Abstract
Stress plays a key role in the development of psychiatric disorders and has a negative impact on sleep integrity. In mice, chronic social defeat stress (CSDS) is an ethologically valid model of stress-related disorders but little is known about its effects on sleep regulation. Here, we investigated the immediate and long-term effects of 10 consecutive days of social defeat (SD) on vigilance states in C57Bl/6J male mice. Social behavior was assessed to identify susceptible mice, i.e., mice that develop long-lasting social avoidance, and unsusceptible mice. Sleep-wake stages in mice of both groups were analyzed by means of polysomnographic recordings at baseline, after the first, third, and tenth stress sessions and on the 5th recovery day (R5) following the 10-day CSDS. In susceptible mice, each SD session produced biphasic changes in sleep-wake states that were preserved all along 10-day CSDS. These sessions elicited a short-term enhancement of wake time while rapid eye-movement (REM) sleep was strongly inhibited. Concomitantly, delta power was increased during non REM (NREM) sleep. During the following dark period, an increase in total sleep time, as well as wake fragmentation, were observed after each analyzed SD session. Similar changes were observed in unsusceptible mice. At R5, elevated high-frequency EEG activity, as observed in insomniacs, emerged during NREM sleep in both susceptible and unsusceptible groups suggesting that CSDS impaired sleep quality. Furthermore, susceptible but not unsusceptible mice displayed stress-anticipatory arousal during recovery, a common feature of anxiety disorders. Altogether, our findings show that CSDS has profound impacts on vigilance states and further support that sleep is tightly regulated by exposure to stressful events. They also revealed that susceptibility to chronic psychological stress is associated with heightened arousal, a physiological feature of stress vulnerability.
Keywords: stress, social defeat, susceptibility, animal model, sleep, arousal, EEG activity, insomnia
Introduction
Mental disorders are a health priority as they are among the major contributors to the global burden of disease due to their high prevalence and high disability (Whiteford et al., 2013; Trautmann et al., 2016). Depressive and anxiety disorders carry the heaviest burden representing ~60% of disability-adjusted life years caused by mental and substance use disorders (Whiteford et al., 2013). The World Health Organization predicts depression as the greatest cause of disability worldwide by 2030, and the economic costs of mental disorders are expected to double. Therefore, there is an urgent need for a better characterization of the cardinal features of mental disorders. Sleep disturbances are a core symptom of psychiatric disorders as set out in the current diagnostic manual DSM-V (www.dsm5.org). Insomnia, defined as a complaint of difficulty initiating or maintaining sleep, is present in over 80% of patients with depression (Armitage, 2007). Furthermore, 40% of patients with insomnia have a coexisting psychiatric condition. Objective measurement of sleep using electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings found characteristic sleep disturbances in patients with depression and anxiety disorders including prolonged sleep latency, increased nocturnal awakenings and early morning awakenings (for reviews see Reynolds and Kupfer, 1987; Benca et al., 1997; Armitage, 2007; Steiger and Kimura, 2010). Sleep efficiency and continuity are also impaired as evidenced by decreased deep slow-wave sleep and slow-wave (delta) activity. Changes in rapid eye-movement (REM) sleep are described in depressed patients with reduced REM sleep latency, prolonged first REM sleep episode and elevated REM density (number of REM per REM sleep time). Similarly, studies on patients with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) report an increase in REM density (Ross et al., 1989, 1994; Mellman et al., 1995; Pillar et al., 2000; Kobayashi et al., 2007; Germain, 2013). However, taken altogether, polysomnographic studies conducted in patient with depression and PTSD have yielded contradictory results, in particular for REM sleep, highlighting the complexity of using sleep biomarkers in these diseases (as described in the following reviews: Pillar et al., 2000; Buysse et al., 2006; Armitage, 2007; Germain, 2013 and for an overview see the following meta-analysis reports: Benca et al., 1992; Kobayashi et al., 2007; Pillai et al., 2011; Baglioni et al., 2016). These conflicting findings may be due to confounding factors, analysis done at different stages of the illness, disease heterogeneity, and comorbidities with other psychiatric conditions. Translational studies in animal models might be useful to define the exact relationship between sleep and affective disorders. For decades, extensive work in preclinical research has generated a large array of animal models sharing relevant endophenotypes of mental disorders. Although much work has been done on depressive- and anxiety-like behaviors, sleep profiling in animal models remains poorly explored in spite of being highly translational. Indeed, polysomnographic and quantitative EEG, the objective gold standard methods for sleep assessment, are carried out and analyzed with the same methods in both humans and animals.
In the recent years, social-stress based models have provided mechanistic insights on how stress shapes socioaffective behaviors. These models are believed to be relevant to the human situation as exposure to stressful life events is a major contributing factor to psychiatric disorders including major depression and anxiety disorders (Yehuda et al., 1998; Liu and Alloy, 2010). In particular, social stressors including social exclusion, low social status, bullying in workplaces or unemployment, have severe consequences on mental health (Björkqvist, 2001). Interestingly, exposure to social stress is translatable to rodents as social hierarchy and confrontation between conspecifics occurs naturally in males. Some studies suggest that chronic social stressors such as, an instable social environment are also applicable to females (Schmidt et al., 2010). Based on brief social subordination sessions in males, the chronic social defeat stress (CSDS) is an ethologically, face, and predictive valid approach for modeling stress-related disorders such as, depression, generalized anxiety, and/or PTSD (Franklin et al., 2012; Slattery and Cryan, 2014). CSDS is a well-studied model that induces long-term physiological and behavioral changes (Hammels et al., 2015). Thus, defeated mice develop a wide range of depressive-like behaviors including anhedonia but also social avoidance that can be normalized by chronic treatment with antidepressants (Berton et al., 2006). They also display anxiety-like behaviors (Berton et al., 2006; Krishnan et al., 2007). Physiological changes in defeated mice include decreased body weight, circadian abnormalities, or sensitized corticosterone reactivity (Krishnan et al., 2007). Some of these features, such as anhedonia, return shortly to normal levels after the stress paradigm. However, others including social avoidance, anxiety-like behavior or social hyperthermia persist up to 4 weeks. Although the majority of defeated mice express these passive coping responses and are considered as susceptible, some show stress resilience manifested by resistance to defeat-induced social avoidance (Krishnan et al., 2007). The mechanisms underlying stress resilience and susceptibility have been proposed to rely on the recruitment of specific neural circuits and neurochemical systems (Tornatzky and Miczek, 1993; Koolhaas et al., 1997; Berton et al., 2006; Krishnan et al., 2007; Vialou et al., 2014, 2015).
Although sleep impairments are important features of stress-related disorders, there are surprisingly few studies on the consequences of CSDS on sleep in mice (Olini et al., 2017; Wells et al., 2017). In the present study, we evaluated the impact of CSDS on sleep-wake stages and sleep EEG in susceptible mice. For this purpose, adult male mice of the C57Bl/6J background were subjected to 10-day CSDS and susceptible mice were subsequently identified in the social interaction test. The effects of social defeat on sleep/wake states were analyzed at different time-points throughout the CSDS paradigm and during the recovery period in susceptible and non-susceptible mice. Our findings reveal that CSDS markedly disturbs sleep and triggers long-lasting EEG changes.
Materials and methods
Animals and housing
Adult male C57Bl/6J (13–19 week-old, bodyweight: 23–30 g) and outbred male Swiss CD1 mice (9–16 week-old, body weight: 30–35 g) were used in this study (Centre d'élevage R. Janvier, Le Genest St. Isle, France). Mice were housed up to five per cage under standard conditions (12 h light/dark cycle; lights on at 7:00 a.m.; 22 ± 2°C ambient temperature; 60% relative humidity; food and water ad libitum). All experiments were performed in strict conformity with the European Union laws and policies for use of animals in neuroscience research (European Committee Council Directive 2010/63/EU) and were authorized by the Ethical Committee for Preclinical Research (Comité d'éthique en expérimentation animale Charles Darwin, CE2A-nb 5) of the French Ministry of Research and High Education (articles R.214-124, R.214-125).
Surgery and electrode implantations
Mice were anaesthetized with ketamine/xylazine [50 and 2 mg/kg, respectively, intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection] before being fixed on a stereotaxic apparatus. All coordinates are adapted from the mouse brain atlas (Franklin and Paxinos, 2008). Mice were implanted with classical set of electrodes (made of enameled nichrome wire, 150 μm in diameter) for polysomnographic sleep monitoring (Boutrel et al., 1999). Briefly, two EEG electrodes were placed onto the dura through holes perforated into the skull over the right parietal cortex (2 mm lateral to midline and 2 mm caudal to bregma suture) and the cerebellum (at midline, 2 mm posterior to lambda), two electrooculogram (EOG) electrodes were located subcutaneously on each side of the orbit and two electromyogram (EMG) electrodes were inserted into the neck muscles. All electrodes were soldered to a miniconnector (Antelec, La Queue en Brie, France). Dental acrylic was used to anchor the electrodes to the skull. Mice were allowed to recover for 10 days and briefly handled daily for habituation. They were acclimated to the recording conditions for 4 days before starting the recordings and the behavioral experiments. Thus, mice were placed in individual recording chambers [custom-made Plexiglas cages (19 × 19 × 30 cm)] and connected to the recording system with a light-weight cable and a swivel allowing free movements.
Sleep recordings and analysis
Recordings
The timeline for sleep recordings is depicted in Figures 1A,B. A 24 h baseline (BL) recording starting at light onset (7 a.m.) was done 2 days before the CSDS protocol. During the 10-day CSDS protocol, mice were recorded immediately after the social defeat (SD) session starting at 09:30 a.m. until the next day (7 a.m.). Mice were gently restrained for disconnection/connection to the recording system before and after the SD session. A 24 h recording starting at light onset (7 a.m.) was also done on the 5th recovery day (R5). Recordings were analyzed on BL, on stress sessions SD1, SD3, and SD10 and on R5.
Scoring
The polysomnographic signals were recorded and digitized with an Embla Module (Medcare, Reykjavik, Iceland). They were sampled at a rate of 200 Hz (EEG) or 100 Hz (EOG and EMG). The EEG signals were analog band-pass filtered at 0.5 and 90 Hz for low- and high-pass filters, respectively, with the presence of a powerline notch filter. EOG and EMG filters were set at 0.5 and 45 Hz for low- and high-pass filters, respectively. Using the Somnologica®software (Medcare, Reykjavik, Iceland), polysomnographic recordings were visually scored every 10 s epoch as wakefulness (W), non REM (NREM) sleep, or REM sleep following classical criteria as previously described (Boutrel et al., 1999).
Sleep parameters
Vigilance states amounts for each animal were expressed as minutes per 3 or 12 h intervals. The sleep architecture was assessed by calculating the mean duration and frequency of vigilance states bouts (a bout could be as short as one epoch).
Power spectra analysis
The EEG (parietal-cerebellum) signal was processed for power spectra analysis on the followings days: BL, SD1, SD3, SD10, and R5. On the basis of visual and spectral analysis, 10-s epochs with artifacts were visually identified and omitted from the spectral analysis. One susceptible mouse showed EEG recordings with wake artifact during more than 20% of the time and was excluded from the spectral analysis. Then, consecutive 10-s epochs were subjected to a fast Fourier transformation routine (FFT), yielding power spectra between 0.4 and 50 Hz with a 0.4 Hz frequency bins (Alexandre et al., 2008). For each animal and vigilance state, a spectrogram was obtained and the values of the power spectra were divided into 5 frequency bands: delta (0.5–4.99 Hz), theta (5–9.99 Hz), alpha (10–12.99 Hz), beta (13–29.99 Hz), and low gamma (30–50 Hz). Mean EEG power spectrum was obtained by averaging the power spectra of all 10-s epochs of a given state in 3 h or 12 h intervals. EEG power spectra for each of the five frequency bands were expressed as a ratio of the mean EEG power over all frequency bands (relative values) and were compared to BL.
Behavioral tests
Chronic social defeat stress paradigm
We first selected aggressive CD1 mice according to published methods (Berton et al., 2006). Upon arrival, CD1 male mice were singly housed to habituate for a week. A C57BL/6J male was introduced into each agressors' cage. Mice were allowed to interact until the first fight occurs or 2 min. Immediately after the first attack mice were separated and latency to first fight was recorded. This was repeated for the next 2 days (3-day screening). At the end of the 3rd day the most aggressive CD1 male mice were kept in large cages (19 × 35 × 14 cm) for the rest of the experiment.
To allow sleep recordings in between SD sessions, we used a modified version of the CSDS paradigm as described in Challis et al. (2013). Experimental C57BL/6J mice were submitted to SD for 10 consecutive days on daily basis (Figure 1A). The mouse was exposed to an unfamiliar and aggressive male CD1 mouse for 5 min before being separated from the aggressor by placing a divider in the same cage for the next 20 min allowing sensory, but not physical, contact (Figure 1B). The SD session lasted 25 min starting at 09:30 a.m. The mouse was then placed back in its recording chamber and connected back to the recording cable until the following SD session.
Social interaction test
Five days before the CSDS paradigm and on recovery day 7 (R7), social avoidance behavior was evaluated in the two-trial social interaction test (Figure 1A). In the first trial (2.5 min), the experimental mouse explored a white Perspex arena (43 × 43 × 26 cm) that has an empty perforated plexiglass cage along one side (“No target” condition) in a 10 lux illuminated room. In the second trial (2.5 min), the experimental mouse was reintroduced into the arena with an unfamiliar CD1 aggressor placed in the plexiglass cage (“Target” condition). Between these two trials, the experimental mice were returned to their home cage for 1 min. The time spent interacting with the unfamiliar CD1 mouse was recorded by video tracking (Viewpoint). Susceptibility to the stress paradigm was defined by two criteria: a social interaction ratio [(interaction time, condition “Target” after the CSDS)/(interaction time, condition “Target” before the CSDS)] smaller than 1 and an interaction time with “Target” less than 45 s.
Statistics
All data were analyzed using Prism 7.0 (GraphPad Software). Normality assumptions were first verified prior to the use of any parametric tests (D'Agostino & Pearson normality test). In case of violation of normality assumption, non-parametric tests were used. Statistical analyses are described in Table S1. For the social interaction test, two-way repeated measures (RM) ANOVAs were performed for stress (repeated measures, before and after CSDS) and susceptibility (susceptible vs. unsusceptible mice). The effects of CSDS on sleep and wake amounts, bout frequency and mean duration, as well as on EEG power spectrum data, were analyzed using one-way ANOVAs with repeated measure over the day of stress (BL, SD1, SD3, or SD10) for each analyzed time window (10–13 h, 13–16 h, or 12 h of the dark period). Two-way RM ANOVAs were also applied to vigilance state data during the recovery phase for stress (repeated measures, BL and R5) and time (repeated measures over 3-h segments). Paired t-test and non-parametric two-tailed paired Wilcoxon test were also employed to analyze EEG power bands during the light or the dark period on R5 compared to BL. When appropriate, ANOVAs were followed by Dunn's, Tukey's, or Sidak's post-hoc tests. Statistical significance was set at 0.05 for all procedures. All tests were performed on raw data.
Results
Identification of susceptible mice
On completion of the sleep recordings (R7), social behavior was evaluated in the social interaction test before and after the stress protocol to identify susceptible and unsusceptible mice (Figure 1). The corresponding statistical analysis is described in Table S1.
Over 17 mice submitted to CSDS, ~60% were considered susceptible according to the inclusion criteria indicated in the legend of Figure 1. Social behavior was greatly affected by the CSDS protocol in the susceptible group (Figure 1C). Time spent in the interaction zone with an unfamiliar CD-1 mouse was significantly decreased after CSDS (−64%, before vs. after CSDS; Figure 1C). In contrast, mice of the unsusceptible group did not display a significant decrease in interaction after the stress protocol (Figure 1C). We then analyzed the consequences of 10-day CSDS on vigilance states in susceptible and unsusceptible mice.
SD induced sleep disturbances throughout the 10-day CSDS in susceptible mice
We evaluated the consequences of stress exposure on sleep-wake patterns during the course of 10-day CSDS. SD sessions were performed during the light “resting” period starting at 9:30 a.m. and vigilance states were analyzed on days SD1, SD3, and SD10 (Figure 2). The corresponding statistical analysis is described in Table S1.
When placed back in their recording chamber, susceptible mice showed a robust increase in wake amounts during the first 3 h period that follows the SD session relative to BL (Figures 2A,B). This wake enhancement reached significance for the different days tested indicating that the effects of SD on wake were maintained throughout the 10-day CSDS (Figure 2B). These changes were not due to modifications in the mean duration of wake bouts or their frequency (Table 1). Concomitantly, SD decreased NREM sleep amounts (Figure 2B). This stress effect was significant on SD1, but not on SD3 and SD10, despite a trend toward reduction. Nevertheless, during this period, REM sleep amounts were drastically reduced for all days tested (Figure 2B), resulting from a decrease in REM sleep bout numbers (Table 1). During the next 3 h period, wake and sleep amounts returned to normal levels compared to BL (Figure 2C). Thus, SD was found to promote arousal and inhibit REM sleep shortly after the SD session.
Table 1.
Number of bouts | Bouts mean duration (min) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | SD1 | SD3 | SD10 | Baseline | SD1 | SD3 | SD10 | ||
WAKE | 0–3 h | 31 ± 3 | 29 ± 3 | 30 ± 5 | 23 ± 3 | 1.6 ± 0.3 | 3.5 ± 0.4 | 4.0 ± 1.4 | 4.3 ± 0.7 |
3–6 h | 26 ± 1 | 34 ± 3 | 30 ± 2 | 29 ± 3 | 1.8 ± 0.1 | 1.4 ± 0.2 | 1.9 ± 0.3 | 2.1 ± 0.3 | |
Dark | 64 ± 4 | 99 ± 7** | 87 ± 5* | 92 ± 8* | 7.7 ± 0.7 | 3.7 ± 0.3** | 4.5 ± 0.5** | 4.4 ± 0.5* | |
NREMS | 0–3 h | 31 ± 3 | 28 ± 3 | 30 ± 5 | 23 ± 3 | 4.1 ± 0.3 | 3.1 ± 0.3 | 3.7 ± 0.4 | 4.5 ± 0.5 |
3–6 h | 26 ± 1 | 34 ± 3 | 30 ± 2 | 29 ± 3 | 4.8 ± 0.3 | 3.9 ± 0.5 | 3.9 ± 0.5 | 4.2 ± 0.5 | |
Dark | 62 ± 4 | 98 ± 7** | 86 ± 5 | 91 ± 8* | 3.9 ± 0.2 | 3.5 ± 0.3 | 3.7 ± 0.2 | 3.7 ± 0.3 | |
REMS | 0–3 h | 11 ± 1 | 1 ± 1*** | 4 ± 1* | 4 ± 1* | 1.3 ± 0.1 | 1.1 ± 0.5 | 1.3 ± 0.1 | 1.1 ± 0.2 |
3–6 h | 10 ± 1 | 8 ± 1 | 9 ± 1 | 9 ± 1 | 1.4 ± 0.1 | 1.2 ± 0.1 | 1.4 ± 0.1 | 1.3 ± 0.1 | |
Dark | 11 ± 2 | 25 ± 2*** | 22 ± 2** | 22 ± 2** | 1.4 ± 0.2 | 1.5 ± 0.1 | 1.3 ± 0.1 | 1.4 ± 0.1 |
The data are given as means ± S.E.M (n = 9). For multiple group comparisons:
P < 0.05,
P < 0.01, and
P < 0.001 compared to BL. The statistical analysis is described in Table S1.
In addition, SD elicited a sleep rebound at the expense of wake levels during the following dark “active” period (Figures 2A,D). Indeed, susceptible mice showed increased time spent in NREM sleep on days SD1, SD3, and SD10 (Figure 2D). This effect was the consequence of an increase in the number of NREM sleep bouts, whose mean duration remained unchanged (Table 1). In parallel, SD markedly enhanced REM sleep amounts throughout the 10 SD sessions reaching a +150% increase on SD1 relative to BL (Figure 2D). This enhancement was due to an increase in the number of REM sleep bouts, with no modification in their mean duration (Table 1). Concomitantly, susceptible mice showed a decrease in wake amounts during the “active” dark period that follows the SD sessions (Figure 2D). Interestingly, the nocturnal wake architecture was also impacted by stress. On SD1, the number of wake bouts increased by +55% whereas their mean duration decreased by −52%. This inability to maintain long wake bouts is indicative of wake instability. As for its diurnal consequences, nocturnal sleep changes (namely sleep rebound and wake instability) were maintained throughout the 10-day CSDS (Figure 2D).
Taken together, these results show that SD sessions induced a powerful disruption of sleep in susceptible mice including: (1) immediate marked arousal and a concomitant drop in REM sleep amounts during the “resting” light period and (2) delayed increase in sleep and wake instability lasting the entire following “active” dark period. These alterations of sleep-wake states were observed from the first to the tenth SD session indicating that stress-induced sleep changes were preserved throughout 10-day CSDS.
SD induced EEG power spectrum changes during NREM sleep all along the 10-day CSDS in susceptible mice
Sleep quality, as revealed by elevated delta power during NREM sleep, depends on the duration of previous wake but also on its nature (Meerlo and Turek, 2001). We therefore analyzed EEG power density during NREM sleep on days SD1, SD3, and SD10 of the 10-day CSDS relative to BL (Figure 3 and see Table S1 for statistics).
During the first 3 h period after the SD session, susceptible mice showed increased EEG activity in the delta range (0.5–4.99 Hz; Figures 3A,B). Conversely, SD consistently elicited a significant decrease in theta to low gamma power during NREM sleep (Figure 3B). Similar changes of a lesser extent were observed during the next 3 h period (Figure 3C). These diurnal modifications were maintained throughout 10-day CSDS (Figures 3B,C). During the dark period, EEG power spectra in defeated mice showed a mirror image with enhanced fast frequencies during NREM sleep on days SD1, SD3, and SD10 (Figure 3D). Thus, alpha power was significantly enhanced on all days tested (Figure 3D). Increased EEG activity was also observed in the beta and low gamma bands but these differences were significant only on days SD1 and SD3, respectively (Figure 3D).
Altogether, these results show that SD produced biphasic changes in quantitative EEG power spectra during NREM sleep in susceptible mice. During the light phase, delta activity was enhanced after the SD sessions suggesting that stress-induced arousal first challenged the sleep homeostatic process. During the subsequent dark phase, defeated mice showed increased high-frequency EEG activity that was mainly observed in the alpha band. As described for sleep-wake states, SD-induced changes in EEG power spectra were, for the most part, maintained throughout the 10-day CSDS.
SD induced sleep and EEG power spectrum changes throughout the 10-day CSDS in unsusceptible mice
The CSDS paradigm makes possible to examine variability in behavioral responses. Beside susceptible individuals, 10-day CSDS produces mice that fail to develop social avoidance, i.e., the unsusceptible mice. To test whether these mice show specific stress-induced sleep changes, we evaluated vigilance states and EEG power density in unsusceptible mice throughout the CSDS. As sleep changes were similar all along the 10-day protocol in susceptible mice, we restricted our analysis to days SD1 and SD10 (Figure 4 and Table 2). The corresponding statistical analysis is described in Table S1.
Table 2.
Number of bouts | Bouts mean duration (min) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | SD1 | SD10 | Baseline | SD1 | SD10 | ||
WAKE | 0–3 h | 25 ± 2 | 30 ± 2 | 22 ± 4 | 2.1 ± 0.3 | 2.7 ± 0.2 | 4.8 ± 1.1 |
Dark | 54 ± 3 | 94 ± 6** | 81 ± 5 | 9.4 ± 0.6 | 4.2 ± 0.6* | 4.9 ± 0.4 | |
NREMS | 0–3 h | 25 ± 2 | 30 ± 2 | 23 ± 4 | 5.0 ± 0.4 | 3.4 ± 0.3* | 4.8 ± 0.6 |
Dark | 52 ± 3 | 93 ± 7** | 80 ± 5 | 4.8 ± 0.3 | 3.4 ± 0.2 | 3.9 ± 0.3 | |
REMS | 0–3 h | 10 ± 1 | 2 ± 1*** | 3 ± 1* | 1.2 ± 0.1 | 0.7 ± 0.1* | 1.0 ± 0.2 |
Dark | 14 ± 3 | 25 ± 4** | 27 ± 3 | 1.1 ± 0.1 | 1.3 ± 0.1 | 1.2 ± 0.1 |
The data are given as means ± S.E.M (n = 7). For multiple group comparisons:
P < 0.05,
P < 0.01, and
P < 0.001 compared to BL. The statistical analysis is described in Table S1.
On days SD1 and SD10, unsusceptible mice displayed enhanced wake and decreased REM sleep amounts during the first 3 h following the SD sessions (Figures 4A,B). REM sleep reduction was accounted for by a decrease in the number of REM sleep bouts on all day tested and a decrease in their mean duration on SD1 (Table 2). Concomitantly, delta activity was significantly increased during NREM sleep on SD1 (Figure 4C). Despite a trend toward enhancement, delta activity remains statistically unchanged on SD10. Conversely, a significant decrease in alpha to beta power during NREM sleep was observed during this 3-h period on days SD1 and SD10 (Figure 4C). During the following dark period, SD elicited an increase in both NREM and REM sleep amounts for all day tested (Figure 4D). In addition to sleep enhancement, wake instability was observed on SD1 as shown by an increase in the number of wake bouts and a decrease in their mean duration (Table 2). Similar, but not significant, trends were observed on SD10 (Table 2).
Altogether, these data show that unsusceptible mice exhibit responses to SD that resemble those observed in susceptible mice including on a short-term basis: wake enhancement, reduced REM sleep and increased delta activity during NREM sleep. During the following dark period, SD promoted sleep and disrupted wake stability.
Long-lasting sleep and EEG power spectrum changes after CSDS in susceptible and unsusceptible mice
We next investigated the effects of the CSDS paradigm on vigilance states on the 5th recovery day (R5) when susceptible mice have developed long-lasting social avoidance (Figure 1C).
On R5, CSDS had limited effects on sleep-wake patterns (Figure 5). Susceptible mice exhibited enhanced wake amounts (+47%; Figure 5A1) together with reduced REM sleep amounts (−27%; Figure 5A3) over the first 3 h of the light period (7–10 a.m.). Despite a trend toward reduction, NREM sleep amounts were not significantly affected during this period (Figure 5A2; for statistics, see Table S1). Analysis of the following 3-h time windows revealed that vigilance states after the CSDS paradigm were not significantly different from those recorded in BL conditions (Figure 5A). In contrast, 10-day CSDS did not affect vigilance states in unsusceptible mice. Thus, wake, NREM sleep and REM sleep amounts remained unchanged across the light/dark cycle on R5 compared with BL (Figures 5B1–B3).
To examine whether 10-day CSDS might disrupt sleep quality, we next performed EEG spectral analysis within NREM sleep and REM sleep in susceptible and unsusceptible mice. In susceptible mice, during NREM sleep, beta to low gamma activity was enhanced during the “active” dark period, while delta power was decreased on R5 (Figure 6A1). Similar trends were observed during the light period with a significant increase in beta rhythms during NREM sleep (Figure 6A1). Minor changes were observed during REM sleep with significant enhanced alpha and low gamma rhythms during the light and the dark phases, respectively (Figure 6B2). However, these changes were detected while the absolute EEG power was globally decreased in both NREM sleep (BL: 3.67 ± 0.24 and R5: 3.32 ± 0.30 in μV2; two-tailed paired t-test: p < 0.05) and REM sleep (BL: 2.12 ± 0.16 and R5: 2.00 ± 0.17 in μV2; two-tailed paired t-test: p < 0.05). In unsusceptible mice, long-term EEG power spectrum changes included reduced delta activity and enhanced theta to beta activity during NREM sleep (Figure 5B1). These changes were mainly observed during the dark “active” phase. Despite a trend toward enhancement, low gamma rhythms were not statistically different on R5 in unsusceptible mice (Figure 5B1). During REM sleep, EEG power spectrum remained unchanged on R5 compared to BL in unsusceptible mice (Figure 5B2). These modifications occur while the absolute EEG power was globally decreased by ~10% in both NREM sleep and REM sleep (two-tailed paired t-test; p < 0.05).
Altogether, our data show that the CSDS has discrete long-lasting consequences on sleep-wake patterns in susceptible mice including: (1) anticipatory arousal (enhanced wake) at the time corresponding to the SD session after cessation of the stress paradigm and (2) hallmarks of decreased sleep quality comprising high frequency activity during sleep. Unsusceptible mice showed no change in sleep-wake states but decreased sleep quality as evidenced by enhanced high frequency activity during NREM sleep on recovery.
Discussion
The present study examined sleep-wake patterns and sleep architecture of mice exposed to CSDS. This model of stress-related disorders has a good face, construct, and pharmacological validity, reproducing in susceptible mice stress-induced social avoidance a behavioral trait commonly found in patients with depression, social phobia and PTSD. Our protocol was designed to analyze sleep-wake patterns and EEG power spectra throughout the 10-day CSDS and during recovery when susceptible mice show social avoidance. Exposure to acute SD stress during the sleep phase produced on defeated mice of both the susceptible and the unsusceptible subgroups a temporally dual effect on sleep that initially involved sustained wake and sleep loss with suppression of REM sleep. These changes were associated with an increase in sleep depth as revealed by higher low-frequency high-amplitude activity (delta power) during NREM sleep. During the following active phase, defeated mice displayed an increase in total sleep time. These overall sleep changes were maintained over the course of 10-day CSDS suggesting that sleep remains tightly regulated during repetition of stress. The majority of these alterations were normalized after cessation of the stress procedure. However, some sleep disturbances analogous to symptoms in patients with mood disorders emerged during recovery. Susceptible and unsusceptible mice display reduced delta power and augmentation of EEG high frequency activity during NREM sleep reminiscent of the EEG features found in insomniacs. Additionally, several days after cessation of the stress paradigm, we observed an anticipatory threat reaction characterized by an enhanced wake that was only present in susceptible mice. This anticipatory wake evokes the excessive anticipatory responses to future threat uncertainty commonly associated with anxiety.
Preserved sleep-wake changes all along the chronic stress protocol
We observed a specific time course for sleep adaptations in response to acute SD stress that was maintained all along the 10-SD sessions of the CSDS. Acute SD stress first exerted a potent wake-enhancing action that lasted for several hours. This type of stress-induced arousal has been classically associated with activation of the autonomic and neuroendocrine systems that enables the organism to face threatening events (Dampney, 2015) and shown to involve wake promoting neurons, including hypocretin (España et al., 2003; Nollet et al., 2011), and norepinephrine (Ceccatelli et al., 1989) neurons. We found that acute SD stress also engaged the homeostatic regulation of sleep as shown by higher delta activity during NREM sleep which could be the consequence of sustained wake. This homeostatic regulation of sleep consists in a compensatory increase in sleep depth after prolonged wake reflected by delta activity, (Borbély, 1982, 2001; Achermann et al., 1993). Accordingly, delta activity peaks at sleep onset and is enhanced after sleep deprivation (Franken et al., 1991; Huber et al., 2000). Our result showing higher delta activity during NREM sleep after acute SD stress is consistent with findings in rats suggesting that homeostatic sleep regulation depends on both the duration and the intensity of prior wake (Meerlo and Turek, 2001; Kamphuis et al., 2015). In particular, sleep deprivation caused by an aggressive interaction had a more powerful impact on delta activity than the one induced by gentle handling (Meerlo and Turek, 2001). More importantly, we found that enhanced sleep depth after acute SD stress was maintained over the course of 10-day CSDS. This observation confirms that the homeostatic mechanisms of sleep regulation remain intact in the context of repeated challenges, as the case occurs during chronic sleep fragmentation (Baud et al., 2015) and chronic sleep restriction (Leemburg et al., 2010). This preserved regulation suggests that NREM sleep may play a fundamental role in the course of recovery, potentially by providing an optimal physiological state for clearing metabolites that have been accumulated during wake (Xie et al., 2013) and regulating extracellular ion homeostasis (Ding et al., 2016). Whether these restorative processes are engaged after the SD stress remains to be established.
In addition to NREM sleep alterations, our results show specific REM sleep changes throughout 10-day CSDS. First, wake enhancement after acute SD stress mainly occurred at the expense of REM sleep amounts. Following initial wake enhancement, there was a sleep rebound during the active period characterized by a large increase in REM sleep. Analogous adaptive sleep regulation has been reported in response to a large variety of stressors (for reviews see Pawlyk et al., 2008; Suchecki et al., 2012). Confrontation with an aggressive conspecific in rats (Meerlo and Turek, 2001), acute restraint stress in mice (Meerlo et al., 2001; Rachalski et al., 2009) and rats (Rampin et al., 1991), footshocks (Pawlyk et al., 2005), brief presentation of ether (Bodosi et al., 2000), or combination of these stressors (Nedelcovych et al., 2015) elicited a similar biphasic sleep response. As found for NREM sleep, these stress-induced REM sleep changes were maintained all along CSDS, indicating the preservation of the adaptive response in spite of stress repetition. A recent study also reported enhanced REM sleep amounts throughout the CSDS paradigm in mice (Wells et al., 2017). However, in contrast with our findings, the REM sleep rebound appeared progressively (Wells et al., 2017). This discrepancy might be due to longer daily bouts of SD (10 vs. 5 min in the present study) and longer protected sensory contact with the aggressor (continuous vs. 20 min in the present study) than in ours. Indeed, the length of the stressor appears to determine the magnitude of the REM sleep rebound, with longer stress inducing smaller REM sleep rebound (Marinesco et al., 1999). Another difference with the study of Wells et al. (2017) is that our protocol is sufficient to disrupt social behavior (see results) as previously shown by others (Challis et al., 2013, 2014). As for NREM sleep, we hypothesize that recurrent REM sleep rebound is part of the processes helping the organism to cope with stress. REM sleep has been proposed to play a fundamental role in the adaptive mechanisms induced by aversive experiences. It is believed to promote emotional memory consolidation and to weaken the emotional content related to a traumatic event (“Sleep to forget and sleep to remember” model, Goldstein and Walker, 2014). Thus, dysregulation of this emotional memory processing could be a core mechanism responsible for PTSD. In line with this model, victims of traumatic events who exhibit more consolidated REM sleep episodes after the trauma do not develop PTSD (Mellman et al., 2007). Further studies should establish the potential role of stress-induced REM sleep rebound in minimizing the negative impact of CSDS.
Overall, our results provide support for the tight relationship between sleep and stress. Importantly, acute SD-induced sleep changes are independent of the future stress outcomes, i.e., the social aversion, as susceptible and unsusceptible mice show similar sleep changes across the 10-day CSDS. Hence, sleep alterations during the CSDS protocol are not predictive of stress vulnerability but may rather help the organism to cope with stress. We thus propose that both NREM and REM sleep underlie restorative processes to face aversive events.
Long-lasting sleep changes reminiscent of stress-related disorders
Initial enhancement of slow wave (delta) activity during NREM sleep and secondary sleep rebound can be considered markers of the ongoing recovery processes triggered by SD stress. We found, in contrast, sleep alterations during recovery that might underlie the long-lasting deleterious effects of chronic stress. Notably, we show that both susceptible and unsusceptible mice display reduced delta activity and enhanced power in higher frequency bands during recovery, a phenotype that emerged after the 10-day CSDS. This altered delta activity during NREM sleep is in line with a recent study showing disrupted sleep homeostasis in response to sleep deprivation in chronically defeated mice (Olini et al., 2017). Collectively, these data position delta activity as a central player in stress responses. Importantly, these EEG features during NREM sleep are reminiscent of primary insomnia. Indeed, EEG spectral characteristics in chronic insomniacs show augmented high-frequency EEG activity (Freedman, 1986; Merica et al., 1998; Perlis et al., 2001; Riedner et al., 2016). Increased power of rapid EEG bands is characteristic of attentional and cognitive processing engaged during wake, and, consequently, their presence during NREM sleep is a sign of persistent activity of the wake systems confirmed by neuroimaging studies in insomniacs (Nofzinger et al., 2004). This heightened wake-like EEG activation is believed to reflect hyperarousal of the brain during sleep and represent the core symptom of chronic insomnia. Interestingly, unsusceptible mice show additional increase in EEG activity in the theta and alpha bands during NREM sleep. A clinical study has reported that patients with primary insomnia exhibit more theta and alpha power during NREM sleep than those with major depression (Perlis et al., 2001). Together, these findings suggest that differences in the EEG activity can help discriminate individuals in light of their stress susceptibility. In summary, our results indicate that CSDS markedly affects NREM sleep, with detrimental effects leading to an insomniac-like pattern in mice. As insomnia is highly prevalent in mood disorders, our results further validate the CSDS paradigm to investigate the mechanisms of these pathologies.
Interestingly, we also found that only susceptible mice anticipated the stress session even after cessation of the CSDS paradigm. Five days after the end of the CSDS paradigm, we found an anticipatory arousal characterized by increment of wake at the time corresponding to the SD session (7–10 a.m.). A comparable trend was observed 3 weeks after cessation of the stress paradigm (data not shown). Anticipation of arousal is classically described under food entrainment in rodents (Mistlberger, 2011; Castro-Faúndez et al., 2016). It usually emerges after a few days of scheduled feeding and depends on food-entrainable oscillators. Our stress paradigm elicited similar temporal sequence as a result of the repeated stress schedule throughout the 10-day CSDS for the purpose of the sleep analysis. Interestingly, excessive anticipatory reactions to potential threat are a common feature across anxiety disorders (Grupe and Nitschke, 2013). Absence of such anticipatory response in unsusceptible mice might reveal physiological adaptations that contribute to coping mechanisms. Conversely, in susceptible mice, anticipatory wake along with long-lasting social aversion, insomniac-like profile, and anxiety-like behavior may offer a more complete picture of the detrimental consequences of chronic stress, paralleling those found in anxiety disorders.
To conclude, the results presented here show that temporal organization of vigilance states is disrupted throughout and following 10-day CSDS. We propose that sleep alterations occurring all along the course of CSDS might participate in the recovery processes to cope with stress or the processing of emotional memory without impeding negative stress outcomes. These long-term deleterious consequences of CSDS in the most vulnerable individuals include hyperarousal during NREM sleep reported in primary insomnia and stress-anticipated arousal found in anxious patients. The CSDS model thus provides significant sleep similarities with stress-related diseases in humans and position sleep as a major player regulating stress consequences.
Authors contributions
FH, VV, SEM, and VF designed research; FH, VV, and VF performed the experiments and analyzed the data. All authors contributed to finalize the manuscript for submission for publication.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Stephanie Daumas for helpful discussion on statistical analysis, Sophie Gautron and Joëlle Adrien for critical comments on the manuscript. We are grateful to Odile Poirel for laboratory managing.
Footnotes
Funding. This research was supported by funds from Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Université Pierre et Marie Curie (UPMC), and Fédération pour la Recherche sur le Cerveau (FRC). FH was supported by a grant from Laboratoire d'Excellence de Biologie pour la Psychiatrie, Bio-Psy. These founders had no rule in the study design, data collection, and analysis, decision to publish, and preparation of the manuscript.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00227/full#supplementary-material
References
- Achermann P., Dijk D. J., Brunner D. P., Borbély A. A. (1993). A model of human sleep homeostasis based on EEG slow-wave activity: quantitative comparison of data and simulations. Brain Res. Bull. 31, 97–113. 10.1016/0361-9230(93)90016-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexandre C., Dordal A., Aixendri R., Guzman A., Hamon M., Adrien J. (2008). Sleep-stabilizing effects of E-6199, compared to zopiclone, zolpidem and THIP in mice. Sleep 31, 259–270. 10.1093/sleep/31.2.259 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armitage R. (2007). Sleep and circadian rhythms in mood disorders. Acta. Psychiatr. Scand. Suppl. 433, 104–115. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00968.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baglioni C., Nanovska S., Regen W., Spiegelhalder K., Feige B., Nissen C., et al. (2016). Sleep and mental disorders: a meta-analysis of polysomnographic research. Psychol. Bull. 142, 969–990. 10.1037/bul0000053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baud M. O., Magistretti P. J., Petit J. M. (2015). Sustained sleep fragmentation induces sleep homeostasis in mice. Sleep 38, 567–579. 10.5665/sleep.4572 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benca R. M., Obermeyer W. H., Thisted R. A., Gillin J. C. (1992). Sleep and psychiatric disorders. A meta-analysis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 651–670. 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080059010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benca R. M., Okawa M., Uchiyama M., Ozaki S., Nakajima T., Shibui K., et al. (1997). Sleep and mood disorders. Sleep Med. Rev. 1, 45–56. 10.1016/S1087-0792(97)90005-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berton O., McClung C. A., Dileone R. J., Krishnan V., Renthal W., Russo S. J., et al. (2006). Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science 311, 864–868. 10.1126/science.1120972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Björkqvist K. (2001). Social defeat as a stressor in humans. Physiol. Behav. 73, 435–442. 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00490-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodosi B., Obál F., Gardi J., Komlódi J., Fang J., Krueger J. M. (2000). An ether stressor increases REM sleep in rats: possible role of prolactin. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 279, R1590–R1598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borbély A. A. (1982). A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1, 195–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borbély A. A. (2001). From slow waves to sleep homeostasis: new perspectives. Arch. Ital. Biol. 139, 53–61. 10.4449/aib.v139i1.204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boutrel B., Franc B., Hen R., Hamon M., Adrien J. (1999). Key role of 5-HT1B receptors in the regulation of paradoxical sleep as evidenced in 5-HT1B knock-out mice. J. Neurosci. 19, 3204–3212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buysse D., Germain A., Nofzinger E., Kupfer D. (2006). Mood disorders and sleep, in The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Mood Disorders, eds Stein D. J., Kupfer D. J., Schatzberg A. F. (Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; ), 717–737. [Google Scholar]
- Castro-Faúndez J., Díaz J., Ocampo-Garcés A. (2016). Temporal organization of the sleep-wake cycle under food entrainment in the rat. Sleep 39, 1451–1465. 10.5665/sleep.5982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ceccatelli S., Villar M. J., Goldstein M., Hökfelt T. (1989). Expression of c-Fos immunoreactivity in transmitter-characterized neurons after stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86, 9569–9573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Challis C., Beck S. G., Berton O. (2014). Optogenetic modulation of descending prefrontocortical inputs to the dorsal raphe bidirectionally bias socioaffective choices after social defeat. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:43. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Challis C., Boulden J., Veerakumar A., Espallergues J., Vassoler F. M., Pierce R. C., et al. (2013). Raphe GABAergic neurons mediate the acquisition of avoidance after social defeat. J. Neurosci. 33, 13978–13988. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2383-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dampney R. A. (2015). Central mechanisms regulating coordinated cardiovascular and respiratory function during stress and arousal. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 309, R429–R443. 10.1152/ajpregu.00051.2015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding F., O'Donnell J., Xu Q., Kang N., Goldman N., Nedergaard M. (2016). Changes in the composition of brain interstitial ions control the sleep-wake cycle. Science 352, 550–555. 10.1126/science.aad4821 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- España R. A., Valentino R. J., Berridge C. W. (2003). Fos immunoreactivity in hypocretin-synthesizing and hypocretin-1 receptor-expressing neurons: effects of diurnal and nocturnal spontaneous waking, stress and hypocretin-1 administration. Neuroscience 121, 201–217. 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00334-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken P., Dijk D. J., Tobler I., Borbély A. A. (1991). Sleep deprivation in rats: effects on EEG power spectra, vigilance states, and cortical temperature. Am. J. Physiol. 261, R198–R208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin K. B. J., Paxinos G. (2008). The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Franklin T. B., Saab B. J., Mansuy I. M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of stress resilience and vulnerability. Neuron 75, 747–761. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman R. R. (1986). EEG power spectra in sleep-onset insomnia. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 63, 408–413. 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90122-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Germain A. (2013). Sleep disturbances as the hallmark of PTSD: where are we now? Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 372–382. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein A. N., Walker M. P. (2014). The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 679–708. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grupe D. W., Nitschke J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 488–501. 10.1038/nrn3524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammels C., Pishva E., De Vry J., van den Hove D. L., Prickaerts J., van Winkel R., et al. (2015). Defeat stress in rodents: from behavior to molecules. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 59, 111–140. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huber R., Deboer T., Tobler I. (2000). Effects of sleep deprivation on sleep and sleep EEG in three mouse strains: empirical data and simulations. Brain Res. 857, 8–19. 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)02248-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamphuis J., Lancel M., Koolhaas J. M., Meerlo P. (2015). Deep sleep after social stress: NREM sleep slow-wave activity is enhanced in both winners and losers of a conflict. Brain Behav. Immun. 47, 149–154. 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.12.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi I., Boarts J. M., Delahanty D. L. (2007). Polysomnographically measured sleep abnormalities in PTSD: a meta-analytic review. Psychophysiology 44, 660–669. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.537.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koolhaas J. M., Meerlo P., De Boer S. F., Strubbe J. H., Bohus B. (1997). The temporal dynamics of the stress response. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 21, 775–782. 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00057-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan V., Han M. H., Graham D. L., Berton O., Renthal W., Russo S. J., et al. (2007). Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell 131, 391–404. 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leemburg S., Vyazovskiy V. V., Olcese U., Bassetti C. L., Tononi G., Cirelli C. (2010). Sleep homeostasis in the rat is preserved during chronic sleep restriction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 15939–15944. 10.1073/pnas.1002570107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu R. T., Alloy L. B. (2010). Stress generation in depression: a systematic review of the empirical literature and recommendations for future study. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 582–593. 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marinesco S., Bonnet C., Cespuglio R. (1999). Influence of stress duration on the sleep rebound induced by immobilization in the rat: a possible role for corticosterone. Neuroscience 92, 921–933. 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00045-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meerlo P., Easton A., Bergmann B. M., Turek F. W. (2001). Restraint increases prolactin and REM sleep in C57BL/6J mice but not in BALB/cJ mice. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 281, R846–R854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meerlo P., Turek F. W. (2001). Effects of social stimuli on sleep in mice: non-rapid-eye-movement (NREM) sleep is promoted by aggressive interaction but not by sexual interaction. Brain Res. 907, 84–92. 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02603-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mellman T. A., Kulick-Bell R., Ashlock L. E., Nolan B. (1995). Sleep events among veterans with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 110–115. 10.1176/ajp.152.1.110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mellman T. A., Pigeon W. R., Nowell P. D., Nolan B. (2007). Relationships between REM sleep findings and PTSD symptoms during the early aftermath of trauma. J. Trauma. Stress 20, 893–901. 10.1002/jts.20246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merica H., Blois R., Gaillard J. M. (1998). Spectral characteristics of sleep EEG in chronic insomnia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 1826–1834. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00189.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mistlberger R. E. (2011). Neurobiology of food anticipatory circadian rhythms. Physiol. Behav. 104, 535–545. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nedelcovych M. T., Gould R. W., Zhan X., Bubser M., Gong X., Grannan M., et al. (2015). A rodent model of traumatic stress induces lasting sleep and quantitative electroencephalographic disturbances. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 6, 485–493. 10.1021/cn500342u [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nofzinger E. A., Buysse D. J., Germain A., Price J. C., Miewald J. M., Kupfer D. J. (2004). Functional neuroimaging evidence for hyperarousal in insomnia. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 2126–2128. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.2126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nollet M., Gaillard P., Minier F., Tanti A., Belzung C., Leman S. (2011). Activation of orexin neurons in dorsomedial/perifornical hypothalamus and antidepressant reversal in a rodent model of depression. Neuropharmacology 61, 336–346. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olini N., Rothfuchs I., Azzinnari D., Pryce C. R., Kurth S., Huber R. (2017). Chronic social stress leads to altered sleep homeostasis in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 327, 167–173. 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.03.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pawlyk A. C., Jha S. K., Brennan F. X., Morrison A. R., Ross R. J. (2005). A rodent model of sleep disturbances in posttraumatic stress disorder: the role of context after fear conditioning. Biol. Psychiatry. 57, 268–277. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pawlyk A. C., Morrison A. R., Ross R. J., Brennan F. X. (2008). Stress-induced changes in sleep in rodents: models and mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 99–117. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlis M. L., Smith M. T., Andrews P. J., Orff H., Giles D. E. (2001). Beta/Gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep 24, 110–117. 10.1093/sleep/24.1.110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillai V., Kalmbach D. A., Ciesla J. A. (2011). A meta-analysis of electroencephalographic sleep in depression: evidence for genetic biomarkers. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 912–919. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillar G., Malhotra A., Lavie P. (2000). Post-traumatic stress disorder and sleep-what a nightmare! Sleep Med. Rev. 4, 183–200. 10.1053/smrv.1999.0095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachalski A., Alexandre C., Bernard J. F., Saurini F., Lesch K. P., Hamon M., et al. (2009). Altered sleep homeostasis after restraint stress in 5-HTT knock-out male mice: a role for hypocretins. J. Neurosci. 29, 15575–15585. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3138-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rampin C., Cespuglio R., Chastrette N., Jouvet M. (1991). Immobilisation stress induces a paradoxical sleep rebound in rat. Neurosci. Lett. 126, 113–118. 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90532-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds C. F., Kupfer D. J. (1987). Sleep research in affective illness: state of the art circa 1987. Sleep 10, 199–215. 10.1093/sleep/10.3.199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riedner B. A., Goldstein M. R., Plante D. T., Rumble M. E., Ferrarelli F., Tononi G., et al. (2016). Regional patterns of elevated alpha and high-frequency electroencephalographic activity during nonrapid eye movement sleep in chronic insomnia: a pilot study. Sleep 39, 801–812. 10.5665/sleep.5632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross R. J., Ball W. A., Dinges D. F., Kribbs N. B., Morrison A. R., Silver S. M., et al. (1994). Rapid eye movement sleep disturbance in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 195–202. 10.1016/0006-3223(94)91152-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross R. J., Ball W. A., Sullivan K. A., Caroff S. N. (1989). Sleep disturbance as the hallmark of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 146, 697–707. 10.1176/ajp.146.1.128-a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt M. V., Scharf S. H., Liebl C., Harbich D., Mayer B., Holsboer F., et al. (2010). A novel chronic social stress paradigm in female mice. Horm. Behav. 57, 415–420. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slattery D. A., Cryan J. F. (2014). The ups and downs of modelling mood disorders in rodents. ILAR J. 55, 297–309. 10.1093/ilar/ilu026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiger A., Kimura M. (2010). Wake and sleep EEG provide biomarkers in depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 242–252. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suchecki D., Tiba P. A., Machado R. B. (2012). REM sleep rebound as an adaptive response to stressful situations. Front. Neurol. 3:41. 10.3389/fneur.2012.00041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tornatzky W., Miczek K. A. (1993). Long-term impairment of autonomic circadian rhythms after brief intermittent social stress. Physiol. Behav. 53, 983–993. 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90278-N [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trautmann S., Rehm J., Wittchen H. U. (2016). The economic costs of mental disorders: do our societies react appropriately to the burden of mental disorders? EMBO Rep. 17, 1245–1249. 10.15252/embr.201642951 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vialou V., Bagot R. C., Cahill M. E., Ferguson D., Robison A. J., Dietz D. M., et al. (2014). Prefrontal cortical circuit for depression- and anxiety-related behaviors mediated by cholecystokinin: role of ΔFosB. J. Neurosci. 34, 3878–3887. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1787-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vialou V., Thibault M., Kaska S., Cooper S., Gajewski P., Eagle A., et al. (2015). Differential induction of FosB isoforms throughout the brain by fluoxetine and chronic stress. Neuropharmacology 99, 28–37. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.07.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wells A. M., Ridener E., Bourbonais C. A., Kim W., Pantazopoulos H., Carroll F. I., et al. (2017). Effects of chronic social defeat stress on sleep and circadian rhythms are mitigated by kappa-opioid receptor antagonism. J. Neurosci. 37, 7656–7668. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0885-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteford H. A., Degenhardt L., Rehm J., Baxter A. J., Ferrari A. J., Erskine H. E., et al. (2013). Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 382, 1575–1586. 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61611-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xie L., Kang H., Xu Q., Chen M. J., Liao Y., Thiyagarajan M., et al. (2013). Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science 342, 373–377. 10.1126/science.1241224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yehuda R., Schmeidler J., Wainberg M., Binder-Brynes K., Duvdevani T. (1998). Vulnerability to posttraumatic stress disorder in adult offspring of Holocaust survivors. Am. J. Psychiatry 155, 1163–1171. 10.1176/ajp.155.9.1163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.