Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Assess. 2017 May 29;30(4):450–458. doi: 10.1037/pas0000479

Patient-Reported Outcome Measures in Huntington Disease: Neuro-QoL Social Functioning Measures

Noelle E Carlozzi 1, Elizabeth A Hahn 2,3, Siera M Goodnight 1, Anna L Kratz 1, Jane S Paulsen 4,5,6, Julie C Stout 7, Samuel Frank 8, Jennifer A Miner 1, David Cella 2,3, Richard C Gershon 2,3,9, Stephen G Schilling 10, Rebecca E Ready 11
PMCID: PMC5708162  NIHMSID: NIHMS867237  PMID: 28557480

Abstract

Social functioning is an essential but poorly understood component of health-related quality of life for people with Huntington disease. We report on the psychometric properties of two Neuro-QoL patient-reported outcome measures to assess social functioning in Huntington disease. Persons with prodromal (n=198) or manifest Huntington disease (n=195 early and n=117 late) completed Neuro-QoL Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities, and Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities. Items from two generic health-related quality of life patient-reported outcome measures were used to create a social functioning composite score; items from the Unified Huntington's Disease Rating Scale and Problem Behaviors Assessment Scale were used to create a clinician-rated composite score of social function. Internal consistencies for the scores on the Neuro-QoL measures were excellent (> .88). Computer adaptive test administration had some advantages over computer-administered static Short Forms. Validity was supported by significant associations between the scores on the Neuro-QoL measures and other self- and clinician-reports of social function. Individuals with prodromal HD had better social functioning than the manifest HD groups; individuals with late-HD had less satisfaction and ability to participate in social roles and activities than the other two groups. Neuro-QoL provides brief, reliable scores of social functioning that measure ability to participate in, and satisfaction with, social roles and activities in persons with prodromal and manifest HD. In addition, test score interpretations of these measures support their validity in people with prodromal and manifest HD. These measurement tools add breadth to treatment outcome measures in HD and can increase understanding of the social implications of living with HD.

Keywords: Neuro-QoL, social participation, community integration, health-related quality of life, HDQLIFE, Huntington disease, prodromal, patient reported outcome (PRO)


Social activity and engagement have long been recognized as integral components of health (World Health Organization, 1946), but have received limited attention in clinical practice and research (Hahn, Cella, Bode, & Hanrahan, 2010; Hahn, Dewalt, Bode, Garcia, Devellis, Correia et al., 2014). The World Health Organization's International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health has helped highlight social participation as both a determinant of health (Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000; Kamiya, Whelan, Timonen, & Kenny, 2010; Sundquist, Lindström, Malmström, Johansson, & Sundquist, 2004) and as a treatment outcome valued by patients, providers, and policy makers (Magasi, Hammel, Heinemann, Whiteneck, & Bogner, 2009). Given these factors, measures of social health will likely play a key role in clinical and research initiatives that emphasize how social activity and engagement influence health (Institute of Medicine, 2003, 2011; Whitehead, 1995).

Huntington disease (HD) is an autosomal-dominant, neurodegenerative condition which affects aspects of motor, cognitive and neuropsychiatric function essential for full participation in the activities of life. The neurodegenerative changes in HD affect physical, emotional, cognitive and behavioral functions that are important for interpersonal relationships, activities of daily living, work performance, and family responsibilities (Aubeeluck & Buchanan, 2006; Carlozzi & Tulsky, 2013; Coulson, Buchanan, & Aubeeluck, 2007; Hans & Koeppen, 1980; Kessler, 1993; Read, Jones, Owen, Leavitt, Coleman, Roos et al., 2013; Rothlind, Bylsma, Peyser, Folstein, & Brandt, 1993; Tyler, Harper, Davies, & Newcome, 1983; Vamos, Hambridge, Edwards, & Conaghan, 2007; Williams, Hamilton, Nehl, McGonigal-Kenney, Schutte, Sparbel et al., 2007). Therefore, social functioning is an important component of health-related quality of life (HRQOL) for people with HD. To date, very little research has focused on social functioning in HD. Furthermore, the field lacks patient-reported outcome (PRO) measures for assessing social functioning in HD. Scores on social functioning measures have potential importance as outcomes for clinical trials, yet no known study has provided validity data to support the clinical utility of HRQOL measures for social functioning either in treatment trials in people diagnosed with HD, nor in preventative trials in people in the premanifest stages of HD.

To this end, the Quality of Life in Neurological Disorders (Neuro-QoL) PRO measurement system was developed to assess HRQOL in neurological conditions (Cella, Lai, Nowinski, Victorson, Peterman, Miller, Bethoux, Heinemann, Rubin, & Cavazos, 2012; Cella, Nowinski, Peterman, Victorson, Miller, Lai et al., 2011). Included within this system are two measures of social functioning: Neuro-QoL Participation in Social Roles and Activities and Neuro-QoL Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities. While there are data to support the validity of the scores on these measures in Parkinson's disease (Nowinski, Siderowf, Simuni, Wortman, Moy, & Cella, 2016), stroke (Sangha, Caprio, Askew, Corado, Bernstein, Curran et al., 2015), multiple sclerosis (Miller, Bethoux, Victorson, Nowinski, Buono, Lai et al., 2016), and adult epilepsy (Victorson, Cavazos, Holmes, Reder, Wojna, Nowinski et al., 2014), validity data have not yet been examined in HD.

This report provides data that support the reliability of the Neuro-QoL social functioning scores, as well as data that support the validity of test score interpretations in a large cohort of people with prodromal and clinically diagnosed HD (Carlozzi, Schilling, Lai, Paulsen, Hahn, Perlmutter et al., 2016). Specifically, we examined internal consistency, floor and ceiling effects, convergent and discriminant validity, and known-groups validity of the scores on the Neuro-QoL social function measures.

Method

Participants

We examined individuals with prodromal HD (a positive test for the HD CAG gene mutation ≥ 36, without an HD clinical diagnosis) and those with a clinical diagnosis of HD. For people with HD, the Total Functional Capacity (Shoulson & Fahn, 1979), as determined by clinician-rated dministration, classified participants as either early stage (sum scores of 7 to 13) or later-stage HD (sum scores of 0 to 6, described below). Participants were at least 18 years of age, were able to read and understand English, and had the ability to provide informed consent. Participants were recruited through eight established HD Clinics, HD specialized nursing home units and support groups, the National Research Roster for Huntington's Disease, online medical record data capture systems (Hanauer, Mei, Law, Khanna, & Zheng, 2015), and articles/advertisements in HD-specific newsletters and websites. Participants were also recruited in conjunction with Predict-HD, a longitudinal observational research study in HD (Paulsen, Hayden, Stout, Langbehn, Aylward, Ross et al., 2006; Paulsen, Langbehn, Stout, Aylward, Ross, Nance et al., 2008; Paulsen, Long, Johnson, Aylward, Ross, Williams et al., 2014). All research was conducted in accordance with local institutional review boards; participants provided informed consent prior to their participation in this study and received $40 compensation for their participation.

PRO Measures

Neuro-QoL Social Functioning Measures

The Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities item bank (45 items) and the Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities item bank (45 items) from the Neuro-QoL measurement system (Cella et al., 2011) were used in this study. Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities provided a measure of self-reported involvement in one's usual social roles and activities, whereas Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities provided self-reported satisfaction with these same roles and activities. The computer-based static Short Form (Cella, Lai, Nowinski, Victorson, Peterman, Miller, Bethoux, Heinemann, Rubin, & Cavazos, 2012) versions of these measures (comprised of 8 items each), as well as the computer adaptive test (CAT) versions (Cella et al., 2011); assessments were administered through www.assessmentcenter.net. CAT administration is a test format where each item that is selected for administration is based on the participant's response to the previous item. Participants completed a minimum of 4 items in each CAT, and test administration stopped after either a standard error (SE) ≤ 0.3 was achieved or the participant answered 12 items. Item response theory based scores for all four of the measures were standardized using a ⊤ metric (M = 50, SD = 10); higher scores indicate better self-reported social health.

Generic PRO HRQOL Comparison Measures

Three generic measures of HRQOL were administered to compare psychometric properties with the Neuro-QoL scales. The WHODAS 2.0 (Ustun, Chatterji, Kostanjsek, Rehm, Kennedy, Epping-Jordan et al., 2010) is a generic self-report measure of HRQOL that includes 12 items examining six subdomains of HRQOL: understanding and communication, self-care, mobility, interpersonal relations, work and household roles, and community and civic roles. Total scores range from 0 (highest level of health) to 48 (low health). The RAND-12 (Hays, Sherbourn, & Mazel, 1995) is also a generic self-report measure of HRQOL; it includes 12 items that examine physical and mental health. The RAND-12 can be used to generate two composite scores: Physical Health (PHC) and Mental Health (MHC). Both composite scores range from 0 (low health) to 100 (highest level of health). The EQ5D (Rabin & de Charro, 2001) is a 6-item self-report measure of generic health status. This measure can be used to generate a Health Scale score (which ranges from 0 [low health] to 100 [highest level of health]) and an Index Value score (which ranges from 0 [low health] to 1 [highest level of health]).

Self-Reported Social Functioning Composite Score

The generic HRQOL measures described above provide an appropriate general comparator for the Neuro-QoL Social Functioning measures, however, compared to the Neuro-Qol, they are broad by design (i.e., they are multi-dimensional). These generic measures are designed to assess physical, mental, and social well-being. The Neuro-QoL measures, in contrast, has several measures, each designed to assess only a single aspect of functioning (i.e., unidimensional), in this case social participation or social ability. Thus, to create a more “pure” generic comparator measure of social function (i.e., a measure that reflected more similar content), we selected items from the generic HRQOL measures to create a composite measure of self-reported social functioning. Items were selected that included face-valid content about social activities and interpersonal relationships. Selected items included 3 items (pertaining to the last 30 days) from the WHODAS (How much of a problem did you have joining in community activities?; How much difficulty did you have in dealing with people you do not know?; and How much difficulty did you have in maintaining a friendship?) and one item from the RAND-12 (During the past 4 weeks, how much of the time has your physical health or emotional problems interfered with your social activities [like visiting friends, relatives, etc.]?). In order to create the composite score, the raw scores for each of the four items (scores were reversed where appropriate so that higher scores indicate better functioning) were transformed to z-scores (this was a linear transformation calculated using the sample SD for each item; resulting z-scores had a M = 0 and the SD = 1). These four z-scores were then averaged and transformed to a ⊤ score metric (M = 50, SD = 10); higher scores indicated better functioning.

Clinician-Rated Assessments of Functioning

The Unified Huntington's Disease Rating Scales (UHDRS; Huntington Study Group, 1996)

The UHDRS is a standardized clinical rating scale that assesses motor, cognitive, behavioral, and functional abilities. We examined four of these measures: Total Functional Capacity (TFC), Total Motor Score (TMS), Independence Scale, and Functional Assessment. The TFC is a 5-item measure that provides an index of day-to-day functioning across the domains of occupation, finances, domestic chores, activities of daily living, and care level; scores range from 0 to 13 (higher scores indicate better functioning). This measure is the most common method for staging HD. The TMS provides ratings of oculomotor function, dysarthria, chorea, dystonia, gait, and postural stability; higher scores indicate greater motor dysfunction. The Independence Scale is rated from 0 to 100; higher scores reflect better functioning/greater independence. The Functional Assessment Scale includes a yes/no checklist of 25 common daily tasks related to occupation, finances, activities of daily living, domestic chores, level of care and physical abilities; scores range from 0 to 25 (higher scores indicate better functioning).

The Problem Behaviors Assessment Scale (PBA-s; Craufurd, Thompson, & Snowden, 2001)

The PBA-s is a clinician administered assessment of behavior. For the purposes of this study, we examined clinician-rated severity for Apathy. The Apathy Severity score was reversed and recoded as follows (8 = 0, 4 = 1, 3 = 2, 2 = 3, 1 = 4, 0 = 5); higher scores indicate less apathy.

Clinician-Rated Composite Score of Social Functioning

As with the self-report generic measures of HRQOL, we wanted to create a more “pure” clinician-rated comparator measure of social function (i.e., a measure that reflected more similar content), and thus, social functioning items from the clinician-rated assessments were used to create a clinician-rated composite score of social function. This clinician-rated composite included the UHDRS Independence Scale and the Apathy Severity score from the PBAs. In order to create the composite score, individual item scores were reversed (where appropriate) and transformed to z-scores (this was a linear transformation calculated using the sample SD for each item; resulting z-scores had M = 0 and SD = 1). These two z scores were then averaged, and transformed to a ⊤ score (M = 50, SD = 10); higher scores indicated better functioning.

Statistical analysis plan

Internal Consistency Reliability

Cronbach's alphas were calculated for the Neuro-QOL social function static form scores (minimal acceptable reliability was specified as ≥ 0.70; Cohen, 1988; DeVellis, 2017).

Floor and Ceiling Effects

Floor and ceiling effects were calculated as the proportion of participants with the lowest or the highest possible scores for the scores on the Neuro-QOL measures (minimal acceptable rates ≤ 20%; Andresen, 2000; Cramer & Howitt, 2004).

Timing Data

Median and SD timing data were examined for both CAT and Short Form versions of the Neuro-QoL measures.

Convergent and Discriminant Validity

A multi-trait multi-method correlation matrix was used to examine interrelationships among the NeuroQoL scores, the clinician-rated composite score, and the self-report composite score. Reliability coefficients were calculated to estimate the reliability of each measure in the matrix (minimal acceptable reliability ≥ 0.70; Cohen, 1988; DeVellis, 2017). Correlations between scores from measures of the same trait should be strongly correlated (i.e., social participation measures with one another); this provides evidence that supports convergent validity for test score interpretation. Correlations between scores of differing traits (i.e., social participation vs. general HRQOL) were examined for interpretation of discriminant validity. In addition, correlations between scores from different methods (self-report with clinician report) should be less strongly correlated due both to method variance and because cognitive problems and anosognosia are common in HD; this also provides evidence for test score interpretation of discriminant validity. Correlations < 0.3 were considered poor, 0.3 to 0.6 adequate, and ≥ 0.6 good/very good evidence for test score interpretation of convergent validity (Campbell & Fiske, 1959).

Known-Groups Analyses

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) methods were used to examine group differences (i.e., prodromal, early- or late-HD) on the Neuro-QoL scores. We hypothesized that prodromal participants would report better functioning than both manifest HD groups, and that early-HD participants would report better functioning that late-HD participants.

Impairment Rates

We examined clinical impairment rates (participants whose scores were > 1 SD worse than the Neuro-QoL normative sample mean [n = 549; M = 50, SD = 10]; Cella, Lai, Nowinski, Victorson, Peterman, Miller, Bethoux, Heinemann, Rubin, Cavazos et al., 2012; Gershon, Lai, Bode, Choi, Moy, Bleck et al., 2012) to determine if individuals with HD were at greater risk than the general population for social impairments. According to the normal curve, 16% of the scores are expected to fall 1 SD below the mean (i.e., impaired); therefore, impairment rates that exceeded 16% indicate greater impairment than would be expected compared to demographically-comparable neurologically healthy peers (Heaton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004).

Effect sizes

Cohen's d was calculated to evaluate the relative influence overall functional severity had on Neuro-QOL social functioning scores (as determined by a median split using the matched “gold standard measure” – the UHDRS Independence Scale). Effect size calculations were computed for each measure based on comparison of each group relative to the means and standard deviations from the Neuro-QoL normative sample (again, n = 549; M = 50, SD = 10; Cella, Lai, Nowinski, Victorson, Peterman, Miller, Bethoux, Heinemann, Rubin, Cavazos, et al., 2012; Gershon et al., 2012). Effect sizes should be larger for the groups with more clinician-rated social functioning impairments.

Demographic Effects

We examined the relationships among age, gender, education and social function using Pearson correlations (for continuous variables) and point biserial correlations (for categorical variables).

Results

We enrolled 510 individuals with prodromal (n = 198) or manifest HD (n = 195 early HD and n = 117 late HD; Table 1). Groups did not differ on gender, χ2 (2, N 510) = 3.53, p = .17. There were small differences for education, F (2, 507) = 15.76, p < 0.0001; the early-HD and late-HD groups had 1 to 1.5 fewer years of education than the prodromal HD group. Groups differed on age, F (2, 507) = 46.47, p < .0001; the prodromal group (M = 42.74, SD = 12.02), was significantly younger than both manifest groups, and the early-HD group (M = 51.98, SD = 12.38) was significantly younger than the late-HD group (M = 55.07, SD = 11.89). Since disease progresses with age, this difference was anticipated.

Table 1. Demographic Information for Huntington disease participants.

Variable Prodromal
(n = 198)
Early
(n = 195)
Late
(n = 117)
All
(N = 510)
Age (Years)
M (SD) 42.74 (12.17) 51.98 (12.38) 55.07 (11.89) 49.10 (13.23)
Gender(%)
Female 63.6 54.4 59.8 59.2
Male 36.4 45.6 40.2 40.8
Race (%)
Caucasian 97.5 96.4 93.2 96.1
Other 2.0 3.6 6.8 3.7
Unknown 0.5 0.0 0.0 0.2
Ethnicity (%)
Not Hispanic or Latino 92.4 92.8 97.4 93.7
Hispanic or Latino 1.5 4.1 0.9 2.4
Not Provided 6.1 3.1 1.7 3.9
Education (# of years)
M (SD) 15.91 (2.94) 14.71 (2.78) 14.22 (2.62) 15.06 (2.89)
Marital Status (%)
Married 67.2 52.8 61.5 60.4

Internal Consistency Reliability

Cronbach's alphas exceeded the minimum standard we established for minimal acceptable reliability for scores on both Neuro-QoL Social Functioning measures and was generally equal to or greater than scores for the general HRQOL scales (Table 2).

Table 2. Descriptive Information and Reliability Data for Study Measures.

Measures n Cronbach's α % of the sample with floor effects (high functioning) % of the sample with ceiling effects (low functioning) M (SD) Administration Time in seconds Mdn (SD)
Neuro-QoL
Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities CAT* 483 -- 19.3 0.0 48.94 (8.49) 37.0(54.97)
Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities SF* 482 0.88 15.8 0.4 48.16 (6.69) 54.0 (40.15)
Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities CAT* 485 -- 11.5 0.0 48.01 (8.63) 46.5 (79.91)
Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities SF* 481 0.94 29.9 0.4 48.87 (8.69) 45.0 (37.83)

COMPARATOR MEASURES
Generic Self-Report Measures
EQ5D Index Score*° 508 0.73 22.8 0.2 0.81 (0.15) -
EQ5D Health Scale*° 508 - 8.9 0.6 79.49 (16.70) -
Rand 12 PHS* 510 0.84 0.0 0.0 52.68 (7.63) 45.0 (67.84)
Rand 12 MHS* 510 0.84 0.0 0.0 53.93 (9.76) 45.0 (67.84)
WHODAS 476 0.94 19.5 0.0 21.13 (9.96) 70.0 (59.88)
Clinician-Rated Measures
UHDRS Motor 496 0.97 9.9 0.2 26.14 (23.56) -
UHDRS Functional Assessment 338 0.95 22.8 0.3 19.03 (6.38) -
UHDRS TFC 508 0.72 11.6 1.0 8.94 (3.30) -

COMPOSITE MEASURES
Social Composite - Self* 479 0.81 0.0 0.0 50.07 (7.98) 36.0 (22.83)
Social Composite - Clinician* 464 0.46 28.9 0.0 50.17 (7.98) -

Note. SRA = Social Roles and Activities; CAT = Computer Adaptive Test; SF = Short Form; PHC = Physical health Composite; MHC = Mental Health Composite; UHDRS = Unified Huntington's Disease Rating Scales; TFC = Total Functional Capacity

*

higher scores = better functioning

°

the paper form of the EQ5D was used for administration and timing data was not recorded

Floor and Ceiling Effects

Floor and ceiling effects were within acceptable limits for scores on all Neuro-QoL measures except for the Neuro-QoL Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities Short Form.

Timing Data

Median administration times for the Neuro-QoL measures were less than one minute (regardless of administration format; Table 3). On average CAT administration utilized fewer than 6 items (Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities M = 5.45 items, SD = 2.84; Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities M = 5.74 items, SD = 3.21).

Table 3. Multi-Trait Multi-Method Correlation Matrix.

Satisfaction with SRA CAT Ability to Participate with SRA CAT Social Composite – Clinician Social Composite – Self




r r r r
Neuro-QoL Satisfaction with SRA CAT 0.92°
Ability to Participate with SRA CAT 0.67 0.95°
Composite Social Composite - Clinician 0.50 0.45 0.46
Social Composite - Self 0.64 0.58 0.51 0.81

Satisfaction with SRA SF Ability to Participate with SRA SF Social Composite – Clinician Social Composite – Self




r r r r

Neuro-QoL Satisfaction with SRA SF 0.88
Ability to Participate with SRA SF 0.71 0.94
Composite Social Composite – Clinician 0.46 0.53 0.46
Social Composite - Self 0.65 0.68 0.51 0.81

Note. SRA = Social Roles and Activities; CAT = computer adaptive test; SF = Short Form; provided correlations are attenuated; numbers in italics reflect reliability coefficients;

°

= CAT scores for the HD sample were simulated, and thus reliability coefficients could not be calculated, the provided reliability coefficients are based on the Neuro-QoL calibration sample (Gershon et al., 2012); numbers in bold reflect validity coefficients; all p < 0.01 level (2-tailed)

Convergent Validity

Reliability coefficients (in italics) for the Neuro-QoL Short Forms were good to excellent (.88 for Satisfaction and .94 for Ability); the reliability coefficient for the self-report composite score was good (r = .81), whereas the reliability coefficient for the clinician rated composite score was sub-optimal (r = .46; Table 3). Validity coefficients (in bold) were adequate between the scores on the Neuro-QoL measures and the scores on the composite self-report measure, and good between the scores on the NeuroQoL measures and the scores on the clinician–rated composites.

Known-Groups Analyses

There were significant group differences on scores on the Neuro-QoL Social Functioning measures among all three HD groups for both Neuro-QoL measures (Table 4); Pillai's Trace =.14 F (4, 960) = 17.55, p < .0001, partial η2 = .07 for the CATs and Pillai's Trace = .17 F (4, 950) = 21.65, p < .0001, partial η2 = .08 for the Short Forms. For the CATs, prodromal HD participants indicated better social functioning (i.e., greater ability to participate and more satisfaction with social roles and activities) than both manifest HD groups, and individuals with early stage HD reported better social functioning than the late stage HD group on both measures. For the Short Forms, prodromal HD participants indicated better social functioning (i.e., greater ability to participate and more satisfaction with social roles and activities) than both manifest HD groups and individuals with early stage HD reported better social functioning ability than the late stage HD group; there were no group differences between individuals with early and late-stage HD on the Satisfaction Short Form.

Table 4. Known Groups Validity for Neuro-QoL Social Functioning Computer Adaptive Tests.

Prodroma HD (CAT and SF n = 195) Impaired Early HD (CAT n = 186 and SF n = 185) Impaired Late HD (CAT n = 102 and SF n = 98) Impaired Overall Impaired F p Partial η2







Mean SD % Mean SD % Mean SD % %
Computer Adaptive Tests
Ability to Participate with SRAa,b,c 51.27 8.03 7.2 47.33 8.11 16.0 43.30 8.03 39.8 17.5 34.07 <0.0001 0.12
Satisfaction with SRAa,b,c 51.67 8.13 5.6 48.49 7.95 11.8 44.52 8.16 38.2 14.9 26.72 <0.0001 0.10
Short Forms
Ability to Participate with SRAa,b,c 52.25 7.95 5.7 48.50 8.27 16.0 42.94 7.63 36.7 16.0 44.38 <0.0001 0.16
Satisfaction with SRAa,b 50.28 6.51 3.1 47.42 6.17 7.0 45.55 6.79 18.0 7.7 19.78 <0.0001 0.08

Note. HD = Huntington disease; CAT = computer adaptive test; SF = Short Form; SRA = Social Roles and Activities; there were significant differences among all three groups for all measures.

a

=prodromal and early,

b

=prodromal and late,

c

=early and late

Impairment rates

Overall impairment rates for our HD participants was comparable to the general population (general population 16%; Neuro-QoL Ability to Participate in Social Roles and Activities impairment rates = 17.5% for the CAT and 16.0% for the Short Form and Neuro-QoL Satisfaction with Social Roles and Activities impairment rates = 14.9% for the CAT and 7.7% for the Short Form). Examination by HD group indicated that impairment rates were within normal limits or exceeded normal rates for the prodromal and early-HD groups, but were elevated in the late-HD group for all measures except the Satisfaction Short Form (Table 3).

Effect sizes

Effect sizes are included in Table 5. As expected, effect sizes were larger for individuals with poorer social functioning.

Table 5. Cohen's d Effect Sizes for Neuro-QoL Social Functioning Computer Adaptive Tests.

NeurQOL Measure UHDRS Independence Scale


Low Functioning High Functioning
Ability to Participate with SRA CAT -0.61 0.04
Satisfaction with SRA CAT -0.52 0.15

Ability to Participate with SRA SF -0.52 0.15
Satisfaction with SRA SF -0.57 0.01

Note. CATs = computer adaptive tests; UHDRS = Unified Huntington's Disease Rating Scales; SRA = Social Roles and Activities.

Demographic Effects

Correlations between scores on social functioning measures and demographic variables were not significant. Specifically, scores on the Neuro-QoL Ability to Participate CAT and Short Form had weak relationships with age (r = -.09 for both), gender (r = -.05 and r = -. 10 for Ability and Satisfaction, respectively) and education (r = .09 and r = .14, for Ability and Satisfaction, respectively). Neuro-QoL Satisfaction CAT and Short Form scores had similar weak correlations with age (CAT r = -.12 and Short Form r = -.10), gender (CAT r = -.07 and short form r = -.05) and education (CAT r = .07 and Short Form r = .08).

Discussion

Findings suggest strong support that the Neuro-QoL measures of social functioning provide an excellent candidate for rigorously developed PROs of social functioning for HD; data indicate that scores on the Neuro-QoL measures of social functioning are reliable and test interpretation supports their validity.

Specifically, internal consistency was excellent (>.88 for both CAT and Short Form scores on the Neuro-QoL measures), and was generally higher than or equivalent to other scores from generic measures of HRQOL. Furthermore, the scores on CAT administrations comfortably exceeded established criterion for floor and ceiling effects (≤ 20%). Together, these findings indicate that these scores provide reliable measures of social function in HD.

Test score interpretations also supported the convergent validity of the Neuro-QoL social functioning measures. Specifically, scores on the Neuro-QoL measures demonstrated moderate relationships with scores on other self-report measures of social function and good relationships with clinician-rated scores of social function. In addition, test score interpretation also supported discriminant validity; correlations were lower for scores across less similar measures (i.e., self-report with clinician report). These findings are especially important because they provide support for a multimodal assessment approach (that includes both PRO and clinician-rated assessments), as well as suggesting that these generic social functioning measures are appropriate for use in individuals with HD.

Furthermore, consistent with the literature, we generally found that individuals with prodromal HD reported better social functioning than either manifest HD group, and individuals with late-HD reported the least satisfaction and ability to participate in social roles and activities of the three groups (Helder, Kaptein, van Kempen, van Houwelingen, & Roos, 2001; Read et al., 2013). The one exception was that the Satisfaction Short Form score was unable to differentiate between individuals with early-and late-stage HD, suggesting that the CAT administration may be better for evaluating social satisfaction than the Short Form administration of this measure. In addition, social functioning “impairment” rates for individuals with HD were similar to or better than rates in the general population, but elevated for individuals with late-HD, again supporting test interpretation of construct validity and confirming previous findings (Helder et al., 2001; Read et al., 2013). Scores on the Neuro-QoL measures were also not related to demographic variables, providing additional evidence for construct validity. Finally, effect sizes for scores from the Neuro-QoL measures were larger for participants with the most social functioning difficulties. Together, the interpretation of these findings strongly support the construct validity of scores from the Neuro-QoL social measures in HD.

We acknowledge some study limitations. First, although we included scores from other self-report and clinician-rated measures/composites of social function as comparators, the selected items were generally non-specific and heterogeneous. In addition, age and disease severity are usually confounded HD (as HD symptoms worsen over time), thus it is difficult to separate their relationships to other variables. Although this confound complicates interpretations, the absence of a relationship between scores on the Neuro-QoL measures and age somewhat mitigates these concerns. Furthermore, while findings may not be generalizable to those prodromal participants with a high school or less education (as education levels for our prodromal participants were higher than the manifest groups), this limitation is somewhat mitigated by the absence of a relationship between the Neuro-QoL scores and education. In addition, although the racial breakdown of our sample is similar to other HD samples (given that HD is a euro-ethnic disease; Pringsheim, Wiltshire, Day, Dykeman, Steeves, & Jette, 2012), these findings may not be generalizable to those individuals with HD that are not Caucasian. These limitations are consistent with previous research and do not mitigate the overall contribution of this study to the field.

The Neuro-QoL social function measures provide brief, reliable scores of the ability to participate in and gain satisfaction from social roles and activities in persons with prodromal and manifest HD. In addition, test score interpretations of these measures support their concurrent, discriminant, and known groups validity in these same individuals. Future studies will examine change over time to determine clinical utility for repeated assessments in clinical trials; such studies will include responsiveness to change over time and establish the minimally important difference for each of these HD groups. In addition, future work should also examine the relationship between these self-reported HRQOL scores and other respondents (e.g., spouses, caregivers, and non-HD comparisons). Ultimately, these PROs of social functioning can provide useful information about social functioning in HD across the full HD spectrum. These measures can add breadth to treatment outcome measure options in HD and can increase understanding of the social implications of living with HD.

Public Significance Statement.

This study is designed to provide support for the reliability and validity of the scores from the Neuro-QoL social functioning patient-reported outcomes measures in Huntington disease. Scales with good reliability and evidence of validity are needed for improving the ability to measure social outcomes in Huntington disease. Furthermore, this paper is designed to encourage the use patient reported outcome measures in clinical research and practice.

Acknowledgments

Work on this manuscript was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01NS077946) and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (UL1TR000433). In addition, a portion of this study sample was collected in conjunction with the Predict-HD study. The Predict-HD study was supported by the NIH, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01NS040068), the NIH, Center for Inherited Disease Research (provided supported for sample phenotyping), and the CHDI Foundation (award to the University of Iowa). We thank the University of Iowa, the Investigators and Coordinators of this study, the study participants, the National Research Roster for Huntington Disease Patients and Families, the Huntington Study Group, and the Huntington's Disease Society of America. We acknowledge the assistance of Jeffrey D. Long, Hans J. Johnson, Jeremy H. Bockholt, and Roland Zschiegner. We also acknowledge Roger Albin, Kelvin Chou, and Henry Paulsen for the assistance with participant recruitment. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

HDQLIFE Site Investigators and Coordinators: Noelle Carlozzi, Praveen Dayalu, Anna L. Kratz, Stephen Schilling, Amy Austin, Matthew Canter, Siera Goodnight, Jennifer Miner, Nicholas Migliore (University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI); Jane S Paulsen, Nancy Downing, Isabella DeSoriano, Courtney Hobart, Amanda Miller (University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA); Kimberly Quaid, Melissa Wesson (Indiana University, Indianapolis, IN); Christopher Ross, Gregory Churchill, Mary Jane Ong (Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD); Susan Perlman, Brian Clemente, Aaron Fisher, Gloria Obialisi, Michael Rosco (University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA); Michael McCormack, Humberto Marin, Allison Dicke (Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ); Joel Perlmutter, Stacey Barton, Shineeka Smith (Washington University, St. Louis, MO); Martha Nance, Pat Ede (Struthers Parkinson's Center); Stephen Rao, Anwar Ahmed, Michael Lengen, Lyla Mourany, Christine Reece, (Cleveland Clinic Foundation, Cleveland, OH); Michael Geschwind, Joseph Winer (University of California – San Francisco, San Francisco, CA), David Cella, Richard Gershon, Elizabeth Hahn, Jin-Shei Lai (Northwestern University, Chicago, IL).

Footnotes

Author Roles: Carlozzi, N.E.: Study Principal Investigator; Data Collection Site; Oversight for Statistical Analysis; Initial draft of Method, Results and Discussion; Incorporation of revisions

Hahn, E.A.: Study Co-Investigator; Assistance with writing the Introduction; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts

Goodnight, S.M. Study Research Coordinator; Primary Data Analyst Responsible for Majority of Statistical Analyses; Assistance with Methods and Results Sections; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts

Kratz, A.L. Study Co-Investigator; Assistance with Analytical Plan and Analysis Write-up; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts

Paulsen, J.S. Study Co-Investigator; Data Collection Site; Predict-HD Principal Investigator; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts (specific contributions to with regard to the shared Predict-HD data, analytic revisions, and relevance to HD literature)

Stout, J.C. Study Consultant; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts (specific contributions to with regard to the analytical revisions and relevance to HD literature)

Frank, S. Study Consultant; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts (specific contributions to Introduction and relevance to HD literature)

Miner, J.A. Study Project Manager; Draft of study protocol for Methods; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts

Cella, D. Study Co-Investigator; Neuro-QoL Investigative Team Principal Investigator; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts (specific contributions to framing the psychometric findings for the Neuro-QoL measures)

Gershon, R.C. Study Co-Investigator; Neuro-QoL Investigative Team Statistical Coordination Lead; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts (specific contributions to framing the psychometric findings for the Neuro-QoL measures)

Schilling, S.G. Co-Investigator; Assistance with Analytical Plan; Oversight of Statistical Analyses; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts

Ready, R.E. Study Consultant; Initial Draft of Abstract; Assistance Writing Discussion Including Integration of Findings with Existing HD Literature; Review and Feedback on manuscript drafts

Conflict of Interest: Carlozzi, N.E. currently has research grants from the NIH; she is also supported by grant funding from the NIH and CHDI.

Hahn, E.A. currently has research grants from the NIH; she is also supported by grant funding from the NIH and PCORI, and by research contracts from Merck and EMMES; she declares no conflicts of interest.

Goodnight, S.M. is supported by grant funding from the NIH and the Craig H. Neilsen Foundation; she declares no conflicts of interest.

Kratz, A.L. currently has research grants from the NIH and the Craig H. Neilsen Foundation; she is also supported by grant funding from the NMSS; she declares no conflicts of interest.

Paulsen, J.S. currently has research grants from the NIH; she is also supported by grant funding from NIH, NINDS, and CHDI; she declares no conflicts of interest.

Stout, J.C. has received research funding in the past three years from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council, University College London, the CHDI Foundation, Prana Biotechnology, and the University of California, Davis.

Frank, S. receives salary support from the Huntington Study Group for a study sponsored by Auspex Pharmaceuticals. There is no conflict of interest.

Miner, J.A. is supported by research grants from the NIH; she declares no conflict of interest.

Cella, D. receives grant funding from the National Institutes of Health and reports that he has no conflicts of interest.

Gershon, R.C. receives research funds from numerous NIH institutes and the Department of Defense.

Schilling, S.G. has a research grant from NSF. He also is supported by grant funding from NIH. He declares no conflicts of interest.

Ready, R.E. declares that she has no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Andresen EM. Criteria for assessing the tools of disability outcomes research. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation. 2000;81(12 Suppl 2):S15–20. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2000.20619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aubeeluck A, Buchanan H. Capturing the Huntington's disease spousal carer experience: A preliminary investigation using the ‘photovoice’ method. Dementia. 2006;5(1):95–116. [Google Scholar]
  3. Berkman LF, Glass T, Brissette I, Seeman TE. From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science and Medicine. 2000;51(6):843–857. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00065-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Campbell DT, Fiske DW. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait- multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin. 1959;56(2):81–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carlozzi NE, Schilling SG, Lai JS, Paulsen JS, Hahn EA, Perlmutter JS, Ross CA, Downing NR, Kratz AL, McCormack MK, Nance MA, Quaid KA, Stout JC, Gershon RC, Ready RE, Miner JA, Barton SK, Perlman SL, Rao SM, Frank S, Shoulson I, Marin H, Geschwind MD, Dayalu P, Goodnight SM, Cella D. HDQLIFE: Development and assessment of health-related quality of life in Huntington disease (HD) Qual Life Res. 2016;25(10):2441–2455. doi: 10.1007/s11136-016-1386-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carlozzi NE, Tulsky DS. Identification of health-related quality of life (HRQOL) issues relevant to individuals with Huntington disease. Journal of Health Psychology. 2013;18(2):212–225. doi: 10.1177/1359105312438109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cella D, Lai JS, Nowinski C, Victorson D, Peterman A, Miller D, Bethoux F, Heinemann A, Rubin S, Cavazos J. Neuro-QOL brief measures of health- related quality of life for clinical research in neurology. Neurology. 2012;78(23):1860–1867. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318258f744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cella D, Lai JS, Nowinski CJ, Victorson D, Peterman A, Miller D, Bethoux F, Heinemann A, Rubin S, Cavazos JE, Reder AT, Sufit R, Simuni T, Holmes GL, Siderowf A, Wojna V, Bode R, McKinney N, Podrabsky T, Wortman K, Choi S, Gershon R, Rothrock N, Moy C. Neuro-QOL: brief measures of health-related quality of life for clinical research in neurology. Neurology. 2012;78(23):1860–1867. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318258f744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cella D, Nowinski C, Peterman A, Victorson D, Miller D, Lai JS, Moy C. The Neurology Quality of Life Measurement (Neuro-QOL) Initiative. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2011;92(Suppl 1):S28–S36. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.01.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd. New York: Academic Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  11. Coulson NS, Buchanan H, Aubeeluck A. Social support in cyberspace: A content analysis of communication within a Huntington's disease online support group. Patient Education & Counseling. 2007;68(2):173–178. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2007.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cramer D, Howitt DL. The Sage dictionary of statistics. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  13. Craufurd D, Thompson JC, Snowden JS. Behavioral changes in Huntington Disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2001;14(4):219–226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. DeVellis R. Scale development: Theory and applications. 4th. Los Angeles, CA: Sage; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gershon RC, Lai JS, Bode R, Choi S, Moy C, Bleck T, Miller D, Peterman A, Cella D. Neuro-QOL: quality of life item banks for adults with neurological disorders: item development and calibrations based upon clinical and general population testing. Qual Life Res. 2012;21(3):475–486. doi: 10.1007/s11136-011-9958-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hahn EA, Cella D, Bode RK, Hanrahan RT. Measuring social well-being in people with chronic illness. Social Indicators Research. 2010;96(3):381–401. [Google Scholar]
  17. Hahn EA, Dewalt DA, Bode RK, Garcia SF, Devellis RF, Correia H, Cella D. New English and Spanish social health measures will facilitate evaluating health determinants. Health Psychology. 2014;33(5):490–499. doi: 10.1037/hea0000055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hanauer DA, Mei Q, Law J, Khanna R, Zheng K. Supporting information retrieval from electronic health records: A report of University of Michigan's nine-year experience in developing and using the Electronic Medical Record Search Engine (EMERSE) Journal of Biomedical Informatics. 2015;55:290–300. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2015.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hans MB, Koeppen AH. Huntington's chorea: Its impact on the spouse. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1980;168(4):209–214. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hays RD, Sherbourn CD, Mazel R. User's manual for the Medical Outcomes Study (MOS) core measures of health-related quality of life. Santa Monica, CA: RAND corporation; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  21. Heaton RK, Miller SW, Taylor JT, Grant I. Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  22. Helder DI, Kaptein AA, van Kempen GM, van Houwelingen JC, Roos RA. Impact of Huntington's disease on quality of life. Movement Disorders. 2001;16(2):325–330. doi: 10.1002/mds.1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Huntington Study Group. Unified Huntington's Disease Rating Scale: Reliability and consistency. Movement Disorders. 1996;11(2):136–142. doi: 10.1002/mds.870110204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Institute of Medicine. The future of the public's health in the 21st century. Washington, D.C: National Academies Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  25. Institute of Medicine. For the public's health: The role of measurement in action and accountability. Washington, D.C: National Academies Press; 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kamiya Y, Whelan B, Timonen V, Kenny RA. The differential impact of subjective and objective aspects of social engagement on cardiovascular risk factors. BMC Geriatrics. 2010;10:81. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kessler S. Forgotten person in the Huntington disease family. American journal of medical genetics. 1993;48(3):145–150. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320480306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Magasi S, Hammel J, Heinemann AW, Whiteneck GG, Bogner J. Participation: A comparative analysis of multiple rehabilitation stakeholders' perspectives. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine. 2009;41(11):936–944. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Miller DM, Bethoux F, Victorson D, Nowinski CJ, Buono S, Lai JS, Wortman K, Burns JL, Moy C, Cella D. Validating Neuro-QoL short forms and targeted scales with people who have multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis. 2016;22(6):830–841. doi: 10.1177/1352458515599450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Nowinski CJ, Siderowf A, Simuni T, Wortman C, Moy C, Cella D. Neuro-QoL health-related quality of life measurement system: Validation in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders. 2016;31(5):725–733. doi: 10.1002/mds.26546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Paulsen JS, Hayden M, Stout JC, Langbehn DR, Aylward E, Ross CA, Guttman M, Nance M, Kieburtz K, Oakes D, Shoulson I, Kayson E, Johnson S, Penziner E. Preparing for preventive clinical trials: The Predict-HD study. Archives of Neurology. 2006;63(6):883–890. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.6.883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Paulsen JS, Langbehn DR, Stout JC, Aylward E, Ross CA, Nance M, Guttman M, Johnson S, MacDonald M, Beglinger LJ, Duff K, Kayson E, Biglan K, Shoulson I, Oakes D, Hayden M. Detection of Huntington's disease decades before diagnosis: the Predict-HD study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2008;79(8):874–880. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.128728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Paulsen JS, Long JD, Johnson HJ, Aylward EH, Ross CA, Williams JK, Nance MA, Erwin CJ, Westervelt HJ, Harrington DL, Bockholt HJ, Zhang Y, McCusker EA, Chiu EM, Panegyres PK. Clinical and biomarker changes in premanifest Huntington disease show trial feasibility: A decade of the PREDICT-HD study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2014;6:78. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Pringsheim T, Wiltshire K, Day L, Dykeman J, Steeves T, Jette N. The incidence and prevalence of Huntington's disease: A systematic review and meta- analysis. Movement Disorders. 2012;27(9):1083–1091. doi: 10.1002/mds.25075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rabin R, de Charro F. EQ-5D: A measure of health status from the EuroQol Group. Annals of Medicine. 2001;33(5):337–343. doi: 10.3109/07853890109002087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Read J, Jones R, Owen G, Leavitt BR, Coleman A, Roos RA, Dumas EM, Durr A, Justo D, Say M, Stout JC, Tabrizi SJ, Craufurd D, Track-HD Investigators. Quality of life in Huntington's disease: A comparative study investigating the impact for those with pre-manifest and early manifest disease, and their partners. Journal of Huntington's Disease. 2013;2(2):159–175. doi: 10.3233/JHD-130051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rothlind JC, Bylsma FW, Peyser C, Folstein SE, Brandt J. Cognitive and motor correlates of everyday functioning in early Huntington's disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1993 doi: 10.1097/00005053-199303000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sangha RS, Caprio FZ, Askew R, Corado C, Bernstein R, Curran Y, Ruff I, Cella D, Naidech AM, Prabhakaran S. Quality of life in patients with TIA and minor ischemic stroke. Neurology. 2015;85(22):1957–1963. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Shoulson I, Fahn S. Huntington disease - Clinical care and evaluation. Neurology. 1979;29(1):1–3. doi: 10.1212/wnl.29.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Sundquist K, Lindström M, Malmström M, Johansson SE, Sundquist J. Social participation and coronary heart disease: a follow-up study of 6900 women and men in Sweden. Social Science & Medicine. 2004;58(3):615–622. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(03)00229-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Tyler A, Harper PS, Davies K, Newcome RG. Family break-down and stress in Huntington's chorea. Journal of Biosocial Science. 1983;15(02):127–138. doi: 10.1017/s0021932000014413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ustun TB, Chatterji S, Kostanjsek N, Rehm J, Kennedy C, Epping-Jordan J, Saxena S, von Korff M, Pull C, Project WNJ. Developing the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2010;88(11):815–823. doi: 10.2471/BLT.09.067231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Vamos M, Hambridge J, Edwards M, Conaghan J. The impact of Huntington's disease on family life. Psychosomatics. 2007;48(5):400–404. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.48.5.400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Victorson D, Cavazos JE, Holmes GL, Reder AT, Wojna V, Nowinski C, Miller D, Buono S, Mueller A, Moy C, Cella D. Validity of the Neurology Quality-of- Life (Neuro-QoL) measurement system in adult epilepsy. Epilepsy Behavior. 2014;31:77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Whitehead M. Tackling inequalities: A review of policy initiatives. In: Benzeval M, Judge K, Whitehead M, editors. Tackling inequalities in health: An agenda for action. London: King's Fund; 1995. pp. 22–52. [Google Scholar]
  46. Williams JK, Hamilton R, Nehl C, McGonigal-Kenney M, Schutte DL, Sparbel K, Birrer E, Tripp-Reimer T, Friedrich R, Penziner E. No one else sees the difference:” Family members' perceptions of changes in persons with preclinical Huntington disease. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2007;144(5):636–641. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. World Health Organization. Constitution of the World Health Organization. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1946. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES