Abstract
Neurofibromatosis Type 1 (NF1) is commonly associated with deficits in executive functions such as working memory and inhibitory control. A valid biomarker to describe the pathological basis of these deficits in NF1 is not available. The aim of this study was to investigate whether any abnormalities in white matter integrity of the executive function related anterior thalamic radiation (ATR), cingulate bundle (CB), and superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) may be regarded as a pathological basis for inhibitory control deficits in adolescents with NF1. Sixteen NF1 patients and 32 healthy controls underwent 3 T DTI MRI scanning. Whole brain-, ATR-, CB-, and SLF-white matter integrity were studied using fractional anisotropy, mean (MD), radial, and axial (DA) diffusivity. Correlation analyses between white matter metrics and inhibitory control (as measured with a computerized task) were performed. Also, verbal and performance abilities (IQ-estimates) were assessed and correlated with white matter metrics. Patients showed significant whole brain- and local microstructural pathology when compared to healthy controls in all measures. In NF1-patients, whole-brain (MD: r = .646 and DA: r = .673) and ATR- (r-range: −.405–.771), but not the CB- (r-range: −.307–.472) and SLF- (r-range: −.187–.406) white matter integrity, were correlated with inhibitory control. Verbal and performance abilities were not associated with white matter pathology. In NF1, white matter abnormalities are observed throughout the brain, but damage to the ATR seems specifically, or at least most strongly related to inhibitory control. Future studies should examine whether reduced white matter integrity in other brain regions or tracts is (more strongly) associated with different aspects of the cognitive-behavioral phenotype associated with NF1.
Keywords: Neurofibromatosis type I, DTI, Anterior thalamic radiation, Executive functions
Introduction
Neurofibromatosis Type 1 (NF1) is a single-gene disorder affecting approximately 1 in 3.500 individuals (National Institutes of Health. 1988). In NF1, a loss of function mutation of the neurofibromin gene leads to increased expression of rat sarcoma (Ras-) proteins (name based on discovery of two cancer-causing viruses in rats). Ras is expressed in all cell lineages and organs (including the brain), and both directly and indirectly, through for instance the MAPK- and PI3K/AKT/mTOR pathways, regulates intracellular signaling. Loss of neurofibromin and subsequent increased Ras-activity have been associated with abnormalities in (neural) cell differentiation, growth, and apoptosis (Dasgupta and Gutmann 2005; National Institutes of Health. 1988; Shilyansky et al. 2010; Tidyman and Rauen 2009).
In line with the pathophysiological loss of control over these cellular processes, including neuronal cells, brain imaging studies have shown various cerebral abnormalities in NF1. These include volumetric abnormalities (mainly increased white matter and subcortical gray matter volumes, and decreased cortical grey matter density) (S. C. Huijbregts et al. 2015) and lower cortical gyrification (Violante et al. 2013). Other frequently-observed abnormalities are focal areas of hyperintensities on T2-weighted images, of which the exact nature is not yet known (Billiet et al. 2014) and reduced integrity of white matter microstructure (Ferraz-Filho et al. 2011; Karlsgodt et al. 2012; van Engelen et al. 2008). Damage to white matter microstructure was found in the entire brain (Karlsgodt et al. 2012).
NF1-patients also exhibit very high rates of cognitive and social impairment. Specifically, 60–80 % of children with NF1 experience (specific) learning disabilities (Hyman et al. 2005), up to 60 % show mild to severe autism symptoms, and up to 50 % qualifies for a diagnosis of Attention Deficit (Hyperactivity) Disorder (Garg et al. 2013; Hyman et al. 2005). Considering cognition, executive dysfunction is a major hallmark of the disease (Diggs-Andrews and Gutmann 2013; Huijbregts et al. 2010b; Rowbotham et al. 2009). Core executive functions include inhibitory control, working memory and cognitive flexibility (Miyake et al. 2000). All of these are affected in NF1 (Huijbregts et al. 2010b; Rowbotham et al. 2009). For executive functioning, positive associations have been reported between executive impairment and subcortical volumes in NF1 (S. C. Huijbregts et al. 2015). With respect to the impact of T2-hyperintensities on cognition assocations with lesion location have been revealed, but not with presence or number (Hyman et al. 2007; Moore et al. 1996).
Surprisingly, no studies have been performed yet examining associations between white matter microstructural abnomalities and the cognitive-behavioral phenotype of NF1, although it has been suggested that presence of T2-hyperintensities might impair functioning of white matter tracts and subsequently cognition (Payne et al. 2014). White matter integrity may be the best neural substrate for functional connectivity, which several recent studies have shown to be abnormal in NF1 (Loitfelder et al. 2015; Tomson et al. 2015). Also, abnormalities in functional connectivity have been related to parent-reported problems with executive functioning in NF1 (Loitfelder et al. 2015).
Reduced microstructural white matter integrity in adult NF1 patients compared to healthy controls was reported to be present in the entire brain, but most prominently in the anterior thalamic radiation (ATR) (Karlsgodt et al. 2012). The ATR connects the thalamus with the frontal cortex and its strategic role in executive functioning has been shown in other disorders, such as CADASIL (Duering et al. 2011), a hereditary stroke disorder, schizophrenia (Mamah et al. 2010), and first episode psychosis (Pérez-Iglesias et al. 2010). Although not specifically highlighted in NF1, other tracts such as the cingulate bundle, projecting from the posterior cingulate cortex to the medial prefrontal cortex (Gordon et al. 2011), and the superior longitudinal fasciculus, which connects the parietal, occipital and temporal lobes with ipsilateral frontal cortices (Schmahmann et al. 2008) have also been associated with executive functioning (Heilbronner and Haber 2014; Mesulam 1998; Nestor et al. 2004; Petrides and Pandya 2002).
In the present study, it was investigated whether whole brain white matter integrity and white matter integrity in tracts specifically associated with executive functions were related to inhibitory control in adolescents with NF1.
Methods
Participants
Sixteen NF1 patients (7 male, age: M = 12.45, SD = 2.75, min-max: 9.3–18.6) and 32 healthy controls (21 male, age: M = 12.43, SD = 2.99, min-max: 9.2–19.0; tage = −0.014, page = .989; χ2 sex = 1.835, psex = .176) underwent structural MRI scanning. All NF1 subjects fulfilled the diagnostic criteria specified by the National Institutes of Health Consensus Conference (National Institutes of Health. 1988).
Scanning procedure
All subjects underwent scanning at the Leiden University Medical Center. Imaging was performed on a Philips 3 Tesla Achieva MRI scanner using an 8 channel SENSE receiver head coil (Philips Healthcare, Best, The Netherlands). In each subject, DTI data were acquired using a 16 directions spin echo sequence with 73 slices without a gap in AC-PC direction (TR 8.36 s, TE 56 ms, flip angle 90 degrees, 2.3 mm isotropic voxels, FOV = 220 × 167.9, b-value 1000s/mm2, total scan time of 183 s, and one B0-image). For the evaluation of T2-hyperintensities, T2-weighted structural scans were acquired (52 slices, voxel size: 0.43 × 0.478, 3 mm slice thickness, FOV: 220x175x156, TE 80 ms, no slice gap, scan duration 236 s). All anatomical scans were reviewed by a neuro-radiologist.
Image processing
All analyses were performed using FMRIB’s Software Library (Smith et al. 2004). Preprocessing included pre-alignment, correction for eddy currents, and brain extraction. FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox was used to reconstruct diffusion tensors, fitting a diffusion tensor model at each voxel. Tensor eigenvalues describing the diffusion strength in primary, secondary and tertiary diffusion directions were extracted. Voxelwise statistical analysis of the fractional anisotropy (FA) data was carried out using Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS (Smith et al. 2006)). All subjects’ FA data were aligned into a common space using the nonlinear registration tool (FMRIB’s non-linear registration tool, (Smith et al. 2004)). Next, the mean FA image was created and thinned to create a mean FA skeleton which represents the centers of all tracts common to the group. Each subject’s aligned FA data was then projected onto this skeleton and the resulting data fed into voxelwise cross-subject statistics. FA, mean diffusivity (MD), axial diffusivity (DA) and radial diffusivity (RD) were used as measures of microstructural white matter integrity. RD was calculated using the average of the secondary and the tertiary eigenvectors (L2 + L3/2). Each additional measure (MD, DA, and RD) was projected on the FA-skeleton. Permutation-based inferences (FSL’s randomize, 5000 permutations, FWE-corrected, p = 0.05) with threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) were carried out for voxelwise analyses of the skeleton. Two sample t-tests were used for the whole brain voxelwise group comparisons (NF1 patients vs. healthy controls).
Thereafter, masks of the ATR, the cingulate bundle (CB) and the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) (Fig. 1) were created using binarised regions of interest, which were based on the John Hopkins University White-Matter Tractography Atlas (Mori et al. 2005). To ensure value extraction in white matter only, the mask was applied to the mean FA skeleton. This confines the statistical analysis exclusively to voxels from the center of the tract, thereby minimizing anatomic inter-subject variability, registration errors, and partial volume effects. Averaged FA, MD, DA, and RD values of the regions of interest were used to examine group differences and the relation with inhibitory control.
Neuropsychological assessment
The Sustained Attention Dots-task from the computerized Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks (ANT) was used to evaluate inhibitory control (S C J Huijbregts et al. 2002). In the Sustained Attention Dots-task (600 trials) participants press the yes-button (i.e. a response with the index finger of the dominant hand) when 4 randomly placed dots appear on the computer screen (200 trials), and the no-button (a response with the index finger of the non-dominant hand) when 3 or 5 randomly placed dots appear (total of 400 trials). As pressing the no-button becomes the predominant (“automatic”) response, inhibitory control is required when responses with the yes-button have to be given. Lack of accuracy of task performance (error rate) was used to represent quality of inhibitory control. Neuropsychological assessment also included the following subtests from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (Wechsler et al. 2011): Vocabulary and Comprehension (verbal comprehension), Block-Design (visual-spatial abilities), Picture Completion (fluid reasoning), Symbol Search, and Coding (processing speed). Verbal abilities were estimated based on the z-transformed subscales vocabulary and comprehension, performance abilities were estimated on the basis of all other z-transformed subtests. Neuropsychological data were collected for 13 NF1 patients and 9 HC.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 21 for Windows (SPSS, Chicago, IL). Whenever appropriate, non-parametric procedures were applied. Correlation analyses between averaged whole brain or averaged tract-based DTI metrics, and inhibitory control, and verbal and performance abilities were conducted (false discovery rate corrected for multiple comparisons, q = .05). Correlation analyses were conducted in patients only. To increase accuracy of sampling estimates, 1000 equally sized random samples with replacement were generated (bootstrapping) using bias corrected and accelerated intervals. The confidence intervals of the bootstrapping approach were used for inferences. Also, differences in DTI-metrics between left and right hemisphere were assessed. To indicate strength of results, relative deviation in white matter microstructure compared to healthy controls, effect size (Cohen’s d) and percent of non-overlap of the group specific distributions were calculated.
T2 hyperintensities
The T2 scans from NF1 patients were visually checked by a neuroradiologist for presence of T2-hyperintensities. Next, hand-labelled masks were created of all voxels showing T2-hyperintensities. To examine the potential influence of thalamic T2-hyperintensities (for which specifically effects on cognition had been shown: (Hyman et al. 2007; Moore et al. 1996; Payne et al. 2014)) on the ATR-microstructure, non-parametric group comparisons using the ATR’s FA, MD,RD, and DA between patients with and without T2-hyperintensities were conducted.
Results
Voxel-wise microstructural analysis of the whole brain
Whole brain voxel-wise permutation analyses revealed a reduction of FA and an increase of MD, RD, and DA throughout the whole brain in NF1 adolescents when compared to controls (Fig. 2). Correlation analyses in NF1 patients revealed an association between the averaged values of the MD and inhibitory control (r = .646, CIbootstrap = 0.034–0.882), and the averaged values of the DA and inhibitory control (r = .673, CIbootstrap = 0.272–0.861) (Fig. 3). No significant correlation was identified for the averaged values of the whole brain DTI metrics and our estimates of verbal, performance and total abilities.
The microstructure of cingulate bundle-, superior longitudinal fasciculus-, and ATR-white matter
Group comparisons using the averaged values of the cingulate bundle, the superior longitudinal fasciculus, and the ATR revealed for all but a few significantly decreased FA and increased MD, RD and DA in NF1 patients when compared to controls (Table 1). The averaged values of the FA, MD, RD, and DA of the CB, the SLF and the ATR showed differences between patients and controls in the range of 0.7 to 12.1 %, being most pronounced in the RD (Fig. 4). Also, large effect sizes (CB range: 0.7–1.2; SLF range: −0.3-1.2; ATR range: −0.8-(1.6)), which were associated with a high percentage of non-overlapping distributions were found (Fig. 4). To examine hemispherical differences in microstructural damage between left and right averaged CB, paired t-tests restricted to NF1 patients revealed differences in the FA (t(15) = 11.091, p < 0.001), RD (t(15) = −5.856, p < 0.001), and DA (t(15) = 3.162, p = 0.006), but not for MD (t(15) = −1.441, p = .170). In the SLF, FA (t(15) = 3.196, p = 0.006), MD (t(15) = −3.037, p = 0.008), and RD (t(15) = −3.738, p = 0.002), but not DA (t(15) = −0.394, p = 0.699) showed hemispherical differences. In the ATR, the FA (t(15) = 3.029, p = .008), MD (t(15) = −3.067, p = .008), and RD (t(15) = −.846, p = .002), but not the DA (t(15) = −.702, p = .494 ) showed significant differences between left and right hemisphere. All tracts indicate less microstructural damage to the left hemisphere, with the exception of the DA of the cingulate bundle. A similar effect was found in control subjects (CB: FA t(31) = 19.849, p < 0.001; MD t(31) = −2.134, p = 0.041; RD t(31) = −10.284, p < 0.001, DA t(31) = 7.867, p < 0.001; SLF: FA t(31) = 5.871, p < 0.001; MD t(31) = −4.385, p < 0.001; RD t(31) = −5.388, p < 0.001; DA t(31) = 1.572, p = 0.126; ATR: FA: t(31) = 5.832, p < 0.001; MD: U = −4.938, p < 0.001; RD: t(31) = −10.413, p < 0.001; DA: U = −3.009, p = 0.003).
Table 1.
left | right | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
df | t | p | df | t | p | ||
Cingulate bundle | FA | 46 | 2.746 | 0.009 | 46 | 2.983 | 0.005 |
MD | 46 | -4.623 | <0.001 | 46 | -3.856 | <0.001 | |
RD | 46 | -4.755 | <0.001 | 46 | -4.199 | <0.001 | |
DA | 46 | -2.241 | 0.030 | 46 | -2.422 | 0.019 | |
Superior longitudinal fasciculus | FA | 46 | 0.995 | 0.325 | 46 | -0.294 | 0.770 |
MD | * | 75.000 | <0.001 | * | 100.500 | 0.001 | |
RD | 46 | -2.467 | 0.017 | * | 188.500 | 0.140 | |
DA | 46 | -3.972 | <0.001 | * | 86.500 | <0.001 | |
Anterior thalamic radiation | FA | 46 | 4.942 | <0.001 | 46 | 4.111 | <0.001 |
MD | * | 21.000 | <0.001 | * | 35.500 | <0.001 | |
RD | * | 23.000 | <0.001 | 46 | -7.307 | <0.001 | |
DA | 46 | -3.603 | 0.001 | 46 | -3.058 | 0.004 |
*U-Test
Relation of the cingulate bundle, the superior longitudinal fasciculus and the ATR with inhibitory control
No DTI-measure of the CB or the SLF revealed a significant correlation with our measure of inhibitory control in NF1 patients (CB: FA left: r = −.203, CIbootstrap = −.785–.424; FA right: r = −.307, CIbootstrap = −.776–.349; MD left: r = .350, CIbootstrap = .-483–.879; MD right: r = −.009, CIbootstrap = −.769–.653; RD left: r = .472, CIbootstrap = −.366–.823; RD right: r = .099, CIbootstrap = −.659–.709; DA left: r = .093, CIbootstrap = −.477–.731; DA right: r = −.131, CIbootstrap = −.778–.595; SLF: FA left: r = .055, CIbootstrap = −.583–.720; FA right: r = −.187, CIbootstrap = −.725–.483; MD left: r = .241, CIbootstrap = .-512–.712; MD right: r = .406, CIbootstrap = −.594–.902; RD left: r = .111, CIbootstrap = −.643–.583; RD right: r = .331, CIbootstrap = −.456–.790; DA left: r = .280, CIbootstrap = −.387–.785; DA right: r = .347, CIbootstrap = −.286–.772). In contrast, for the ATR associations with inhibitory control were evident for FA (r = −.405, CIbootstrap = −0.752-(−0.060); not significant after correction for multiple comparisons), MD (r = .707, CIbootstrap = 0.196–0.930), and RD (r = .771, CIbootstrap = 0.490–0.919), all in the left hemisphere (Fig. 3). No significant correlations between FA, MD, RD, and DA of the CB, the SLF or the ATR and our IQ-estimates were found.
Neuropsychological results
When compared to controls, NF1 patients showed significantly lower scores on our measure of performance abilities (MHC = 0.606, SDHC = 0.638; MNF1 = −0.474, SDNF1 = 0.812; t(18) = 3.250, p = .004) and total abilities (MHC = 0.551, SDHC = 0.620; MNF1 = −0.402, SDNF1 = 0.730; t(18) = 3.107, p = .006). A trend was found for verbal abilities (MHC = 0.442, SDHC = 0.680; MNF1 = −0.259, SDNF1 = 0.899; t(18) = 1.928, p = .070). Despite relatively large absolute differences in error rate, no significant group differences were found for inhibitory control between patients and controls (Mcontrols = 20.11 ± 15.36, MNF1 = 32.85 ± 27.37, t(20) = −1.259, p = .222).
T2-hyperintensities
T2-hyperintensities were identified in 62.5 % (n = 10 of 16) of the patients, of which 31.3 % (n = 5) showed T2-hyperintensities in the thalamus, 37.5 % (n = 6) in the cerebellum, 25 % (n = 4) in the pallidum, 18.8 % (n = 3) in the brainstem and in the cortical grey matter, 6.3 % (n = 1) in the putamen and amygdala, and 31.3 % (n = 5) in the cerebral white matter. Also, 12.5 % (n = 2) subjects had one T2- hyperintensity in the left ATR. To determine whether thalamic T2-hyperintensities have an effect on the microstructural integrity of ATR, subjects were separated in two groups, thalamic T2-hyperintensities present (n = 5) and absent (n = 11). Non-parametric group comparisons revealed no significant differences in the ATR FA (left: p = 0.267, right: p = 0.377), MD (left: p = 0.320, right; p = 0.913), RD (left; p = 0.743, right: p = 0.913) or DA (left; p = 0.115, right: p = 0.267).
Discussion
In this study we found extensive global and local white matter abnormalities in NF1 adolescents. The significance of white matter pathology and its impact on cognitive functioning has already been recognized in various neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders (Duering et al. 2011; Mamah et al. 2010; Pérez-Iglesias et al. 2010; Yu et al. 2012). In NF1, white matter pathology has also been observed before (Ferraz-Filho et al. 2011; Karlsgodt et al. 2012; van Engelen et al. 2008), but its functional significance as a biomarker for the quantification of impaired executive functioning had not yet been investigated. We found a number of associations between whole brain white matter integrity and inhibitory control, one of the core executive functions, in NF1. In order to achieve more specific results, we examined white matter integrity in three tracts, the cingulate bundle, the superior longitudinal fasciculus, and the anterior thalamic radiation, which have all been associated with executive functioning (Duering et al. 2011; Gordon et al. 2011; Heilbronner and Haber 2014; Mamah et al. 2010; Nestor et al. 2004; Pérez-Iglesias et al. 2010; Petrides and Pandya 2002; Schmahmann et al. 2008). Whereas indeed we found strong associations between white matter integrity of the ATR and inhibitory control, no significant associations were observed for the CB and the SLF. A possible explanation for the apparent specificity of the association between ATR WM-integrity and inhibitory control in this study might lie in the nature of the task that was used, or, in other words, in the type of inhibitory control measured by the Sustained Attention Dots-task -task. Inhibitory control as measured by the Sustained Attention Dots-task may be classified as “cool” executive functioning: it is “decontextualized”, and does not involve affect or motivation (Griffith-Lendering et al. 2012; Stephan C J Huijbregts et al. 2008; Rubia 2011). The ATR connects the thalamus and the frontal cortex and does not seem to involve projections from/to cortical or subcortical brain regions that have specifically been related to affect/emotion and motivation, whereas the CB connects brain regions associated with executive functions, decision-making and emotion (Gordon et al. 2011). The CB might therefore be associated more strongly with so-called “hot” executive functioning, i.e. executive functioning with an affective or motivational component or within an affective or motivational context (Griffith-Lendering et al. 2012; Stephan C J Huijbregts et al. 2008; Rubia 2011). Alternatively, however, the CB is one of the brain’s major white matter pathways and its specific architecture may have to be considered in more detail. Heilbronner and Haber (Heilbronner and Haber 2014) found evidence for a cingulate bundle architecture involving four compartments, with some connections to limbic structures (such as the amygdala), and others not connected to such structures associated with affect/emotion and/or motivation. Therefore, it seems possible that white matter integrity of some but not all compartments of the CB is related to “cool” executive functioning, while white matter integrity of other compartments may be associated more strongly with “hot” executive functioning. A similar explanation may be suggested for the lack of significant associations between SLF white matter integrity and inhibitory control, the SLF also being a large bundle of white matter fibers, present in each hemisphere, and with potentially different functional significance in different segments. Such an explanation would corroborate with the fact that we found associations between whole brain white matter integrity and inhibitory control as well, as the contribution of the ATR WM-integrity (possibly together with the non-significant contributions of the CB and the SLF) to whole brain WM-integrity may be sufficient to render these correlations significant. However, in order to provide more definite answers future studies should segment further what are now considered unitary regions of interest, and include neuropsychological tasks that measure other executive functions such as working memory and cognitive flexibility and tasks that measure “hot” executive functions (see (Griffith-Lendering et al. 2012; Stephan C J Huijbregts et al. 2008; Rubia 2011)). One (further) reason for investigating this in more detail is that it has convincingly been shown that NF1-patients have extensive socio-emotional problems (Garg et al. 2013; S. C. Huijbregts et al. 2015) as well as impaired social information processing (Huijbregts et al. 2010a; Pride et al. 2014), and it seems reasonable to hypothesize that white matter integrity of tracts other than the ATR are more strongly associated with such problems.
Another important topic for future studies is to integrate what is known at neurobiological or cellular signaling level regarding NF1-pathophysiology and what has consistently been found at neuroanatomical level, and to investigate interrelations between different neuroanatomical abnormalities observed in NF1. Globally, we found mean and axial diffusivity to be negatively associated with inhibitory control. Mean diffusivity is a measure of the average molecular motion independent of any directionality and is affected by cellular size and integrity (Cercignani et al. 2001). Axial diffusivity was reported to be specifically sensitive to axonal degeneration (Alexander et al. 2007). In NF1, DTI-metrics have been related to T2-hyperintensities, although white matter abnormalities were also observed in lesion-free brain regions (Eastwood et al. 2001; Ferraz-Filho et al. 2011). The results of our study indicated that presence of T2-hyperintensities did not determine abnormalities of the microstructure in ATR in NF1-adolescents. Moreover, apart from some evidence showing associations between lesions at specific (subcortical) locations and cognitive outcomes, the hypothesis stating that T2-hyperintensities influence cognition lacks empirical support (S. C. Huijbregts et al. 2015; Hyman et al. 2007; Moore et al. 1996). Although results to date suggest only a relatively marginal role for T2-hyperintensities in both white matter microstructure and cognitive outcomes, whilst white matter microstructure itself seems to play a significant role in cognitive outcomes, it should be noted that the sample size of patients with and without T2-hyperintensities was limited, thus warranting further studies on this issue. Altogether, however, results of this and other studies suggest that concurrent pathological (e.g. neurobiological/cellular) processes affect white matter microstructure in NF1 patients.
As noted, the main limitation of this study was its sample size. However, the sample size is comparable to other imaging studies in NF1 (Karlsgodt et al. 2012; Pride et al. 2014; Violante et al. 2013). Perhaps more importantly, the strong involvement of white matter integrity of the ATR in inhibitory control was corroborated by high effect sizes (all above 0.8) and non-overlapping distributions when comparing NF1-patients and controls. Also, estimation of verbal and performance abilities and inhibitory control was not available for all subjects (with those data particularly not collected among healthy controls). Group differences in absolute numbers, which are similar to, or even larger than those observed in other studies (Huijbregts et al. 2010b; Rowbotham et al. 2009), indicating lower scores on IQ-subtests for NF1-patiens and more inhibitory control errors for NF1-patients than controls, may not always have reached significance due to limited power. Lastly, to underpin our speculations on different aspects of executive functioning possibly being associated with specific white matter tracts, tasks incorporating different executive function-dimensions have to be included in future studies.
It is concluded that, in NF1, microscopic white matter damage to the ATR is strongly associated with inhibitory control deficits. White matter integrity of the ATR may be predictive of other impairments in executive functioning characterizing NF1 as well, and thus represent an important target in therapeutic interventions. Although there is still much to be clarified, the single-gene disorder NF1 provides an extremely relevant model for studying associations between neurobiology, cell signaling, neuroanatomy and cognition/behavior as well.
Acknowledgments
Open access funding provided by Medical University of Graz.
Compliance with ethical standards
All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Grants
This study was funded by grants from an Erwin Schroedinger Fellowship from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF: J3500 B23) to MK, the Dutch Neurofibromatosis Association to SH, and by the Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition. SR is supported by a Vici grant (016130677) from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). Study sponsors were not involved in study design, data collection, analyses, or interpretation of the data. MK, SR, IV, MB, SH declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
References
- Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Field AS. Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics: the journal of the American Society for Experimental NeuroTherapeutics. 2007;4(3):316–329. doi: 10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billiet T, Mädler B, D’Arco F, Peeters R, Deprez S, Plasschaert E, et al. Characterizing the microstructural basis of “unidentified bright objects” in neurofibromatosis type 1: a combined in vivo multicomponent T2 relaxation and multi-shell diffusion MRI analysis. Neuro Image. Clinical. 2014;4:649–658. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cercignani M, Inglese M, Pagani E, Comi G, Filippi M. Mean diffusivity and fractional anisotropy histograms of patients with multiple sclerosis. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2001;22(5):952–958. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dasgupta B, Gutmann DH. Neurofibromin regulates neural stem cell proliferation, survival, and astroglial differentiation in vitro and in vivo. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2005;25(23):5584–5594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4693-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diggs-Andrews KA, Gutmann DH. Modeling cognitive dysfunction in neurofibromatosis-1. Trends in Neurosciences. 2013;36(4):237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duering M, Zieren N, Hervé D, Jouvent E, Reyes S, Peters N, et al. Strategic role of frontal white matter tracts in vascular cognitive impairment: a voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study in CADASIL. Brain. 2011;134(Pt 8):2366–2375. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eastwood JD, Fiorella DJ, MacFall JF, Delong DM, Provenzale JM, Greenwood RS. Increased brain apparent diffusion coefficient in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Radiology. 2001;219(2):354–358. doi: 10.1148/radiology.219.2.r01ap25354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferraz-Filho JRL, da Rocha AJ, Muniz MP, Souza AS, Goloni-Bertollo EM, Pavarino-Bertelli EC. Diffusion tensor MR imaging in neurofibromatosis type 1: expanding the knowledge of microstructural brain abnormalities. Pediatric Radiology. 2011;42(2):449–454. doi: 10.1007/s00247-011-2274-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garg S, Lehtonen A, Huson SM, Emsley R, Trump D, Evans DG, Green J. Autism and other psychiatric comorbidity in neurofibromatosis type 1: evidence from a population-based study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2013;55(2):139–145. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon EM, Lee PS, Maisog JM, Foss-Feig J, Billington ME, Vanmeter J, Vaidya CJ. Strength of default mode resting-state connectivity relates to white matter integrity in children. Developmental Science. 2011;14(4):738–751. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01020.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffith-Lendering MFH, Huijbregts SCJ, Vollebergh WAM, Swaab H. Motivational and cognitive inhibitory control in recreational cannabis users. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2012;34(7):688–697. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.668874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilbronner SR, Haber SN. Frontal cortical and subcortical projections provide a basis for segmenting the cingulum bundle: implications for neuroimaging and psychiatric disorders. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2014;34(30):10041–10054. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5459-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbregts SCJ, de Sonneville LMJ, Licht R, van Spronsen FJ, Verkerk PH, Sergeant JA. Sustained attention and inhibition of cognitive interference in treated phenylketonuria: associations with concurrent and lifetime phenylalanine concentrations. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(1):7–15. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00078-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbregts SCJ, Warren AJ, de Sonneville LMJ, Swaab-Barneveld H. Hot and cool forms of inhibitory control and externalizing behavior in children of mothers who smoked during pregnancy: an exploratory study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(3):323–333. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9180-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbregts S, Jahja R, De Sonneville L, de Breij S, Swaab-Barneveld H. Social information processing in children and adolescents with neurofibromatosis type 1. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2010;52(7):620–625. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03639.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbregts S, Swaab H, de Sonneville L. Cognitive and motor control in neurofibromatosis type I: influence of maturation and hyperactivity-inattention. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2010;35(6):737–751. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.508670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbregts SC, Loitfelder M, Rombouts SA, Swaab H, Verbist BM, Arkink EB, et al. Cerebral volumetric abnormalities in Neurofibromatosis type 1: associations with parent ratings of social and attention problems, executive dysfunction, and autistic mannerisms. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2015;7:32. doi: 10.1186/s11689-015-9128-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SL, Shores A, North KN. The nature and frequency of cognitive deficits in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Neurology. 2005;65(7):1037–1044. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000179303.72345.ce. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SL, Gill DS, Shores EA, Steinberg A, North KN. T2 hyperintensities in children with neurofibromatosis type 1 and their relationship to cognitive functioning. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2007;78(10):1088–1091. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.108134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsgodt KH, Rosser T, Lutkenhoff ES, Cannon TD, Silva A, Bearden CE. Alterations in white matter microstructure in neurofibromatosis-1. PloS One. 2012;7(10):e47854. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loitfelder M, Huijbregts SCJ, Veer IM, Swaab HS, Van Buchem MA, Schmidt R, Rombouts SA. Functional connectivity changes and executive and social problems in Neurofibromatosis type I. Brain Connectivity. 2015;5(5):312–320. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mamah D, Conturo TE, Harms MP, Akbudak E, Wang L, McMichael AR, et al. Anterior thalamic radiation integrity in schizophrenia: a diffusion-tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Research. 2010;183(2):144–150. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesulam MM. From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998;121:1013–1052. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore BD, Slopis JM, Schomer D, Jackson EF, Levy BM. Neuropsychological significance of areas of high signal intensity on brain MRIs of children with neurofibromatosis. Neurology. 1996;46(6):1660–1668. doi: 10.1212/WNL.46.6.1660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori, S., Wakana, S., Nagac-Poetscher, L., & van, PCZ. (2005). MRI Atlas of Human White Matter. (Elsevier, Ed.) NeuroImage, Amstedam.
- National Institutes of Health Consensus development conference statement. Neurofibromatosis. Archives of Neurology. 1988;45(5):575–578. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1988.00520290115023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestor PG, Kubicki M, Gurrera RJ, Niznikiewicz M, Frumin M, McCarley RW, Shenton ME. Neuropsychological correlates of diffusion tensor imaging in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(4):629–637. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.4.629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne JM, Pickering T, Porter M, Oates EC, Walia N, Prelog K, North KN. Longitudinal assessment of cognition and T2-hyperintensities in NF1: an 18-year study. American journal of medical genetics. Part A. 2014;164A(3):661–665. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.36338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Iglesias R, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, McGuire PK, Barker GJ, Roiz-Santiañez R, Mata I, et al. White matter integrity and cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;167(4):451–458. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Pandya DN. Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human and the macaque ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and corticocortical connection patterns in the monkey. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;16(2):291–310. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2001.02090.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pride NA, Korgaonkar MS, Barton B, Payne JM, Vucic S, North KN. The genetic and neuroanatomical basis of social dysfunction: lessons from neurofibromatosis type 1. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35(5):2372–2382. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowbotham I, Pit-ten Cate IM, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Huijbregts SCJ. Cognitive control in adolescents with neurofibromatosis type 1. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(1):50–60. doi: 10.1037/a0013927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubia K. “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: a review. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):e69–e87. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD, Smith EE, Eichler FS, Filley CM. Cerebral white matter: neuroanatomy, clinical neurology, and neurobehavioral correlates. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1142:266–309. doi: 10.1196/annals.1444.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shilyansky C, Lee YS, Silva AJ. Molecular and cellular mechanisms of learning disabilities: a focus on NF1. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2010;33:221–243. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23(Suppl 1):S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Mackay CE, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. NeuroImage. 2006;31(4):1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tidyman WE, Rauen KA. The RASopathies: developmental syndromes of Ras/MAPK pathway dysregulation. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development. 2009;19(3):230–236. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2009.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomson SN, Schreiner MJ, Narayan M, Rosser T, Enrique N, Silva AJ, et al. Resting state functional MRI reveals abnormal network connectivity in neurofibromatosis 1. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36(11):4566–4581. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Engelen SJPM, Krab LC, Moll HA, de Goede-Bolder A, Pluijm SMF, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, et al. Quantitative differentiation between healthy and disordered brain matter in patients with neurofibromatosis type I using diffusion tensor imaging. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2008;29(4):816–822. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A0921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Violante IR, Ribeiro MJ, Silva ED, Castelo-Branco M. Gyrification, cortical and subcortical morphometry in neurofibromatosis type 1: an uneven profile of developmental abnormalities. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2013;5(1):3. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-5-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D, Petermann F, Petermann U. Wechsler intelligence scale for children - fourth edition. 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Yu HJ, Christodoulou C, Bhise V, Greenblatt D, Patel Y, Serafin D, et al. Multiple white matter tract abnormalities underlie cognitive impairment in RRMS. NeuroImage. 2012;59(4):3713–3722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]