Abstract
Objective
To compare the attentional bias of depressed patients and non-depressed control subjects and examine the effects of age using eye-tracking technology in a free-viewing set of tasks.
Methods
Patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) and non-depressed control subjects completed an eye-tracking task to assess attention of processing negative, positive and neutral facial expressions. In this cross-sectional study, the tasks were separated in two types (neutral versus happy faces and neutral versus sad faces) and assessed in two age groups (‘young’ [18–30 years] and ‘middle-aged’ [31–55 years]).
Results
Compared with non-depressed control subjects (n = 75), patients with MDD (n = 90) had a significant reduced positive attentional bias and enhanced negative attentional bias irrespective of age. The positive attentional bias in ‘middle-aged’ patients with MDD was significantly lower than in ‘young’ patients, although there was no difference between the two age groups in negative attentional bias.
Conclusions
These results confirm that there are emotional attentional biases in patients with MDD and that positive attentional biases are influenced by age.
Keywords: Depression, attentional bias, eye-tracking
Introduction
Attentional bias for emotional stimuli has become recognised as a key mechanism of affective disorders and is an important factor in the persistence of depressive symptoms.1 Although the aetiology and phenomenology of depression has been the focus of research into attentional bias,2 experimental evidence of attentional bias in depressive patients is inconsistent.3–13
Some studies support a negative attentional bias in depressive patients. For example, using free-viewing tasks, one study found that compared with control subjects, depressed patients had a longer gaze on negative pictures and had difficulty relieving attention from negative stimuli.3 Another study using visual search tasks also reported that patients with depression were more likely to pay attention to negative stimuli compared with control subjects.4 A meta-analysis of 33 studies using eye-tracking research concluded that the attentional bias for negative stimuli in patients with depression was significantly higher than in non-depressed subjects.5
Using happy and sad faces as stimuli to investigate depressive disorders in hospitalized patients, researchers found that compared with never-depressed controls, patients with depression failed to inhibit negative information but positive material was not affected.6 In one study where subjects were free to browse neutral and emotional images of the same face (i.e. happy, sad or angry), investigators showed that compared with never-depressed controls, patients with depression showed attentional bias to sad faces but not to angry faces.7 These results indicated that patients with depression tended to look at the stimuli that were consistent with their mood and confirmed that they had significant negative attentional bias.
However, other studies using emotional faces have shown that compared with controls, depressed patients have a lack of attentional bias for positive stimuli.8,9 A study of event-related evoked potentials found that patients with depression had less attention to positive information.10 Another study using a free-viewing paradigm, reported that while currently depressed and remitted depressed participants paid attention to sad faces significantly more than never-depressed participants, only the depressed participants showed a significant lack of attentional bias to happy faces.11 Nevertheless, although one study using a browsing paradigm to study the attentional bias of patients with depression showed that compared with controls patients with depression had an inadequate positive attentional bias,5 another study using eye-tracking research did not find any significant attentional bias toward positive stimuli in patients with depression.7 Moreover, some studies have failed to find any attentional biases in patients with depression.12,13
People of different ages will undoubtedly have differences in their psychological and physiological makeup that will lead to differences in emotional perception. For example, one study showed that with increasing age, the impact of negative emotions gradually decreased and that of positive emotions remained constant.14 Another study found that older people were more likely to look at happy faces and avoid angry faces whereas younger people only showed a tendency towards afraid faces.15 Studies have also found that with increasing age, attention to negative expressions gradually decreases.16,17 To date, research on the effect of age on emotional attention has tended to focus on healthy individuals and not on depressed individuals. Interestingly, one study compared the clinical treatment of depressed adults from different age groups and found that the dose of medication for middle-aged patients with depression is often larger than that for the young and the elderly.18 This may indicate that depression in middle-aged depressed patients is more severe than in other age groups. Furthermore, antidepressant medications may increase positive emotional conditioning and reduce the negative emotional conditioning that may affect attention to different emotional images.19,20
The current study was designed to assess attentional bias in depressed patients and non-depressed subjects when presented with negative, positive, and neutral facial expressions in a free-viewing paradigm. Importantly, stimulation time is a crucial factor in any study of attentional bias. Studies have shown that in depressed patients, negative attentional biases are related to the time of stimulus and are not found with short presentation times (i.e. 500 ms) but are present with longer presentation times.21–24 The presentation time chosen for this present study was 3000 ms.
We hypothesized that compared with non-depressed, control subjects, depressed patients would have an inadequate positive attentional bias and an enhanced negative attentional bias.2 Our second hypothesis was that compared with controls, both ‘young’ and ‘middle-aged’ depressed patients would show inadequate positive attentional bias and enhanced negative attentional bias.3 The third hypothesis was that compared with ‘young’ patients with depression, ‘middle-aged’ patients would have significant inadequate positive attentional bias and enhanced negative attentional bias.
Patients and methods
Study participants
For this cross-sectional study, all study participants were recruited from the Department of Automation, Faculty of Information Technology, Beijing University of Technology, and the Mood Disorders Centre & China Clinical Research Centre for Mental Disorders, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China. The control subjects (non-depressed) were recruited following advertisement. All prospective participants were screened by one of two psychiatrists (L.F & B.B.F) using the Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) to identify cases of major depressive disorder (MDD).25 The control subjects had never suffered with depression.
Inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) ≥18 years of age; (ii) right-handed; (iii) Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) score >5;26 (iv) no drugs or alcohol in the previous 2 weeks; (v) normal or corrected to normal vision. Exclusion criteria included: (i) colour blindness; (ii) presence of concurrent eye disease; (iii) inability to complete the eye-tracking experiment; (iv) presence of an obstacle to facial expression recognition such as inability to have normal language exchange. All study participants provided signed informed consent and this study was approved by the Ethics Committee at Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China (no. 2014065).
Experimental procedures
A set of images that included 12 happy faces (six males, six females), 12 sad faces (five males, seven females) and 24 neutral faces (11 males, 13 females) were selected from the NimStim Set of Facial Expressions image library.27 All images were processed by Photoshop software and the size, gradation and resolution were all consistent (i.e. image size 354 × 472 pixels; 8 × 6 cm). Stimuli consisted of pairs of images that included an emotional and a neutral facial expression made by the same person (Figure 1). There were two types of tasks separated into 12 neutral versus happy faces and 12 neutral versus sad faces and they appeared in random order. The positions of the two images in each task type were counterbalanced. The total task took 144 s to complete.
Eye-tracking information was collected using a Tobii T120 Eye Tracker (Tobii Pro, Stockholm, Sweden) with a sampling frequency of 120 Hz. The experimental stimuli were shown on a 17 in liquid crystal display monitor. Participants’ eyes were kept at a distance of approximately 60 cm from the monitor and 60 cm from the eye tracker.
Practice trials preceded the main study to ensure the participants were familiar with the experimental procedures and the data were collected correctly. Each trial began with a white cross on a grey background presented as the prompt for 1 s in the centre of the screen and the participant was required to pay attention to the symbol and fixate on the cross. Afterwards, a pair of faces was displayed for 3 s and the participants were instructed to view them freely. A rest time of 2 s preceded the next trial.
Initially, an area in each image was selected as an area of interest and the fixation time on that area was designated the total fixation time. All measurements were made in milliseconds (ms) and collected by two researchers (J. Xu & J. Xue). The data were analysed by one researcher (J. Xu) The total fixation time on happy faces was denoted th, on sad faces ts, and on neutral faces tn. Therefore, happiness bias score was th – tn, and sadness bias score was ts – tn Scores greater than zero indicated an attentional bias for emotional faces while scores less than zero indicated a preference toward neutral faces. Score indices based on differences such as these have been used in previous research.7
Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS® software, version 20.0 for Windows® (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA). Data were expressed as mean ± SE. A 2 group (i.e. depressed, non-depressed) × 2 group (i.e. emotional category: happy, sad) × 2 group (i.e. age: young, middle-aged) mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyse the data. Differences between groups in attentional bias scores of total fixation time were analysed using a two sample t-test. A P-value < 0.05 (2-sided) was considered statistically significant.
Results
In total, 75 (27 men, 48 women) patients with depression were enrolled into the study; their ages ranged from 18– 55 years (mean ± SE age, 42.5 ± 12.0 years) and they had attended school for a mean ± SE of 12.9 ± 3.4 years. In addition, 90 (40 men, 50 women) non-depressed control subjects were recruited whose ages ranged from 18– 55 years (mean ± SE age, 31.2 ± 9.9 years) and they had attended school for a mean ± SE of 14.6 ± 3.2 years.
Study participants were subdivided into ‘young’ and ‘middle-aged’. In the depressed group, there were 17 (7 men, 10 women) ‘young’ patients whose ages ranged from 18 – 30 years (mean ± SE age, 24.9 ± 3.1 years) and they had been in education for a mean ± SE of 14.7 ± 2.4 years. There were 58 (20 men, 38 women) ‘middle-aged’ patients with depression whose ages ranged from 31 – 55 years (mean ± SE age, 44.2 ± 7.7 years) and they had been in education for a mean ± SE of 12.5 ± 3.6 years. In the non-depressed control group, there were 56 (26 men, 30 women) ‘young’ subjects whose ages ranged 18 – 30 years (mean ± SE age, 24.6 ± 3.0 years) and they had been in education for a mean ± SE of 15.8 ± 2.1 years. There were 34 (14 men, 20 women) ‘middle-aged’ control subjects whose ages ranged from 31 – 55 years (mean ± SE age, 42.2 ± 7.1 years) and they had been in education for a mean ± SE of 12.7 ± 3.8 years.
In the three factor (i.e. 2 × 2 × 2) ANOVA, group factor was not significant (F [1, 161] = 0.18, P ≥ 0.05, η2 = 0.001). However, expression factor was significant (F [1, 161] = 111.14, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.26), but age factor was not significant (F [1, 161] = 0.17, P ≥ 0.05, η2 = 0.001). Group × expression interaction was significant (F [1, 161] = 12.00, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.04), group × age interaction was not significant (F [1, 161] = 0 .40, P ≥ 0.05, η2 = 0.003), but expression × age interaction was significant (F [1, 161] = 5.74, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.02). Finally, group × expression × age interaction was significant (F [1, 161] = 4.15, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.03).
Results from the 2 (group: depressed, non-depressed) × 2 (emotional category: happy, sad) ANOVA showed a nonsignificant main effect for the group (F [1, 163] = 0.41, P ≥ 0.05, η2 = 0 .003) and a significant interaction between emotion and group (F [1, 163] = 54.80, P < 0.05, η2 = 0 .25). Analysis of attentional bias scores showed that by comparison with control subjects, depressed patients had a reduced attentional bias for happy faces and an enhanced attentional bias for sad faces (Table 1).
Table 1.
Depressed patients n = 75 | Non-depressed control subjects n = 90 | t | Statistical significancea | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Happiness bias score,b ms | 247 ± 125 | 1003 ± 166 | −6.97 | P < 0.05 |
Sadness bias score,b ms | −570 ± 184 | −1447 ± 189 | 6.39 | P < 0.05 |
Data are presented as mean ± SE.
Two sample t-test.
Happiness bias score was th–tn and sadness bias score was ts–tn, where total fixation time on happy faces was th, on sad faces was ts and on neutral faces was tn.
Analysis of the ‘young’ participants (aged 18–30 years) from the 2 (group: depressed, non-depressed) × 2 (emotional category: happy, sad) ANOVA showed a nonsignificant group effect (F [1, 71] = 0.06, P ≥ 0.05, η2 = 0.001) and a significant interaction between emotion and group (F [1, 71] = 9.33, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.12). Likewise, analysis of the ‘middle-aged’ participants (aged 31–55 years) from the 2 (group: depressed, non-depressed) × 2 (emotional category: happy, sad) ANOVA showed a nonsignificant group effect (F [1, 90] = 0.14, P ≥ 0.05, η2 = 0.002) and a significant interaction between emotion and group (F [1, 90] = 35.39, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.28). Analysis of attentional bias scores showed that both ‘young’ and ‘middle-aged’ depressed patients had a reduced attentional bias for happy faces and an increased attention bias for sad faces when compared with non-depressed control subjects (Tables 2 and 3).
Table 2.
Depressed patients n = 17 | Non-depressed control subjects n = 56 | t | Statistical significancea | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Happiness bias score,b ms | 305 ± 158 | 992 ± 167 | −3.07 | P < 0.05 |
Sadness bias score,b ms | −804 ± 206 | −1435 ± 187 | 2.43 | P < 0.05 |
Data are presented as mean ± SE.
Two sample t-test.
Happiness bias score was th–tn and sadness bias score was ts – tn, where total fixation time on happy faces was th, on sad faces was ts and on neutral faces was tn.
Table 3.
Depressed patients n = 58 | Non-depressed control subjects n = 34 | t | Statistical significancea | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Happiness bias score,b ms | 162 ± 110 | 1031 ± 162 | −6.26 | P < 0.05 |
Sadness bias score,b ms | −440 ± 175 | −1361 ± 186 | 4.87 | P < 0.05 |
Data are presented as mean ± SE.
Two sample t-test.
Happiness bias score was th–tn and sadness bias score was ts – tn, where total fixation time on happy faces was th, on sad faces was ts and on neutral faces was tn.
For the depressed patients, results from the 2 (age: young, middle-aged) × 2 (emotional category: happy, sad) ANOVA showed a non-significant effect of group (F [1, 73] = 1.21, P ≥ 0.05, η2 = 0.016) and a significant interaction between emotion and age (F [1, 73] = 4.42, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.057). For the non-depressed control subjects, results from the 2 (age: ‘young’, ‘middle-aged’) × 2 (emotional category: happy, sad) ANOVA showed a significant group factor interaction between emotion and age (F [1, 88] = 4.04, P < 0.05, η2 = 0.440). Analysis of attentional bias scores showed that ‘middle-aged’ patients with depression had a significantly reduced attentional bias for happy faces when compared with younger patients, but there was no significant difference between the age groups with regard to sadness (Table 4). Finally, there was no significant difference between ‘young’ and ‘middle-aged’ non-depressed control subjects in attentional bias for happy or sad faces (Table 5).
Table 4.
Young depressed patientsa n = 17 | Middle-aged depressed patientsa n = 58 | t | Statistical significanceb | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Happiness bias score,c ms | 305 ± 158 | 162 ± 110 | 2.03 | P < 0.05 |
Sadness bias score,c ms | −804 ± 206 | −440 ± 175 | –0.83 | NS |
Data are presented as mean ± SE.
Young = 18–30 years; middle-aged = 31–55 years.
Two sample t-test.
Happiness bias score was th–tn and sadness bias score was ts – tn, where total fixation time on happy faces was th, on sad faces was ts and on neutral faces was tn.
NS, no significant between-group difference (P ≥ 0.05).
Table 5.
Young non-depressed subjectsa n = 56 | Middle-aged non-depressed subjectsa n = 34 | t | Statistical significanceb | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Happiness bias score,c ms | 992 ± 167 | 1031 ± 162 | 0.94 | NS |
Sadness bias score,c ms | −1435 ± 187 | −1361 ± 186 | –1.18 | NS |
Data are presented as mean ± SE.
Young = 18–30 years; middle-aged = 31–55 years.
Two sample t-test.
Happiness bias score was th – tn and sadness bias score was ts – tn, where total fixation time on happy faces was th, on sad faces was ts and on neutral faces was tn.
NS, no significant between-group difference (P ≥ 0.05).
Discussion
The results of this study confirmed our first hypothesis that compared with non-depressed control subjects, depressed patients showed inadequate positive attentional bias and enhanced negative attentional bias. These current results were consistent with findings from a previous study that used a visual browsing paradigm.28 The present results are also partially consistent with another study that also used a visual browsing paradigm and found patients with depression had more negative attentional bias than controls.7 However, in contrast with the present findings, the previous study did not find inadequate positive attentional bias in depressed patients.7 A possible explanation for the difference in study outcomes may be the variation in sample size; the previous study only included 16 depressed patients, which may have been too small to detect a difference.7 In our opinion, it is reasonable for patients with depression to have inadequate attention to positive stimuli since anhedonia is a typical feature of patients with depression and it can result in patients’ decreased sensitivity to reward stimulation,29,30 which will lead to decreased attention to positive experiences.31
Our second hypothesis was also supported by these current results in that irrespective of age, patients with depression showed inadequate positive attentional bias and enhanced negative attentional bias compared with non-depressed control subjects. However, the third hypothesis with regard to differences in attentional biases of ‘young’ and ‘middle-aged’ patients with depression was only partially supported. Compared with ‘young’ depressed patients, ‘middle-aged’ patients showed a significant deficiency in positive attentional bias but there was no difference between the two age groups in negative attentional bias. Previous studies have reported that in terms of behaviour, the sensitivity of adolescents to positive stimuli was significantly increased while that to negative stimuli was decreased.32 Changes in the structure and function of the brain with age also follow the trajectory of these behavioural changes.33,34 Interestingly, a previous study reported that the depressive symptoms in ‘middle-aged’ patients were more severe than in younger patients.18 Perhaps the discrepancy in these current results was due to differences in sample size between the ‘young’ and ‘middle-aged’ groups, which may have affected statistical power.
Limitations of this study include the small sample size of the group of ‘young’ patients with depression and the imbalance of the ratio of women to men with depression (i.e. 47 women and 28 men). Studies have shown that there are significant differences between men and women with depression in emotional experience, prevalence and age of onset.35–37 A further limitation of the study was the imbalance of the number of male and female faces in sad and neutral images, which may have affected the study outcome. Finally, although all drug and alcohol was withdrawn for at least 2 weeks before the study started, some of the patients with depression may have previously taken long-term antidepressants, which may have influenced the results.
In summary, this study showed that patients with depression had inadequate positive attentional bias and enhanced negative attentional bias compared with the non-depressed control subjects. In addition, the positive attentional bias of patients with depression was significantly affected by age: the positive attentional bias in ‘middle-aged’ patients was significantly lower than in ‘young’ patients. The results of this study may help us to understand more fully the emotional bias of patients with depression and provide direction for clinical treatment of these patients. For example, it may be possible to adjust the emotional state of depressed patients and reduce their depressive mood by repeated training of paying attention to positive stimuli. Training times would be increased appropriately for older patients.
Declaration of conflicting interests
The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.
Funding
This work is supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (61602017), the Beijing Natural Science Foundation (4164080), the Beijing Outstanding Talent Training Foundation (2014000020124G039), the National Basic Research Programme of China (2014CB744600), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (61420106005), the International Science & Technology Cooperation Programme of China (2013DFA32180), the Funding of Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University (YJ2015009), the Special fund of Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Commission (Z151100003915117), Beijing Municipal Administration of Hospitals Clinical Medicine Development of Special Funding Support (ZYLX201607 and ZYLX201403), and Beijing Municipal Administration of Hospitals’ Ascent Plan (DFL20151801).
References
- 1.Beck AT. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders, New York: International Universities Press, 1976, pp. 47–48. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Hogan ME, et al. The temple–Wisconsin cognitive vulnerability to depression project: lifetime history of Axis I psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. J Abnorm Psychol 2000; 109: 403–418. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Eizenman M, Yu LH, Grupp L, et al. A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res 2003; 118: 117–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gotlib IH, Krasnoperova E, Yue DN, et al. Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. J Abnorm Psychol 2004; 113: 127–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Armstrong T, Olatunji BO. Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2012; 32: 704–723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Goeleven E, De Raedt R, Baert S, et al. Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. J Affect Disord 2006; 93: 149–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Duque A, Vázquez C. Double attention bias for positive and negative emotional faces in clinical depression: evidence from an eye-tracking study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2015; 46: 107–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Joormann J, Gotlib IH. Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. J Abnorm Psychol 2007; 116: 80–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Peckham AD, McHugh RK, Otto MW. A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depress Anxiety 2010; 27: 1135–1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nandrino JL, Dodin V, Martin P, et al. Emotional information processing in first and recurrent major depressive episodes. J Psychiatr Res 2004; 38: 475–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Soltani S, Newman K, Quigley L, et al. Temporal changes in attention to sad and happy faces distinguish currently and remitted depressed individuals from never depressed individuals. Psychiatry Res 2015; 230: 454–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.McCabe SB, Gotlib IH. Selective attention and clinical depression: Performance on a deployment-of-attention task. J Abnorm Psychol 1995; 104: 241–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cheng P, Preston SD, Jonides J, et al. Evidence against mood-congruent attentional bias in Major Depressive Disorder. Psychiatry Res 2015; 230: 496–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Mayr U, et al. Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000; 79: 644–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Isaacowitz DM, Wadlinger HA, Goren D, et al. Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study [J]. Psychol Aging 2006; 21: 40–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Charles ST, Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J Exp Psychol Gen 2003; 132: 310–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychol Sci 2003; 14: 409–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Blažeković-Milaković S, Stojanović-Špehar S, Katić M, et al. Comparison of depression treatment among different age groups in primary care setting. Psychiatr Danub 2011; 23: 183–188. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Outhred T, Das P, Felmingham KL, et al. Impact of acute administration of escitalopram on the processing of emotional and neutral images: a randomized crossover fMRI study of healthy women. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2014; 39: 267–275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wells TT, Clerkin EM, Ellis AJ, et al. Effect of antidepressant medication use on emotional information processing in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171: 195–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Waechter S, Nelson A, Wright C, et al. Measuring attentional bias to threat: Reliability of dot probe and eye movement indices. Cognit Ther Res 2014; 38: 313–333. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Waechter S, Stolz JA. Trait anxiety, state anxiety, and attentional bias to threat: Assessing the psychometric properties of response time measures. Cognit Ther Res 2015; 39: 441–458. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Taylor JL, John CH. Attentional and memory bias in persecutory delusions and depression. Psychopathology 2004; 37: 233–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Donaldson C, Lam D, Mathews A. Rumination and attention in major depression. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45: 2664–2678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV [Internet]. 4th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association; 1994 [cited 2010 Mar 8]. 866 p. Available from: http://allpsych.com/disorders/dsm/.
- 26.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med 2001; 16: 606–613.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res 2009; 168: 242–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sears CR, Newman KR, Ference JD, et al. Attention to emotional images in previously depressed individuals: an eye tracking study. Cognit Ther Res 2011; 35: 517–528. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pechtel P, Dutra SJ, Goetz EL, et al. Blunted reward responsiveness in remitted depression. J Psychiatr Res 2013; 47: 1864–1869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schiller CE, Minkel J, Smoski MJ, et al. Remitted major depression is characterized by reduced prefrontal cortex reactivity to reward loss. J Affect Disord 2013; 151: 756–762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Watson D, Naragon-Gainey K. On the specificity of positive emotional dysfunction in psychopathology: evidence from the mood and anxiety disorders and schizophrenia/schizotypy. Clin Psychol Rev 2010; 30: 839–848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Arnett J. Reckless behavior in adolescence: a developmental perspective. Dev Rev 1992; 12: 339–373. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Luna B, Sweeney JA. The emergence of collaborative brain function: FMRI studies of the development of response inhibition. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2004; 1021: 296–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lewis DA, Cruz D, Eggan S, et al. Postnatal development of prefrontal inhibitory circuits and the pathophysiology of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2004; 1021: 64–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Li M, Lu S, Wang G, et al. The effects of gender differences in patients with depression on their emotional working memory and emotional experience. Behav Neurol 2015; 2015: 807343–807343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory. Psychol Bull 2001; 127: 773–796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Seedat S, Scott KM, Angermeyer MC, et al. Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the world health organization world mental health surveys. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66: 785–795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]