Abstract
Graph theoretic analyses applied to examine the brain at rest have played a critical role in clarifying the foundations of the brain’s intrinsic and task-related activity. There are many opportunities for clinical scientists to describe and predict dysfunction using a network perspective. This primer describes the theoretical basis and practical application of graph theoretic analysis to resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Major practices, concepts, and findings are concisely reviewed. The theoretical and practical frontiers are highlighted with observations about major avenues for opportunity in theoretical development and clinical translation.
Keywords: Resting state fMRI, graph theory, connectome, neuroimaging, networks, network analysis
Introduction
Given its complexity [1], the brain is perhaps the quintessential example of a network. Neural elements are interconnected by a large number of connections that can be studied across several orders of spatio-temporal magnitude. We are apt to seek a framework that can help us describe and understand brain organization, dynamics, and cognitive-behavioral phenomena. From the perspective of network science[2], the brain is a special case of a larger space of natural and possible networks [3]. The mathematical basis in which we can represent and study networks is graph theory, which provides fundamental mathematical knowledge and a generalizable basis in which to study networks.
In human brain networks, we can apply graph theoretic analysis to anatomical or functional networks at multiple scales to study the connectome [4]. An essential tool in our effort to understand functional brain networks is functional magnetic resonance imaging (BOLD fMRI), which allows us to indirectly study neural activity using the hemodynamic relationship between blood flow and neural firing in the brain [5]. BOLD fMRI is often used to examine contrasts between different cognitive conditions to examine changes in functional signal amplitudes and connectivity associated with behavior. In addition, and the focus of the current review, it can be used to understand the intrinsic [6] organization of functional brain networks to study the brain at rest [7] and its relationship with cognition and behavior.
To say that a living brain is ever at “rest” is incorrect; indeed, brains persistently bustle with complex neural and metabolic activity. “Resting state” fMRI (rsfMRI) is broadly used to refer to data acquired when subjects are instructed to look at a crosshair fixation or engage in no task in particular while in the scanner [7]. In contrast to studies that involve tightly controlled experimental paradigms, rsfMRI studies involve data that represent any number of cognitive-emotional mental activities that participants may engage in while at rest. Indeed, rsfMRI is used to investigate processes such as mind-wandering [8], introspection [9], and imaginative thought [10]. For these reasons, it has been suggested that less leading terminology such as “intrinsic connectivity” may be preferable in referring to “resting state” data [11]. Broadly, we can examine properties of network organization observable within rsfMRI to identify correlates of other cognitive variables and markers of dysfunction. While care should be applied when interpreting rsfMRI connectomes, we know that rsfMRI networks and network statistics are overall reliable enough to afford robust analysis of connectomic organization to examine major systems [12, 13]. Here, I review major approaches to and findings from graph theoretic analysis of rsfMRI data.
Data collection and preprocessing
Data collection for rsfMRI typically involves asking subjects to lie in the scanner with eyes open, closed, or fixated on a target in the center of participants’ visual field over several minutes of fMRI data acquisition [14] (see “Not all rsfMRI is equal”, below). “Preprocessing” techniques to ensure that data meet several assumptions prior to analysis. The most standard steps to preprocessing rsfMRI data include slice timing correction [15], motion correction ([16, 17]), realignment [18], coregistration of anatomical and functional images [19], spatial normalization [20], and smoothing [21]. Smoothing increases signal to noise, normalizing error distributions, and accomodates anatomical and functional variation between subjects. In addition, global signal regression (GSR), which refers to the statistical removal of the average signal across all voxels in the brain, is a contentious issue in rsfMRI analysis [22, 23]. On the one hand, GSR can increase the detection of localized neural signals and improve functional connectivity analysis specificity [24, 25]. On the other, GSR introduces negative correlations mean-centered around zero and may exclude important neural signatures [26, 22, 27, 28]. Finally, rsfMRI time series are often examined after applying a bandpass filter to BOLD data to reduce influences of nonphysiological and physiological nuisance signals (often between 0.01 Hz to 0.1 Hz or similar). Because no model can perfectly separate physiological nuisance variables from neurally-related signals, we must be cautious in our selection of GSR and bandpassing preprocessing techniques and appropriately discuss the limitations of each selection in empirical work. Readers are encouraged to consult primary sources for in depth empirical analysis and discussion of preprocessing issues.
Graph theoretic analysis of rsfMRI data
Once data are obtained and preprocessed, any number of techniques can be applied to examine the organization of rsfMRI data. The primary conceptual distinction between graph theoretic analysis of rsfMRI and other approaches is that the former directly links rsfMRI to much broader efforts with deep mathematical foundations in graph theory. This allows us to share concepts and language with investigators interested in other types of networks and encourages the potential for innovative crosstalk. In this section, I provide basic intuition for major ideas in graph theory analysis for rsfMRI data and findings from applied analysis.
Graphs in rsfMRI analysis
Graph theoretic analysis is a specific approach to analyzing brain networks in which the brain network is represented in the mathematical “graph”. A graph G is composed of N nodes (or vertices) and E edges (region-region relationships). The graph G can be encoded in an adjacency matrix, A, whose (i,j)th element represents the weight of the edge between node i and node j. The edges of a graph can be binary (including only 0’s and 1’s) or weighted (including a range of other values). Graphs can be undirected, where the association between regions is bidirectional, or directed, where the association between regions may vary across directions. In rsfMRI analysis, the elements of the adjacency matrix A most often include full correlation coefficients representing the strength of functional communication between two regions. The elements can alternatively include covariance, partial correlation, coherence, and mutualized information shared between pairs of brain regions.
To construct A, one must define the nodes between which edges are calculated. This is typically achieved by selecting a parcellation of rsfMRI data voxels into coarse-grained units called parcels, which reduces the number of nodes in the system for simplicity, statistical economy, and computational efficiency. Several notable parcellations are available defined by functionally [29, 30, 12] or anatomically validated features. The proliferation of and continued interest in developing parcellations highlights the fact that there is no “perfect” parcellation established to date. Indeed, each parcellation relies on statistical optimization or anatomically-based boundary definitions paired with our intuitions about what matters in brain organization. It is essential to note that the absolute value of statistics in rsfMRI network analysis vary depending on the parcellation [31]. Thus, caution should be used in inter-parcellation comparisons and we should be well aware of the nature of the parcellation we select and rationale for selecting it.
Once the adjacency matrix is defined, we can apply a rich range of concepts and tools from graph theory to examine brain networks (See Fig. 1). In the following sections, I introduce commonly applied network statistics across scales of network organization. The reader is also referred to other excellent reviews concerning graph theoretic analysis in general [32] and specific to brain networks [33, 34].
Micro, meso, and macroscale network analysis
One of the advantages of applying a graph theoretic perspective is that it gives the ability to study intermediate and high levels of organization across the network as a whole. This is a fundamental distinction between rsfMRI and other approaches to functional brain network analysis: by allowing us to examine higher level statistics, we can identify properties of networks that are not evident in any particular node or edge and potentially what cognitive-behavioral processes they may support. The scales of network analysis can coarsely be separated into micro, meso, and macroscale, representing the configuration of the elements of the network, their modular configuration, and the overall topology of the network as a whole, respectively.
Microscale
The micro scale refers to the organization of nodes and edges in the network. In rsfMRI data, each node statistic emphasizes complementary information about brain region roles in the functional connectome. One major notion in networks is that hubs serve central roles in network organization and information processing. To represent this, hub coefficients [35, 36] constructed from measures of node centrality have been used to quantify nodes’ connectedness in the network (degree or strength in weighted networks; see 2), involvement in short paths across the network (betweenness centrality), connectedness with local neighbors (clustering coefficient or local efficiency), connectedness to important nodes in the network (eigenvector centrality), and interactions with multiple communities in the functional network (participation coefficient). Each of these statistics emphasizes a distinct aspect of nodes’ varying roles in organizing information procecssing across the brain. In rsfMRI data, we can examine variation in node centrality across the brain and distinct groups to identify characteristic roles for nodes.
Perhaps the simplest measure of node centrality is degree. In binary networks, degree is defined as the number of edges connected to a node. In weighted networks, the weighted degree (or strength) is the sum of edge weights connected to a node. Nodes of high degree or strength centrality are thought to be particularly influential on the network’s function. In rsfMRI networks, nodes with high degree are referred to as network hubs, and are thought to be critical for general information transmission and circuit-level computing [37, 38].
Other centrality statistics capture a node’s role in network organization beyond node-to-node connections. Commonly used examples include betweenness centrality, closeness centrality, and eigenvector centrality. Betweenness centrality quantifies the extent to which a node participates in shortest paths throughout the network. A shortest path is the path between node i and node j that traverses the fewest (or in weighted networks the fewest high-weight) edges. Nodes with high betweenness centrality are thought to be particularly influential across different efficient pathways of the network as a whole rather than just local direct connections. Closeness centrality quantifies the average shortest path between a given node and all other nodes in the graph. As a result, closeness centrality is used as a measure of a node’s ability to communicate quite broadly to every node in the network. Eigenvector centrality uses the eigenspectrum of the adjacency matrix to quantify the influence of a node based on its connectedness with other high-scoring nodes in a network (See Fig. 3). This statistic recursively captures the importance of a node: nodes that are connected to important nodes will rank higher in eigenvector centrality. Because each of these statistics putatively identify highly central nodes, nodes that score high on these statistics are sometimes referred to as hubs given their theoretical role in network function [39].
Some node statistics are designed to capture the role of nearby neighbors in the neighborhood (one step in topological distance) around the node. The clustering coefficient, which describes how close its nearest neighbors are to being a completely connected subgraph or clique). One specific example is the local clustering coefficient, which can be defined as the number of triangles in the network containing a node, divided by the number of connected triples containing that same node [40]. With this definition, the local clustering coefficient gives the density of local connections involving a given node and is often used to probe the node’s ability to participate in local information integration. A complementary notion is that of node “efficiency”, which assesses the connectedness of the edges among neighbors of a given node, thus offering a notion of the network’s local robustness to a node’s removal [41].
In addition to quantifying features of a node and its role in the network, one might also be interested in quantifying features of an edge. Perhaps the simplest statistic for an edge is its weight, which provides information about the strength of the relationship between two nodes. Moving beyond pairwise information, one can also compute something like the edge betweenness centrality, which measures the number of shortest paths between all possible pairs of nodes that pass through the edge of interest. Similar to the betweenness centrality of a node, the betweenness centrality of an edge is thought to represent its importance in efficient information transfer in networks. These are just a couple of examples of useful statistics for edges that can help one to understand the role of a node-node relationship in the broader network.
Mesoscale
Mesoscale organization refers to the arrangement of nodes into modules or communities. Mesoscale structure is that which is not easily characterized at either the local (node and edge) or global (entire network) scales. Networks may exhibit varying degrees and qualities of modular structure – also known as community organization – which refers to high within- and low between- module connectivity. A network exhibits a varying degree of modularity that can be quantified as the extent to which nodes exhibit non-trivial community organization relative to network null models [42].
A contrasting mesoscale organization in networks is core-periphery structure, which refers to the tendency for a network to have a core of densely interconnected nodes surrounded by a periphery of nodes that connect to the core but not to one another. Put differently, an extreme of thiswould be a single large module with more sparsely connected individual nodes attached to the module. More modular networks theoretically maintain a balance of information integration within and segregation between modules, while core-periphery structure can offer relatively more centralized processing [43].
In natural networks, mesoscale networks can exhibit community organization in which individual modules have mixtures of characteristic modular, core-periphery, or assortative organization. An assortative network is one in which like-degree nodes tend to connect to one another, and a disassortative network is one in which unlike-degree nodes tend to connect to one another. In some networks, assortativity can be a marker of robustness: the removal of one high-degree node can be overcome by the interconnectedness of the others [44, 45]. Promising techniques such as stochastic block models [46] allow us to identify the organization of network communities along these organizational subtypes and describe their potential diversity of network function.
Macroscale
Another key notion in graph theory is that networks can be represented holistically, potentially described with single scalar values representing a property of the network. This is a form of emergent feature of a system that is not represented in any particular part. To represent this, global statistics are important to characterize overarching network organizational principles. Perhaps the simplest statistic is a network’s density: the number of existing edges relative to the number of possible edges. Many natural networks exhibit low density because edges are costly [47]. Relatedly, one often studied characteristic of a network is the shape of its degree (or strength) distribution. Long-tailed degree distributions indicate that unexpectedly large hubs exist. As within individual modules, it is often interesting to ask whether these hubs preferentially connect to one another across the entire network, forming a basis for macroscale assortativity analysis [44].
In addition, we can examine whether a network is scale-free, which means that a network has a degree distribution that follows a power law [48]. Scale-free networks can emerge from theoretical mechanisms including preferential attachment [49] and node fitness [50], or rules that copy a fraction of links to existent nodes [51]. Regardless of the mechanism of generation, scale-free networks are notable because they represent relatively economical architectures for information transmission [47]. They are also robust against random damage but especially vulnerable to targeted attack to high-degree nodes [52]. Many natural networks demonstrate approximately scale-free degree distributions [53] that are characterized by power laws over some regimes of their degree distribution [54].
Another way to describe macroscale organization is to average the values of nodal statistics. For example, a network’s global clustering coefficient is equal to the average clustering coefficient of that network’s nodes. High global clustering in a network indicates that nodes tend to be highly connected across all possible cliques larger than two nodes. The characteristic path length refers to the average shortest path between all pairs of nodes in the network. A short characteristic path length is thought to represent the potential for high integration across the network [55]. Global efficiency is a notion that is complementary to the characteristic path length, and can be calculated as the inverse of the harmonic mean of the shortest paths in the network [41, 56]. A network with a smaller characteristic path length has a higher global efficiency, indicating a theoretical ability to communicate quickly across the network as a whole [57].
A composite statistic representing a network’s tendency to exhibit higher than expected clustering and shorter than expected path lengths is famously known as small-worldness [55]. Small-world network organization theoretically supports a balance of local segregated information processing in modules in conjunction with long distance integrating processing across modules. Many small-world networks are also scale-free [58]. One problem with calling a network “small-world” is that all networks fall on a continuum of this property and it is not often robustly defined in network analysis [59]. Because small-worldness is a continuous property that is defined by the clustering and path length in networks, it is important to interpret the contribution of both of these statistics to the small-worldness of the system. A statistically rigorous definition of small-worldness for weighted networks is small world propensity, which has been validated but not yet widely applied in brain network analysis [59].
If one is interested in studying not just the shortest path but also longer paths or walks through the network, then one might examine a generalization of network communicability [60]. Communicability describes all shortest paths as well as all walks – steps along the network including revisitations to nodes and edges – connecting two nodes. This accounts for the possibility that communication can occur in a network involving both the shortest path as well as longer and indirect paths across the network. This property may be interesting as a complement to other measures associated with network resilience. For example, nodes and networks with increased communicability may exhibit more robustness to the loss of specific edges because multiple pathways for communication exist across the nodes.
One feature of global network organization is the minimum spanning tree (MST), which is a subset of edges in a connected, weighted, undirected graph that connects all nodes together without any cycles and the minimum possible total edge weight [61]. Thus, the MST of a graph represents the least total cost architecture associating all nodes to one another with no redundancy. The size and configuration of a graph’s MST can be analyzed to identify efficient organization in network topology and its temporal and cross-sectional variation.
In practical network analysis, it is important to note that raw measures of global network organization are heavily influenced by the network’s density (or total node strength), and it is therefore important to normalize or statistically control for graph density (strength) prior to making statistical inferences across networks or in relation to extrinsic variables such as clinical status or cognition in the context of rsfMRI analysis. This allows us to examine the unique contributions of network topology above and beyond simple network density.
Major findings in rsfMRI graph theoretic analysis
Graph theoretic analysis has been applied broadly in rsfMRI in healthy and clinical populations to examine intrinsic network organization and how variations in intrinsic graph properties relate to cognition and clinical syndromes. From these studies, it is known that (1) not all rsfMRI is created equal (2) that human rsfMRI networks exhibit a complex organization that supports cognitive activity and (3) is altered in clinical populations.
Not all rsfMRI is equal
As alluded to previously, “resting state” fMRI is a misnomer given that the brain is a persistently active system with a rich cognitive repertoire. Neurophysiologically, the link between neural field potentials and BOLD is greater in eyes closed relative to open conditions in animal models, suggesting that visual and attention-related cognitive demands modulate hemodynamic coupling [26, 62]. In addition, important differences in connectivity can be observed across eyes closed, eyes open, and fixation cross rsfMRI collection designs. In eyes closed conditions, connectivity within the auditory network is higher than the other conditions. Connectivity within default-mode, attention, and auditory networks are more reliable when eyes are fixated on a cross. In an eyes open condition without a fixation cross, visual network connectivity is most reliable [14]. This highlights the fact that while rsfMRI data are thought to be relatively “task-free”, differences in how the rsfMRI paradigm is administered are associated with different network configurations, presumably as a function of differences in cognitive processing demands across the conditions. In addition, not all rsfMRI represents waking brain activity: as much as 30% of rsfMRI data may be acquired during transient or sustained periods of sleep in the scanner [14]. While eyes open and especially fixation cross designs reduce the incidence of sleep, it is an important potential confound to all rsfMRI analysis [14]. Thus, we should consider using fixation paradigms to help reduce the incidence of sleep in the scanner. We can also use technologies such as eye tracking, self report, and machine learning procedures to identify potential sleep in the scanner [14].
Human brain organization maintains a complex balance between randomness, small worldness, and modularity
In healthy humans, some major findings have resulted from rsfMRI network analysis. At a high level of organization, it is clear that healthy human brain networks exhibit economical sparse connectivity [47] and maintain a balance along extremes on dimensions of order, degree diversity, and heirarchy [47, 63]. Relatively high order is reflected by the high clustering and modularity of brain networks, but the presence of some randomness is reflected in short path lengths that connect modules [55, 64]. rsfMRI networks exhibit truncated power law distributions [31], demonstrating the presence of scale-free organization through some of the network’s regime with a pronounced “rich-club” of hubs that are highly connected to one another [65]. Hubs are variably defined in the literature, but are typically characterized by an unexpectedly high number of connections given all connections observed in the entire network, within specific modules, and/or between modules [36], and are thought to play key roles in regulating information processing across the network [36]. Interestingly, step-wise functional connectivity analyses in rsfMRI data corroborate the view that the brain is organized hierarchically, involving connections spreading from regions in primary and secondary sensorimotor regions to multimodal integration regions and finally converging on cognitive hubs in the cortex. This suggests that classically defined neuropsychological functions across these regions are represented in a low-cost hierarchy that privileges dense communication between cognitive control and default mode regions and the rest of the brain.
Resting state networks form a stable organization supporting cognitive function within and between individuals
rsfMRI studies demonstrate a rich mesoscale organization involving reliably detectable intrinsic networks that can be observed during rest [29] (See Fig. 7). These are often thought to represent systems with distinct cognitive roles, including fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular control networks, dorsal and ventral attention networks, a salience network, a default mode network, primary somato-motor systems, and subcortical systems. The cognitive relevance of these networks is suggested by the fact that they co-activate as units during distinct cognitive tasks [68]. Notably, connections between the fronto-parietal control and other networks reconfigure most prominently across tasks, most notably in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [68]. In addition, functional connections across the brain robustly predict cognitive activations among these systems during various tasks, suggesting that these systems form basic building blocks in high-level cognitive organization [69]. Interestingly, patterns of connectivity can be used to accurately identify individuals and intersubject variability in intelligence from rsfMRI data connectivity “fingerprints” [70], demonstrating a high information content in the brain’s intrinsic profile.
From Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Adeyemo B, Huckins JF, Kelley WM, Petersen SE. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cerebral cortex. 2014;p. bhu239.
Resting state network graph characteristics are altered in numerous clinical populations
While major networks and organization can be identified in healthy rsfMRI analyses, we can also examine whether graph theoretic analysis contribute to our characterization and understanding of clinical syndromes. A major review is outside the focus of this primer, and the reader is encouraged to consult several excellent reviews concerning graph theoretic analysis in neurological and psychiatric syndromes [71, 63, 72, 73, 74]. So far, early studies span a wide range of syndromes with developmental, psychiatric, and neurological mechanisms of origin. Findings indicate that rsfMRI analysis can indeed identify altered network topology in clinical syndromes over the lifespan. Emerging findings from rsfMRI analysis suggest that psychiatric and neurological disorders across the lifespan are associated with altered intrinsic network organization. Across syndromes, disruptions in hub node activity are frequently identified, with common disruptions in cognitive control and default mode systems but notable differences in specific nodes across disorders [63, 73]. Network efficiency is often disrupted via alterations of path length and mesoscale community organization [73, 47]. Cognitively, general conscious activity may be supported by segregated information processing facilitated by specific functional hub organization [75, 76], and global cognitive function may depend on key long distance connections [77, 78, 79] and “connector” hubs that communicate between systems [80]. Emotionally, disrupted connectivity involving subcortical and frontal are associated with mood and anxiety syndromes and symptom profiles [81, 82]. While the number of studies for any specific disorder remains limited, it appears clear that rsfMRI can contribute a network-level perspective to characterize features associated with clinical diseases and disorders.
Open frontiers in rsfMRI analysis
With the increase in rsfMRI analysis and its early promise in identifying correlates of clinical syndromes and symptoms, it is useful to clarify important theoretical frontiers and opportunities. We can potentially make great strides by studying the neural basis of rsfMRI graph organization, its theoretical contributions to cognitive and clinical neuroscience, its clinical utility, and integration with other techniques.
What does rsfMRI graph organization represent?
It is prudent to reflect on the nature of rsfMRI data to evaluate its epistemological value. Because rsfMRI analysis is based on the BOLD signal, there are fundamental limits to its neural interpretation from the micro- to macroscale. rsfMRI data are thought to represent high and low frequency local field potentials [26]. Beyond hemodynamic responses, how can we understand the specific network configurations we observe in healthy brains? The fact that rsfMRI topology represents major cognitive systems may have a basis in environmental influences over development. There is evidence that rsfMRI network organization is a result of experience-dependent plasticity in humans following co-activation between brain regions [83]. This feature appears to persist in mature brain networks, where a relatively simple connectivity model of activity flow among brain regions predicts network-level activation [69]. Combined with observations about the neurovascular coupling that drives widespread rsfMRI activity, it is speculatively possible that intrinsic network organization is truly a robust marker of gross neurocognitive organization within and between individuals. This may be why cognitive variability can be associated with intrinsic network activity. However, we must still be wary about drawing inferences about any specific cognitive activity during any particular scanning session.
One prevailing limitation in many studies is that BOLD-based graph edges are demonstrably low-dimensional when computed along entire time series and thus very limited in their ability to represent brain interactions [84]. However, time-varying graph [85] and trial-wise analysis [86] are promising open areas that may add new dimensions of information to potentially link clinical nosology to brain network dynamics (See Fig. 8). As these efforts develop, it will remain critical to pair rsfMRI analysis with good experimental designs, genetic and environmental analyses, and replication and cross-validation studies.
Adapted from Mattar MG, Betzel RF, Bassett DS. The flexible brain. Brain. 2016;139(8):2110–2112.
The unique theoretical value of graph theory rsfMRI analysis is at present unknown. Do the quantities afforded to us represent something cognitively meaningful in principle? Can they provide us theoretical or predictive information we otherwise would not see? It is clear that we can continue to study brain organization and dysfunction without using graph theory. It is also possible, as it commonly done, to rely heavily on reverse inference [87, 88] to interpret the graph statistics we observe and previously known ideas from cognitive neuroscience. To identify specific value of graph theory, we should search for consilience between graph theoretic analysis and other neuroscientific approaches [89].
Optimism for clinical identification, prediction, and translation
Nevertheless, cautiously optimistic view of rsfMRI for some clinical purposes is justified if we maintain several priorities. The features that distinguish useful clinical research include problem base, context placement, information gain, pragmatism, patient centeredness, value for money, feasibility, and transparency [90]. Many studies do not satisfy these features and fail to provide value because of their design. It has been suggested that a major reform could address the forces that drive this problem, and the reader is strongly encouraged to consult one particularly incisive perspective [90]. As it stands, careful hypothesis testing for specific clinical end-goals should increase in general [90].
Toward applied clinical utility, rsfMRI graph statistics analyzed can be combined with machine learning to build models that identify autism [91], Alzheimer’s disease [92, 93], schizophrenia [94] depression [95], relative to controls, in some cases with very high sensitivity and specificity. In general, the opportunities to apply machine learning to any type of neuroimaging data or derived measure are enormous and can provide novel observations [96]. The challenge in rsfMRI analysis is that in order to have clinical value, these approaches must be valid and replicable, portable to clinical environments, and either equally accurate to but cheaper than existing techniques, or demonstrably more accurate.
Integrated multimodal strategies offer potential value for treatment. For example, rsfMRI connectivity has been evaluated to guide or predict outcomes in neurosurgical approaches in brain injury [97], brain tumors [98], and epilepsy [99], and deep brain stimulation [100]. In addition, the effects of noninvasive brain stimulation have been linked to the connectivity profiles of intrinsic networks across many diseases. Specifically, suppressive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is effective if the site of stimulation is positively connected with the target, whereas facilitative TMS is effective in the opposite case [101]. Importantly, rsfMRI graph theoretic analyses have not been systematically linked to brain stimulation approaches to provide either neurocognitive criteria (e.g., target brain network states for stimulation) or surgical or brain stimulation targeting methods. This is substantial potential for development on this front. It is possible that computer simulation models that use simplifications of neural dynamics in anatomical networks to predict rsfMRI connectivity [102, 103, 104] and the effects of stimulation [105] may contribute to this effort.
Conclusion
Graph theoretic analyses in rsfMRI are becoming a significant tool for describing the human brain and characterizing clinical syndromes. It is becoming clear that clinical syndromes are marked by dysfunction in major brain networks and distinct hubs in intrinsic functional connectivity patterns. The specific contributions of rsfMRI to clinical nosology and translation will likely be understood with the benefits of hindsight. At present, our challenge is to embrace opportunities to robustly characterize network dysfunction and search for a meaningful framework to connect rsfMRI network analysis to clinical practice.
KEY POINTS.
-
-
Graph theory is the mathematical basis of network science and is now widely applied to study rsfMRI networks
-
-
Several major themes have emerged in applied rsfMRI graph theoretic analysis
-
-
rsfMRI graph theoretic analysis has revealed several key principles of healthy and dysfunctional brain network organization
-
-
Open frontiers in rsfMRI graph theoretic analysis include evaluating the contributions of graph theory and potential for direct clinical applications
SYNOPSIS.
Graph theoretic analyses applied to examine the brain at rest have played a critical role in clarifying the foundations of the brain's intrinsic and task-related activity. There are many opportunities for clinical scientists to describe and predict dysfunction using a network perspective. This primer describes the theoretical basis and practical application of graph theoretic analysis to resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Major practices, concepts, and findings are concisely reviewed. The theoretical and practical frontiers are highlighted with observations about major avenues for opportunity in theoretical development and clinical translation.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors has no disclosures to report.
References and Notes
- 1.Bassett DS, Gazzaniga MS. Understanding complexity in the human brain. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2011;15(5):200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lewis TG. Network science: Theory and applications. John Wiley & Sons; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bassett DS, Bullmore E. Small-world brain networks. The neuroscientist. 2006;12(6):512–523. doi: 10.1177/1073858406293182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sporns O, Tononi G, Kötter R. The human connectome: a structural description of the human brain. PLoS Comput Biol. 2005;1(4):e42. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0010042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ogawa S, Lee TM, Kay AR, Tank DW. Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1990;87(24):9868–9872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.24.9868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(9):2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2001;98(2):676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN. Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science. 2007;315(5810):393–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Delamillieure P, Doucet G, Mazoyer B, Turbelin MR, Delcroix N, Mellet E, et al. The resting state questionnaire: an introspective questionnaire for evaluation of inner experience during the conscious resting state. Brain research bulletin. 2010;81(6):565–573. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Beaty RE, Benedek M, Wilkins RW, Jauk E, Fink A, Silvia PJ, et al. Creativity and the default network: A functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia. 2014;64:92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg A, Keller J, Glover GH. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Adeyemo B, Huckins JF, Kelley WM, Petersen SE. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cerebral cortex. 2014:bhu239. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Shehzad Z, Kelly AC, Reiss PT, Gee DG, Gotimer K, Uddin LQ, et al. The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable. Cerebral cortex. 2009;19(10):2209–2229. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Patriat R, Molloy EK, Meier TB, Kirk GR, Nair VA, Meyerand ME, et al. The effect of resting condition on resting-state fMRI reliability and consistency: a comparison between resting with eyes open, closed, and fixated. Neuroimage. 2013;78:463–473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Parker D, Liu X, Razlighi QR. Optimal slice timing correction and its interaction with fMRI parameters and artifacts. Medical image analysis. 2017;35:434–445. doi: 10.1016/j.media.2016.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Patriat R, Reynolds RC, Birn RM. An improved model of motion-related signal changes in fMRI. Neuroimage. 2017;144:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ciric R, Wolf DH, Power JD, Roalf DR, Baum G, Ruparel K, et al. Benchmarking confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. arXiv preprint arXiv: 160803616. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Poline J, Grasby P, Williams S, Frackowiak RS, et al. Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited. Neuroimage. 1995;2(1):45–53. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ashburner J, Friston K. Multimodal image coregistration and partitioning—a unified framework. Neuroimage. 1997;6(3):209–217. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ashburner J, Friston KJ, et al. Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions. Human brain mapping. 1999;7(4):254–266. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:4<254::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hagler DJ, Saygin AP, Sereno MI. Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data. Neuroimage. 2006;33(4):1093–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage. 2009;44(3):893–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Murphy K, Fox MD. Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102(27):9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106(31):13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Schölvinck ML, Maier A, Frank QY, Duyn JH, Leopold DA. Neural basis of global resting-state fMRI activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(22):10238–10243. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913110107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Saad ZS, Gotts SJ, Murphy K, Chen G, Jo HJ, Martin A, et al. Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain connectivity. 2012;2(1):25–32. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Weissenbacher A, Kasess C, Gerstl F, Lanzenberger R, Moser E, Windischberger C. Correlations and anticorrelations in resting-state functional connectivity MRI: a quantitative comparison of preprocessing strategies. Neuroimage. 2009;47(4):1408–1416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 2011;72(4):665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Shen X, Tokoglu F, Papademetris X, Constable RT. Groupwise whole-brain parcellation from resting-state fMRI data for network node identification. Neuroimage. 2013;82:403–415. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wang J, Wang L, Zang Y, Yang H, Tang H, Gong Q, et al. Parcellation-dependent small-world brain functional networks: a resting-state fMRI study. Human brain mapping. 2009;30(5):1511–1523. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Newman MEJ. Networks: An Introduction. Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2010;52(3):1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bassett DS, Sporns O. Network neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20(3):353–364. doi: 10.1038/nn.4502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E. A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. The Journal of neuroscience. 2006;26(1):63–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2013;17(12):683–696. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sporns O. Networks of the Brain. MIT press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Betzel RF, Gu S, Medaglia JD, Pasqualetti F, Bassett DS. Optimally controlling the human connectome: the role of network topology. arXiv preprint arXiv: 160305261. 2016 doi: 10.1038/srep30770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sporns O, Honey CJ, Kötter R. Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks. PloS one. 2007;2(10):e1049. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Onnela JP, Saramäki J, Kertész J, Kaski K. Intensity and coherence of motifs in weighted complex networks. Physical Review E. 2005;71(6):065103. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.065103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Latora V, Marchiori M. Efficient behavior of small-world networks. Physical review letters. 2001;87(19):198701. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.87.198701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mucha PJ, Richardson T, Macon K, Porter MA, Onnela JP. Community structure in time-dependent, multiscale, and multiplex networks. Science. 2010;328(5980):876–878. doi: 10.1126/science.1184819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Holme P. Core-periphery organization of complex networks. Physical Review E. 2005;72(4):046111. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.72.046111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Newman ME. Assortative mixing in networks. Physical review letters. 2002;89(20):208701. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.89.208701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Newman ME. Mixing patterns in networks. Physical Review E. 2003;67(2):026126. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.67.026126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Martin T, Ball B, Newman ME. Structural inference for uncertain networks. Physical Review E. 2016;93(1):012306. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.93.012306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bullmore E, Sporns O. The economy of brain network organization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13(5):336–349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Barabási AL, Albert R. Emergence of scaling in random networks. Science. 1999;286(5439):509–512. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5439.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Barabási AL, Albert R, Jeong H. Mean-field theory for scale-free random networks. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications. 1999;272(1):173–187. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Albert R, Barabási AL. Statistical mechanics of complex networks. Reviews of modern physics. 2002;74(1):47. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kumar R, Raghavan P, Rajagopalan S, Sivakumar D, Tomkins A, Upfal E. Foundations of Computer Science, 2000. Proceedings. 41st Annual Symposium on. IEEE; 2000. Stochastic models for the web graph; pp. 57–65. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Albert R, Jeong H, Barabási AL. Error and attack tolerance of complex networks. Nature. 2000;406(6794):378–382. doi: 10.1038/35019019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Barabási AL. Scale-free networks: a decade and beyond. Science. 2009;325(5939):412–413. doi: 10.1126/science.1173299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Clauset A, Shalizi CR, Newman ME. Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM review. 2009;51(4):661–703. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Watts DJ, Strogatz SH. Collective dynamics of â€~small-world’networks. Nature. 1998;393(6684):440–442. doi: 10.1038/30918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Latora V, Marchiori M. Economic small-world behavior in weighted networks. The European Physics Journal B. 2003;32:249–263. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Achard S, Bullmore E. Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS Comput Biol. 2007;3:e17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Amaral LAN, Scala A, Barthelemy M, Stanley HE. Classes of small-world networks. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2000;97(21):11149–11152. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200327197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Muldoon SF, Bridgeford EW, Bassett DS. Small-world propensity and weighted brain networks. Scientific reports. 2016:6. doi: 10.1038/srep22057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Estrada E, Hatano N. Communicability in complex networks. Physical Review E. 2008;77(3):036111. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.77.036111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Graham RL, Hell P. On the history of the minimum spanning tree problem. Annals of the History of Computing. 1985;7(1):43–57. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Pan WJ, Thompson GJ, Magnuson ME, Jaeger D, Keilholz S. Infraslow LFP correlates to resting-state fMRI BOLD signals. Neuroimage. 2013;74:288–297. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Stam CJ. Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014;15:683–695. doi: 10.1038/nrn3801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bassett DS, Bullmore ET. Small-world brain networks revisited. The Neuroscientist. 2016:1073858416667720. doi: 10.1177/1073858416667720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Van Den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Rich-club organization of the human connectome. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(44):15775–15786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Zuo XN, Ehmke R, Mennes M, Imperati D, Castellanos FX, Sporns O, et al. Network centrality in the human functional connectome. Cerebral cortex. 2012;22(8):1862–1875. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Sepulcre J. Stepwise Connectivity of the Modal Cortex Reveals the Multimodal Organization of the Human Brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:10649–10661. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0759-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature neuroscience. 2013;16(9):1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Cole MW, Ito T, Bassett DS, Schultz DH. Activity flow over resting-state networks shapes cognitive task activations. Nature Neuroscience. 2016 doi: 10.1038/nn.4406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Finn ES, Shen X, Scheinost D, Rosenberg MD, Huang J, Chun MM, et al. Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nature neuroscience. 2015 doi: 10.1038/nn.4135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Lee MH, Smyser CD, Shimony JS. Resting-state fMRI: a review of methods and clinical applications. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2013;34(10):1866–1872. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10(3):186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Braun U, Muldoon SF, Bassett DS. On human brain networks in health and disease. eLS. 2015 [Google Scholar]
- 74.Van Den Heuvel MP, Pol HEH. Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. European neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;20(8):519–534. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Uehara T, Yamasaki T, Okamoto T, Koike T, Kan S, Miyauchi S, et al. Efficiency of a â€oesmall-worl d†brain network depends on consciousness level: a resting-state fMRI study. Cerebral cortex. 2014;24(6):1529–1539. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Achard S, Delon-Martin C, Vértes PE, Renard F, Schenck M, Schneider F, et al. Hubs of brain functional networks are radically reorganized in comatose patients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(50):20608–20613. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208933109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. An anatomical substrate for integration among functional networks in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(36):14489–14500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2128-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Markov NT, Ercsey-Ravasz M, Lamy C, Gomes ARR, Magrou L, Misery P, et al. The role of long-range connections on the specificity of the macaque interareal cortical network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(13):5187–5192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1218972110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Pandit AS, Expert P, Lambiotte R, Bonnelle V, Leech R, Turkheimer FE, et al. Traumatic brain injury impairs small-world topology. Neurology. 2013;80(20):1826–1833. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182929f38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Warren DE, Power JD, Bruss J, Denburg NL, Waldron EJ, Sun H, et al. Network measures predict neuropsychological outcome after brain injury. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111(39):14247–14252. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322173111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Zhang J, Wang J, Wu Q, Kuang W, Huang X, He Y, et al. Disrupted brain connectivity networks in drug-naive, first-episode major depressive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 2011;70(4):334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Ye M, Yang T, Qing P, Lei X, Qiu J, Liu G. Changes of functional brain networks in major depressive disorder: a graph theoretical analysis of resting-state fMRI. PloS one. 2015;10(9):e0133775. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Guerra-Carrillo B, Mackey AP, Bunge SA. Resting-state fMRI: a window into human brain plasticity. The Neuroscientist. 2014;20(5):522–533. doi: 10.1177/1073858414524442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Marrelec G, Messé A, Giron A, Rudrauf D. Functional Connectivity’s Degenerate View of Brain Computation. PLoS computational biology. 2016;12(10):e1005031. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Mattar MG, Betzel RF, Bassett DS. The flexible brain. Brain. 2016;139(8):2110–2112. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Liu X, Chang C, Duyn JH. Decomposition of spontaneous brain activity into distinct fMRI co-activation patterns. Frontiers in systems neuroscience. 2013;7:101. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Poldrack RA. Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: from reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron. 2011;72(5):692–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Poldrack RA. Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in cognitive sciences. 2006;10(2):59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Mill RD, Ito T, Cole MW. From connectome to cognition: The search for mechanism in human functional brain networks. NeuroImage. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Ioannidis JP. Why most clinical research is not useful. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002049. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Zhou Y, Yu F, Duong T. Multiparametric MRI characterization and prediction in autism spectrum disorder using graph theory and machine learning. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e90405. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Koch W, Teipel S, Mueller S, Benninghoff J, Wagner M, Bokde AL, et al. Diagnostic power of default mode network resting state fMRI in the detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of aging. 2012;33(3):466–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Khazaee A, Ebrahimzadeh A, Babajani-Feremi A. Identifying patients with Alzheimer’s disease using resting-state fMRI and graph theory. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2015;126(11):2132–2141. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.02.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Fekete T, Wilf M, Rubin D, Edelman S, Malach R, Mujica-Parodi LR. Combining classification with fMRI-derived complex network measures for potential neurodiagnostics. PloS one. 2013;8(5):e62867. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Sacchet MD, Prasad G, Foland-Ross LC, Thompson PM, Gotlib IH. Support vector machine classification of major depressive disorder using diffusion-weighted neuroimaging and graph theory. Frontiers in psychiatry. 2015:6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Varoquaux G, Thirion B. How machine learning is shaping cognitive neuroimaging. GigaScience. 2014;3(1):28. doi: 10.1186/2047-217X-3-28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Böttger J, Margulies DS, Horn P, Thomale UW, Podlipsky I, Shapira-Lichter I, et al. A software tool for interactive exploration of intrinsic functional connectivity opens new perspectives for brain surgery. Acta neurochirurgica. 2011;153(8):1561–1572. doi: 10.1007/s00701-011-0985-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Sair HI, Yahyavi-Firouz-Abadi N, Calhoun VD, Airan RD, Agarwal S, Intrapiromkul J, et al. Presurgical brain mapping of the language network in patients with brain tumors using resting-state fMRI: Comparison with task fMRI. Human brain mapping. 2016;37(3):913–923. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Osipowicz K, Sperling MR, Sharan AD, Tracy JI. Functional MRI, resting state fMRI, and DTI for predicting verbal fluency outcome following resective surgery for temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Neurosurgery. 2016;124(4):929–937. doi: 10.3171/2014.9.JNS131422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Figee M, Luigjes J, Smolders R, Valencia-Alfonso CE, Van Wingen G, De Kwaasteniet B, et al. Deep brain stimulation restores frontostriatal network activity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Nature neuroscience. 2013;16(4):386–387. doi: 10.1038/nn.3344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Fox MD, Buckner RL, Liu H, Chakravarty MM, Lozano AM, Pascual-Leone A. Resting-state networks link invasive and noninvasive brain stimulation across diverse psychiatric and neurological diseases. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111(41):E4367–E4375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1405003111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Honey C, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran JP, Meuli R, et al. Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106(6):2035–2040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811168106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Hermundstad AM, Brown KS, Bassett DS, Aminoff EM, Frithsen A, Johnson A, et al. Structurally-constrained relationships between cognitive states in the human brain. PLoS Comput Biol. 2014;10(5):e1003591. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Hermundstad AM, Bassett DS, Brown KS, Aminoff EM, Clewett D, Freeman S, et al. Structural foundations of resting-state and task-based functional connectivity in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(15):6169–6174. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219562110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Muldoon SF, Pasqualetti F, Gu S, Cieslak M, Grafton ST, Vettel JM, et al. Stimulation-based control of dynamic brain networks. PLoS Comput Biol. 2016;12(9):e1005076. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]