Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2017 Apr 11;29(4):1483–1498. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000402

The mediating roles of cortisol reactivity and executive functioning difficulties in the pathways between childhood histories of emotional insecurity and adolescent school problems

Meredith J Martin a, Patrick T Davies b, E Mark Cummings c, Dante Cicchetti d
PMCID: PMC5699223  NIHMSID: NIHMS919831  PMID: 28397610

Abstract

This study tested a hypothesized cascade in which children’s insecure representations of the interparental relationship increase their school problems by altering children’s cortisol reactivity to stress and their executive functioning. Participants included 235 families. The first of five measurement occasions occurred when the children were in kindergarten (M age = 6 years), and they were followed through the transition to high school. The results indicated that children’s histories of insecure representations of the interparental relationship during the early school years were associated with executive functioning difficulties in adolescence (M age = 14 years). This in turn predicted subsequent increases in school adjustment difficulties 1 year later. In addition, elevated cortisol reactivity to interadult conflict mediated the association between early histories of insecurity and subsequent executive function problems in adolescence.


Witnessing destructive interparental conflict characterized by hostility, negative escalation, and detachment is stressful for children and increases their risk for adjustment problems (Cummings & Davies, 2010). In addressing why and how interparental discord promotes psychological problems, prevailing theories point to children’s negative internal representations as a key mediating mechanism (i.e., Davies & Cummings, 1994; Grych & Fincham, 1990; Johnston, Roseby, & Kuehnle, 2009; Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, 2002). For example, emotional security theory (EST; Cummings & Davies, 1996; Davies & Cummings, 1994) suggests that exposure to destructive interparental conflict undermines children’s sense of safety and security in the family. Children’s concerns for security (i.e., emotional insecurity) are proposed to manifest in insecure representations of the interparental relationship, or implicit appraisals of parental conflicts as having threatening implications for themselves and for the integrity of the family unit. Although insecure representations have the adaptive function of heightening children’s ability to efficiently scan for, interpret, and respond to cues of impending interparental conflicts, the effort of managing persistent concerns for security is proposed to take a physical and psychological toll, ultimately undermining children’s ability to meet subsequent developmental challenges (Davies, Sturge-Apple, & Martin, 2013; Davies, Winter, & Cicchetti, 2006).

In support of EST, research documents insecure representations of the interparental relationship as a consistent mediator explaining the association between early exposure to destructive interparental conflict and a wide array of psychological problems (Buehler, Lange, & Franck, 2007; Coe, Davies, Sturge-Apple, in press; Davies & Cummings, 1998; Shelton & Harold, 2008; Sturge-Apple, Davies, Winter, Cummings, & Schermerhorn, 2008). However, far less is known about how and why emotional insecurity increases children’s vulnerability to psychological problems. Developmental psychopathology models of early adversity illustrate how short-term ways of processing and responding to harsh conditions in childhood can trigger an unfolding series of processes that, over time, confer risk for psychopathology (e.g., Masten & Cicchetti, 2010; Repetti, Robles, & Reynolds, 2011). In an effort to provide further direction and specificity in characterizing these developmental cascades, the reformulation of emotional security theory (EST-R) proposes that a history of insecure representations of the interparental relationship in childhood ultimately leads to psychopathology and school difficulties in adolescence by disrupting neuropsychological functioning (Davies & Martin, 2013). Drawing on physiological models of risk, alterations to the stress response system (i.e., the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis) are further proposed to serve as the biological mechanism underlying this link. Accordingly, as shown in Figure 1, the goal of this study is to utilize EST-R as a guide in elucidating an understanding of children’s adrenocortical reactivity to stress and neuropsychological functioning as mediators of the pathway between children’s insecure representations and their psychological problems.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

An illustration of the developmental cascade from emotional insecurity to psychological adjustment problems outlined by the reformulation of emotional security theory (Davies & Martin, 2013). The specific construct listed underneath each box represents the process we chose to represent each component of the proposed cascade in the current study. HPA, hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal.

Executive Functioning as a Mediator in the Link Between Children’s Insecure Representations and Their Psychological Problems

Evolutionary life history theories and resource models of self-control share in common the underlying concept that organisms need to efficiently distribute limited resources (e.g., caloric energy, mental effort, and time) to support the developmental and cognitive tasks necessary for survival and reproductive success across the life span (e.g., Del Giudice, Elis, & Shirtcliff, 2013; Inzlicht & Schmeichel, 2012; Korte, Koolhaas, Wingfield, & McEwen, 2005). From this perspective, biases toward pursuing selective goals will necessarily limit the resources that can be devoted to other significant domains of functioning. For example, repeated exposure to interparental conflicts may promote a “better safe than sorry” strategy in which children devote significant efforts toward vigilantly processing cues signaling the potential for threat and coping with negative affect in order to manage their insecurity. Accordingly, EST-R proposes that insecure representations of the interparental relationship, over time, tip the balanced allocation of psychobiological resources toward investing in immediate personal safety at the cost of equal investment in mastering longer term goal pursuits, such as gaining knowledge of the physical world or developing strong social bonds (Davies & Martin, 2013). The resulting attenuation of resources is proposed to have a negative impact on the executive domains of neuropsychological functioning that require considerable motivation, effort, planning, and cognitive sophistication (Cowell, Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2015; Hofmann, Schmeichel, & Baddeley, 2012; Kaplan & Berman, 2010).

As a demanding, higher order set of processes, executive functioning is one component of neuropsychological functioning that may be particularly likely to serve as an intermediary mechanism linking childhood insecurity and adjustment problems. Executive functioning refers to a multidimensional set of goal-directed, purposeful skills in domains of planning, problem solving, working memory, self-monitoring, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control (Zelazo et al., 2013). Prior research clearly demonstrates that family adversity in childhood, including exposure to interparental conflict, sets the stage for executive functioning impairments (e.g., Bernier, Carlson, & Whipple, 2010; Blair, Raver, & Berry, 2014; Gustafsson, Coffman, & Cox, 2015; Hinnant, El-Sheikh, Keiley, & Buckhalt, 2013; Lengua et al., 2014). However, although interparental conflict and children’s insecure representations are lawfully related, multiple theories underscore that family factors (e.g., exposure to interparental conflict and parental hostility) and children’s processing and coping (e.g., internal representations and emotional reactivity) operate in distinctive ways in models of psychopathology (e.g., Davies & Cummings, 1994; Grych & Fincham, 1990; Repetti et al., 2002). The current study is designed to build on previous research by providing a first test of whether executive functioning serves as an intermediary mechanism in the association between children’s early histories of insecure representations of interparental relations and their adjustment problems in adolescence.

In turn, impairments in executive functioning have been linked with a wide array of problems including poor emotional, behavioral, peer, and academic functioning in childhood and adolescence (e.g., Blair & Raver, 2015; Ellis, Rothbart, & Posner, 2004; Holmes, Kim-Spoon, & Deater-Deckard, 2016; Latzman, Elkovitch, Young, & Clark, 2010; Zelazo & Müller, 2010). Difficulties in regulating impulses, engaging in strategic planning, flexibly tailoring responses to environmental input, and marshalling attentional resources are all proposed to broadly increase children’s risk for self-regulation difficulties (Cowell et al., 2015; Hofmann et al., 2012). Given the multiple regulatory demands required to manage the challenges of adjusting to school, particularly during a transition period, we examined the hypothesized cascading role of executive functioning in influencing adolescent adjustment following the transition to high school. Successful school adjustment requires not only academic engagement and achievement but also the ability to regulate emotions and behavior, and to effectively navigate peer challenges and opportunities. Therefore, we utilized a comprehensive index of school adjustment composed of functioning in academic, social, and psychopathology (i.e., internalizing and externalizing symptoms) domains.

Cortisol Reactivity as an Explanatory Mechanism

If executive functioning is an explanatory mechanism in the pathway between children’s insecurity and their school adjustment, as EST-R proposes, it raises the further question of how and why children’s concerns about security increase their risk for executive functioning difficulties. In drawing on the psychobiological literature (e.g., Blair et al., 2011; Repetti et al., 2011), EST-R further posits that alterations in the stress response system serve as the central biological mechanism explaining how early histories of insecurity ultimately undermine cognitive control processes (see Figure 1). The hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis is a central component of the stress-response system (Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, 2009). With its end product of cortisol, the HPA axis primes homeostatic defense mechanisms by mobilizing energy (e.g., glucose and oxygen) and modulating the processing, encoding, and memory consolidation of emotionally significant events. Altered HPA axis activity is regarded as a key mediator of associations between early adverse experiences and dimensions of executive functioning, including planning, problem solving, working memory, and inhibitory control (Blair et al., 2011; Suor, Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, & Manning, 2015). For example, De Bellis and Thomas (2003) posited that adverse family experiences result in alterations in the HPA axis that damage neuronal pathways in the prefrontal cortex, ultimately compromising the development of executive functions. Likewise, Loman and Gunnar (2010) noted that brain regions that serve to organize automatic cognitive and behavioral responses to threat are strongly interconnected with the HPA axis. As a result, significant alterations in cortisol production may disrupt children’s executive functioning by impacting the operation of the prefrontal cortex.

Building on these findings, EST-R posits that insecure representations reflecting pervasive concerns about the threat posed by interparental conflict will alter the stress-response system and its biological function of coordinating physiological responses to environmental challenges (Davies, Sturge-Apple, & Cicchetti, 2011; Davies et al., 2013). Accordingly, several studies have demonstrated that emotional insecurity serves as the more proximal predictor of the pathogenic effects of interparental conflict on children’s stress response (Davies, Sturge-Apple, Cicchetti, & Cummings, 2008; Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, & Manning, 2012). However, models of allostatic load offer two differing hypotheses on how adrenocortical functioning may mediate the pathway between early family adversity and children’s vulnerability to executive functioning difficulties (Susman, 2006). The attenuation hypothesis proposes that chronic stress may compromise neuropsychological functioning by dampening cortisol output when confronted with subsequent stressors (i.e., subsequent interparental difficulties). In contrast, the sensitization hypothesis suggests that the internalization of adverse experiences sensitizes the HPA axis, with the resulting elevations in cortisol reactivity increasing children’s vulnerability to psychological adjustment problems (Davies, Sturge-Apple, et al., 2011; Gunnar & Vazquez, 2006; Susman, 2006).

Determining the relative viability of sensitization and attenuation models in understanding the cascading sequelae of insecurity is made difficult by the paucity of studies examining cortisol reactivity as a mediator of associations between histories of family adversity and neuropsychological functioning. Components of emotional insecurity (e.g., emotional reactivity and involvement in interparental disputes) have been identified as correlates of both heightened and diminished adrenocortical reactivity to interparental conflict (Davies et al., 2011; Davies, Sturge-Apple, et al., 2008; Koss et al., 2013). No studies to date have explicitly examined children’s cortisol reactivity to conflict as predictive of their neuropsychological functioning. However, lower basal cortisol levels in children have been related both to better executive functioning and self-regulation (e.g., Blair, Grange, & Razza, 2005; Spinrad et al., 2009) and, in some studies, to cognitive impairments (e.g., Blair et al., 2011). Given these gaps in knowledge, the present study represents the first effort to examine cortisol reactivity to interparental conflict as an explanatory mechanism in the mediational pathway involving children’s history of insecurity in the interparental relationship, executive functioning impairments, and school adjustment difficulties.

Developmental Considerations

We specifically examined whether children’s history of insecurity during the early school years (i.e., 6 through 8 years old) and subsequent individual differences in cortisol reactivity to conflict (i.e., 8 years old) would be linked with increases in school difficulties during the transition from eighth to ninth grade (i.e., 14 to 15 years old) through its association with earlier executive functioning impairments (i.e., 14 years old). The early school years are regarded as a particularly sensitive developmental window for emotional insecurity. Compared to older children, early school age children report experiencing more fear and threat in response to interparental conflict (Kitzmann, Gaylord, Holt, & Kenny, 2003). Children’s increasing capabilities in social-perspective taking, representing the family, and understanding and communicating affective states may also heighten awareness and concerns about the quality of family relations beyond the parent–child subsystem (i.e., interparental relationship; Davies, Sturge-Apple, Winter, Cummings, & Farrell, 2006). Moreover, early adolescence is a period of considerable plasticity and growth in executive functions (Steinberg, 2005). For example, brain imaging work indicates that this developmental period is marked by synaptic pruning and myelination in the frontal lobes and prefrontal cortex and accompanying increases in executive functioning abilities (Luciana, Conklin, Hooper, & Yarger, 2005). Heightened plasticity of neurological pathways involved in executive functioning is also theorized to amplify vulnerability to enduring psychosocial (i.e., insecurity in the interparental relationship) and neurobiological (i.e., cortisol reactivity) stress (Lupien et al., 2009). Finally, school adjustment during the transition to high school is viewed as a pivotal foundation for long-term prosperity in academic, occupational, interpersonal, and emotional domains of functioning during a challenging developmental period in which adolescents experience a peak in the onset of mental health disorders (Langenkamp, 2010; Steinberg, 2005).

The Present Study

Little is known about the potential operation of neuropsychological impairments in accounting for the vulnerability of adolescents with histories of insecurity. As the closest test of the proposed cascade to date, Davies, Woitach, Winter, and Cummings (2008) found that insecure representations predicted greater school problems through their association with attention difficulties in a sample of children in the school years. However, as noted in the limitations of this study, assessment of attention difficulties does not sufficiently capture executive functioning difficulties (e.g., planning and working memory) that are central to testing the proposed pathway in EST-R. Thus, our paper is designed to provide the first comprehensive test of the theoretically guided hypothesis that insecurity in the interparental relationship increases children’s school adjustment problems by undermining their executive functioning abilities. In breaking additional ground, we also examine whether cortisol reactivity to interparental conflict further mediates the cascade of executive functioning and school adjustment difficulties proposed to develop from children’s enduring experiences with insecurity in the interparental relationship. Finally, we expand beyond the previous short-term longitudinal study on attention problems in childhood, by tracing the longer term significance of insecurity over the course of multiple developmental periods.

To reduce common method and informant variance and increase our ability to identify the temporal sequence in our proposed cascade, we drew upon a multimethod, multiagent assessment battery nested within five measurement occasions spanning childhood (i.e., kindergarten; 6 years old) through middle adolescence (i.e., ninth grade; 15 years old). Observational assessments of children’s insecure representations of the interparental relationship across the first three annual waves of data collection were specified as a predictor of a computerized measure of adolescent executive functions during the fourth measurement occasion. Executive functioning, in turn, was specified as a predictor of teacher reports of academic, social, and psychological domains of school adjustment at the fifth measurement occasion, 1 year later. Autoregressive analyses, which controlled for prior values of school adjustment, were also conducted to derive more rigorous tests of directionality in the proposed mediational pathways. Although assessments of executive functioning were not available in childhood, a childhood assessment of attention was included as a covariate to reduce the plausibility that the proposed cascade was an artifact of early histories of attention problems. Children’s cortisol reactivity to simulated interparental conflicts during the third wave of assessment (i.e., age 8) was further specified as a mediating mechanism linking children’s earlier histories of insecurity with their executive functioning during adolescence. Finally, child and demographic characteristics (i.e., gender, socioeconomic status, and exposure to interparental conflict) were included as covariates in light of their potential associations with children’s insecure representations, cortisol reactivity to conflict, executive function abilities, and school adjustment (Cummings & Davies, 2010; Hill, Laird, & Robinson, 2014; Loney, Butler, Lima, Counts, & Eckel, 2006; Lupien, King, Meaney, & McEwen, 2001).

Methods

Participants

Participants for this study were drawn from a larger project that consisted of 235 children, their parents, and teachers, recruited through schools and the community centers in a metropolitan area in the Northeast and a small city in the Midwest. Interested families were included in the project if they had a child in kindergarten and lived together with two primary caregivers for at least the preceding 3 years. The longitudinal design occurred in two stages. In the first, families participated in three annual measurement occasions beginning when children were in kindergarten (M age = 6 years). In the second stage, families returned for two additional waves of data collection when children were in eighth (M age = 14 years) and ninth (M age = 15 years) grades, respectively. Girls comprised 55% of the sample. Median household income of the families was between $40,000 and $54,999. On average, mothers and fathers completed comparable years of education (14.47 years, SD = 2.33) and 14.60 years, SD = 2.69, respectively). Most parents (i.e., 92%) were married at the outset of the study. The majority of the participants were White (74%), followed by smaller percentages of Black (16%), multiracial (3%), and other races (3%). Four percent of the sample identified as Hispanic or Latino. Children lived with their biological mother in most cases (95%), with the remainder of the sample living with either an adoptive mother (3%) or a stepmother or female guardian (2%). In addition, children lived with their biological father in the majority of cases (87%), with the remainder of the sample living with either an adoptive father (4%) or a stepfather or male guardian (9%). Families were compensated for their participation on a graduated basis (i.e., payments across two visits within each measurement occasion ranged from $120 in Wave 1 to $155 in Wave 6). Teachers also received $25 payments for completing questionnaires. Retention rates across contiguous waves of data collection averaged 94% (range = 88%–97%) across the five waves.

Procedures and measures

At each of five waves of data collection, families visited the laboratory at one of the two sites. Laboratories at each site were designed to be comparable to each other in size and quality and included (a) an observation room that was designed to resemble a living room and equipped with audiovisual equipment to capture family interactions, and (b) interview rooms for completing confidential interview and survey measures. The study was approved by the institutional review board at each research site.

History of emotional insecurity

At Waves 1 (i.e., kindergarten), 2 (i.e., first grade), and 3 (i.e., second grade), children’s history of emotional insecurity in the interparental relationship were assessed through the revised version of the MacArthur Story Stem Battery (MSSB-R; Davies et al., 2006). Consistent with the original version of the MSSB (Bretherton, Oppenheim Buchsbaum, Emde, & MacArthur Narrative Group, 1990), the MSSB-R is a narrative storytelling technique designed to assess children’s internal representations of family relationships. However, unlike the original MSSB, the MSSB-R is designed to systematically capture children’s representations of the interparental conflict as a threat to themselves and their families through the presentations of three story stems about interparental conflict. To facilitate engagement in the task, experimenters used dramatic, animated voices, various toy props, and family action figures matching the child’s sex and race. After the experimenter presented each story stem, children completed the story with the assistance of the action figures, props, and experimenter probes. The three specific stories assessing insecure representations of interparental conflict included (a) a mild interparental conflict regarding a lost set of keys, (b) an intense interparental conflict regarding a messy kitchen, and (c) a relatively calm interparental disagreement about one of the parents returning home late. Child stories were videotaped for later coding.

Coders rated the videotaped records of each of the three MSSB-R interparental narratives at each wave along four 5-point scales assessing child representations of the implications of interparental difficulties for the welfare of child and family. Maternal and paternal caregiver incompetence ratings indexed the extent to which each parent failed to use available resources in ways that protected or improved the well-being of the child and family. Whereas “very competent” (1) ratings reflect that child portrayals of the caregivers as sources of support and protection for the child and family in the face of interparental disagreements, “very incompetent” (5) ratings reflect that caregivers are depicted as completely inept or threatening. As a third assessment, the relationship quality code captured child representations of the long-term emotional impact of the conflict on the interparental relationship. At the low end of the scale, “intense harmony” (1) was designated for stories where relationships were predominantly characterized as harmonious. Conversely, “intense discord” (5) reflected expectations of protracted, intense problems between parents. For the final assessment, emotional insecurity was a global rating designed to assess collective representations of the interparental relationship as a source of support or threat. Whereas “strong security” (1) ratings were characterized by depictions of parents resolving challenges in a manner that facilitated family harmony and the physical and emotional welfare of the child, “strong insecurity” (5) ratings portrayed family relationships as persistently in conflict and as posing a clear threat to the child’s security.

To assess interrater reliability at each wave, trained coders independently overlapped in their ratings of 20% of the MSSB-R interviews. Intraclass correlation coefficients for each of the four rating scales within each story ranged from 0.85 to 0.95 (M = 0.90). The four MSSB-R ratings (i.e., maternal and paternal incompetence, relationship quality, and emotional insecurity) were averaged together within each wave to obtain a comprehensive assessment of children’s emotional insecurity in the interparental relationship at Waves 1, 2, and 3. The resulting composites evidenced high internal consistency (α = 0.95 at each wave). Accordingly, the three composites were specified as manifest indicators of a latent construct of children’s history of insecurity in the interparental relationship.

Childhood attention problems

Although our childhood assessment battery did not contain executive function measures, children did complete the Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II; Conners, 2000), a computerized assessment of children’s difficulties controlling and sustaining attention, at Wave 2. Consistent with standard procedures for the CPT-II, children were instructed to watch a series of letters and respond to all letters except the target letter X by pressing the spacebar as quickly as possible. Each letter was displayed for a 250-ms time period. To obtain a more sensitive measure of attention difficulties in the face of changing task demands, the time between stimulus (i.e., letter) presentations varied by 1-, 2-, and 4-s intervals across the task. Following a practice session, children completed six blocks of letter presentations, each lasting approximately 2 min. Each block in turn contained three subblocks of 20 trials each. We utilized two key measures as indicators of attentional difficulties. First, the hit-rate reaction time score indexed the mean response time to all letters except the target letter X across all six CPT session blocks. Longer latencies in reaction time are indicative of attention problems. Second, the hit reaction time standard error score was calculated as the standard error of responses to all the letters except the target letter X. Thus, higher scores reflect more erratic responding characteristic of inattention and impulsivity. The variables were transformed into T scores, adjusting for child age and gender. Each of these CPT variables has demonstrated adequate reliability (i.e., test–retest) and validity across comparable samples (e.g., Conners, 2000).

Adolescent executive function problems

At Wave 4, three subtests of a computerized battery of tests from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (Cambridge Cognition) were used to assess teen executive function (e.g., Luciana et al., 2005). First, the Spatial Span Task assesses the spatial short-term memory component of working memory. Children viewed up to nine boxes that change color in a sequence and then reproduced the original order of the sequence. If participants made a mistake, they were given a second chance to complete the sequence. The length of the span increases with each correctly reproduced sequence. We derived a composite measure based on the average of two standardized measures: (a) the spatial span score, defined as the longest sequence of boxes correctly recalled; and (b) total usage errors, characterized by the number of times the subject selected an incorrect box in the sequence (see Gau & Shang, 2010). To maintain consistency with the other assessments of childhood executive function problems, the spatial span variable was reverse scored prior to calculating the composite so that higher values indicated more executive function problems.

Second, the Spatial Working Memory Task is a self-ordered task that measures executive planning and working memory (Luciana & Nelson, 2002). The object of the task is to efficiently search for tokens hidden inside a number of colored boxes. Because tokens appear in each box only once, efficiency is maximized by remembering boxes that have been previously searched. Consistent with previous approaches (e.g., Gau & Shang, 2010), executive function problems were assessed by total errors, calculated based on the number of times the participant chose a box they had already found to be empty (within) or returned to boxes that had previously contained a token (between).

Third, the Stockings of Cambridge Test was used to assess children’s planning and spatial working memory. Participants transported colored balls into three socks in the minimum number of moves possible to reproduce a model display of colored balls. Problems vary in complexity, requiring anywhere from one to five moves in order to obtain a successful solution. In accord with prior research (e.g., Luciana & Nelson, 2002), we calculated the reverse score of the number problems solved in the minimum number of moves to obtain an indicator of children’s executive function problems.

Cortisol reactivity to interadult conflict

To assess cortisol reactivity, children provided two saliva samples during the third wave of data collection (i.e., second grade). Children provided the sample using the passive drool technique prior to and after witnessing a series of seven videotaped conflicts depicting conflict between adults. Children were asked to imagine the disagreements were taking place between their parents. The vignettes, which were each approximately 1 min in duration, were ordered in the following way: (a) two unresolved conflicts depicting moderate displays of anger between the adults, (b) a constructive conflict characterized by mild expressions of adult anger that were fully resolved, (c) an escalating conflict characterized by the progressive intensification of hostility and anger, and (d) a child-rearing conflict depicting unresolved displays of anger over a child-related issue. To reduce any distress experienced by children, the task concluded with a videotaped depiction of the adults fully resolving the issues raised in the unresolved conflict vignettes. Experimenters collected the preconflict saliva sample immediately prior to children viewing the videotaped vignettes. Consistent with previous research indicating that salivary cortisol peaks 20 to 40 min after the stressful event (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004), the postconflict saliva sample was obtained 25 min after exposure to the final unresolved (i.e., child-rearing) conflict. Saliva collection took place during the afternoon and early evening to limit the effects of the natural diurnal pattern of cortisol (M sampling time 3:43 p.m., SD = 2 hr).

All samples were assayed for salivary cortisol using a highly sensitive immunoassay at Salimetrics Inc. (State College, PA). The assay test process utilized 25 µl of saliva; samples were tested in duplicate form. The test had a lower test sensitivity of 0.007 µg/dl and an upper test sensitivity of 3.0 µg/dl. The average intraassay coefficient was 4.2% for the current sample. After removing three outlier values (3.5 SD from the mean) from the data, preconflict and postconflict cortisol variables were transformed using the logarithmic calculation to normalize cortisol distributions. Consistent with previous procedures (e.g., Granger, Weisz, McCracken, Ikeda, & Douglas, 1996), a multiple regression analysis was then conducted to assess children’s cortisol reactivity to the interparental conflict vignettes. In the analysis, preconflict cortisol level was specified as a predictor of postconflict cortisol. As an additional control for variability resulting from differences in the diurnal rhythm of cortisol, time since awakening was also specified as a predictor in the first step of analyses. The cortisol reactivity variable used in our primary analyses consisted of the standardized residual score from the regression analysis, reflecting the difference between the observed postconflict score and the predicted score based on preconflict and time since awakening variables. Thus, higher scores reflect greater cortisol reactivity following exposure to conflict vignettes.

Adolescent school problems

At Waves 4 and 5, teachers completed surveys assessing adolescent psychopathology and school functioning. An assessment of scholastic competence consisted of three scales, including the Hyperactivity/ Distractibility Scale from the Child Behavior Scale consisting of four items indexing concentration and attention problems in school (Ladd & Profilet, 1996; e.g., “poor concentration”); the seven-item cooperative participation subscale of the Teacher Rating Scale of School Adjustment assessing engagement and compliance in classroom activities (Birch & Ladd, 1997; e.g., “Follows teacher’s directions”); and the Academic Competence Scale of the Teacher’s Rating Scale of Child’s Actual Behavior (TRSCAB; Harter, 1988), composed of two items assessing children’s academic abilities (e.g., “This individual does well at schoolwork.”). Internal consistencies for the three scales across the two waves ranged from 0.73 to 0.92 (M = 0.84). To obtain a parsimonious indicator of academic problems at each wave, the three scales were standardized and summed together within each measurement occasion after reverse scoring the TRSCAB Academic Competence and Teacher Rating Scale of School Adjustment Cooperative Participation scales. Supporting this practice, the scale-level α coefficient for the academic problems composite was 0.86 at both waves.

Three scales assessing the social dimension of school functioning included the Social Competence Scale of the TRSCAB (Harter, 1988), containing 2 items assessing children’s ability to get along with peers (e.g., “This individual does have a lot of friends”); the Peer Problems Scale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman & Scott, 1997), containing 5 items indexing interpersonal difficulties with peers (e.g., “Picked on or bullied by other children”); and the 11-item Social Problems Scale (e.g., “Doesn’t get along with other children”) from the Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach, Dumenci, & Rescorla, 2003). Across the two waves, internal consistencies for the three scales ranged from 0.77 to 0.86 (M = 0.80). A composite of social problems was created at each wave by standardizing and summing the three scales together after reverse scoring the TRSCAB Social Competence Scale (scale-level α = 0.84 and 0.81 for the Wave 4 and 5 composites, respectively).

The final dimension consisted of teacher reports of adolescent psychopathology, including both internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Internalizing symptoms were assessed using two scales: the TRF Anxious/Depressed Scale (Achenbach et al., 2003), consisting of 16 items (e.g., “There is very little s/he enjoys”); and the 5-item SDQ Emotional Problems Scale (e.g., “Many worries,” “Often unhappy, downhearted”). Alpha coefficients for the scales ranged from 0.63 to 0.86 across the two waves (M = 0.74). Externalizing problems were assessed using three scales: the Aggression Scale of the TRF (Achenbach et al., 2003), consisting of 25 items (e.g., “Gets into many fights”); the TRF Delinquency Scale, including 8 items (e.g., “Steals”); and the 5-item Conduct Problems Scale from the SDQ (e.g., “Often fights with other youth or bullies them” and “Often lies or cheats”). Alpha coefficients for these three scales ranged from 0.68 to 0.94 across the two waves (M = 0.80). Consistent with the other school dimensions, these five scales were standardized and summed together to create a composite assessment of adolescent psychopathology at Waves 4 (scale-level α = 0.59) and 5 (scale-level α = 0.62).

Covariate: Demographic characteristics

Child gender and a multiple-indicator construct of socioeconomic status (SES) were included as covariates. At Wave 1, parents completed a demographic survey to obtain SES indices of maternal education (years), paternal education (years), and parental occupational level using the socioeconomic index (Entwistle & Astone, 1994). Higher socioeconomic index scores reflect greater parental occupational prestige.

Covariate: Interparental conflict

At each of the first three waves, we created a multimethod composite of interparental conflict based on maternal reports and observer ratings of interparental conflict. Maternal reports of destructive interparental conflict over the past year were derived from three well-established measures: the Frequency Scale from the Conflict and Problem-Solving Scales (CPS; Kerig, 1996), the CPS Verbal Aggression Scale, and the O’Leary-Porter Scale (Porter & O’Leary, 1980). The CPS Frequency Scale consists of the number of times the primary caregiver reports engaging in minor (e.g., “spats”) and major (e.g., “big fights”) conflicts over the past year, whereas the CPS Verbal Aggression Scale contains 16 items indexing the extent to which mothers and their partners engage in verbally aggressive conflict tactics such as yelling, accusing, and insulting (e.g., “Raise voice, yell, shout”). Finally, the 10-item O’Leary–Porter Scale assesses children’s exposure to interparental hostility (e.g., “How often do you and/or your partner display verbal hostility [raised voices, etc.] in front of your child?”). Internal consistencies for the three scales across the three waves ranged from α = 0.72 to 0.90.

Observer ratings of interparental conflict were derived from an interparental interaction task in which mothers and fathers engaged in two interparental disagreements that they viewed as problematic in their relationship. Following similar procedures in previous research (i.e., Du Rocher Schudlich, Papp, & Cummings, 2004), parents conferred to select two common, difficult conflict topics that they both felt comfortable discussing. The couples subsequently discussed each topic for 10 min while they were alone in the laboratory room. Consistent with the use of similar interaction tasks in prior research, the aim of the interparental interaction task was to assess parents’ characteristic ways of managing conflict in the interparental relationship (Du Rocher Schudlich et al., 2004). Trained coders independently rated videotaped records of the couples’ interactions using three interparental hostility and negativity codes from the System for Coding Interactions in Dyads (Malik & Lindahl, 2004). Specifically, coders separately rated maternal and paternal negativity and conflict, which reflects the extent to which each individual in the dyad displays anger, frustration, and tension. At a dyadic level of analysis, coders rated negative escalation, defined as the degree to which the couple as a unit has a tendency to reciprocate or escalate expressions of anger, hostility, and negativity. Each code was rated along 5-point dimensional scales, ranging from 1 (very low) to 5 (high). To assess interrater reliability, coders independently rated at least 20% of the tapes at each wave. The resulting intraclass correlation coefficients across the three waves ranged from 0.73 to 0.87 (M = 0.82). The six measures of interparental conflict within each of the three waves were standardized and aggregated to form multimethod composites of interparental conflict. Internal consistencies for the interparental conflict composites were satisfactory: 0.80 at Wave 1, 0.83 at Wave 2, and 0.82 at Wave 3. Therefore, each of the three composites served as indicators of a larger latent construct reflecting children’s history of exposure to interparental conflict.

Results

Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations, and correlations among the main variables in the primary analyses. Due to moderate kurtosis, the cortisol reactivity measure was log transformed to increase normality in the distribution prior to the calculation of the correlations and the primary analyses. As indicated by the correlations in Table 1, associations between the proposed manifest indicators of the latent constructs were all significant and generally moderate in magnitude. The moderate associations evident between children’s representations of the interparental relationship across time (i.e., rs = .30–.46) are consistent with previous findings regarding stability in children’s appraisals and representations of interparental conflict in childhood (Davies et al., 2006; Grych, Harold, & Miles, 2003; Toth, Rogosch, Sturge-Apple, & Cicchetti, 2009). Structural equation models were estimated using full-information maximum likelihood in Amos 22.0 to retain the full sample for primary analyses (Enders, 2001).

Table 1.

Means, standard deviations, and correlations of the primary variables in the analyses

Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
Wave 2 attention problems
  1. CPT hit reaction time 53.57 11.07
  2. CPT hit reaction time SE 57.52 10.55 .72*
History of emotional insecurity
  3. Wave 1 3.09 0.75 .11 .24*
  4. Wave 2 2.68 0.72 .15* .22* .34*
  5. Wave 3 2.67 0.72 .12 .17* .30* .46*
Cortisol reactivity
  6. Wave 3 0.00 0.99 .05 .09 .14* .15* .15*
Wave 4 executive functioning problems
  7. SWM total errors 23.80 15.43 .26* .30* .26* .27* .18* .17*
  8. SST digit span problems 0.00 0.85 .26* .25* .14 .17* .21* .26* .47*
  9. SOC solved (reversed) 3.75 1.76 .23* .21* .01 .04 .03 .17 .40* .43*
Wave 4 school problems
10. Academic difficulties 0.00 0.88 .30* .32* .27* .19* .11 .02 .25* .20* .16
11. Social difficulties 0.00 0.87 .15 .21* .18* .17* .07 .07 .21* .15 .21* .53*
12. Psychopathology symptoms 0.00 1.53 .19* .20* .21* .13 .08 .00 .26* .22* .20* .50* .68*
Wave 5 school problems
13. Academic difficulties 0.01 0.90 .27* .31* .18* .21* .17* .21* .38* .23* .19* .52* .44* .42*
14. Social difficulties 0.00 0.85 .01 .06 .10 .17* .09 .25* .35* .16 .22* .16 .63* .37* .46*
15. Psychopathology symptoms 0.03 1.51 .20* .24* .11 .14 .05 .01 .33* .17* .25* .31* .46* .50* .54* .57*

Note: CPT, Continuous Performance Test; SWM, Spatial Working Memory Task; SST, Spatial Span Task; SOC, Stocking of Cambridge Test.

p < .10.

*

p ≤ .05.

Executive functioning as a mediator of the sequelae of children’s insecurity

Figure 2 provides a depiction of the structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis of adolescents’ executive function problems as a mediator of associations between childhood insecure representations of the interparental relationship and their school difficulties. As shown in the figure, a latent construct assessing children’s history of insecure representations over the three measurement occasions was specified as a predictor of each of the downstream endogenous variables comprising Wave 3 cortisol reactivity, Waves 4 and 5 school problems, and most important for mediational tests, Wave 4 executive function difficulties. In reflecting the second link in the mediational chain, Wave 4 teen executive function difficulties in turn served as a predictor of the latent construct of school problems 1 year later at Wave 5 after controlling their school problems at Wave 4. To provide a more stringent test of the hypothesized mediational pathway, we also specified pathways among (a) the three covariates (i.e., child gender, SES, and interparental conflict history) and the endogenous variables consisting of Wave 3 cortisol reactivity, Wave 4 executive function problems, and Waves 4 and 5 school problems; and (b) attention difficulties in childhood and school problems at Waves 4 and 5. The light-colored, dashed lines in Figure 2 illustrate the nonsignificant pathways that were included in the structural model. Because our first aim was to test executive functioning as a mediator of children’s insecurity prior to examining cortisol reactivity as an explanatory mechanism, predictive pathways from Wave 3 cortisol reactivity to Wave 4 adolescent executive functioning and Waves 4 and 5 school problems were constrained to 0.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(Color online) Structural equation model examining adolescent executive functioning mediating early histories of insecure representations of the interparental relationship and later school functioning. All path coefficients are standardized values. Light-colored dotted lines indicate structural paths that were included in the model, but were not significant at p < .05. CPT, Continuous Performance Test; SWM, Spatial Working Memory Task; SST, Spatial Span Task; SOC, Stocking of Cambridge Test; SES, socioeconomic status.

The model provided a good representation of the data, χ2 (175, N = 235) = 238.49, p < .001, root mean square error of approximation = 0.04, and χ2/df ratio = 1.36, and comparative fit index ¼ 0.95. Consistent with hypotheses, children’s history of emotional insecurity predicted adolescent executive function problems 7 years later at Wave 4 (β = 0.33, p = .02). Moreover, this pathway was robust in a broader model that also controlled for childhood attention problems (β = 0.29, p = .01), SES, child gender, and interparental conflict history as predictors of teen executive functioning problems. In turn, adolescent executive function problems at Wave 4 predicted their school adjustment problems 1 year later at Wave 5 (β = 0.33, p = .01). In addition, this association was evident in a broader analysis that controlled for the autoregressive path for adolescent school problems (β = 0.54, p < .001), the covariates, and childhood executive function problems. Further supporting the mediational hypothesis, bootstrapping tests utilizing the PRODCLIN software program indicated that the indirect path involving childhood history of insecurity, Wave 4 executive function difficulties, and Wave 5 school difficulties was significantly different from 0, 95% confidence interval (CI) [0.01–0.33] (MacKinnon, Fritz, Williams, & Lockwood, 2007). Finally, supporting the possibility that cortisol reactivity to interparental conflict may mediate the first part of the cascade between childhood insecurity history and Wave 4 executive functioning, childhood insecurity also predicted greater cortisol reactivity to the videotaped vignettes of interparental conflict (β = 0.35, p = .002).

The role of adrenocortical reactivity in the cascade of emotional insecurity

Given the evidence supporting mediation, we proceeded to examine a more complex cascade whereby childhood history of insecure representations increased vulnerability to teen executive function difficulties and, ultimately, school problems through its association with heightened cortisol reactivity to conflict. Thus, in addition to estimating all the pathways specified in Figure 2, we also freed up the constraints placed on the predictive paths for Wave 3 cortisol reactivity. Pathways were specifically estimated between Wave 3 cortisol reactivity to the interparental conflict vignettes and adolescent Wave 4 executive functioning and Waves 4 and 5 school problems. The model, which is presented in Figure 3, fit the data well, χ2 (172, N = 235) = 227.58, p < .01, root mean square error of approximation = 0.04, χ2/df ratio = 1.32, and comparative fit index ¼ 0.96. In support of the first link in the proposed mediational chain, children’s cortisol reactivity at Wave 3 was predicted by their insecure representations of interparental conflict from Waves 1 through 3 (β = 0.33, p < .01), even with the inclusion the covariates in the model. In the second link in the mediational chain, children’s cortisol reactivity to conflict at Wave 3, in turn, was associated with greater Wave 4 executive function problems (β = 0.27, p = .01) in the broader model that also specified SES, interparental conflict history, childhood executive function problems, and children’s gender and insecurity as predictors. Moreover, the original structural path between childhood insecurity and Wave 4 executive function impairments in Figure 2 (i.e., β = 0.33, p = .02) dropped by 52% to β = 0.16, ns, following the inclusion of Wave 3 cortisol reactivity as a predictor (see Figure 3). Finally, consistent with the results for analyses presented in Figure 2, poor executive function at Wave 4 continued to predict higher levels of school maladjustment 1 year later after estimating for the autoregressive path, covariates, and cortisol reactivity as predictors (β = 0.29, p = .03).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

(Color online) Structural equation model including cortisol reactivity as an intervening mechanism explaining the mediating role of adolescent executive functioning between early histories of insecure representations of the interparental relationship and later school functioning. All path coefficients are standardized values. Light-colored dotted lines indicate structural paths that were included in the model, but were not significant at p < .05. CPT, Continuous Performance Test; SWM, Spatial Working Memory Task; SST, Spatial Span Task; SOC, Stocking of Cambridge Test; SES, socioeconomic status.

Our findings on the multichain cascade of processes involving emotional insecurity can be further dichotomized into two interlocking indirect pathways. In the first part of the cascade, Wave 3 cortisol is hypothesized to mediate prospective associations between childhood history of emotional insecurity and Wave 4 executive function problems. Consistent with this prediction, the PRODCLIN bootstrapping analyses indicated that the indirect path in Figure 3 involving childhood history of insecurity, Wave 3 cortisol reactivity, and Wave 4 executive function problems was significantly different from 0, 95% CI [0.28–4.37]. In the subsequent part of the cascade, the results in Figure 3 also highlighted the plausibility of Wave 4 executive function problems serving as an intermediary process linking Wave 3 cortisol reactivity to Wave 5 school problems. Supporting this proposed pathway, the results of the PRODCLIN indicated that the mediational link involving Wave 3 cortisol reactivity, teen executive function problems at Wave 4, and their school problemsat Wave 5 was also significantly different form 0, 95% CI [0.003–0.12] (MacKinnon et al., 2007).

Finally, in the context of research highlighting the potential influence of both dampened and heightened cortisol levels on executive functioning (e.g., Blair et al., 2005, 2011), an alternative interpretation is that the linear association between greater cortisol reactivity and poorer executive functioning may be masking a more complex curvilinear relationship. To investigate this possibility, we retested our SEM model after including a manifest indicator representing a curvilinear (i.e., squared) term for cortisol reactivity. In support of original findings, the curvilinear term did not add unique explanatory power as a predictor of Wave 4 executive functioning impairments (β = –0.03, p = .85). In addition, the linear index of cortisol reactivity at Wave 3 continued to significantly predict Wave 4 executive functioning impairments (β = 0.29, p = .04).

Discussion

In spite of its consistent documentation as a mediator in the association between interparental conflict and children’s psychological problems, little is known about how and why children’s insecure representations of the interparental relationship increase their risk for adjustment problems (Cummings & Davies, 2010). Research in the broader literature has provided important clues to addressing this question by identifying dimensions of interparental conflict (e.g., hostility and aggression) that alter adrenocortical functioning and impair executive functioning (e.g., Blair et al., 2011; Repetti et al., 2011). In integrating these two literatures, EST-R hypothesizes that insecure representations of the interparental relationship increase children’s vulnerability to psychological problems by altering their cortisol reactivity to subsequent conflicts and executive functioning (Davies & Martin, 2013). This study provided a first test of this theoretically driven hypothesis whereby children’s cortisol reactivity and executive functioning difficulties serve as unfolding mediators in the link between their insecure representations and school adjustment problems (see Figure 1). Consistent with the first link in the mediational chain, the findings indicated that children’s insecure representations of the interparental relationship across three annual measurement occasions in childhood predicted greater executive functioning problems during early adolescence, even after controlling for childhood attention difficulties, interparental conflict exposure, family SES, and child gender. Adolescent executive functioning difficulties, in turn, were associated with teacher reports of school problems1 year later, even after accounting for the autoregressive path and covariates. Further supporting the developmental cascade of the processes underlying this link, children’s cortisol reactivity to conflict in childhood mediated the relationship between their insecure representations and later executive functioning difficulties in adolescence.

Executive functioning mediates insecure representations and later school adjustment

EST-R specifically proposes that insecure representations increase adjustment problems over time by prioritizing quick and efficient self-defense over more effortful, goal-directed skills characterized by flexible planning, working memory, and behavioral organization and flexibility (e.g., Davies & Martin, 2013). Our documentation of a prospective association between insecure representations in childhood and executive functioning problems in early adolescence provides initial support for the first link in the mediational chain. Moreover, these results are broadly consistent with both evolutionary and resource allocation proposals highlighting the mental health implications of individual differences in how organisms apportion limited psychobiological resources to multiple developmental tasks (e.g., Del Giudice et al., 2013; Inzlicht & Schmeichel, 2012). Interpreted within this framework, insecure representations represent the heavy dedication of affective and information processing capacities toward identifying and responding to threats in the interparental subsystem. Although highly specialized and elaborative ways of processing danger in the family may be adaptive for children in high-conflict homes, this can occur at the expense of a more balanced investment of resources toward mastery of other important goal pursuits (Davies & Martin, 2013). Children drawing on insecure representations as blueprints for interpreting and responding to subsequent interpersonal interactions are likely to be narrowly attentive and motivated toward defending against threat, and this may initially be manifested in decrements to higher order, sophisticated, and goal-oriented executive functions.

The second link in this mediational chain, our finding that neuropsychological impairments predict difficulties across academic, social, and psychological domains of school adjustment, contributes to the growing literature emphasizing the central role of executive functioning for healthy adjustment (e.g., Rothbart & Posner, 2015; Suor et al., 2015). For example, although longitudinal studies are largely limited to early childhood, empirical findings have identified children’s executive functioning abilities as predictors of subsequently higher levels of academic abilities, social competence, and emotional well-being in school contexts (Blair & Raver, 2015). Likewise, conceptualizations of executive functioning underscore their role in flexibly coordinating and shifting attention, affect, and cognition in a goal-directed manner that facilitates the enactment of contextually sensitive, reflective responses across challenging and novel settings, ultimately supporting academic, emotional, and social well-being (Blair, 2002; Snyder, 2013).

Cortisol reactivity mediates the link between insecure representations and executive functioning

Although resource allocation models provide a general foundation for understanding why insecurity may be associated with deficits in executive function, they offer less guidance as to the precise mechanisms that may explain how or why neuropsychological impairments would develop in the wake of early histories of insecurity. To address this substantive gap, EST-R draws on canalization and risky families models in proposing that this cascading pathway is underpinned by neurobiological mechanisms. According to these frameworks, early histories of family adversity alter neural connections between the limbic system (e.g., amygdala) and the prefrontal cortex in ways that increase the reflexive processing of environmental threats at the expense of neural structures that organize top-down control (i.e., executive functions; Blair et al., 2005, 2011; Repetti et al., 2011). Consistent with these broader models, EST-R posits that cortisol, as a central component of the stress response system, interferes with executive functioning in the wake of interpersonal threats (Davies et al., 2011, 2013). Thus, to further trace the legacy of childhood histories of insecurity, our second aim was to test the hypothesis that children’s cortisol reactivity to interadult conflict served as an explanatory mechanism in the first link of our proposed mediational chain (see Figure 1). Supporting the iatrogenic role of heightened cortisol, our findings indicated that children’s higher cortisol reactivity to videotaped simulations of interadult conflict during late childhood mediated the link between their insecure representations earlier in childhood and their subsequent executive functioning impairments in adolescence. Additional analyses revealed that the linear, prospective association between cortisol reactivity and executive functioning difficulties was not simply an artifact of a more complex curvilinear relationship whereby children with dampened or heightened cortisol reactivity exhibited the poorest executive functioning.

Neurobiological theory and research vary in their support of whether associations between early adversity and executive functioning difficulties are mediated by dampened cortisol, elevated cortisol, or both (Blair et al., 2011; Gunnar & Vazquez, 2001; Koss et al., 2013; Susman, 2006). Therefore, it is important to further address the question of why higher, rather than lower, cortisol reactivity to conflicts was identified as a risk mechanism. In drawing on neuroscience research, heightened cortisol activity has been consistently designated as having toxic effects on brain regions mediating executive functioning. A common hypothesis shared by multiple frameworks is that pronounced increases in cortisol may lead to reduced sensitivity and efficiency of neuronal pathways in the prefrontal cortex, particularly given the dense population of glucorticoid receptors in the brain region (Blair et al., 2005; De Bellis & Thomas, 2003; Lupien et al., 2005). Consistent with this hypothesis, Blair et al. (2005) found that preschool children who evidenced higher cortisol levels prior to meeting an unfamiliar adult and at the midpoint of the assessment with the unfamiliar adult exhibited poorer fluid cognitive abilities that, in turn, were associated with teacher reports of poorer school (e.g., social and academic competence) adjustment. Our findings are also consistent with other studies linking greater cortisol activity or reactivity with poorer executive functioning for both adults (e.g., Gonzalez, Jenkins, Steiner, & Fleming, 2012; Monk & Nelson, 2002) and children (e.g., Blair et al., 2011). Even conceptual proposals highlighting links between attenuated cortisol and psychological problems commonly implicate heightened cortisol output as a primary agent of toxicity. For example, as a key pathway in the attenuation hypothesis (Susman, 2006), downregulation of the HPA axis is postulated to thwart the chronic overarousal of the stress response system (e.g., higher cortisol) and its toxic effects on the brain. Thus, the toxic effects of HPA axis sensitization in the early or middle stages of contending with histories of threat may account for why dampened cortisol is associated with poorer executive functioning in some studies.

Limitations and conclusions

Our findings must be interpreted within the context of several methodological limitations. First, our community sample of families was predominantly White and from working- or middle-class backgrounds. Therefore, the cascading pathway identified in the present study may not necessarily generalize to families with other racial, ethnic, or social (e.g., poverty and family violence) backgrounds. Addressing questions about the applicability of our findings to other samples is particularly important given empirical evidence that children from higher risk backgrounds (e.g., economic impoverishment) may experience multiple pathogenic forms of HPA functioning (e.g., attenuated levels; Blair et al., 2011; Lupien et al., 2001; Suor et al., 2015). Second, although the timing of our measures was broadly consistent with the expected sequence of unfolding processes in the proposed cascade, the study design did not permit a full prospective analysis of change at each link in the mediational chain. For example, because repeated measures of children’s cortisol reactivity to interadult conflict were not obtained in our study, we could not examine whether children’s histories of insecure representations predicted subsequent increases in their cortisol reactivity over time. Likewise, although our inclusion of attention measures reduces the plausibility that the identified pathways were an artifact of early attention difficulties, we could not test a more rigorous autoregressive analysis of executive functions due to the absence of neuropsychological assessments in our measurement battery during childhood. Third, even with the inclusion of an autoregressive path for school adjustment and multiple covariates, we cannot rule out the possible operation of other extraneous variables in accounting for mediational pathways. For example, it is possible that more complex, transactional chains of processes are operating whereby executive functioning deficits increase children’s insecure representations of interparental relations (Beevers, 2005).

Fourth, future research would benefit from employing more extensive assessments of physiological reactivity and executive functioning than were available in our data set. Although our multitask assessment of executive functioning captured planning, problem solving, and spatial working memory, it still only captures a subset of the broader multidimensional construct. Thus, broadening neuropsychological assessments to also include indices of self-monitoring, task switching, and inhibitory control would provide a more comprehensive account of executive functions (Zelazo et al., 2013). Likewise, complementing performance-based measures, like those used in this study, with self- or informant-report measures of executive functioning will be important for future work. Improvements in the assessment of physiological reactivity beyond our pre–post measure of adrenocortical reactivity could also be achieved by the collection of more cortisol samples within and across measurement occasions within a more diverse physiological assessment battery (e.g., sympathetic nervous system reactivity).

Despite these limitations, the longitudinal findings of this multimethod, multilevel study were designed to break new ground in several ways. As the first comprehensive test of the role of neuropsychological functioning in EST-R, the findings provided support for a developmental pathway whereby insecure representations of interparental relations predicted greater executive functioning impairments in early adolescence that, in turn, were linked with subsequent increases in school adjustment problems following the transition to high school. Toward the goal of identifying the neurobiological underpinnings of this cascade, our SEM analyses further revealed that higher levels of cortisol reactivity to interadult conflict mediated the prospective links between insecurity in childhood and executive functioning and school adjustment difficulties in adolescence. Together, these findings begin to articulate a more precise understanding of the target mechanisms linking insecurity and school adjustment problems across multiple levels of analysis (e.g., psychological, cognitive, and physiological). Ultimately, articulating this complex cascade of developmental processes has the potential to inform interventions designed to stop the pathogenic consequences of early histories of emotional insecurity.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants R01 MH57318 and 2R01 MH57318 (to P.T.D., E.M.C., and D.C.). We are grateful to the children, parents, teachers, and school administrators who participated in this project. Our thanks also to the staff on the project, including Karin Gasaway, and to the graduate and undergraduate students at the University of Rochester and the University of Notre Dame.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, Dumenci L, Rescorla LA. DSM-oriented and empirically based approaches to constructing scales from the same item pools. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2003;32:328–340. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3203_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Beevers CG. Cognitive vulnerability to depression: A dual process model. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:975–1002. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bernier A, Carlson SM, Whipple N. From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development. 2010;81:326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Birch SH, Ladd GW. The teacher-child relationship and children’s early school adjustment. Journal of School Psychology. 1997;35:61–79. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(96)00029-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blair C. School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist. 2002;57:111–127. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blair C, Granger DA, Razza RP. Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development. 2005;76:554–567. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00863.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blair C, Granger DA, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Greenberg MT, Fortunato CK. Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development. 2011;82:1970–1984. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blair C, Raver CC. School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66:711–731. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blair C, Raver CC, Berry DJ. Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50:554–565. doi: 10.1037/a0033647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bretherton I, Oppenheim D, Buchsbaum H, Emde RN, MacArthur Narrative Group . MacArthur Story-Stem Battery. 1990. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  11. Buehler C, Lange G, Franck KL. Adolescents’ cognitive and emotional responses to marital hostility. Child Development. 2007;78:775–789. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01032.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Coe JL, Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML. The multivariate roles of family instability and interparental conflict in predicting children’s representations of insecurity in the family system and early school adjustment problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0164-6. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Conners CK. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II: Technical guide. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cowell RA, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL. Childhood maltreatment and its effect on neurocognitive functioning: Timing and chronicity matter. Development and Psychopathology. 2015;27:521–533. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cummings EM, Davies P. Emotional security as a regulatory process in normal development and the development of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:123–139. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cummings EM, Davies PT. Marital conflict and children: An emotional security perspective. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  17. Davies PT, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:387–411. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davies PT, Cummings EM. Exploring children’s emotional security as a mediator of the link between marital relations and child adjustment. Child Development. 1998;69:124–139. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06138.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davies PT, Martin MJ. The reformulation of emotional security theory: The role of children’s social defense in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:1435–1454. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Cicchetti D. Interparental aggression and children’s adrenocortical reactivity: Testing an evolutionary model of allostatic load. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:801–814. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM. Adrenocortical underpinnings of children’s psychological reactivity to interparental conflict. Child Development. 2008;79:1693–1706. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01219.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Martin MJ. Family discord and child health: An emotional security formulation. In: Lansdale NS, McHale SM, Booth A, editors. Families and child health. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 45–74. [Google Scholar]
  23. Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Winter MA, Cummings EM, Farrell D. Child adaptational development in contexts of interparental conflict over time. Child Development. 2006;77:218–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00866.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Davies PT, Winter MA, Cicchetti D. The implications of emotional security theory for understanding and treating childhood psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:707–735. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Davies PT, Woitach MJ, Winter MA, Cummings EM. Children’s insecure representations of the interparental relationship and their school adjustment: The mediating role of attention difficulties. Child Development. 2008;79:1570–1582. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01206.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. De Bellis MD, Thomas LA. Biologic findings of post-traumatic stress disorder and child maltreatment. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2003;5:108–117. doi: 10.1007/s11920-003-0027-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA. Making sense of stress: An evolutionary-developmental framework. In: Laviola G, Macri S, editors. Adaptive and maladaptive aspects of developmental stress. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 23–43. [Google Scholar]
  28. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130:355–391. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Du Rocher Schudlich TD, Papp LM, Cummings EM. Relations of husbands’ and wives’ dysphoria to marital conflict resolution strategies. Journal of Family Psychology. 2004;18:171–183. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ellis LK, Rothbart MK, Posner MI. Individual differences in executive attention predict self-regulation and adolescent psychosocial behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021:337–340. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Enders CK. The impact of nonnormality on full information maximum-likelihood estimation for structural equation models with missing data. Psychological Methods. 2001;6:352–370. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.6.4.352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Entwistle DR, Astone N. Some practical guidelines for measuring youth’s race-ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Child Development. 1994;65:1521–1540. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00833.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gau SSF, Shang CY. Executive functions as endophenotypes in ADHD: Evidence from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:838–849. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02215.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gonzalez A, Jenkins JM, Steiner M, Fleming AS. Maternal early life experiences and parenting: The mediating role of cortisol and executive function. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51:673–682. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Goodman R, Scott S. Child psychiatry. Oxford: Blackwell Science; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  36. Granger DA, Weisz JR, McCracken JT, Ikeda SC, Douglas P. Reciprocal influences among adrenocortical activation, psychosocial processes, and the behavioral adjustment of clinic-referred children. Child Development. 1996;67:3250–3262. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01912.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grych JH, Fincham FD. Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: A cognitive-contextual framework. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;108:267–290. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.2.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Grych JH, Harold GT, Miles CJ. A prospective investigation of appraisals as mediators of the link between interparental conflict and child adjustment. Child Development. 2003;74:1176–1193. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gunnar MR, Vazquez DM. Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: Potential indices of risk in human development. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:515–538. doi: 10.1017/S0954579401003066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gunnar MR, Vasquez DM. Stress neurobiology and developmental psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Developmental neuroscience. 2. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2006. pp. 533–577. [Google Scholar]
  41. Gustafsson HC, Coffman JL, Cox MJ. Intimate partner violence, maternal sensitive parenting behaviors, and children’s executive functioning. Psychology of Violence. 2015;5:266–274. doi: 10.1037/a0037971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Harter S. Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. Denver, CO: University of Denver Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hill AC, Laird AR, Robinson JL. Gender differences in working memory networks: A BrainMap meta-analysis. Biological Psychology. 2014;102:18–29. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hinnant JB, El-Sheikh M, Keiley M, Buckhalt JA. Marital conflict, allostatic load, and the development of children’s fluid cognitive performance. Child Development. 2013;84:2003–2014. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Holmes CJ, Kim-Spoon J, Deater-Deckard K. Linking executive function and peer problems from early childhood through middle adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;44:31–42. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0044-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Inzlicht M, Schmeichel BJ. What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2012;7:450–463. doi: 10.1177/1745691612454134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Johnston JR, Roseby V, Kuehnle K. In the name of the child: A developmental approach to understanding and helping children of conflicted and violent divorce. 2. New York: Springer; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kaplan S, Berman MG. Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2010;5:43–57. doi: 10.1177/1745691609356784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kerig PK. Assessing the links between interparental conflict and child adjustment: The conflicts and problem-solving scales. Journal of Family Psychology. 1996;10:454–473. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.10.4.454. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kitzmann KM, Gaylord NK, Holt AR, Kenny ED. Child witnesses to domestic violence: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:339–352. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.2.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Korte SM, Koolhaas JM, Wingfield JC, McEwen BS. The Darwinian concept of stress: Benefits of allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2005;29:3–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Koss KJ, George MR, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, Sturge-Apple ML. Patterns of children’s adrenocortical reactivity to interparental conflict and associations with child adjustment: A growth mixture modeling approach. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:317–326. doi: 10.1037/a0028246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ladd GW, Profilet SM. The Child Behavior Scale: A teacher-report measure of young children’s aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:1008–1024. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.6.1008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Langenkamp AG. Academic vulnerability and resilience during the transition to high school: The role of social relationships and district context. Sociology of Education. 2010;83:1–19. doi: 10.1177/0038040709356563. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Latzman RD, Elkovitch N, Young J, Clark LA. The contribution of executive functioning to academic achievement among male adolescents. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2010;32:455–462. doi: 10.1080/13803390903164363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lengua LJ, Kiff C, Moran L, Zalewski M, Thompson S, Cortes R, Ruberry E. Parenting mediates the effects of income and cumulative risk on the development of effortful control. Social Development. 2014;23:631–649. doi: 10.1111/sode.12071. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Loman MM, Gunnar MR. Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulation in children. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34:867–876. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Loney BR, Butler MA, Lima EN, Counts CA, Eckel LA. The relation between salivary cortisol, callous-unemotional traits, and conduct problems in an adolescent non-referred sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:30–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01444.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Luciana M, Conklin HM, Hooper CJ, Yarger RS. The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Development. 2005;76:697–712. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00872.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Luciana M, Nelson CA. Assessment of neuropsychological function through use of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery: Performance in 4- to 12-year-old children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2002;22:595–624. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2203_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lupien SJ, Fiocco A, Wan N, Maheu F, Lord C, Schramek T, Tu M. Stress hormones and human memory function across the life-span. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:225–242. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:653–676. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:434–445. doi: 10.1038/nrn2639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. MacKinnon DP, Fritz MS, Williams J, Lockwood CM. Distribution of the product confidence limits for the indirect effect: Program PRODCLIN. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:384–389. doi: 10.3758/BF03193007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Malik NM, Lindahl KM. System for coding interactions in dyads (SCID) In: Kerig PK, Baucom DH, editors. Couple observational coding systems. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2004. pp. 173–188. [Google Scholar]
  67. Masten AS, Cicchetti D. Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:491–495. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Monk CS, Nelson CA. The effects of hydrocortisone on cognitive and neural function: A behavioral and event-related potential investigation. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:505–519. doi: 10.1038/S0893-133X(01)00384-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Porter B, O’Leary KD. Marital discord and childhood behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1980;8:287–295. doi: 10.1007/BF00916376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Repetti RL, Robles TF, Reynolds B. Allostatic processes in the family. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:921–938. doi: 10.1017/S095457941100040x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:330–366. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rothbart MK, Posner MI. The developing brain in a multitasking world. Developmental Review. 2015;35:42–63. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Shelton KH, Harold GT. Interparental conflict, negative parenting, and children’s adjustment: Bridging links between parents’ depression and children’s psychological distress. Journal of Family Psychology. 2008;22:712–724. doi: 10.1037/a0013515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Snyder HR. Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139:81–132. doi: 10.1037/a0028727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Granger DA, Eggum ND, Sallquist J, Haugen RG, Hofer C. Individual differences in preschoolers’ salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase reactivity: Relations to temperament and maladjustment. Hormones and Behavior. 2009;56:133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Steinberg L. Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sturge-Apple ML, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Manning LG. Interparental violence, maternal emotional unavailability and children’s cortisol functioning in family contexts. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48:237–249. doi: 10.1037/a0025419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sturge-Apple ML, Davies PT, Winter MA, Cummings EM, Schermerhorn A. Interparental conflict and children’s school adjustment: The explanatory role of children’s internal representations of interparental and parent-child relationships. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:1678–1690. doi: 10.1037/a0013857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Suor JH, Sturge-Apple ML, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Manning LG. Tracing differential pathways of risk: Associations among family adversity, cortisol, and cognitive functioning in childhood. Child Development. 2015;86:1142–1158. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Susman EJ. Psychobiology of persistent antisocial behavior: Stress, early vulnerabilities and the attenuation hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30:376–389. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Toth SL, Rogosch FA, Sturge-Apple M, Cicchetti D. Maternal depression, children’s attachment security, and representational development: An organizational perspective. Child Development. 2009;80:192–208. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01254.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J, Wallner-Allen K, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2013;78 doi: 10.1111/mono.12032. 4, Serial No. 309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Zelazo PD, Müller U. Executive function in typical and atypical development. In: Goswami U, editor. The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. 2. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell; 2010. pp. 574–603. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES