’re much more likely to use it. If the results showed an aid was valid, I guess I would be prepared to try it and see whether I thought it was valid in my hands, in my practice’. (026) |
4 | |
Goals | My goal is to optimise patient outcomes. | Facilitator | ‘Certainly, surgeons want results. If you say you are going to reduce our risk, then why wouldn’t we be happy with that’. (012) | 20 |
*‘−’ denotes that the belief may be either a facilitator or barrier depending on how an aid is implemented.
TKA, total knee arthroplasty. BMI, body mass index. CMI, clinically meaningful improvement
Results
Participants
Aggregate data describing the demographics of the sample is provided to protect individual anonymity. The sample comprised of 15 consultant surgeons and 5 registrars. The surgeons’ total experience performing TKA ranged from 6 months to 30 years (mean±SD: 12.9±9.3), and the number of TKAs performed each month ranged from less than 1 to 12 (mean±SD: 5.9±3.0).
Relevant domains
Across the 20 interview transcripts, 628 utterances were coded into the 14 domains. Eleven domains of the TDF were identified as relevant: knowledge, behavioural regulation, memory attention and decision processes, beliefs about capabilities, skills, goals, social/professional role and identity, intention, beliefs about consequences, environmental context and reinforcement. Table 2 presents the relevant domains, with specific belief statements supported by example quotes.
The seven themes are described below, illustrated by interview extracts found in table 3 and denoted in the text as ‘Quote’ numbers, for example, Q1.
Table 3.
Quote number | Quote (participant code) |
Q1 | ‘Ultimately, we will always do our best for the patient’. (024) |
Q2 | ‘I don’t count it, but you get an impression. Around 10 per cent of my patients would be saying they are not entirely satisfied by surgery’. (016) |
Q3 | ‘Often, to please you, patients say that it is doing better than it really is. So I would think my outcomes are better than 20 per cent, but I am aware of the glasses that I see it through as well as what patients might tell me’. (014) |
Q4 | ‘There’s always a difference between how well you are doing and how well you think you are doing. Having formal feedback on patient outcomes gives you the opportunity to change things if you are not doing as well as you want to’. (023) |
Q5 | ‘If patients choose not to come back, the only way you have got to track them is looking at your results from the registry. But I want to know the answers to the clinical questions – are you happy? Is your pain better than it was pre-op? How you ask the question matters’. (028) |
Q6 | ‘If you received feedback that the rate of clinically meaningful improvement reported by your patients is not as high as you think it should be, you have to look at whether you are not picking the right patients, or you are operating on patients that are not going to do well. I think it would be more likely to be the way the question is asked. I would want to check who is asking the questions, what they are asking and how they are asking it’. (023) |
Q7 | ‘To me a good result is: they are going to have some intermittent ache in the knee, they are not going to be able to kneel or squat, they are going to be aware that it is there. That to me is a good result. Now others on some assessment scale they might say well that is in our system considered a failure thing, so you have get those parameters right’. (010) |
Q8 | ‘At the end of the day if there is a pathology that can be deleted by surgery and the patient accepts some improvement then that means that the surgery will happen’. (025) |
Q9 | ‘If the patients’ expectations are not meeting mine, I won’t do the operation because then the patient isn’t happy and sometimes they have 2/10 pain and they are not happy’. (013) |
Q10 | ‘It is patient factors more than anything else. Because it is very easy for me to look at xrays and use the Kellgren-Lawrence scale: 1, 2, 3, 4 for disease severity. There is not much of an argument over that. It is about the patient factors, the psychology and behavioural aspects of it which is more what you want reassurance for’. (016) |
Q11 | ‘You spend all your life looking at patients and assessing them and you start to develop a bit of a gut feeling as to what might be happening when you sit in front of a patient and you might be saying you know you are telling me this but actually I know something else is happening’. (015) |
Q12 | ‘I don’t think it would really influence my surgical decision making, I think it would more affirm my decision to not offer a patient an operation’. (029) |
Q13 | ‘If I think they are OK and they score badly I will relook at it and say why is that? Am I missing something obvious? But at the end of the day if the tool says one thing and my sniff test says there is something not right, I am still following my nose’. (010) |
Q14 | ‘Not every tool is perfect and it may not capture every patient… the danger is we may end up refusing to do something because of this tool and therefore the patient may not receive the appropriate treatment based on a decision aid and nothing is 100 per cent so you have to expect some patients would fall through the cracks’. (019) |
Q15 | ‘I think people are mistrustful of things that come out of other institutions but I would trust that a study from [the Department] would be a rigorous design. Where people are invested in something, they are much more likely to use it. If the results showed the tool was valid, I guess I would be prepared to try it and see whether I thought it was valid in my hands, in my practice’. (026) |
Q16 | ‘I never trust evidence because you only have to go to Dr x …even in research, there is a lot of doubtful stuff and you have got to be careful about basing something totally on results. I know we have got to be evidenced based but the evidence may apply to a certain situation in a certain individual at a period in time and there is always variations or exceptions around that. So I would try and correlate them in my own mind and if after a while I am seeing well that person is a bit odd and they are scoring badly on that, well ok, this has legs’. (010) |
Q17 | ‘I think that the main benefit of a tool would be making the patient understand if I am saying no to the surgery it is not because I don’t like him or her, it is because there is data written black on white that they are not going to do well…It will not just be my gut feeling. I can give them data and say "sorry it is written here. It is not me it is the computer". So it backs up what I am saying’. (013) |
Q18 | ‘It comes back down to getting patient consent, as part of that I would incorporate it into my consent form and say preoperatively you have a 50:50 chance and that has been discussed with a validated tool. If the patient wishes to go ahead, they can make that informed decision’. (021) |
Q19 | ‘A patient may be so severely impacted that a 1 in 2 shot is worth it…it is totally patient dependent’. (023) |
Q20 | ‘You have got to be 95 per cent and above. I wouldn’t accept anything less than that. I wouldn’t offer the operation. It is too big an operation, too big a deal, too big a cost’. (024) |
Q21 | ‘You have to think the medico-legal implications of a patient having a risk value documented in their notes. If they don’t have a good result and then some have the lawyers look through and say you had this tool that was validated and you still went ahead where would we lie medico-legally?’. (024) |
Q22 | ‘I guess the ethicists would say you are denying patient-centred care, so that is where there is a potential for a can of worms’. (021) |
Q23 | ‘I don’t think it can become compulsory because it takes away patient-centred care’. (025) |
Q24 | ‘If you could use the tool to triage patients and push them some where else, it would be more effective for the patient and there would be cost savings for the hospital and the community’. (016) |
Q25 | ‘You have to be able to say: "although we don’t think you would benefit from surgery, we are going to put you in this intense physiotherapy program with dieticians and this is how we are going to improve your knee pain’" They need to be offered something. The problem is these things are available at an individual component level - we have got dieticians and physiotherapists and exercise groups, but I don’t think there is anything formally put in place that patients can be referred from arthroplasty clinics into these program’. (029) |
Q26 | ‘A lot of surgeons would say in their hands they will get better results, that is just an inherent bias associated with surgical procedures and surgeons themselves so it would be hard to agree on a level’. (019) |
Q27 | ‘Well compulsory to have it? Ok. That would be easy to do and surgeons wouldn’t care as long as they didn’t have to do any work. Making it compulsory to follow it would be dangerous. Because we are all individuals, what you are doing is taking the human experience aspect of the consultation out and then you turn us into proceeduralists that just look at a tick box and operate on someone’. (016) |
Q28 | ‘I can imagine something working on the phone, an app. It needs to be simple and intuitive - so you put in a little info - BMI, age, degree of arthritis etc… tick tick tick. And then it gives you the number, bang’. (013) |
Q29 | ‘I think it is something that should be done by the surgeon. It is also part of the process where the surgeon gets to know the patient as well - not just their xrays and physical examination but also their psychosocial situation’. (019) |
Q30 | ‘I would want the tool to be applied within the consultation. Because I would never believe a value until I have seen the person. Because we might just have one of those weird situations that fall out of the ‘normal’ range’. (010) |
Q31 | ‘When you have got 10 minutes for a consultation you don’t have time to spend another 10 minutes going through the tool. So it would have to be either the patient themselves or secretarial person prior to the consultation’. (012) |
Q32 | ‘I have a lot of patients look me up on my website. You could have a thing on your website saying: ‘sometimes patients with certain problems may not be appropriate for a TKA, this test can give you a rough idea of your success rate’. You could put it out there before they even come to see you. ‘Is this operation for you?’ type of thing’. (028) |
Themes identified
Knowledge of one’s own patient outcomes
(Relevant TDF domains: Goals, Knowledge, Behavioural regulation and Beliefs about capabilities)
The goal of all participations was to optimise outcomes for their patients (Q1). While almost all participants (n=19) were aware of the literature that up to 20 per cent of patients undergoing TKA have no clinically meaningful improvement from surgery, most believed that this percentage was significantly lower in the patients they operated on (Q2). All participants based this estimation on patient presentations at postsurgical follow-up appointments. However, participants acknowledged the ‘rose-coloured glasses’ they saw their patients through, citing the subtleties of rapport that made patients less likely to report dissatisfaction with surgery (Q3).
It was also recognised that patients with poor post-surgical outcomes may be less likely to attend follow-up appointments, choosing to seek care elsewhere. Participants believed that tracking long-term patient outcomes through an existing joint registry could counteract this (Q4 and Q5).
However, a key problem identified by many participants was how to define clinically meaningful improvement. Seven surgeons emphasised the importance of ‘asking the right question in the right way’ (Q6 and Q7). The discrepancy between surgeons’ perception of clinically meaningful improvement and that of their patients was commonly attributed to mismatched surgeon–patient pre-surgical expectations of TKA. Surgeons reported their expectations of TKA to be a resolution of joint pathology with some corresponding improvement in pain. While disease and pain severity were key considerations in surgical decision making, all participants acknowledged that patient expectations were important (Q8 and Q9).
Reliance on ‘clinical intuition’
(Relevant TDF domains: Memory, attention and decision processes and Skills)
The accurate assessment of patient expectations presented a challenge for many surgeons. While the physical aspects of the clinical assessment were routine skills that all participants believed they had mastered well, many junior and senior surgeons reported difficulties assessing the psychological aspects of the patients’ presentation (Q10).
Only a few participants were aware of any validated tools to assess pre-surgical patient predictive factors. One participant had prior knowledge of a decision aid but had not used it. None of the other participants were aware of any decision aid for TKA. Participants relied on their ‘clinical intuition’ for patients who were less likely to do well. A ‘gut-feeling’ for patients was developed with experience over time (Q11).
The role of aids in supporting clinical decision making
(Relevant TDF domains: Intention and Reinforcement)
Fifteen of the participants believed an aid would support decision making, like a ‘barometer’ or weather app to forecast outcomes, particularly in patients they were unsure about (Q12, Q13). However, most participants (n=13) believed an aid would be insensitive to nuances at an individual level and could therefore not replace their clinical acumen (Q14).
All participants expected to be provided with evidence that a decision aid had been rigorously validated and shown to have high specificity and sensitivity before considering using it. Participants were more likely to trust this evidence if it came from their own institution (Q15). However, half of the participants reported that evidence supporting the validity of a decision aid would not be sufficient to convince them to use it. Instead they would need to see how the tool correlated with their own clinical decision making (Q16).
Implications of a decision aid for patient–surgeon communication and shared decision making
(Relevant TDF domains: Beliefs about consequences and Memory attention and decision processes)
Nine participants perceived that a decision aid would give them an evidence-based approach for saying ‘no’ to patient, particularly for those participants who reported difficulty declining for surgery (Q17). Many participants (n=10) believed an aid could be an important component of patient informed consent, providing patients with objective data on their likely risk outcomes, enabling them to have the appropriate expectations to weigh up the risk and benefits of surgery for themselves. In this way, a decision aid was seen as a valuable support to shared decision making (Q18).
When asked how they would feel about operating if a decision aid predicted a patient had a 50 per cent chance of not responding to TKA, surgeons were divided in their responses. While 11 believed that decisions to proceed would need to be shared with the patient (Q19), eight reported they would not consider surgery unless there was a greater than 80% per cent chance the patient would respond (Q20).
Ethical and legal concerns about decision aids
(Relevant TDF domain: Beliefs about consequences)
Eight participants had medicolegal concerns about documenting a specific risk value in patient records, with a few believing that such information would have to be deliberately withheld from patients in the case it fell into the ‘wrong’ hands (Q21). While some participants believed it would be unethical not to use a decision aid if it had been shown to improve patient outcomes, others were concerned about the ethical implications of a tool if imposed cut-offs were used to deny patients’ surgery (Q22 and Q23).
Available resources and organisational culture as barriers to uptake
(Relevant TDF domains: Environmental context and resources, Beliefs about consequences and Social/professional role and identity)
Almost all participants expressed concerns about making an aid compulsory and imposing mandatory cut-off levels. While many recognised that implementing the tool in this way would have the potential to improve the use of valuable health resources and save costs (Q24), several participants commented that mandatory cut-offs would only be possible if an effective, non-operative alternative was made available for patients that were denied surgery. The existing lack of effective alternative to surgery was seen as a key barrier (Q25).
There was widespread agreement among surgeons (n=17) that the logistics of agreeing on a cut-off value for acceptable risk, and the threat to surgeons’ professional identity as a patient-centred practitioner, were insurmountable barriers to imposing mandatory cut-offs (Q26 and Q27).
Format and content of a decision aid
(Relevant TDF domain: Environmental context and resources)
Finally, physically integrating an aid into clinical practice was not seen as a key barrier to implementation. An electronic or online format was seen as the most likely way an aid could be implemented, particularly given the strict time constraints placed on outpatient clinics (Q28).
Most believed that an aid would be best used within the patient–surgeon consultation (Q29 and Q30), while a couple suggested that an aid could be designed for patients to use on their own or with a support network to save time in the clinical consultation (Q31 and Q32).
Irrelevant domains
Three domains did not appear to have a salient influence on the target behaviour. These were: Optimism, Emotion and Social influences. Optimism was closely connected to the domain reinforcement; that is, surgeons reported being neither optimistic nor pessimistic about the utility of an aid until they had seen evidence of its effectiveness. The Emotion domain did not appear relevant as described by one surgeon: ‘I am not worried about the implications of a tool. TKA is wellness surgery, in the worst case scenario they don’t get a new knee’. The Social influence of patients did not appear relevant as surgeons believed patients were accustomed to filling out questionnaires. While surgeons were more likely to trust an aid if it had been developed by their peers, they preferred to test the tool ‘in their own hands’ and therefore the social influences of other surgeons using an aid appeared limited.
Discussion
These findings have implications for the future design and implementation of decision aids into surgical clinical practice. It is possible that current decision-making biases may be key barriers to uptake. Surgical decision making involves the consideration of the risks versus benefits of surgery.30 In this study, participants expressed confidence in their ability to successfully remove diseased tissue and correct joint deformity, and the likelihood of some corresponding improvement in pain severity was perceived to be high. At the same time, participants perceived that the risk their patients would not respond to TKA was low. One potential explanation for this is an apparent lack of clarity around the definition of ‘clinically meaningful improvement’ and ‘non-response’. The literature suggests that patients’ perceptions of ‘clinically meaningful improvement’ depend on their satisfaction with improvements in pain and function, closely linked to their expectations of surgery.31 In contrast, the surgeons in this study believed that a resolution in joint pathology and some corresponding improvement pain could be considered ‘meaningful improvement’. This lack of clarity is important, as surgeons who believe that their patient outcomes are already optimised may be less motivated to use a decision aid.
Another explanation may be that surgeons in this sample exhibited an overconfidence bias. Indeed, most participants believed their own patient outcomes were better than that reported in the literature. While it is possible that this belief is accurate and outcomes among this sample of surgeons are indeed above average, similar observations were made in a survey study involving 700 orthopaedic surgeons in Europe and North America.32 In that study, 83 per cent of surgeons surveyed considered themselves to be above-average diagnosticians, 74 per cent believed they were above average surgeons and 25 per cent believed they were in the top 5 per cent of surgeons.32 This overconfidence bias may lead to a confirmation bias in which surgeons only notice things that agree with their point of view and are less attentive to alternative viewpoints due to cognitive dissonance.33 A confirmation bias can have consequences for the uptake of a decision aid as surgeons may be less likely to consider evidence that contradicts their clinical experience.
Surgical decision making also involves weighing up of the risks versus benefits of non-operative interventions.30 The participants highlighted a lack of effective non-operative interventions for end-stage knee OA. A study among Dutch orthopaedic surgeons similarly documented a lack of confidence in the efficacy of non-surgical treatments associated with a decreased referral rate.34 Indeed, there is currently a dearth of evidence-based, non-operative interventions for patients with end-stage knee OA either as an alternative to surgery or as a definitive management when the patient is deemed unsuitable for TKA. In the absence of a treatment alternative, surgery with all its risks and costs is often the default intervention. This is an important barrier to uptake as evidence suggests surgeons will not use an aid if they do not have the resources to carry out the decisions28 or if the aid does not support their own view about effective treatments.35
The participants in this study believed a decision aid could promote shared decision making and enhance informed consent for TKA. Similar attitudes towards decision aids were identified in a previous review9 and a survey of the wider population of orthopaedic surgeons.18 It may be that surgeons can be motivated to uptake a decision aid, because it is the ethical thing to do.36 However, our study identified concerns about the ethical implications of imposing mandatory cut-offs that would determine eligibility for TKA. These concerns may reflect the participants’ desire to defend professional discretion and autonomy. We take the opportunity to emphasise the function of a decision aid is to support, not replace, clinical decision making. However, concerns were also expressed about the legal implications of using a decision aid. This finding contrasts with a study involving US orthopaedic surgeons who believed decision aids may reduce litigation and malpractice insurance premiums by enhancing shared decision making.37 It may be that these concerns are unique to the medicolegal context of Australia. Certainly, surgeons will be less likely to be uptake a tool if they perceive it makes them vulnerable to medicolegal action.
Implications
The Ready, Willing and Able model posits that these three preconditions must be satisfied before a decision aid will be adopted.38 ‘Ready’ refers to the perceived benefit of changing the status quo and adopting an aid to support decision making. ‘Willing’ refers to the perceived legitimacy of a decision aid and a willingness to overcome objections and concerns. ‘Able’ refers to the ability to adopt the decision aid given available resources.39 Implications for enhancing the readiness, willingness and ability of Australian orthopaedic surgeons to uptake a TKA decision aid are described below.
To enhance readiness to uptake, current decision-making biases may need to be addressed. Information to counter these biases could be provided through audit and feedback. Audit and feedback is defined as a ‘summary of the clinical performance of healthcare provider(s) over a specified period of time’ (Ivers et al, p5).40 It may be particularly effective when clinicians’ ability to accurately self-assess is limited.41 This assumes that clinicians are motivated to improve care but lack intention to change current behaviour because they are unaware of their suboptimal performance. While effect sizes may depend on the format and context in which feedback is provided, findings from a Cochrane review suggest that audit and feedback can yield important improvements in professional practice.40 Clarity and consensus on the definition of ‘clinically meaningful improvement’ and ‘non-response’ to TKA is likely to influence the success of an audit and feedback intervention in this context. Future research is needed to explore how audit and feedback is best delivered in this context including the immediacy and frequency of feedback, and the potential to incorporate other approaches such as the use of surgeon incentives.42
To enhance willingness to uptake, an audit and feedback intervention before and after using a decision aid may be necessary for orthopaedic surgeons to gain confidence in its legitimacy. The findings suggest that validating a decision aid in large multicentre trials may not be sufficient to encourage uptake, as surgeons believed such trials do not account for variations in case load and years of experience. Surgeons perceived themselves as scientists and ‘artists’, believing that surgical success depended, in part, on the ‘hands’ that performed it. This suggests that orthopaedic surgeons may need to be given the opportunity to validate a decision aid ‘in their own hands’.
Addressing concerns about the medicolegal implications of a decision aid may also be necessary to enhance willingness to uptake. This may require the consultation of multiple stakeholders including policy makers, lawyers and institutional administration before the implementation of a decision aid. Legislative changes in the USA have recognised decision aids as a higher standard of informed consent,8 and in the UK, incentives to use decision aids are being developed.9 There is evidence that mandating the use of a decision aid as a requirement for gaining informed consent results in significantly improved patient outcomes including a reduction in the rate of TKA by 38 per cent in a 6-month period.43 Future studies are required to explore if policy changes and/or incentives might influence willingness to uptake a decisions aid in the Australian context.
To enhance ability to uptake, it appears important to address the dearth of non-operative alternatives to surgery for end-stage knee OA so that surgeons have the resources to carry out decisions. Future research to design and implement effective, evidence-based, non-operative interventions should be a priority. Targeting orthopaedic surgeons’ beliefs about the efficacy of such interventions through education strategies will also be important. The study findings suggest that ability to uptake may be enhanced if a decision aid is packaged in an electronic or online format that is quick and easily accessible. Focus groups involving surgeons and patients to gain feedback on prototypes of a decision aid during the design phase is important to ensure that the format and the content are accessible to a range of end users.
Limitations
Qualitative studies involving orthopaedic surgeons are rare. The strength of this study is the 100 per cent participation rate by orthopaedic surgeons in one tertiary hospital setting. While the sampling strategy means the generalisability of these findings to other contexts may be limited, we have documented significant similarities with international studies. We acknowledge that while beliefs, attitudes and intentions can predict behaviours with a degree of accuracy, they are distinct from actual behaviour.44 Thus, the themes elicited in this study do not provide evidence of the actual influences on uptake of a decision aid.
Conclusions
Using a theoretical framework to systematically explore barriers and facilitators to uptake, this study will inform the design and implementation of future TKA decision aids.13 The findings suggest that a multifaceted approach will be required to ensure that orthopaedic surgeons are ready, willing and able to use a decision aid that can reduce the suffering and economic burden of ‘failed’ TKA. Research exploring patients’ beliefs and attitudes towards a TKA decision aid is required prior to implementation.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge Pascal Buenzli for assistance with the data management software and inter-rater reliability calculation. We extend our thanks to all the surgeons for kindly donating their time to participate in this study.
Footnotes
Contributors: SB: led the study design, data collection, data analysis and manuscript preparation. EN contributed to the data analysis. AS, PC and MD contributed to the study design, data analysis and manuscript preparation. SF contributed to the study design and manuscript preparation.
Funding: This study was funded by the University of Melbourne FBE/MDHS Collaboration Seed Funding Grant and by the National Health & Medical Research Council (APP1122983). A/Prof Dowsey holds an National Health & Medical Research Council Career Development Fellowship (APP1122526).
Competing interests: None declared.
Ethics approval: This study was carried out in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration. Approval was given by St Vincent’s Hospital Melbourne, Australia (QA 019 /_16).
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Data sharing statement: Requests regarding the datasets used in this study can be directed to the first author. Full interview transcripts will not be shared to protect the anonymity of the study participants.
References
- 1.Cobos R, Latorre A, Aizpuru F, et al. . Variability of indication criteria in knee and hip replacement: an observational study. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2010;11:249 10.1186/1471-2474-11-249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dowsey MM, Smith AJ, Choong PFM. Latent class growth analysis predicts long term pain and function trajectories in total knee arthroplasty: a study of 689 patients. Osteoarthritis Cartilage 2015;23:2141–9. 10.1016/j.joca.2015.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Australian Orthopaedic Association. Annual report. Adelaide: National Joint Replacement Registry, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Riddle DL, Jiranek WA, Hayes CW. Use of a validated algorithm to judge the appropriateness of total knee arthroplasty in the United States: a multicenter longitudinal cohort study. Arthritis Rheumatol 2014;66:2134–43. 10.1002/art.38685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Curtis AJ, Wolfe R, Russell CO, et al. . Determining priority for joint replacement: comparing the views of orthopaedic surgeons and other professionals. Med J Aust 2011;195:699–702. 10.5694/mja10.11052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Frankel L, Sanmartin C, Hawker G, et al. . Perspectives of orthopaedic surgeons on patients' appropriateness for total joint arthroplasty: a qualitative study. J Eval Clin Pract 2016;22:164–70. 10.1111/jep.12449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gooberman-Hill R, Sansom A, Sanders CM, et al. . Unstated factors in orthopaedic decision-making: a qualitative study. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2010;11:213 10.1186/1471-2474-11-213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kuehn BM. States explore shared decision making. JAMA 2009;301:2539–41. 10.1001/jama.2009.867 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jayadev C, Khan T, Coulter A, et al. . Patient decision aids in knee replacement surgery. Knee 2012;19:746–50. 10.1016/j.knee.2012.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.van den Bemt BJ, Zwikker HE, van den Ende CH. Medication adherence in patients with rheumatoid arthritis: a critical appraisal of the existing literature. Expert Rev Clin Immunol 2012;8:337–51. 10.1586/eci.12.23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Feldman-Stewart D, Brennenstuhl S, McIssac K, et al. . A systematic review of information in decision aids. Health Expect 2007;10:46–61. 10.1111/j.1369-7625.2006.00420.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sacks GD, Dawes AJ, Ettner SL, et al. . Impact of a risk calculator on risk perception and surgical decision making: a randomized trial. Ann Surg 2016;264:889–95. 10.1097/SLA.0000000000001750 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dowsey MM, Spelman T, Choong PF. Development of a prognostic nomogram for predicting the probability of nonresponse to total knee arthroplasty 1 year after surgery. J Arthroplasty 2016;31:1654–60. 10.1016/j.arth.2016.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Barsoum WK, Murray TG, Klika AK, et al. . Predicting patient discharge disposition after total joint arthroplasty in the United States. J Arthroplasty 2010;25:885–92. 10.1016/j.arth.2009.06.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kattan MW. Nomograms are difficult to beat. Eur Urol 2008;53:671–2. 10.1016/j.eururo.2007.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Stacey D, Bennett C, Barry M, et al. . Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011;10:CD001431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Knops AM, Legemate DA, Goossens A, et al. . Decision aids for patients facing a surgical treatment decision: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann Surg 2013;257:860–6. 10.1097/SLA.0b013e3182864fd6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Adam JA, Khaw FM, Thomson RG, et al. . Patient decision aids in joint replacement surgery: a literature review and an opinion survey of consultant orthopaedic surgeons. Ann R Coll Surg Engl 2008;90:198–207. 10.1308/003588408X285748 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Phillips CJ, Marshall AP, Chaves NJ, et al. . Experiences of using the theoretical domains framework across diverse clinical environments: a qualitative study. J Multidiscip Healthc 2015;8:139–46. 10.2147/JMDH.S78458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Eccles M, Grimshaw J, Walker A, et al. . Changing the behavior of healthcare professionals: the use of theory in promoting the uptake of research findings. J Clin Epidemiol 2005;58:107–12. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Noar SM, Zimmerman RS. Health Behavior Theory and cumulative knowledge regarding health behaviors: are we moving in the right direction? Health Educ Res 2005;20:275–90. 10.1093/her/cyg113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S. Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement Sci 2012;7:37 10.1186/1748-5908-7-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sansom A, Donovan J, Sanders C, et al. . Routes to total joint replacement surgery: patients' and clinicians' perceptions of need. Arthritis Care Res 2010;62:1252–7. 10.1002/acr.20218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Michie S, Johnston M, Abraham C, et al. . Making psychological theory useful for implementing evidence based practice: a consensus approach. Qual Saf Health Care 2005;14:26–33. 10.1136/qshc.2004.011155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dobson F, Bennell KL, French SD, et al. . Barriers and facilitators to exercise participation in people with hip and/or knee osteoarthritis. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2016;95:1–89. 10.1097/PHM.0000000000000448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.French SD, Green SE, O’Connor DA, et al. . Developing theory-informed behaviour change interventions to implement evidence into practice: a systematic approach using the Theoretical Domains Framework. Implement Sci 2012;7:38 10.1186/1748-5908-7-38 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Patey AM, Islam R, Francis JJ, et al. . Anesthesiologists' and surgeons' perceptions about routine pre-operative testing in low-risk patients: application of the Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) to identify factors that influence physicians' decisions to order pre-operative tests. Implement Sci 2012;7:52 10.1186/1748-5908-7-52 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sepucha KR, Mulley AG. Extending decision support: preparation and implementation. Patient Educ Couns 2003;50:269–71. 10.1016/S0738-3991(03)00048-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Buenzli P. Codesort software. 2012.
- 30.Sacks GD, Dawes AJ, Ettner SL, et al. . Surgeon perception of risk and benefit in the decision to operate. Ann Surg 2016;264:896–903. 10.1097/SLA.0000000000001784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hawker G, Bohm ER, Conner-Spady B, et al. . Perspectives of Canadian stakeholders on criteria for appropriateness for total joint arthroplasty in patients with hip and Kknee osteoarthritis. Arthritis Rheumatol 2015;67:1806–15. 10.1002/art.39124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Teunis T, Janssen S, Guitton TG, et al. . Do orthopaedic surgeons acknowledge uncertainty? Clin Orthop Relat Res 2016;474:1360–9. 10.1007/s11999-015-4623-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. New York, USA: Macmillan, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hofstede SN, Marang-van de Mheen PJ, Vliet Vlieland TP, et al. . Barriers and facilitators associated with non-surgical treatment use for osteoarthritis patients in orthopaedic practice. PLoS One 2016;11:e0147406 10.1371/journal.pone.0147406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Elwyn G, Rix A, Holt T, et al. . Why do clinicians not refer patients to online decision support tools? Interviews with front line clinics in the NHS. BMJ Open 2012;2:e001530 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tai-Seale M, Elwyn G, Wilson CJ, et al. . Enhancing shared decision making through carefully designed interventions that target patient and provider behavior. Health Aff 2016;35:605–12. 10.1377/hlthaff.2015.1398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Weinstein JN, Clay K, Morgan TS. Informed patient choice: patient-centered valuing of surgical risks and benefits. Health Aff 2007;26:726–30. 10.1377/hlthaff.26.3.726 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lesthaeghe R, Ready VC. willing and able: A conceptualization of transitions to new behavioral forms : Casterline J, Diffusion processes and fertility transition: selected perspectives. Washington DC: National Academies Press, 2001:240–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Shultz CG, Jimbo M. Decision aid use in primary care: an overview and theory-based framework. Fam Med 2015;47:679–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ivers N, Jamtvedt G, Flottorp S, et al. . Audit and feedback: effects on professional practice and healthcare outcomes (Review). Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2012;6:CD000259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Davis DA, Mazmanian PE, Fordis M, et al. . Accuracy of physician self-assessment compared with observed measures of competence: a systematic review. JAMA 2006;296:1094–102. 10.1001/jama.296.9.1094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Emanuel EJ, Ubel PA, Kessler JB, et al. . Using behavioural economics to design physician incentives that deliver high-value care. Ann Intern Med 2016;164:114–9. 10.7326/M15-1330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Arterburn D, Wellman R, Westbrook E, et al. . Introducing decision aids at Group Health was linked to sharply lower hip and knee surgery rates and costs. Health Aff 2012;31:2094–104. 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Azjen I, Madden T. Prediction of goal-directed behaviour: attitudes, intentions and perceived behavioural control. J Exp Soc Psychol 1986;22:453–74. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
bmjopen-2017-018614supp001.pdf (34.8KB, pdf)