Abstract
Using 15 years of data from a stable population of wild Eurasian beavers (Castor fiber), we examine how annual and lifetime access to food resources affect individual age-related changes in reproduction and somatic condition. We found an age-related decline in annual maternal reproductive output, after a peak at age 5–6. Rainfall, an established negative proxy of annual resource availability for beavers, was consistently associated with lower reproductive output for females of all ages. In contrast, breeding territory quality, as a measure of local resource history over reproductive lifetimes, caused differences in individual patterns of reproductive senescence; animals from lower quality territories senesced when younger. Litter size was unrelated to maternal age, although adult body weight increased with age. In terms of resource effects, in poorer years but not in better years, older mothers produced larger offspring than did younger mothers, giving support to the constraint theory. Overall, our findings exemplify state-dependent life-history strategies, supporting an effect of resources on reproductive senescence, where cumulative differences in resource access, and not just reproductive strategy, mediate long-term reproductive trade-offs, consistent with the disposable soma and reproductive restraint theories. We propose that flexible life-history schedules could play a role in the dynamics of populations exhibiting reproductive skew, with earlier breeding opportunities leading to an earlier senescence schedule through resource dependent mechanisms.
Introduction
Senescent declines in reproductive success, somatic condition and cohort survival rate [1,2] influence the schedule of lifetime breeding success [3–7] in mammal species. A variety of theories have been conceived to explain the mechanism of senescence [8–14], many of which are based on the observation that because the cumulative risk of extrinsic mortality increases over time, the force of selection reduces with increasing age [9,10] as future reproductive value declines. In addition, natural selection is hypothesised to mould senescence synchronously across traits [9], but see [7]. In this study, we examine the effects of resource availability on senescence in reproductive and somatic traits in relation to four theories that have been proposed to explain the prevalence of senescence, or commonly observed changes in reproduction, somatic condition and survival with age: the disposable soma theory [11–13]; the reproductive restraint theory [13]; the constraints theory [15–17]; and the selection theory [16].
In wild-living populations, subject to natural selection, the annual and life-time availability of environmental resources can ‘stress’ organisms [18], either directly through actual insufficient availability, or indirectly through physiological constraints on an organism’s capacity to acquire them sufficiently [19–21]. This leads to trade-offs in energy allocation, especially when food supply is limited, with a division between reproduction and investment in somatic maintenance and repair, to ensure continued survival (or ‘longevity assurance mechanisms’, [22]). Theories linked to resource-dependency include the ‘disposable soma’ theory ‘DS’ [11–13] and ‘reproductive restraint’ theory ‘RR’ [13]. DS posits that the trade-off between energy allocated to reproduction and investment in somatic maintenance and repair leads to deterioration in the organism’s body with age [11]. RR posits that when extrinsic mortality risk relative to intrinsic mortality are low later in life reduced reproductive effort may arise in order to enhance survival probability and provide additional future breeding opportunities. In wild populations it can be difficult to disentangle the trade-off involved in these theories from genetically mediated age-related effects. Both DS and RR predict declines in female reproductive output with increasing age, while DS also predicts a decline in somatic (body) condition. To the extent that any trade-off occurs, individuals consistently exposed to poorer resource availability over their reproductive lifespan (their resource history)–for example because they live in habitat that is of lower quality than their conspecifics–will exhibit a concomitant reduction in investment in at least one of reproduction or somatic repair and maintenance. Therefore, an influence of individual resource histories on senescence is predicted from both DS and RR theories.
Conversely, lower reproductive success in early life may be explained by the constraint theory (‘CT’, [15–17], which predicts that poor physiological condition, or inexperience, can limit breeding in younger individuals [16]). Thus CT predicts lower early-life reproductive output and somatic condition.
Potentially pertinent too is that, at the population level, less fit individuals such as those less able to acquire resources are likely to die sooner, causing selection for superior phenotypes to inflate per capita reproductive rate in the surviving population; termed the selection theory (‘ST’, [16]). A cohort would thus become comprised by a greater proportion of fitter individuals over time [23–25]. Like CT, ST predicts higher late-life reproductive output and somatic condition. Unlike CT however, these patterns will only be detectable at the population level.
To investigate these potential resource-dependent effects on reproductive and somatic senescence we analysed 15 years of data from a wild Eurasian beaver (Castor fiber; henceforth ‘beaver’) population, collected in Telemark, Norway. The beaver provides an informative model species for examining senescence, being long-lived (up to 20 years in the wild; noting that in our study population, mean age at death for dominant adults was c. 7 years [26]). Beavers are also highly territorial, where territory borders can remain stable for several years [27], and exhibit changes in breeding success with age [28]. In terms of dietary resources, beavers are large (> 20 kg), herbivorous, semi-aquatic rodents [29,30] that forage predominantly on deciduous trees, consuming leaves, twigs and bark [31,27]. Pertinent here is that previous research on this same study population has found that higher rainfall correlated with lower resource availability, due to trees near water level producing poorer forage in wetter years, due to water logging [31]. This resulted in lower body weights for adults, yearlings and offspring (kits), fewer kits weaned per breeding female, and lower weaned kit and yearling survival rates [26,31].
In terms of socio-spatial distribution and reproductive patterns, beavers are behaviourally monogamous, tending to remain faithful until a mate is displaced or dies [30,32] (but see [33]). Only the dominant pair in a family group breeds [27,29], creating reproductive skew (sensu [34]). Extended family groups can develop due to the retention of philopatric offspring [27,35], although during the study period groups rarely (<10% of occasions) exceeded five individuals (mean group size in this population = 3.42 ± 1.73 SD), although we recorded 11 individuals in one territory in one year. Allo-parenting thus has the potential to play some role in reproductive success, or offspring quality. Litters of 1–5 kits (although litters of 4 or more are unusual in the wild) are born around mid-May in Norway [27,36] and emerge from the natal den or lodge when weaned, one to two months later [29]. Sexual maturity is attained at age 1.5–2.5 [29], whereupon offspring may disperse to fill any available territory vacancies [27,37].
Here we examine changes in adult body condition in both sexes along with changes in reproductive success with age in females (breeding frequency and offspring number and quality). We also examine to what extent long-term resource (food) availability, mediated by territory quality, influences reproductive senescence relative to short-term resource availability, mediated by rainfall.
Materials and methods
Study area
The study site was centred on three rivers, the Straumen (N59.30, E9.09 to N59.91, E9.19), the Gvarv (N59.41, E 9.12 to N59.37, E9.20) and the Sauar (N59.47, E9.31 to N59.37, E9.25), in southern Norway, with one beaver family group per 2km of river length [27]. Riparian woodland bordered these rivers, even along agricultural stretches, dominated by grey alder (Alnus incana) and, to a lesser extent, willow (Salix sp.) and bird cherry (Prunus padus). The beaver population in the study area is open to immigration from similar contiguous riparian habitat. There is no reason to believe that the beavers outside the study area would be any different in terms of life-history strategy. Beavers have been in this area since 1920s and there were no substantial changes in population density over the study period [38]. For further details on the study area, see [31].
Beaver trapping and sampling
Between March and November from 1998 to 2012, beavers were live-trapped from a motor-boat using hand-nets [39]. Mean captures per year per individual varied from 1.7 to less than one. Captured beavers were restrained in cloth sacks, sexed by the colour of the anal gland secretion [40], weighed (to the nearest 200 g), and body length (cm) was measured along the spine from nose-tip to base of tail. Beavers were tagged with a microchip (Avid or Trovan) and marked with colour-plastic (Dalton) and metal (National Band and Tag Co.) ear-tag combinations. An animal was assumed to be resident if it was trapped or sighted in the same territory more than once >24h apart, or was seen interacting non-agonistically with other known residents [26]. We defined trapping effort per territory as the number of nights spent trapping along each river section (Straumen, Gvarv, upper Sauar and lower Sauar) in year n and n+1.
Age-class was assigned on first capture, based either on weight [38] (see also [30]) or previous trapping history (year 0 = kit, year 1 = yearling, year 2 = sub-adult and ≥ year 3 = adult, where ‘year’ ended on 31st Dec). Animals first trapped as kits or yearlings, and those sub-adults that could be aged with confidence, were assigned their actual age–referred to here as ‘known-age’ individuals [38]. Many of the individuals measured were adult when first trapped (e.g. 78% of individuals in the model of somatic senescence and 82% of dominant females in models of reproductive senescence), either because they were already adult at the beginning of the study, they evaded detection over their first three years, or they were recruited from outside the study area. Animals first trapped as adults were assumed to be three years of age (minimum age) and sub-adults two years of age. Although including minimum age in addition to known-age individuals made it possible that the age at which senescence commenced could be underestimated, (but not over-estimated), this allowed us to include a larger sample of females. The probability of detecting senescence was therefore more stringent, permitting us to identify relationship minima with confidence; that is, any signal for senescence had to be stronger to be apparent. Only one dominant female occupies each territory, identifiable from greater body weight than other same sex group members and signs of lactation (nipple length > 0.5cm). These were assigned maternal status, based on their trapping and sighting history. Furthermore, females dispersing into a territory were assigned the dominant breeding position (generally corroborated by the disappearance of the previous incumbent). Without contrary evidence, we assumed individuals maintained their dominant status until they disappeared or died [26]. Since dominant adult beavers in the study area very rarely move out of their territory to breed elsewhere (see below), we assumed that disappearances arose from death of the individual. Of the 39 breeding females used here, 13 were still alive at the end of the study. Fifteen of the females held their dominant adult breeding positions at the start of observations on their territory, of which three were removed from the study area as part of an international reintroduction programme, 11 disappeared or died during the study, and one was alive at the end of the study, having been under observation for 14 years. Thus we observed the entire reproductive lifespan for the remaining 12 (31%) of the females, only the latter part of the reproductive lifespan for 11 (28%) and only the early part of the reproductive lifespan for 12 (31%).
Metrics of reproductive success were derived from the number of kits trapped per year plus the number of yearlings trapped the following year that were not trapped as kits previously (mean first year mortality 8%, [26]). Two-year olds (mean mortality age 1–2 28%, [26]) and unmarked offspring clearly resident in the territory were also included in reproduction metrics. Over the whole study, these comprised respectively 48%, 25%, 17% and 10% of all observed offspring. Offspring body metrics were taken from the first records of individuals per year, controlling for season to account for on-going growth. Sample sizes are provided along with each model, below.
Territory quality
Territory borders show little change between years and dominant adult beavers rarely change territory [27]; therefore, territory quality provided a consistent proxy of resource history for each reproductive female. Territory quality was defined as the total availability of deciduous saplings (principal food source) within each territory (S1 File). Based on this metric, we divided territories into four quality categories (TQ4): 1 (lowest quality) to 4 (highest quality). Since territory borders typically remain stable over time, only four of 39 females changed TQ4 over the study period, where one change was because the female moved territory and the other changes resulted from shifts in territory borders [27].
Rainfall variables
Higher rainfall has been established to correlate with lower resource availability to beavers, due to trees near water level being prone to water logging and thus producing poorer forage in wetter years [29]; therefore, we used rainfall as a proxy of annual variation in resources. Daily rainfall data, 1997 to 2012, were sourced from the Lifjell weather station (354m a.s.l., 59°455 N, 09°037 E) using Eklima (http://eklima.met.no/), which correlated well with rainfall measured on the study rivers [31]. To account for exceptionally high rainfall in July 2007 [31], we adjusted for spring-summer (Apr–Sept) run-off; this was unnecessary for autumn rainfall (Aug–Oct).
Theory and calculation
Studying wild aquatic rodents, able to retreat into lodges, is challenging. Therefore, studies of senescence on wild-living animals have largely been limited to more tractable ungulate species. Consequently analytical procedures had to be adapted to best utilise available data.
For analyses of annual reproduction, we excluded seven territories with less than four years of data. Due to stable monogamy, breeding males and females were the same (minimum) age in 75 of 132 (57%) instances, with only 11 (8%) instances, 1998–2008, where an adult of either sex ≥8 years was paired with a mate of ≤5 years. Due to the sexes aging in parallel, we focused on the influence of female age on reproductive success, whereas for analyses of adult body weight, we were able to examine the influence of age for both sexes. Relevant sample sizes are provided along with each model, below.
All statistical analyses were conducted in the R environment [41]. Unless otherwise stated, all linear mixed models (LMMs) and generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) were run using function lmer in the R package lme4 [42]. From each global model, we specified a subset of candidate models that included all possible combinations of fixed effects, retaining only essential variables in all candidate models (S1 File). Akaike Information Criterion, controlling for small sample sizes (AICc) was used to weight models within candidate sets in the R package MuMIn (v. 1.7.11, see [43]). We used Akaike weight based averaging, over all models within AICc = 4 of the single most supported (lowest AICc) model (the top model set), to calculate parameter estimates and associated 95% confidence intervals (CIs) [44], where the exclusion of zero inferred significance. Predicted values, with 95% prediction intervals calculated from the most supported models, were then used to explore significant interactions. Little or no overlap of prediction intervals inferred a significant effect. Global models were assessed for over-dispersion (Gaussian models) and homogeneity of variance (non-Gaussian models). Conditional R2 (R2c) values were calculated on global models [45] to asses overall fit of the model candidate set. Methods concerning the inclusion of variables in the global models are provided in S1 File, with descriptions of the variable codes in Table 1.
Table 1. Description of variables used in the global models.
Variable | Data type | Description |
---|---|---|
lnBW | ordinal | ln body weight (kg), offspring range -0.967–2.526, adult range 2.542–3.401 |
lnBL | ordinal | ln body length (cm), offspring range 0.478–4.220, adult range 4.241–4.489 |
year | categorical | year, 14 levels |
day | ordinal | day of year, range 75–325 |
age | ordinal | minimum age, range 3–14 (ages ≥ 14 were combined) |
sex | binomial | sex |
preg | binomial | female reproductive status (1 = pregnant, 0 = not pregnant) |
kage | binomial | age is minimum age (0) or known age (1) |
maxage | ordinal | maximum age at which an individual was measured, range 2–15 |
death | binomial | Individual died in the same year or the year following the last measurement of body weight and length |
TQ4 | ordinal | Territory quality, range 1–4 (see S1 File) |
RP | binomial | offspring detected post emergence (1) or not (0) each year |
RS | ordinal | number of offspring produced each year, range 0–4 |
mother | categorical | identity of mother, 39 levels |
mmage | ordinal | minimum maternal age, range 2–15 |
rain | ordinal | rainfall (mm) in the previous Aug–Oct, range 164–515 |
RPY | binomial | RP in the same territory in the previous year |
TE | ordinal | Trap effort in the current and previous year, range 2–51 |
mmage39 | ordinal | minimum maternal age (≥3 years) where ages ≥9 were combined into one age, range 3–9 |
seasonage | categorical | Offspring age: first year summer (Jul-Aug); first year autumn (Sept-Nov); second year spring (Mar-May); second summer (Jun-Aug), 4 levels |
LS | ordinal | Litter size (1, 2, ≥3 offspring) |
rainbin | binomial | total rain Apr-Sept in birth year: < 570mm (-1), ≥ 570mm (+1) |
Somatic senescence
We used data from 93 individuals (45 female and 48 male), each sampled on 2–8 occasions (mean = 3.29, thus 306 observations), to investigate the relationship between age and body weight (controlling for body length and time of year) for sexually mature, full-grown beavers (age ≥ 3 years); using a LMM, with the function lme in the R package nlme (v. 3.1–102, [46]). Of these, 20 (8 female and 12 male) were of known age and 35 (including 7 of known age) died in the same year, or the year following the last measurement of their body weight and length. We did not include our measure of resource history (TQ4) because this was either unknown or changed during the life of the majority of individuals. We included a random intercept for individual, with a continuous AR(1) correlation structure to account for repeated measures within individuals.
Our global model (Table 1; S1 File) was:
Factors influencing senescence in female reproductive output
We estimated annual female breeding success over 36 territories, 25 with TQ4 indices. We observed 65 litters totalling 106 offspring. Due to accession of the dominant breeding position, our analyses included 39 dominant (territory holding) females (minimum age 2–15) observed over 169 occasions. We included individuals where age was not known at the first observation in all models of reproductive output.
Reproduction per year (RP)
We used a GLMM, with a binomial error structure and a logit-link function, to investigate the probability that a dominant (territory holding) female reproduced, or not (binary response), in a given year (RP), noting potential for a quadratic (non-linear) relationship between RP and minimum maternal age. While variation in group sizes could modify the effects of territory quality (TQ4), group size within each territory was highly dependent on reproductive success in the previous year (RPY, S1 File) and was therefore not included in the model. Due to the smaller sample of breeding females compared with individuals used in the model of somatic senescence, we removed kage from the global model to avoid problems arising from a low ratio of sample size to predictors. We included random intercepts for mother. The global model was (Table 1; S1 File):
Reproductive success (RS)
To scrutinise the relationship between maternal age and reproductive output, we repeated this analysis, replacing the binary response of RP with the number of offspring produced by each dominant female per year (i.e., reproductive success, RS). Litter size ranged from 0–4, leading to a Poisson distribution and a log-link function in the model. The predictors included in the global model were otherwise identical, and we again included random intercepts for mother.
Litter size (LS)
To investigate the relationship between maternal age and litter size (LS), limited to the instances when females bred successfully, we constructed a GLMM with a Poisson error structure and a log-link function, specifying a random intercept for mother, and included TE as a control variable. Furthermore, we included measures of resources as used in models of RP and RS. The global model was therefore:
Where mmage39 is maternal minimum age class ranging 3–8 and ≥9 years (S1 File).
Offspring quality
Given that longer body-length beavers (BL) are more likely to achieve dominant breeding positions in this population [38], we assessed the effect of maternal age, litter size and resources (rainfall and territory quality) on (i) individual offspring body weight (BW, relative to body length and age–as an index of somatic condition, able to decrease as well as increase) and (ii) individual offspring BL (as an index of skeletal size, stabilising at maturity), using a LMM with a Gaussian error structure with an identity link function. These data were collected for 80 offspring (63 measured as kits and 17 as yearlings) in 53 litters from 26 mothers in 24 territories. We included a random intercept for mother. These global models included two-way interaction terms between maternal age and all other predictors except control variables (Table 1; S1 File):
And:
Ethics statement
The study, including all handling and tagging procedures (for details see above), was reviewed and approved by the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management (permit id 2012/1191, id 2008/14367, id 05/9639, id 04/9793, id 04/334, id 2002/6758, id 2001/7552, id 2002/841, id 2001/635, id 99/1880) and the Norwegian Experimental Animal Board (id 2579, id 2170, id742, id 2005/48612, id 2004/14671, id S-32/03 id S-168-01), which also granted permission to conduct fieldwork in our study area. The owners of the land gave permission to conduct the studies on their properties.
Results
Somatic senescence
We found no evidence for somatic senescence in beavers; instead, once fully grown, body weight (BW, controlling for body length and season) increased continuously with age, although the rate of increase abated (Table 2; Fig 1). Although males exhibited significantly lower BW than females, they did not show a different pattern of weight increase compared to females (non-significant negative age × sex interaction, Table 2; Fig 1; retained in 16 of the 21 most supported models (total Akaike weights = 0.493, Table 3). R2c for the global model was 0.559. The null model had a ΔAICc of +129.3. Although our sample size declined with age (mean 50 individuals per age class from minimum age 3–6 versus mean 12 individuals minimum age 10–13), the mean and SD of BW did not change markedly (21.7 ± 1.1 kg and 21.2 ± 1.1 kg in the 3–6 and 10–13 minimum age classes respectively).
Table 2. Model estimates of effects on adult body weight.
95% CI | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | lower | upper | t | ||
Intercept | -1.25 | -2.32 | -1.79 | -2.24 | * |
Day-of-year | 3.63E-04 | 2.21E-04 | 5.05E-04 | 5.10 | * |
lnBL | 0.982 | 0.739 | 1.226 | 7.94 | * |
Sex (male) | -5.72E-02 | -8.75E-02 | -2.70E-02 | -3.84 | * |
Known age | -3.26E-02 | -7.44E-02 | -0.93E-02 | -1.48 | * |
Age | 0.112 | -0.100 | 0.324 | 1.83 | |
Age2 | -3.79E-02 | -10.4E-02 | 2.80E-02 | -2.17 | |
Pregnant | 0.64E-02 | -1.83E-02 | 3.10E-02 | ||
Max age | 1.93E-03 | -4.61E-03 | 8.45E-03 | ||
Died | -0.89E-02 | -3.97E-02 | 2.19E-02 | ||
Age × sex (male) | -2.18E-02 | -6.02E-02 | 1.66E-02 | -2.03 | |
Age × known age | 0.154 | 0.025 | 0.282 | 2.96 | * |
Age × max age | -0.20E-02 | -2.39E-03 | 1.99E-02 | ||
Age2 × sex (male) | -0.06E-02 | -2.01E-02 | 1.88E-02 | ||
Age2 × known age | -2.47E-02 | -4.73E-02 | -0.21E-02 | -2.20 | * |
Age2 × max age | 4.28E-03 | -0.90E-03 | 9.45E-03 |
Model averaged estimates for LMM models describing ln body weight (kg) of beavers ≥3 years old. Estimates where the 95% confidence intervals do not include zero (indicating significance) are marked with an asterisk. Values of t are included from the single most supported model. Not all variables in the top model set were in this model.
Table 3. Model selection tables examining the effects on body weight, controlling for body length and time of year.
Inter-cept | age | age2 | kage | inBL | maxage | died | preg | sex | day | year | age× kage | age× maxage | age× sex | age2× kage | age2× maxage | age2× sex | AICc | ΔAICc | Akaike weight |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
-1.20 | 0.062 | -0.015 | -0.030 | 0.972 | + | 3.63E-04 | + | 0.171 | + | -2.53E-02 | -693.1 | 0.082 | |||||||
-1.25 | 0.234 | -0.079 | -0.031 | 0.985 | 1.13E-03 | + | 3.69E-04 | + | 0.166 | -9.30E-03 | + | -2.53E-02 | 4.37E-03 | -692.9 | 0.23 | 0.073 | |||
-1.33 | 0.047 | -0.027 | -0.032 | 0.997 | 3.03E-03 | + | 3.63E-04 | + | 0.160 | 6.73E-03 | + | -2.37E-02 | -692.3 | 0.82 | 0.054 | ||||
-1.23 | 0.052 | -0.015 | -0.030 | 0.979 | + | 3.64E-04 | + | 0.164 | -2.49E-02 | -691.3 | 1.83 | 0.033 | |||||||
-1.17 | 0.064 | -0.015 | -0.030 | 0.965 | + | + | 3.65E-04 | + | 0.171 | + | -2.52E-02 | -691.1 | 2.05 | 0.029 | |||||
-1.19 | 0.063 | -0.015 | -0.030 | 0.970 | + | + | 3.61E-04 | + | 0.167 | + | -2.46E-02 | -691.1 | 2.06 | 0.029 | |||||
-1.21 | 0.241 | -0.080 | -0.031 | 0.978 | 1.16E-03 | + | + | 3.71E-04 | + | 0.166 | -9.81E-03 | + | -2.52E-02 | 4.48E-03 | -690.9 | 2.27 | 0.026 | ||
-1.21 | 0.062 | -0.015 | -0.031 | 0.974 | 6.64E-04 | + | 3.64E-04 | + | 0.171 | + | -2.52E-02 | -690.8 | 2.36 | 0.025 | |||||
-1.20 | 0.060 | -0.014 | -0.030 | 0.973 | + | 3.64E-04 | + | 0.170 | + | -2.52E-02 | + | -690.7 | 2.41 | 0.024 | |||||
-1.24 | 0.235 | -0.079 | -0.032 | 0.983 | 1.09E-03 | + | + | 3.67E-04 | + | 0.163 | -9.35E-03 | + | -2.47E-02 | 4.36E-03 | -690.6 | 2.48 | 0.024 | ||
-1.36 | 0.037 | -0.027 | -0.032 | 1.004 | 2.89E-03 | + | 3.63E-04 | + | 0.152 | 6.66E-03 | -2.32E-02 | -690.4 | 2.69 | 0.021 | |||||
-1.25 | 0.233 | -0.079 | -0.031 | 0.985 | 1.12E-03 | + | 3.69E-04 | + | 0.166 | -9.33E-03 | + | -2.52E-02 | 4.38E-03 | + | -690.4 | 2.70 | 0.021 | ||
-1.19 | 0.101 | -0.023 | -0.048 | 0.974 | + | 3.45E-04 | + | 0.047 | + | -690.3 | 2.80 | 0.020 | |||||||
-1.30 | 0.199 | -0.072 | -0.031 | 0.994 | 1.25E-03 | + | 3.68E-04 | + | 0.157 | -7.24E-03 | -2.44E-02 | 3.78E-03 | -690.3 | 2.84 | 0.020 | ||||
-1.31 | 0.048 | -0.027 | -0.032 | 0.992 | 3.10E-03 | + | + | 3.64E-04 | + | 0.159 | 6.66E-03 | + | -2.36E-02 | -690.1 | 2.99 | 0.018 | |||
-1.33 | 0.048 | -0.027 | -0.033 | 0.995 | 2.99E-03 | + | + | 3.61E-04 | + | 0.156 | 6.64E-03 | + | -2.32E-02 | -690.1 | 3.04 | 0.018 | |||
-1.34 | 0.082 | -0.036 | -0.050 | 1.002 | 3.45E-03 | + | 3.45E-04 | + | 0.043 | 7.11E-03 | + | -690.0 | 3.12 | 0.017 | |||||
-1.26 | 0.251 | -0.082 | -0.050 | 0.990 | 1.78E-03 | + | 3.49E-04 | + | 0.042 | -7.20E-03 | + | 3.91E-03 | -690.0 | 3.16 | 0.017 | ||||
-1.34 | 0.047 | -0.027 | -0.032 | 0.997 | 3.03E-03 | + | 3.63E-04 | + | 0.160 | 6.73E-03 | + | -2.37E-02 | + | -689.8 | 3.28 | 0.016 | |||
-1.20 | 0.053 | -0.015 | -0.030 | 0.973 | + | + | 3.65E-04 | + | 0.163 | -2.47E-02 | -689.2 | 3.95 | 0.011 | ||||||
-1.23 | 0.053 | -0.015 | -0.030 | 0.977 | + | + | 3.62E-04 | + | 0.160 | -2.42E-02 | -689.2 | 3.97 | 0.011 |
Due to our estimate of inferred age tending to over-estimate (but not under-estimate) age, BW of known age individuals was initially lower than those of inferred-age, with a later slowing in BW rate increase (significant negative age2 × kage interaction, Table 2; Fig 1). Age, age2, kage and the age × kage interaction were retained in all 21 of the most supported models (Akaike weights = 0.589) while the age2 × kage interaction was retained in 18 of the most supported models (Akaike weights = 0.535, Table 3). Proximity to death did not influence the detection of somatic senescence, with the age × death and age2 × death interactions not retained in any of the most supported models. Pregnancy status did not influence female body weight (Table 2) and was retained in only four of the most supported models (Akaike weights = 0.082).
The lack of a significant positive interaction between maxage and age (retained in 10 of the most supported models, Akaike weights = 0.424, Table 2; Table 3), or age2 (retained in 5 models, Akaike weights = 0.233) implied that this increase in BW occurred at the individual, and not at the population level, i.e., not due to differential survival (ST).
Senescence in female reproductive output
Reproduction per year (RP)
RP was influenced by a quadratic effect of maternal minimum age (mmage2), a negative effect of rain, positive effect of TQ4 and a positive mmage × TQ4 interaction. All of these predictors, except mmage2, were retained in each of the top seven models (ΔAICc <4) (Table 4, Akaike weights = 0.534), where 95% CIs did not span zero (Table 5). Models including mmage2 comprised six of the top models (Akaike weights = 0.480). RPY, maxage and the interactions of RPY, rain and maxage with mmage were retained in fewer of the top models (Table 4); 95% CIs indicated non-significance (Table 5). R2c for the global model was 0.512 and the null model had a ΔAICc of +23.6.
Table 4. Model selection for probability of reproduction and reproductive success.
Response | Intercept | TE | mmage | mmage2 | rain | RPY | TQ4 | maxage | mmage × rain | mmage × RPY | mmage × TQ4 | mmage × maxage | AICc | ΔAICc | Akaike weight |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Probability of reproduction (0,1) |
-0.042 | 0.170 | 2.642 | -0.666 | -0.511 | 0.747 | 0.664 | 206.7 | 0.00 | 0.189 | |||||
-0.049 | 0.144 | 2.535 | -0.658 | -0.510 | 0.750 | 0.109 | 0.673 | 208.7 | 2.04 | 0.068 | |||||
-0.121 | 0.166 | 2.582 | -0.653 | -0.525 | 0.161 | 0.729 | 0.643 | 208.7 | 2.04 | 0.068 | |||||
-0.049 | 0.160 | 2.631 | -0.671 | -0.540 | 0.749 | -0.112 | 0.672 | 208.7 | 2.06 | 0.068 | |||||
-0.177 | 0.200 | -0.162 | -0.497 | 0.712 | -0.278 | 0.573 | -0.707 | 209.2 | 2.51 | 0.054 | |||||
-0.052 | 0.157 | 2.761 | -0.641 | -0.485 | -0.022 | 0.800 | -0.624 | 0.737 | 209.3 | 2.66 | 0.050 | ||||
-0.032 | 0.176 | 1.640 | -0.425 | -0.518 | 0.731 | -0.117 | 0.643 | -0.392 | 209.9 | 3.25 | 0.037 | ||||
Total | 0.534 | ||||||||||||||
Reproductive success (N offspring) |
-0.402 | 0.102 | 1.401 | -0.343 | -0.326 | 0.372 | 0.274 | 211.0 | 0.00 | 0.096 | |||||
-0.380 | 0.115 | 1.347 | -0.333 | -0.334 | 0.261 | 211.6 | 0.60 | 0.071 | |||||||
-0.404 | 0.081 | 1.269 | -0.331 | -0.322 | 0.365 | 0.159 | 0.282 | 212.3 | 1.37 | 0.048 | |||||
-0.374 | 0.111 | 1.606 | -0.352 | -0.294 | -0.114 | 0.426 | -0.485 | 0.338 | 212.7 | 1.71 | 0.041 | ||||
-0.382 | 0.097 | 1.222 | -0.321 | -0.330 | 0.252 | 0.145 | 213.1 | 2.11 | 0.033 | ||||||
-0.405 | 0.098 | 1.380 | -0.342 | -0.333 | 0.369 | -0.040 | 0.273 | 213.1 | 2.16 | 0.032 | |||||
-0.403 | 0.101 | 1.400 | -0.343 | -0.326 | 0.002 | 0.372 | 0.274 | 213.2 | 2.23 | 0.031 | |||||
-0.389 | 0.120 | 1.537 | -0.370 | -0.332 | 213.4 | 2.39 | 0.029 | ||||||||
-0.384 | 0.111 | 1.323 | -0.331 | -0.341 | 0.259 | -0.045 | 213.7 | 2.71 | 0.025 | ||||||
-0.400 | 0.111 | 1.331 | -0.329 | -0.339 | 0.050 | 0.256 | 213.7 | 2.75 | 0.024 | ||||||
-0.485 | 0.129 | -0.102 | -0.323 | 0.381 | -0.015 | 0.243 | -0.345 | 214.0 | 3.02 | 0.021 | |||||
-0.446 | 0.139 | -0.096 | -0.333 | 0.269 | -0.028 | -0.364 | 214.0 | 3.05 | 0.021 | ||||||
-0.372 | 0.119 | 1.498 | -0.339 | -0.318 | -0.037 | 0.277 | -0.365 | 214.4 | 3.37 | 0.018 | |||||
-0.394 | 0.091 | 1.027 | -0.267 | -0.326 | 0.361 | 0.097 | 0.273 | -0.118 | 214.4 | 3.39 | 0.018 | ||||
-0.395 | 0.097 | 1.361 | -0.352 | -0.329 | 0.189 | 214.4 | 3.40 | 0.017 | |||||||
-0.372 | 0.098 | 1.503 | -0.344 | -0.291 | -0.115 | 0.418 | 0.119 | -0.453 | 0.340 | 214.5 | 3.54 | 0.016 | |||
-0.408 | 0.077 | 1.244 | -0.330 | -0.329 | 0.363 | 0.159 | -0.043 | 0.280 | 214.5 | 3.55 | 0.016 | ||||
-0.401 | 0.082 | 1.272 | -0.332 | -0.321 | -0.008 | 0.366 | 0.159 | 0.283 | 214.6 | 3.62 | 0.016 | ||||
-0.684 | 0.146 | -0.180 | -0.305 | 0.443 | 0.259 | 214.8 | 3.81 | 0.014 | |||||||
-0.367 | 0.109 | 0.870 | -0.229 | -0.336 | 0.248 | 0.064 | -0.167 | 214.8 | 3.85 | 0.014 | |||||
-0.366 | 0.115 | 1.638 | -0.355 | -0.287 | -0.126 | 0.431 | 0.031 | -0.499 | 0.343 | 214.9 | 3.96 | 0.013 | |||
Total | 0.614 |
Model selection results for GLMM model sets describing probability of reproduction (binomial) and reproductive success (number of offspring, Poisson). Only models within AICc 4 of the top model are included. Trap effort was included in all candidate models within each set.
Table 5. Model estimates on the probability of reproduction and reproductive success.
Response: | Probability of reproduction (0,1) | Reproductive success (N offspring) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
95% CI | 95% CI | |||||||||
Predictor | Estimate | lower | upper | z | Estimate | lower | upper | z | ||
Intercept | -0.068 | -0.596 | 0.461 | -0.17 | -0.403 | -0.762 | -0.044 | -2.41 | * | |
Trap effort (TE) | 0.167 | -0.196 | 0.530 | 0.94 | 0.106 | -0.114 | 0.326 | 0.93 | ||
Maternal age (mmage) | 2.278 | -0.599 | 5.154 | 2.17 | 1.228 | -0.366 | 2.822 | 2.01 | ||
mmage2 | -0.642 | -1.206 | -0.079 | -2.44 | * | -0.335 | -0.638 | -0.031 | -2.27 | * |
rain | -0.513 | -0.889 | -0.136 | -2.71 | * | -0.324 | -0.551 | -0.097 | -2.86 | * |
RPY | 0.084 | -0.690 | 0.857 | -0.048 | -0.501 | 0.405 | ||||
TQ4 | 0.746 | 0.290 | 1.201 | 3.26 | * | 0.338 | 0.021 | 0.656 | 2.42 | * |
maxage | -0.075 | -0.736 | 0.586 | 0.111 | -0.277 | 0.499 | ||||
mmage × rain | -0.112 | -0.653 | 0.429 | -0.031 | -0.351 | 0.288 | ||||
mmage × RPY | -0.624 | -1.591 | 0.344 | -0.457 | -1.049 | 0.135 | ||||
mmage × TQ4 | 0.660 | 0.094 | 1.226 | 2.34 | * | 0.286 | -0.056 | 0.627 | 1.60 | |
mmage × maxage | -0.578 | -1.301 | 0.144 | -0.262 | -0.723 | 0.199 |
Model averaged results from the top (ΔAICc <4) set of GLMM models describing probability of reproduction (binomial) and reproductive success (number of offspring; Poisson). Estimates where the 95% confidence intervals do not include zero (indicating significance) are marked with an asterisk. Values of z are included from the single most supported model for each. Not all variables in the top model set were in these models.
These models (Fig 2, see S1 Fig for plots of the response surface, sample size and mean values) imply that a substantial reduction in RP occurred with age, particularly in lower quality (TQ4 < 4) territories (e.g., for TQ4 = 1, upper prediction intervals at mmage 10 are lower than lower prediction intervals at mmage four).
A decline in RP with age for TQ4 = 4 was less apparent; the peak prediction interval was at the minimum age six (0.44–0.80), compared to at minimum age 12 (0–0.61). For TQ4 > 1, RP was also lower for younger animals, but again this was non-significant (e.g., when TQ4 = 4 and mmage = 2, RP = 0–0.86). Wide prediction intervals for the youngest mothers might relate to a smaller sample size.
Overall, with lower TQ4, we observed a lower peak in RP, which also occurred at a younger age. Consequently, from minimum age six, mothers in territories with TQ4 = 1 were significantly less likely to produce offspring compared to those occupying highest quality territories (TQ4 = 4). Moving from the highest to lowest quality territories, model predictions indicate the highest probability of reproduction was at minimum ages 6–7, 5, 3–4 and 3 years (Fig 2).
From the 39 mothers examined, sample sizes were smaller for older age classes with a mean sample size per age class of seven individuals over minimum ages 8–15, versus a mean of 20 individuals over minimum ages 2–7. Nevertheless, the general reduction in RP with age was clear (44% bred per year over minimum ages 2–7 and 25% over minimum ages 8–15) while five (13%) survived for ≥3 years (mean = 6 years ± 3.6 SD, range = 3–11) after their last successful reproductive event.
Reproductive success
Modelling RS (number of offspring recorded for each dominant / breeding female in each year) in place of RP changed the biological interpretation of the model selection process somewhat (Table 4). Mmage and rain contributed to all of the 21 top models (total Akaike weights = 0.614, Table 4), TQ4 contributed to 19 (Akaike weights = 0.568), while mmage2 contributed to 18 (Akaike weights = 0.558). The mmage × TQ4 interaction contributed to 12 of the top models (Akaike weights = 0.362 Table 4) but was marginally non-significant (CI = -0.056–0.627, Table 5). Averaging these model estimates reaffirmed a negative effect of mmage2, a negative effect of rain, and a positive effect of TQ4 on reproductive success (Table 5). The mmage and mmage2 estimates suggest that peak RS (assuming all other variables are at their mean or median) occurred at minimum age five, when mean age-specific RS = 0.75 offspring / year (95% CIs = 0.35 to 1.14). R2c for the global model was 0.505 and the null model had a ΔAICc of +184.
Litter size (LS)
Mean litter size over the study period was 1.66 ± 0.84 SD. We found no effect of individual maternal age (mmage39) or of either resource proxy (rain and TQ4) on LS, on occasions when females bred successfully; with greatest support for the null model (containing only TE, Akaike weight = 0.325). R2c for the global model was 0.407.
Models including mmage39 had less influence (three of the six top models, total Akaike weights = 0.220). Similarly, there was little support for models including rain (two of the top models, Akaike weights = 0.170) or TQ4 (two of the top models, Akaike weights = 0.155), while there was scant support for the models that also included mmage392 and interactions of mmage39×rain and mmage39× TQ4 (all outside the top model set with ΔAICc > 4). Averaging from the top models confirmed no effect of mmage39 (estimate = 0.066, 95% CIs = -0.129 to 0.261), rain (estimate = -0.085, 955% CIs = -0371 to 0.202) or TQ4 (estimate = -0.040, 95% CIs = -0.249 to 0.169).
Offspring quality
We found little support for any influence of mmage39 or TQ4 on offspring BW: of the 14 top models with a ΔAICc < 4 (total Akaike weights = 0.790), mmage39 contributed in seven (Akaike weights = 0.252) and TQ4 in four (Akaike weights = 0.164). This was corroborated by model averaged estimates: mmage39 estimate -0.023, 95% CIs = -0.105 to 0.058; TQ4, estimate = -0.023, 95% CIs = -0.081 to 0.034. Apart from the control variables (lnBL and seasonage), only rainbin (‘dry’ versus ‘wet’ years) effected offspring BW, with a negative influence (estimate = -0.045, 95% CIs = -0.087 to -0.003); where rainbin contributed to 10 of the top models (Akaike weights = 0.581). Litter size (LS) contributed to seven of the top models (Akaike weights = 0.459), although the effect was not quite significant (estimate = -0.039, 95% CIs = -0.080 to 0.001). R2c for the global model was 0.907 and the null model displayed a ΔAICc of +12.8.
Effects on body length (BL) of mmage39, mmage392, LS and rainbin were modified by interactions with mmage39 with rainbin (positive, evident in all 16 top ΔAICc <4 models, Akaike weights = 0.980, Table 6; Table 7) and mmage39 with LS (negative, six top models, Akaike weights = 0.382). R2c for the global model was 0.736 and the null model displayed a ΔAICc of +77.4. The predictions of the averaged top models implied that:
Table 6. Model selection for offspring body weight and body length.
inter-cept | lnBL | age | LS | mmage | mmage2 | rain | TQ4 | mmage × LS | mmage × rain | mmage × TQ4 | AICc | Δ AICc | Akaike weight | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BC (ln BM) | -5.271 | 1.790 | + | -0.034 | -0.039 | -48.3 | 0.0 | 0.137 | ||||||
-5.040 | 1.735 | + | -0.051 | -48.2 | 0.1 | 0.129 | ||||||||
-5.829 | 1.927 | + | -0.047 | -47.7 | 0.6 | 0.102 | ||||||||
-5.018 | 1.732 | + | -0.025 | -0.052 | -47.0 | 1.3 | 0.073 | |||||||
-5.234 | 1.783 | + | -0.032 | -0.021 | -0.041 | -46.6 | 1.7 | 0.060 | ||||||
-5.278 | 1.790 | + | -0.037 | -0.039 | -0.028 | -46.5 | 1.8 | 0.057 | ||||||
-5.030 | 1.731 | + | -0.051 | -0.022 | -46.1 | 2.2 | 0.045 | |||||||
-5.843 | 1.929 | + | -0.049 | -0.026 | -46.0 | 2.3 | 0.043 | |||||||
-5.820 | 1.927 | + | -0.045 | -0.018 | -45.8 | 2.5 | 0.039 | |||||||
-5.700 | 1.898 | + | -44.9 | 3.4 | 0.025 | |||||||||
-5.215 | 1.781 | + | -0.025 | -0.047 | -0.009 | -44.5 | 3.8 | 0.021 | ||||||
-5.620 | 1.880 | + | -0.035 | -0.021 | -0.031 | -0.016 | -44.5 | 3.8 | 0.021 | |||||
-5.015 | 1.731 | + | -0.023 | -0.052 | -0.009 | -44.4 | 3.9 | 0.019 | ||||||
-5.017 | 1.732 | + | -0.027 | 0.000 | -0.052 | -44.3 | 4.0 | 0.019 | ||||||
Total | 0.790 | |||||||||||||
3.943 | + | 0.0117 | 0.007 | -0.040 | -0.018 | 0.037 | -160.7 | 0.00 | 0.121 | |||||
lnBL | 3.938 | + | 0.011 | -0.039 | 0.036 | -160.7 | 0.08 | 0.116 | ||||||
3.956 | + | 0.158 | -0.022 | -0.045 | -0.020 | 0.042 | -160.5 | 0.19 | 0.110 | |||||
3.959 | + | 0.123 | -0.018 | -0.043 | 0.040 | -160.5 | 0.24 | 0.108 | ||||||
3.958 | + | 0.0047 | 0.141 | -0.019 | -0.043 | -0.019 | -0.019 | 0.042 | -160.2 | 0.58 | 0.091 | |||
3.96 | + | 0.0066 | 0.108 | -0.016 | -0.042 | -0.018 | 0.041 | -159.7 | 1.07 | 0.071 | ||||
3.933 | + | 0.017 | -0.040 | -0.015 | 0.037 | -159.4 | 1.33 | 0.062 | ||||||
3.935 | + | 0.0103 | 0.016 | -0.039 | -0.019 | -0.020 | 0.038 | -159.3 | 1.48 | 0.058 | ||||
3.94 | + | 0.0105 | 0.011 | -0.041 | 0.036 | -159.2 | 1.54 | 0.056 | ||||||
3.956 | + | 0.0060 | 0.107 | -0.015 | -0.044 | 0.039 | -158.1 | 2.63 | 0.033 | |||||
3.933 | + | 0.024 | -0.040 | -0.028 | 0.037 | -0.015 | -157.9 | 2.84 | 0.029 | |||||
3.954 | + | 0.0037 | 0.150 | -0.021 | -0.046 | -0.020 | 0.041 | -157.9 | 2.88 | 0.029 | ||||
3.954 | + | 0.144 | -0.019 | -0.044 | -0.022 | 0.041 | -0.005 | -157.8 | 2.90 | 0.028 | ||||
3.934 | + | 0.0098 | 0.016 | -0.042 | -0.014 | 0.037 | -157.8 | 2.97 | 0.027 | |||||
3.936 | + | 0.0087 | 0.020 | -0.039 | -0.024 | -0.018 | 0.038 | -0.009 | -157.2 | 3.50 | 0.021 | |||
3.958 | + | 0.0047 | 0.143 | -0.020 | -0.043 | -0.019 | -0.019 | 0.043 | 0.001 | -157.1 | 3.61 | 0.020 | ||
Total | 0.980 |
Model selection results for LMM model sets describing offspring ln body weight (BW) and ln body length (BL). Only models within AICc 4 of the top model are included. Age was included in all candidate models within each set while lnBL was included in all candidate models describing lnBW so that lnBW equates to body condition (BC).
Table 7. Model estimates on offspring body weight and body length.
lnBW | lnBL | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
95% CI | 95% CI | |||||||||
Predictor | Estimate | lower | upper | t | Estimate | lower | upper | t | ||
Intercept | -5.334 | -6.983 | -3.685 | -6.71 | * | 3.948 | 3.905 | 3.990 | 226 | * |
lnBL | 1.807 | 1.392 | 2.222 | 9.01 | * | |||||
Age: summer 0 | -0.233 | -0.345 | -0.120 | -4.76 | * | -0.121 | -0.165 | -0.077 | -5.97 | * |
Age: spring 1 | 0.022 | -0.091 | 0.136 | 0.54 | 0.137 | 0.079 | 0.195 | 4.57 | * | |
Age: summer 1 | 0.126 | -0.114 | 0.367 | 1.19 | 0.332 | 0.236 | 0.428 | 7.30 | * | |
Age: autumn 1 | 0.342 | 0.079 | 0.606 | 2.62 | * | 0.275 | 0.159 | 0.392 | 5.46 | * |
Litter size (LS) | -0.039 | -0.080 | 0.001 | -1.73 | 0.008 | -0.010 | 0.027 | 1.28 | ||
Maternal age (mmage) | -0.023 | -0.103 | 0.058 | 0.073 | -0.079 | 0.226 | 0.74 | |||
mmage2 | 0.000 | -0.040 | 0.041 | -0.019 | -0.039 | 0.002 | ||||
Rain | -0.045 | -0.087 | -0.003 | -1.88 | * | -0.042 | -0.061 | -0.022 | -4.04 | * |
TQ4 | -0.023 | -0.081 | 0.034 | -0.019 | -0.040 | 0.002 | ||||
mmage × LS | -0.019 | -0.035 | -0.002 | -2.10 | * | |||||
mmage × Rain | -0.013 | -0.049 | 0.024 | 0.039 | 0.021 | 0.057 | 4.38 | * | ||
mmage × TQ4 | -0.008 | -0.031 | 0.016 |
Model averaged results from the top (ΔAICc <4) set of LMM models describing ln body weight (BW) and ln body length (BL). Estimates where the 95% confidence intervals do not include zero (indicating significance) are marked with an asterisk. Values of t are included from the single most supported model for each. Not all variables in the top model set were in these models.
Offspring BL was unaffected by maternal age in drier years, but increased markedly with maternal age in wetter years. Thus, wetter (resource poor) years impaired offspring BL only for younger mothers (Fig 3).
Offspring BL, in litters of one kit, was slightly longer among kits born to older mothers, although the magnitude of the increase was less with larger litter sizes (Fig 4). The effect of maternal age and its interaction with litter size was marginal however, as evidenced by the interaction appearing in fewer of the top models, and exhibiting a large overlap of prediction intervals (Fig 4).
Offspring BL was unaffected by territory quality, irrespective of the age of the mother.
Discussion
While several seminal studies have reported a range of life-history trade-offs with senescence (e.g. [7, 25, 47–54]) our study contributes rare evidence supporting resource-dependent theories of senescence (see S2 File for discussion in relation to other studies on Castor spp.). No signal for body weight senescence was apparent for adult beavers, and dominant females that achieved an older age in the population were no less likely to breed in a given year, and did not produce fewer offspring per annum than those that died when younger.
Importantly, the fact that the clear signal of reproductive senescence was not mirrored by somatic senescence lends support to Nussey et al.’s [7] contention that heterochrony is widespread in natural populations (see also Hayward et al. [54]), and counters Williams’s [9] prediction of synchrony in the schedule of senescent traits.
Age-related changes in body condition and offspring production
Somatic senescence
Rather than body weight senescence, (controlling for body length and season) beavers of both sexes from minimum age three exhibited an increase in body weight. This contrasts with several other studies on mammals [53,55,56] that found declines in body weight with age. For example, Alpine marmot (Marmota marmota) males exhibited terminal decline in body weight (controlling for season), although females did not show any detectable senescence in body weight [53]. In a detailed cross-species analysis, Nussey et al. [55] also found a significant body-mass decline with age in populations of wild bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis), roe deer (Capreolus capreolus) and Soay sheep (Ovis aries). While they report that the selective disappearance of light individuals contributed to variation in the body-weights they observed with age following the selection theory (ST [16]), we found no evidence for this effect. Although our sample size declined with age, potentially affecting our ability to detect changes in BW, the between-individual variance was similar across age classes and the increase in BW was significant even controlling for the maximum age attained, indicating that this occurred at the level of individuals and not from the selective mortality of lower quality animals at younger ages, and so did not accord ST, [16]. We found no evidence that somatic condition declined in the year prior to death, with the 35 individuals that died in the same or following year after measurement exhibiting the same pattern of increasing body weight as those individuals that survived for longer after the last measurement was recorded.
While we do not show here that body weight influenced fitness directly, our previous studies on this same beaver population have shown significant and consistent weather effects on body weight, reproduction and survival probability [26,31], suggesting that body weight is related to fitness. Nevertheless, future studies on this species could examine changes in skeletal density, muscle mass and mass of internal organs in more detail to establish whether the body weight pattern observed here is mirrored across other physical traits.
Given that both sexes (especially both breeding and non-breeding females) exhibited an increase in BW with age, we propose that this body-weight result may arise due to foraging experience following the constraints theory (CT [15–17]), with older individuals able to secure food resources more successfully, and was not linked to early-life breeding restraint, which would evidence somatic conservation following the disposable soma and reproductive restraint theories (DS and RR [11–13]).
Reproductive investment and age
Reproduction was already close to maximal at primiparity, or at the earliest observation where age was unknown. Although we detected an initial increase in both probability of annual reproduction and reproductive success (hereafter ‘reproduction’) with age for mothers in high quality territories (up to minimum age 5–6 years), this increase was not significant, contrary to CT [15–17].
However, overlaying annual resource effects, we did find that while the offspring born to younger mothers (≤5 years minimum age) during resource-poor years (high rainfall) were significantly smaller (juvenile body length), this was not the case for offspring born to older mothers (≥6). This suggests a greater influence of resource availability on younger mothers, consistent with CT and potentially ST (though see below). Crucially, large adult body size determines whether a beaver gains dominant breeding status and juvenile beavers may prioritise investment in gaining body-length at the expense of body condition [38]. The greater experience [57] or superior body condition of older mothers [58] might influence their capacity to respond to more challenging conditions (but see below), because conversely, in resource rich (dry) years (see [31]), mothers of all ages produced offspring with similar body sizes.
Reproductive senescence
That we were able to observe only 31% of dominant females over their entire reproductive life-span inevitably restricts our ability to investigate reproductive senescence fully. Nevertheless, we observed a significant decline in annual reproduction, after minimum age five. This decline in annual reproduction with age is predicted by DS [11] RR, [13], and congruent with other mammalian studies [50,59,60]. The disappearance of faster-breeding or lower quality, and thus shorter-lived individuals from the population (following ST, [16]) does not adequately explain the actuarial pattern of senescence we observed: dominant females that achieved an older age in the population were no more or less likely to breed in a given year, and did not produce greater or fewer offspring per annum, than those that died when younger.
In terms of resource affects, overlain on a general senescent decline in reproduction, reproductive metrics were consistently lower in low quality territories, irrespective of maternal age and annual rainfall. Annual variation in rainfall did not, however, influence the onset of reproductive senescence (the age at which the trait measured begins to show a year-on-year decline) directly, with similar responses for females of all age. Consequently, older (and higher BW, see above) mothers responded to short-term (inter-annual) resource variation similarly to younger mothers in terms of reproductive output, despite also exhibiting a senescent decline in reproduction. In contrast, older mothers are less affected by short-term resource variation, as measured by offspring size. This dual response could arise if older mothers accumulate greater energy reserves (accruing capital, sensu [61]) during one, or more, barren years, in order to mitigate unexpected poor conditions in the birth year.
Reproductive senescence onset
Williams [9] proposed that senescence should begin immediately after maturation (i.e. at the start of the decline in reproductive potential). By contrast, we observed a mismatch between sexual maturity (occurring at age 2–3) and the earliest breeding opportunity. In beavers, only dominant pairs (territory holders, determined by body size, [38]) breed, creating strong reproductive skew, and thus not all beavers begin breeding upon reaching sexual maturity at around age two [28,29]. In our study system territories are acquired at the mean age of five (S1 Fig); the age beyond which senescence in annual reproduction probability also commenced. Indeed, based on a dominant adult annual mortality rate of 13% per annum in this same population [26], 50% of beavers that achieved dominant breeding positions will die before age seven; approximating the age at which reproductive success started to decline. This corroborates Jones et al. [6] who found that for terrestrial vertebrate species in which the mean female reproductive age is 5–7, the relationship between age at onset of senescence (both actuarial and reproductive) and mean reproductive age was close to synchronous.
Resource history and onset of senescence
Studies have reported a trade-off between early and late reproduction in species as diverse as flycatcher (Ficedula albicollis) [47] and female red deer (Cervus elaphus) [60], but without reference to whether this trade-off was mediated by resources, as predicted by DS. In this territorial species, we found that individuals that experienced impoverished resource availability through their reproductive lifespan, measured as territory quality, experienced a relatively earlier onset of annual reproductive senescence (RP: litter produced, or not) than individuals in higher quality territories, although reproductive success (RS: annual number of offspring produced) was not affected. It is plausible that females in poor quality territories may allocate more absolute resources to reproduction (not just a relatively larger proportion of available resources) than those in high quality territories as a strategy to counter a lower realised longevity.
We could not measure resource allocation directly, only reproductive output; however, the mechanism involved may relate to physiological stress arising from the elevated relative energy expenditure required to raise litters successfully in poor quality territories. This could potentially affect longevity assurance mechanisms [22], induce greater oxidative stress, exacerbate telomere shortening rates [62], or induce apoptosis [63]. Alternatively, following RR [13], this pattern may result from females in poor quality territories reducing reproductive effort due to poor somatic condition, in order to extend their remaining lifespan and take advantage of breeding opportunities in later years. Indeed, this decoupling of somatic condition and reproductive success through reproductive restraint could explain the asynchrony between somatic and reproductive senescence we detected.
Notably, there was no evidence that the link between territory quality and senescence onset could be explained by higher quality individuals residing in better territories (S2 Fig). The difference in the response of RP and RS could arise if older individuals in lower quality territories are more likely to incur whole-litter breeding failures, rather than partial breeding failures (i.e. resource history and age interact to influence the probability that RS = 0 versus ≥1 but not the probability that RS = 1, 2 or 3). In support of this, we found no evidence that maternal age or territory quality influenced litter sizes.
These results exemplify state-dependent life-history strategies [64]; that is, age-dependent reproductive profiles differed between individuals depending on individual circumstances. Local resource competition also affects earlier onset and higher rate of physical senescence in male red deer in Norway [65]. Similarly, guillemots (Uria aalge) experiencing harsher environmental conditions early in life undergo more rapid senescence [52]. Together, these demonstrate generally that a trade-off between somatic maintenance and reproduction can arise as a direct result of resource limitation (supporting resource-dependent hypotheses of senescence, such as DS).
Conclusions: The general influence of resources and population structure on senescence
Nussey et al. [7] highlight how life-histories differ substantially depending on the environment in which they are measured, and therefore that patterns of aging cannot be characterised without reference to the specific environmental conditions in which they are also measured. By examining the relationship between reproductive senescence and metrics of long- and short-term environmental quality (resources) from this wild beaver population, we demonstrate (see [52,65]) that resource limitation (i.e. residence in low quality territories over the reproductive life-span), and not just reproductive life-cycle phase, can mediate trade-offs in long-term reproductive cost, exemplifying state-dependent life-history strategies [64]. Under these natural circumstances we establish support for resource-dependent hypotheses of ageing (i.e., DS and RR).
We also emphasise how individual-level effects can influence reproduction and somatic maintenance; for example, the offspring of older individuals were not smaller (shorter body length) in resource-poor (high rainfall) years, whereas those of younger mothers were smaller. Thus the life experience of individuals may relate to their ability to acquire resources to invest in somatic maintenance and reproduction, especially in resource-poor years (see [66,67]).
Under circumstances where only territory holding pairs breed, and low turnover in these pairs limits recruitment, the retention of non-breeding philopatric offspring can arise [27]. Where environmental conditions are stable, as in this study (c.f. [68]), and annual mortality rate is low (here ≤36% for philopatric adults, [26]) this can result in reproductive decline prior to achieving breeding status. Termed the ‘Florida effect’ [68], this phenomenon describes progressively older breeding individuals holding territories, and aged individuals still waiting for a breeding opportunity. As a consequence, adaptive life-history schedules would appear to play a crucial role in population dynamics, where under conditions of limited territory availability, selective pressure drives delayed senescence in order to enhance the life-time reproductive success of individuals that begin breeding late in life, due to a lack of territories. Early breeding opportunities presented to individuals arsing from the background mortality of incumbent territory holders could, however, result in a forward shift in the individuals’ ageing schedule arising from resource dependent senescence mechanisms. We suggest that necessity for flexible life-history strategies to maximise fitness under varying social and environmental conditions might explain the heterochrony in senescence traits observed here and in other studies [7].
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank: Frode Bergan, Bjørnar Hovde, Lasse Asmyhr, Claire Buchanan, Hannah B. Cross, Laura Daniells, Manuel E. Echeverria, Rita Goncalves, Patricia M. Graf, Orsi Haarberg, Jan Herr, Kristian Ingdal, Bård, A. Lassen, Elisabeth Mølmen, Leigh Murray, Bruno Pinto, Christian A. Robstad, Jørn-Ingar Sanda, Matthias Scherger, Jörg Schlichter, Fiona Sharpe, Kjartan Sjulstad, Magnhild Snaprud, Liat R. Thomsen, Helga Veronica Tinnesand, Jan-Marc Türschmann, Lisa Wallis, Akkerhaugen brygge and the local residents of the three rivers. We also thank Kirstin Bilham and four anonymous reviewers for comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript, Lutz Fromhage for useful discussion on the reproductive restraint hypothesis and Paul Johnson for statistical insight. This study was financially supported by a University College of Southeast Norway grant to RDC and FR and through a grant from the Peoples Trust for Endangered Species to DWM., also supporting CN.
Data Availability
All relevant data are available within the paper and its Supporting Information.
Funding Statement
This study was financially supported by a University College of Southeast Norway grant to RDC and FR and through a grant from the Peoples Trust for Endangered Species to DWM, also supporting CN.
References
- 1.Abrams PA. Does increased mortality favor the evolution of more rapid senescence? Evolution. 1993; 47: 877–887. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb01241.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hayflick L. How and Why We Age. Ballantine Books, New York: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Clutton-Brock TH. Reproductive Success. Studies of Individual Variation in Contrasting Breeding Systems. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ricklefs RE. Evolutionary theories of aging: confirmation of a fundamental prediction, with implications for the genetic basis and evolution of life span. Amer. Nat. 1998; 152:24–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Merilä J, Sheldon BC. Lifetime reproductive success and heritability in nature. Amer. Nat. 2000; 155:301–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Jones OR, Gaillard JM, Tuljapurkar S, Alho JS, Armitage KB, Becker PH, et al. Senescence rates are determined by ranking on the fast-slow life-history continuum. Ecol. Lett. 2008; 11:664–673. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2008.01187.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Nussey DH, Froy H, Lemaitre J-F Gaillard J-M, Austad SN. Senescence in natural populations of animals: Widespread evidence and its implications for bio-gerontology. Ageing Res. Rev. 2013; 12:214–225. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2012.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Medawar PB. An Unsolved Problem of Biology. H. K. Lewis, London: 1952. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Williams GC. Pleiotropy, Natural Selection, and the Evolution of Senescence. Evolution 1957; 11:398–411. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hamilton WD. The moulding of senescence by natural selection. J. Theor. Biol. 1966; 12:12–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kirkwood TBL. Evolution of ageing. Nature 1977; 270:301–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kirkwood TBL, Rose MR. Evolution of Senescence: Late Survival Sacrificed for Reproduction. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. [Biol.] 1991; 332:15–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.McNamara JM, Houston AI, Barta Z, Scheuerlein A, Fromhage L. Deterioration, death and the evolution of reproductive restraint in late life. Proc. R. Soc. Lond [Biol.] 2009; 276:4061–4066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Charmantier A, Brommer JE, Nussey DH. Quantitative genetics of senescence in wild animals In Quantitative genetics in the wild (eds Charmantier A, Garant D, Kruuk LEB). 2014; pp. 68–83. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Curio E. Why do young birds reproduce less well? Ibis 1983; 125:400–404. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nol E, Smith JNM. Effects of age and breeding experience on seasonal reproductive success in the song sparrow. J. Anim. Ecol. 1987; 56:301–313. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dugdale HL, Pope LC, Newman C, Macdonald DW, Burke, T. Age-specific breeding success in a wild mammalian population: selection, constraint, restraint and senescence. Mol. Ecol. 2011; 20:3261–3274. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05167.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wilbur HM, Tinkle DW, Collins JP. Environmental certainty, trophic level, and resource availability in life history evolution. Amer. Nat. 1974; 108:805–817. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Williams GC. Natural Selection, the Costs of Reproduction, and a Refinement of Lack's Principle. Amer. Nat. 1966; 100:687–690. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Van Noordwijk AJ, Jong G. Acquisition and allocation of resources: their influence on variation in life history tactics. Amer. Nat. 1986; 128:137–142. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Boggs CL. Resource allocation: exploring connections between foraging and life history. Funct. Ecol. 1992; 6:508–518. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Speakman JR, Król E. The heat dissipation limit theory and evolution of life histories in Endotherms—Time to dispose of the disposable soma theory? Integr. Comp. Biol. 2010; 50:793–807. doi: 10.1093/icb/icq049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lemaître JF, Berger V, Bonenfant C, Douhard M, Gamelon M, Plard F, Gaillard JM. Early-late life trade-offs and the evolution of ageing in the wild. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2015; 282: 20150209 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Forslund P, Pärt T. Age and reproduction in birds–hypotheses and tests. Trends Ecol. Evolut. 1995; 10:374–378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nussey DH, Coulson T, Festa‐Bianchet M, Gaillard JM. Measuring senescence in wild animal populations: towards a longitudinal approach. Functional Ecology. 2008; 22:393–406. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Campbell RD, Nouvellet P, Newman C, Macdonald DW, Rosell F. The influence of mean climate trends and climate variance on beaver survival and recruitment dynamics. Global Change Biol. 2012;18:2730–2742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Campbell RD, Rosell F, Nole B, Dijkstra V. Territory and group sizes in Eurasian beavers (Castor fiber): echoes of settlement and reproduction? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2005; 58:597–607. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rosell F, Pedersen KV. Bever. Landbruksforlaget, Oslo: In Norwegian; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wilsson L. Observations and experiments on the ethology of the European beaver (Castor fiber L.). Vitrevy 1971; 8:115–266. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Műller-Schwarze D, Sun L. The beaver: natural history of a wetlands engineer. Cornell University Press, Ithaca: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Campbell RD, Newman C, Macdonald DW, Rosell F. Proximate weather patterns and spring green-up phenology effect Eurasian beaver (Castor fiber) body mass and reproductive success: the implications of climate change and topography. Global Change Biol. 2013; 19:1311–1324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Svendsen GE. Pair formation, duration of pair-bonds, and mate replacement in a population of beavers (Castor canadensis). Can. J. Zool. 1989; 67:336–340. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Crawford JC, Liu Z, Nelson TA, Nielsen CK, Bloomquist CK. Microsatellite analysis of mating and kinship in beavers (Castor canadensis). J. Mammal. 2008; 89:575–581. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Clutton-Brock TH. Reproductive skew, concessions and limited control. Trends Ecol. Evolut. 1998; 13:288–292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nolet BA, Rosell F. Territoriality and time budgets in beavers during sequential settlement. Can. J. Zool.1994; 72:1227–1237. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Parker H, Rosell F. Parturition dates for Eurasian beaver Castor fiber: When should spring hunting cease? Wildl. Biol. 2001; 7:237–241. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hartman G. Notes on age at dispersal of beaver (Castor fiber) in an expanding population. Can. J. Zool. 1997; 75:959–962. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Campbell RD. Demography and life-history of the Eurasian beaver Castor fiber. Unpublished D. Phil thesis. University of Oxford, Oxford, UK: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Rosell F, Hovde B. Methods of aquatic and terrestrial netting to capture Eurasian beavers. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 2001; 29:269–274. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rosell F, Sun L. Use of anal gland secretion to distinguish the two beaver species Castor canadensis and C. fiber. Wildl. Biol. 1999; 5:119–123. [Google Scholar]
- 41.R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2011 [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999999–0. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4. 2009.
- 43.Barton K. MuMIn: Multi-model inference. R package version 1.7.11. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn. 2012.
- 44.Burnham KP, Anderson D. Model Selection and Multi-Model Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach, 2nd ed: Springer. New York, USA: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed‐effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2013; 4:133–42. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkara D. and the R Development Core Team. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1–102. 2011.
- 47.Gustafsson L, Pärt T. Acceleration of senescence in the collared flycatcher Ficedula albicollis by reproductive costs. Nature 1990; 347:279–281. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Broussard DR, Risch TS Dobson FS, Murie JO. Senescence and age-related reproduction of female Columbian ground squirrels. J. Anim. Ecol. 2003; 72: 212–219. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Isaac JL, Johnson CN. Terminal reproductive effort in a marsupial. Biol. Lett. 2005; 1:271–275. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0326 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bowen WD, Iverson SJ, McMillan JI, Boness DJ. Reproductive performance in grey seals: age-related improvement and senescence in a capital breeder. J. Anim. Ecol. 2006; 75:1340–1351. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2006.01157.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Nussey DH, Kruuk LE, Donald A, Fowlie M, Clutton-Brock TH. The rate of senescence in maternal performance increases with early-life fecundity in red deer. Ecol. Lett. 2006; 9:1342–1350. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2006.00989.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Reed TE, Kruuk LEB, Cunningham EJA, Wanless S, Frederiksen M, Harris MP. Reproductive senescence in a long-lived seabird: rates of decline in late-life performance are associated with varying costs of early reproduction. Amer. Nat. 2008; 171:E89–E101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tafani M, Cohas A, Bonenfant C, Gaillard JM, Lardy S, Allainé D. Sex-specific senescence in body mass of a monogamous and monomorphic mammal: the case of Alpine marmots. Oecol. 2013; 172: 427–436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hayward AD, Moorad J, Regan CE, Berenos C, Pilkington JG, Pemberton JM, Nussey DH. Asynchrony of senescence among phenotypic traits in a wild mammal population. Experimental gerontology. 2015; 71:56–68. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2015.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Nussey DH, Coulson T, Delorme D, Clutton-Brock TH, Pemberton JM, Festa-Bianchet M, Gaillard J-M. Patterns of body mass senescence and selective disappearance differ among three species of free-living ungulates. Ecology. 2011; 92: 1936–1947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Beirne C, Delahay R, Young A. Sex differences in senescence: the role of intra-sexual competition in early adulthood. Proc. R. Soc. Lond [Biol.]. 2015; 282: 20151086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Green W. Reproductive effort and associated costs in bison (Bison bison): do older mothers try harder? Behav. Ecol. 1990; 1:148–160. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Marshall D., Heppel SS Munch SB, Warner RR. The relationship between maternal phenotype and offspring quality: do older mothers really produce the best offspring? Ecol. 2010; 91:2862–2873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.de Magalhães JP, Toussain, O. The evolution of mammalian aging. Exp. Geront. 2002; 37:769–775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Nussey DH, Kruuk LEB, Donald A, Fowlie M, Clutton-Brock TH. The rate of senescence in maternal performance increases with early-life fecundity in red deer. Ecol. Lett. 2006; 9:1342–1350. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2006.00989.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Jönsson KI, Jonsson KI. Capital and income breeding as alternative tactics of resource use in reproduction. Oikos 1997; 78:57–66. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hall ME, Nasir L, Daunt F, Gault E, Croxal, JP, Wanless S, Monaghan P. Telomere loss in relation to age and early environment in long-lived birds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond [Biol.] 2004; 271:1571–1576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kujoth GC, Hiona A, Pugh TD, Someya S, Panzer K, Wohlgemuth SE, Hofer T, Seo AY, Sullivan R, Jobling WA, Morrow JD, Van Remmen H, Sedivy JM, Yamasoba T, Tanokura M, Weindruch R, Leeuwenburgh C, Prolla TA. Mitochondrial DNA mutations, oxidative stress, and apoptosis in mammalian aging. Science. 2005; 309: 481–484. doi: 10.1126/science.1112125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.McNamara JM, Houston AI. State-dependent life histories. Nature 1996; 380:215–221. doi: 10.1038/380215a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mysterud A Yoccoz NG, Stenseth NC, Langvatn R. Effects of age, sex and density on body weight of Norwegian red deer: evidence of density–dependent senescence. Proc. R. Soc. Lond [Biol.] 2001; 268:911–919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Bérubé CT, Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT. Individual differences, longevity, and reproductive senescence in bighorn ewes. Ecology 1999; 80:2555–2565. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Reznick D, Nunney L,Tessier A. Big houses, big cars, superfleas and the costs of reproduction. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2000; 15:421–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Penteriani V, Ferrer M, Otalora F, Delgado MdM. When individuals senesce: the 'Florida effect' on stable populations of territorial, long-lived birds. Oikos. 2009; 118:321–327. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are available within the paper and its Supporting Information.