Abstract
Indirect genetic effects (IGEs) describe the effect of the genes of social partners on the phenotype of a focal individual. Here, we measure indirect genetic effects using the “coefficient of interaction” (Ψ) to test whether Ψ evolved between Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans. We compare Ψ for locomotion between ethanol and nonethanol environments in both species, but only D. melanogaster utilizes ethanol ecologically. We find that while sexual dimorphism for locomotion has been reversed in D. simulans, there has been no evolution of social effects between these two species. What did evolve was the interaction between genotype‐specific Ψ and the environment, as D. melanogaster varies unpredictably between environments and D. simulans does not. In this system, this suggests evolutionary lability of sexual dimorphism but a conservation of social effects, which brings forth interesting questions about the role of the social environment in sexual selection.
Keywords: Drosophila, indirect genetic effects, locomotion, sexual dimorphism
1. INTRODUCTION
Indirect genetic effects (IGEs) occur when individual phenotypes are affected by the genotype of conspecifics with which they interact (Wolf, Brodie, Cheverud, Moore, & Wade, 1998). Essentially, the phenotype of an individual depends on its own genes (direct genetic effects) but also on the genes of its social partners (IGEs). When the social environment contains genes, it can evolve and be an important component of heritability (Bijma, 2014; Dingemanse & Araya‐Ajoy, 2015; Moore, Brodie, & Wolf, 1997). In general, social effects are expected to alter the evolutionary trajectory of other traits such that they evolve differently than expected based on measures of selection and direct additive genetic effects (Wolf et al., 1998). This phenomenon has been demonstrated in a number of species, including laying hens (Brinker, Bijma, Visscher, Rodenburg, & Ellen, 2014; Peeters, Eppink, Ellen, & Visscher, 2012), pigs (Camerlink, Turner, Bijma, & Bolhuis, 2013; Camerlink, Ursinus, Bijma, Kemp, & Bolhuis, 2015), and Drosophila (Chenoweth, Rundle, & Blows, 2010; Saltz, 2013; Signor, Abbasi, Marjoram, & Nuzhdin, 2017).
Indirect genetic effects can be described by Ψ, the “coefficient of interaction,” which is the partial regression coefficient of the focal individual's behavior on the behavior of its social partner (Moore et al., 1997). Ψ measures the effect of a trait in the social partner on a trait in a focal individual, such that if it is zero, there is no effect of the social partner on the focal individual. It has been shown previously that Ψ is evolvable (Bleakley & Brodie, 2009; Chenoweth et al., 2010; Kazancıoğlu, Klug, & Alonzo, 2012; Marie‐Orleach et al., 2017). Here, we will focus on the evolution of Ψ measured for locomotion in two species of Drosophila.
While few studies have focused on locomotion, it has been found to be an important component of sexual selection and fitness (Ferguson, O'Neill, Audsley, & Isaac, 2015; Husak & Fox, 2008; Lailvaux, Alexander, & Whiting, 2003; Long & Rice, 2007; Perry, 1996; Peterson & Husak, 2006). In D. melanogaster, locomotion is sexually antagonistic and is thought to have a shared genetic basis in males and females (Long & Rice, 2007). It is advantageous for males of this species to move more and for females to move less, as higher activity males mate more and higher activity in females’ acts as a stimulus for male courtship (Bateman, 1948; Maklakov & Arnqvist, 2009; Olson‐Manning, Wagner, & Mitchell‐Olds, 2012; Partridge, Green, & Fowler, 1987; Tompkins, Gross, Hall, Gailey, & Siegel, 1982). In D. melanogaster, male courtship results in a general reduction in female lifetime reproductive success, likely due to higher rates of remating and interference during oviposition (Bateman, 1948; Fiumera & Dumont, 2006; Kuijper, Stewart, & Rice, 2006; Long & Rice, 2007; Partridge et al., 1987; Tompkins et al., 1982). Sexually antagonistic interactions, and IGEs expressed in those interactions, could potentially have a large impact on the evolutionary trajectory of a given trait (Marie‐Orleach et al., 2017; Moore & Pizzari, 2005).
While the effect of locomotion on fitness has not been explicitly tested in D. melanogaster's sister species D. simulans, there is evidence to suggest that locomotion is not sexually antagonistic in this species. As aforementioned, higher activity level in D. melanogaster results in increased mating, and multiple matings are detrimental to females and beneficial to males (Fiumera & Dumont, 2006; Kuijper et al., 2006; Long & Rice, 2007; Maklakov & Arnqvist, 2009; Partridge et al., 1987; Tompkins et al., 1982). The relationship between locomotion and mating in D. simulans is not known, but there is evidence that undergoing multiple matings is beneficial in D. simulans (Bateman, 1948; Fiumera & Dumont, 2006; Kuijper et al., 2006; Maklakov & Arnqvist, 2009; Taylor, Wigmore, Hodgson, Wedell, & Hosken, 2008; Tompkins et al., 1982). While we cannot assume that the relationship between movement and mating is the same, if it were in D. simulans, it would suggest that locomotion is not sexually antagonistic. Overall, the data indicate that sexual selection and trade‐offs vary considerably between Drosophila species. Indeed, sexual dimorphism of activity level is evolutionarily labile—for example, in D. suzukii females are 4× more active than males, while in D. melanogaster males are 3× more active than females (Ferguson et al., 2015; Long & Rice, 2007; Signor et al., 2017).
In a previous study, we used measures of Ψ for the effect of male locomotion on female activity to show that there is variation in Ψ between abiotic environments in D. melanogaster and found that Ψ was positive. This means that male and female locomotory phenotypes will covary, and females will resemble their male social partners without direct genetic effects (Signor et al., 2017). The two environments were ethanol‐ and nonethanol‐exposed, and while Ψ was higher without ethanol, when Ψ was measured in individual genotypes it varied differently in different genotypes between environments. Here, we will compare Ψ for locomotion in D. simulans and D. melanogaster in ethanol‐ and nonethanol‐exposed environments to determine whether there has been evolution of social effects between the two species.
Ethanol was chosen as an environmental variable because D. melanogaster and D. simulans have a different ecological history with regard to ethanol‐rich substrates. D. melanogaster is well adapted to ethanol, regularly utilizing resources with ethanol concentrations greater than 8% (Fry, 2014; Gibson, May, & Wilks, 1981; Hoffmann & McKechnie, 1991; Zhu & Fry, 2015). Ethanol tolerance is variable in D. melanogaster, and polymorphisms are maintained in the species at multiple loci involved in ethanol metabolism (Chakir, Peridy, Capy, Pla, & David, 1993; Chakraborty & Fry, 2016; Fry, Bahnck, Mikucki, Phadnis, & Slattery, 2004; Fry, Donlon, & Saweikis, 2008). There is a long history of research on the differences in alcohol tolerance between D. simulans and D. melanogaster, as well as their relative exploitation of this habitat type (David & Bocquet, 1976; Dickinson, Rowan, & Brennan, 1984; McKenzie & McKechnie, 1979; Parsons, 1977). In general, the niche of D. simulans is narrower than that of D. melanogaster, and it is not found pupating or feeding on high‐ethanol substrates (although larvae may have a higher tolerance) (Chakir et al., 1993; David & Bocquet, 1976; Dickinson et al., 1984; Gibson & Wilks, 1988; Gibson et al., 1981; Joshi & Thompson, 1997; McKenzie & McKechnie, 1979; Mercot, Defaye, Capy, Pla, & David, 1994; Parsons & King, 1977; Parsons & Spence, 1981; Thomson, Jacobson, & Laurie, 1991). Thus, we can investigate the role of social effects in environments to which these two species are, or are not, adapted.
To construct a comprehensive account of Ψ in different environments, we measure movement in D. simulans in sets of one female and two males. Across all experiments, female genotype is the same and the male genotype varies (but are identical within a replicate), allowing for genetic replication. We compare these results to those for D. melanogaster to investigate how Ψ, and differences in Ψ between environments, has evolved between these two closely related species (Signor et al., 2017). We find that locomotion is remarkably different in D. melanogaster and D. simulans, with D. simulans exhibiting little dimorphism in its level of activity. However, we find that Ψ is almost the same in the two species, indicating that for locomotion sexual dimorphism has evolved, but in this case Ψ did not. This has two implications that Ψ may have a partially separate genetic basis from locomotion in these species and that the differences in Ψ between environments are not due to differences in ethanol tolerance. Lastly, we find that while there is a Ψ × environment interaction in D. melanogaster, this is not present in D. simulans, which has interesting implications for the maintenance of variation in populations.
2. METHODS
2.1. Drosophila simulans genotypes
Male genotypes were collected from the Zuma organic orchard in Zuma beach, CA in the spring of 2012. They were inbred by 15 generations of full‐sib crosses. A subset of six genotypes were used for behavioral assays. Females used in the behavioral assays were an inbred laboratory strain y 1 w 1 (San Diego Species stock number 14021–0251.013).
2.2. Drosophila melanogaster genotypes
Males were collected from lines originating from an orchard in Winters, California in 1998 that were made isogenic by at least 40 generations of full‐sibling inbreeding (Campo et al., 2013; Yang & Nuzhdin, 2003). A subset of six genotypes were used for behavioral analysis. Female flies were an inbred laboratory strain y 1 w 1 (Bloomington stock number 1495).
2.3. Heterozygous crosses
Heterozygous crosses were used to generate the male flies used in the behavioral assays. In general, flies collected from the field will be heterozygotes, so in order to more closely replicate these conditions, the genotypes from Zuma Beach and Winters were crossed to a single reference genotype (D. simulans: w 501 San Diego Species stock number 14021–0251.011, D. melanogaster: w 1118, Bloomington stock number 3605), and the F1 flies from this cross were used in the behavioral assays (Nuzhdin, Friesen, & McIntyre, 2012). This allows both the use of heterozygous flies that are more similar to wild flies and the replication of behavioral observations as the flies are genetically identical (Brakefield, 2003; Wahlsten, 2001).
2.4. Rearing conditions
All flies were reared on a standard medium at 25°C with a 12‐h light/12‐h dark cycle. Mothers were crossed at 1 day old in groups of 10 males and 10 females. F1 males and females were collected as virgins and maintained separately by sex in vials at a density of 24–30 individuals (males) or 12–15 individuals (females). Males and females were then aged 3–4 days before use in the behavioral assays. To more closely mimic natural situations in which most of the female flies will be mated, 3–5 males of a single standard genotype were added to the virgin females the day before the behavioral observation. There is potential that not all females will have mated, but this effect will be random across genotypes and will not create systematic differences in the data.
2.5. Experiment setup
All assays were performed within 2 hr of dawn to avoid any unnecessary variation introduced by circadian rhythms, and replicates were conducted randomly under standardized conditions (25°C, 70% humidity). Each group of two males and one female was put into a single chamber using a paintbrush, for a total of 12 chambers per assay (each chamber is 2.54 cm, VWR cat. no. 89093‐496 see (Signor et al., 2017)). The females were all of the standard y 1 w 1 genotypes, and male genotypes varied across replicates but not within them. Each chamber contained 5 ml of either standard grapefruit medium or medium in which 15% of the water has been replaced with ethanol.
To avoid introducing artifacts from the use of CO2 for sedation, the flies were sedated at 4°C for 10 min. The flies were allowed to acclimate in the chambers for 10 min before commencing recording with PointGrey Grasshopper digital cameras. This is long enough for any initial startle responses to ethanol to have concluded and is standard for behavioral assays (Cho, Heberlein, & Wolf, 2004; Grosjean et al., 2011; Li, Fink, El‐Kholy, & Roeder, 2015). FlyCapture (PointGrey) was used to properly set up the assays for filming, which was automated using VideoGrabber (http://code.google.com/p/video-grabber). Flies were filmed for 10, 20, or 30 min for three replicates each of the ethanol‐ and nonethanol‐exposed conditions, thus each genotype was filmed in 216 arenas. The different time intervals are intended for an analysis of the transcriptome of flies exposed to ethanol for different time periods, which will be reported in subsequent papers. Thus, for each genotype, three replicates of these 12 chambers were performed for each of three time periods in ethanol‐ and nonethanol‐exposed conditions (12 chambers per replicate, three replicates, three time periods, six genotypes, and two conditions). Arenas with damaged flies or mating events were excluded from the final analysis.
2.6. Automatic tracking
In brief, we used a background subtraction method for each frame of the video and a Gaussian mixture model to determine the exact position of each fly. Matching flies in consecutive frames created a representation of fly movement by recording the number of pixels traversed each second by each fly. Please see (Signor et al., 2017) for a more thorough description of the automatic tracking of fly movement. The background subtraction method was modified from that in (Signor et al., 2017), and it is described in the Appendix S1.
2.7. Validation of methods
To obtain a measure of error for our tracking algorithms, we randomly selected 360 20‐s intervals from the videos and compared it with the recorded tracking data. We evaluated the frequency of errors using two metrics: (1) how often the tracking switched between two different males (i.e., the fly was incorrectly identified) and (2) how often the sex of the fly was incorrectly inferred.
2.8. Movement
The phenotype for each arena was defined as the average movement in pixels per second for each 5‐min interval. For each 5‐min interval, the male phenotype was averaged between the two individuals, as they are of the same genotype. Thus, for each interval, there are two data points, one for females and one for the average of the two males.
2.9. Analysis of movement rates and sexual dimorphism
Male and female movements were analyzed separately. We used a t‐test to test for differences between activity levels between species. We used measurements from 779 arenas and normalized the data using a logarithmic transformation. We accounted for repeated measures (the 5‐min intervals for each arena), and fit a linear mixed‐effects model. Random effects included genotype and interactions between genotype, time, and environment. The ID of individual arenas was nested within genotype as a random effect, but it was included with only an intercept term to reflect individual baseline movement but not change over time. Fixed effects were day, environment, time, and the interaction between environment and time. The day of the assay was included to control for any batch effects.
Model fit was evaluated using the lme (nlme) function in R (See Appendix S1 for the R commands). Significance of fixed effect variables was assessed using an F test with the anova (nlme) function. Using the same function, significance of variables of random effect was assessed using a likelihood ratio test to compare model fits with and without the variable of interest. We used REML (restricted maximum likelihood) to fit the lme model parameters. Our fixed effects are common across models, and thus, REML is an appropriate test.
2.10. Calculation of Ψ for different abiotic environments
We used the following formula to estimate Ψ:
See Table 1 for a description of all of the terms. Ψ is the regression coefficient from this model, which we estimate for each abiotic environment with male phenotype as a fixed effect. It is possible that there is feedback between males and females as well; however, we could not evaluate this with our model. The model was fitted with lme (nlme) in R, and REML was used to fit the lme model parameters.
Table 1.
A description of the terms used in each model
Symbol | Meaning | |
---|---|---|
Z jk | Phenotype of the female from the kth trial with the jth male genotype | |
Ψ | Partial regression coefficient of the focal individual on its social partner | |
a | Effect of female genotype and environment | |
|
Mean male movement across all trials containing genotype j | |
X jk | Male movement for the kth trial with the jth movement | |
Ψj | Ψ including both the effects of shared environment and differences in Ψ due to genotype (j) | |
|
Error term |
2.11. Ψj for individual genotypes
To estimate the effect each genotype has on female behavior, we fit a separate regression for each genotype for the effect of male movement on female movement. While Ψ has been reported in the literature for individual genotypes, male and female behaviors can covary for reasons other than Ψ (Bleakley & Brodie, 2009). That is, they can vary due to their shared environment, or due to Ψ, and the two factors will always be confounded. We have used extensive measures to control for environment, so if environmental covariation is affecting our estimates, it should be equivalent across experiments. This does not rule out the possibility that there are genetic differences in the sensitivity to shared environment. We calculate it here with the caveat that measures are confounded with shared environment and refer to Ψ as Ψj. We used the following formula to estimate Ψj:
See Table 1 for a description of the variables. The model was fitted as previously described, using lme (nlme) with REML to fit the lme model parameters, accounting for day effect and repeated measures. To estimate Ψj, genotype was included as a random effect, we calculated the slope of the male phenotype effect for each genotype (Appendix S1).
3. RESULTS
In the following section, we will include a selection of results from Signor et al. (2017) so that we may explicitly compare between these sister species.
3.1. Validation of methods
For D. simulans, when flies were far enough from each other to be distinct (~0.5 mm), our annotations were 98% accurate for both measures of error (track switching and sex misidentification). If the flies are in very close proximity, the accuracy of tracking is 5% lower because assigning tracks consistently to adjacent flies is more difficult. However, such errors have little effect on the overall movement rates because the flies occupy the same position. In D. melanogaster, our error rates are approximately 1% better in both situations (Signor et al., 2017).
3.2. Analysis of movement rates and sexual dimorphism
Overall male movement level in D. simulans was lower than in D. melanogaster, in both ethanol‐ and nonethanol‐exposed conditions (Figure 1) (Difference in mean movement = 15.23 pixel/s, t 1148 = 41.232, p < 10−4). However, the effect of genotype and ethanol was significant for male activity level in this species (Figure 1, Table 2) (male movement, genotype, χ2 (df = 1) = 65.88, p < 10−4; male movement, environment, F 1,705 = 11.89, p = .0006)). There was also a much larger decrease in movement in D. simulans in response to ethanol than in D. melanogaster (Figure 1) (D. melanogaster = 10% decrease, D. simulans = 42% decrease, F 1,1646 = 61.36, p < 10−4). In D. simulans, we observed a positive slope of movement over time (Table 2) (male movement, time, F 1,1870 = 57.13, p < 10−4), and the effect was genotype‐specific (Table 2) (male movement, genotype × time, χ2 (df = 1) = 61.26, p < 10−4), similar to D. melanogaster. Ethanol also affected different genotypes differently (Figure 1, Table 2) (male movement, genotype × environment, χ2 (df = 1) = 12.95, p = .0003). However, unlike D. melanogaster, we did not observe an effect of ethanol on the slope of movement over time in D. simulans (Table 2) (male movement, environment × time, F 1,1870 = 0.32, p = .57). There was also a three‐way interaction, in that the effect of the environment varied over time between genotypes (Table 2) (male movement, genotype × environment × time, χ2 (df = 1) = 42.46, p = .0036). Thus, in both D. simulans and D. melanogaster, there was an effect of genotype on movement and interactions between genotype, environment, and time.
Figure 1.
Male and female movements over time in ethanol and nonethanol environments. (a) The log‐transformed average over all male genotypes for movement in D. melanogaster and D. simulans. The solid and dashed lines represent the linear model fitted to the movement of flies over time in ethanol and nonethanol environments, respectively. (N = 1325 measures of movement). (b) Movement broken down by male genotype for D. simulans. There is genetic variation in the movement of male flies in different environments (G × E). The x‐axis is time in minutes, the y‐axis is the log‐transformed movement of male flies in pixels/second. The solid and dashed lines represent the linear model fitted to the movement of flies over time in ethanol and nonethanol environments, respectively. N = 240 measures of movement (c) The same results as shown in (b) but for female flies. These females are all the same genotype, and thus, differences in movement will be largely due to male genotype
Table 2.
Results of the full model for male movement
Fixed effect | df | F‐value | p‐Value | Random effect | df | LRT‐χ2 | p‐Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
T | 1 | 57.13 | <10−4 | G m | 1 | 65.88 | <10−4 |
E | 1 | 11.88 | .0006 | G m × E | 2 | 12.95 | .0003 |
E × T | 2 | 0.31 | .57 | G m × T | 2 | 61.26 | <10−4 |
Day | 63 | 5.47 | <10−4 | G m × E × T | 3 | 42.46 | .0036 |
Arena | 1 | 599.09 | <10−4 |
The variables are time (T), environment (E), and genotype (G). For fixed‐effect variables, the results of the F test are shown, and for random‐effect variables, the results of the likelihood ratio test (LRT) to compare model fits are shown.
Female movement was affected by the presence of ethanol (Table 3) (female movement, ethanol: F 1,705 = 8.09, p = .0046). It was also affected by male genotype; however, there was no interaction between the two terms indicating that the effect of genotype varied in the same way between environments (Table 3) (female movement, male genotype: χ2 (df = 1) = 16.99, p = .0019) (female movement, male genotype × environment: χ2 (df = 1) = 0.013, p = .91). For female movement, there was, however, an interaction between male genotype and time (Table 3) (female movement, male genotype × time: χ2 (df = 1) = 16.99, p = .0002).
Table 3.
Results of the full model for female movement
Fixed effect | numdf | dendf | F‐value | p‐Value | Random effect | df | LRT‐χ2 | p‐Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
T | 1 | 1870 | 2.06 | .15 | G m | 1 | 16.99 | .0019 |
E | 1 | 705 | 8.09 | .0046 | G m × E | 2 | 0.013 | .91 |
E × T | 1 | 1870 | 3.19 | .075 | G m × T | 2 | 16.99 | .0002 |
Day | 67 | 705 | 4.85 | <10−4 | G m × E × T | 3 | 3.82 | .051 |
Arena | 1 | 398.35 | <10−4 |
The variables are time (T), environment (E), and genotype (G). For fixed‐effect variables, the results of the F test are shown; for random‐effect variables, the results of the likelihood ratio test (LRT) to compare model fits are shown.
Locomotion in D. melanogaster was sexually dimorphic, and we previously confirmed the results of other studies that males move approximately three times more than females (Long & Rice, 2007) (Figure 1a–c). In each species, a single female genotype was used, so any overall variation in movement is due to the abiotic environment and to females’ interaction partners. Sex and genotype are confounded because females are a different genotype than males. We tested the significance of the difference between males and females in D. simulans using a linear regression model and found that males moved slightly less than females. This is in contrast with D. melanogaster, where males move 2.7× as much as females (0.77× vs 2.7×) (F 1,1642 = 35.29, p < 10−4).
3.3. Calculation of Ψ for different abiotic environments
Here, we estimated Ψ in D. simulans and compared Ψ between D. melanogaster and D. simulans (Figure 2a,b). Male phenotype was defined, as before, as the average movement of a male in either ethanol‐ or nonethanol‐exposed conditions. In D. simulans, we estimated Ψ = 0.13 in nonethanol environments and Ψ = 0.06 in ethanol environments. This is very similar to the estimates of Ψ in D. melanogaster Ψ (0.11 and 0.04, respectively) (Figure 2a,b). The authors note that in both cases a single female genotype was used, and it is possible that the inclusion of additional genotypes would alter this comparison.
Figure 2.
Ψ calculated for each environment in (a) D. melanogaster and (b) D. simulans. The fitted values were calculated using generalized mixed model (see Methods)
3.4. Ψj for individual genotypes
To determine whether the Ψ is genotype‐specific, and whether that varies with environment, we tested for variation in Ψj (Tables 4 and 5, Appendix S1). With ethanol, we found significant differences between genotypes (genotype × male movement, LRT result for models with and without a genotype × male movement term: χ2 (df = 1) = 4.95, p =.03). This effect is not significant in environments without ethanol (genotype × male movement, LRT results for models with and without a genotype × male movement term: χ2 (df = 1) = .01, p = .91) (Tables 2 and 5). What is unique to D. simulans is that they did not vary in unexpected ways between environments, but rather all changed in a consistent manner. That is to say that measures of Ψj were reduced uniformly across genotypes upon exposure to ethanol (genotype × male movement × environment, χ2 (df = 1) = 0.00002, p = .99) (Tables 2 and 5). In D. melanogaster, Ψj changed differently in different genotypes between environments (genotype × male movement × environment χ2 (df = 1) = 18.15, p < 10−4) (Table 4) (Signor et al., 2017).
Table 4.
Ψj for each genotype in Drosophila melanogaster
Genotype | N | ETOH | N | Non‐ETOH |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 82 | 0.25 | 81 | 0.27 |
2 | 101 | 0.33 | 81 | 0.39 |
3 | 77 | 0.31 | 84 | 0.16 |
4 | 96 | 0.18 | 75 | 0.41 |
5 | 79 | 0.25 | 80 | 0.36 |
6 | 96 | 0.19 | 84 | 0.54 |
Shared environment may be conflated with estimates of Ψj. This table is reproduced from (Signor et al., 2017).
Table 5.
Ψj for each genotype in Drosophila simulans
Genotype | N | ETOH | N | Non‐ETOH |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 47 | 0.29 | 92 | 0.79 |
2 | 41 | 0.16 | 75 | 0.78 |
3 | 38 | 0.38 | 100 | 0.70 |
4 | 42 | 0.38 | 73 | 0.83 |
5 | 49 | 0.37 | 97 | 0.75 |
6 | 47 | 0.15 | 78 | 0.66 |
Shared environment may be conflated with estimates of Ψj.
4. DISCUSSION
The role of IGEs and how they vary among environments and species is an important part of understanding the impact of social environments in evolution (Bailey & Zuk, 2012). Few studies have measured IGEs using Ψ for any trait, and this is the only study that compares Ψ for closely related species (Kent, Azanchi, Smith, Formosa, & Levine, 2008; Bleakley & Brodie, 2009; Chenoweth et al., 2010; Bailey & Zuk, 2012; Kazancıoğlu et al., 2012; Bailey & Hoskins, 2014; Bailey et al. 2013). We have demonstrated that despite a reversal in sexual dimorphism for locomotion, Ψ did not evolve between these species. We have shown that for locomotion in D. simulans and D. melanogaster, Ψ is context‐specific, varying in different environments. However, in D. melanogaster, there was a significant interaction with the environment, while in D. simulans there was not, indicating that this interaction can evolve between species.
There is a different relationship between male and female locomotions in D. melanogaster as compared to D. simulans. In D. melanogaster, males move 2.7× more than females, while in D. simulans, males move only 0.77× as much as females. This difference in sexual dimorphism could have implications for sexual selection in each system given that in D. melanogaster, selection on locomotion is sexually antagonistic (Long & Rice, 2007). Absence of sexual dimorphism does not necessarily indicate a lack of sexual conflict, although this has not been investigated in D. simulans. Locomotion in other members of this species group has not been well characterized, although there is some evidence that D. melanogaster is more active than its close relatives overall (Cobb, Connolly, & Burnet, 1987). D. simulans and D. melanogaster do have more divergent courtship behaviors than other members of their species group, likely because they are both cosmopolitan species that occupy many of the same substrates (Cobb, Burnet, & Connolly, 1986; Cobb, Connolly, & Burnet, 1985).
The evolution of locomotory behavior, and of sexual dimorphism in locomotory behavior, has not been well characterized in many systems. The best‐studied systems are lizards and D. melanogaster, and in both, locomotion is an important component of sexual selection and fitness (Colomb & Brembs, 2014; Husak & Fox, 2008; Lailvaux et al., 2003; Perry, 1996; Peterson & Husak, 2006). In general, this manifests as males that move faster, but there are exceptions to this that also demonstrate the evolutionary lability of locomotory behavior. For example, in D. suzukii, females move 4× more than males, and in one species of lizard movement was not sexually dimorphic despite more active males having higher reproductive success (Ferguson et al., 2015; Peterson & Husak, 2006).
One large difference between selection regimes for females of D. melanogaster and D. simulans is that in D. simulans, multiple matings are beneficial for female fecundity and neutral with respect to longevity, while in D. melanogaster, it is neutral to fecundity and deleterious to longevity (Chapman, Liddle, Kalb, & Wolfner, 1995; Kuijper et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 2008; Wigby & Chapman, 2004). Furthermore, other metrics of sexual selection, such as the cost of female choice on longevity or fecundity, indicate that they are largely neutral in D. simulans but are costly in D. melanogaster (Friberg & Arnqvist, 2003; Pitnick, 1991; Pitnick & Garcia‐Gonzalez, 2002; Taylor, Wedell, & Hosken, 2007; Taylor et al., 2008). While the present work does not measure variation in fitness and therefore cannot make any conclusions about this in relation to locomotion, it may be an interesting question for future research to consider that this may be a manifestation of different selective regimes in the two species. For example, because multiple matings are beneficial to both females and males in D. simulans, selection toward a shared level of activity may be beneficial. In contrast, D. melanogaster has been selected for higher activity levels because of sexually antagonistic selection (Long & Rice, 2007). This would suggest a lack of sexually antagonistic selection for locomotion in D. simulans.
It is interesting that despite these differences in dimorphism and patterns of context‐specific change in locomotion overall, estimated Ψ‐values are approximately equal in D. melanogaster and D. simulans. This supports two conclusions, (1) that overall differences between abiotic environments for Ψ are not due variation in ethanol tolerance (2) Ψ and locomotion within each species likely have a separate genetic basis, as levels of activity are very different between species and Ψ is not. Thus, the two traits would be able to evolve independently, contributing to sexually antagonistic selection in D. melanogaster and sexual selection in D. simulans. It is clear that despite a reversal in sexual dimorphism, and different sexual dynamics, Ψ has not evolved between these two species.
In light of the fact that D. melanogaster is adapted to substrates with high concentrations of ethanol, while D. simulans is not, it is interesting that it is D. melanogaster that exhibits a Ψj × environment interaction. There is considerable spatial heterogeneity in the ethanol content of the environment for Drosophila, which implies that not all genotypes will encounter ethanol‐rich substrates (Hoffmann & McKechnie, 1991; McKenzie & McKechnie, 1979). Polymorphisms for ethanol tolerance are widespread in Drosophila species. It has previously been shown that variable exposure to ethanol in D. melanogaster maintains a balanced polymorphism in the Aldehyde dehydrogenase gene responsible for detoxifying acetaldehyde derived from dietary ethanol (Chakraborty & Fry, 2016). Furthermore, there is a long history of documenting variation and latitudinal clines in Alcohol dehydrogenase, which transforms ethanol into acetaldehyde (Dorado & Barbancho, 1984; Gibson et al., 1981; Mercot et al., 1994; Zhu & Fry, 2015; Ziolo & Parsons, 1982). Thus, it would be interesting to consider that adaptations for Ψ on ethanol substrates could be maintained as polymorphisms in the population, including locomotion. If this were the case, this would not have occurred in D. simulans due to its avoidance of substrates containing high concentrations of ethanol. While it is slightly counterintuitive to imagine than a lack of selection maintains less variation in a trait, this is the expectation if spatially variable selection is common, and polymorphisms are conditionally beneficial.
Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans are both cosmopolitan species commonly found in the same habitats. D. simulans readily evolves increased ethanol tolerance in the laboratory, so it may be that selection for whatever benefit ethanol provides resulted in different trade‐offs in D. melanogaster compared to D. simulans (Joshi & Thompson, 1997; Lefèvre, de Roode, Kacsoh, & Schlenke, 2012). For example, polymorphisms at the Aldehyde dehydrogenase locus in D. melanogaster are detrimental in the absence of ethanol as they result in a reduction in the efficacy of processing other targets (Chakraborty & Fry, 2016). However, ethanol‐rich substrates provide some protection against parasites for D. melanogaster, while D. simulans appears to mount a stronger immune response instead (i.e., fight or flight) (Lefèvre et al., 2012; Milan, Kacsoh, & Schlenke, 2012). The relationship between ethanol, parasite resistance, adult and larval tolerance, and caloric benefit is not entirely clear despite a number of studies on the subject (Chakir et al., 1993; Dorado & Barbancho, 1984; Fry et al., 2004, 2008; Garcin, Cote, Lau You Hin, Chawla, & Radouco‐Thomas, 1986; Hodges, Laskowski, Squadrito, De Luca, & Leips, 2013; Kerver & Rotman, 1987; McKechnie & Morgan, 1982; McKenzie & McKechnie, 1979; Mercot et al., 1994; Milan et al., 2012; Muhammed‐Ali & Burnet, 1995; Thomson et al., 1991).
Attempting to estimate Ψj requires confounding the effect of shared environment and the effect of different genes in the environment. As such it is an overestimation of the effect of individual genotypes, which is most likely why values of Ψj are consistently higher than Ψ. However, there is no other way to attempt to calculate this term, and extensive environmental controls were used such that the effect of shared environment should have been constant across assays. This does not preclude the existence of genetic variability in the response to shared environment in males, and thus, it is possible that even the extensive environmental controls do not eliminate this effect. As such Ψj is a valuable contribution to understanding the patterns in our data, however the authors stress that absolute values are not interpretable and it must be considered with caution.
In conclusion, we have explored differences in Ψ for locomotion between abiotic environments for two closely related species, D. simulans and D. melanogaster. We have found extensive evidence for context‐specific Ψ that varies between species, with D. melanogaster having interaction terms with abiotic environment that are lacking in D. simulans. We have found that Ψ is positive in both abiotic environments for both species, and approximately equal within a given abiotic environment between species. We have also found evidence for differences in sexual dimorphism in locomotion despite similarities in Ψ between species. These findings present interesting pathways for future research into the evolution of sexual antagonism and sexual selection, and the role of IGEs in both of these processes.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
None declared.
DATA ACCESSIBILITY
Data deposited in the dryad repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.p3215 and https://doi.org/datadryad.org/resource/10.5061/dryad.bf278.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
SS performed the experiments, wrote the manuscript, and created the figures. MA analyzed the data. PM and SN conceived of the experiments and contributed to their design.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Thanks to J. Butler and G. Mackerel for assistance during the production of this manuscript. Thanks to Nicole Shadman, Victoria Paterson, Zachary Polonus, Kristina Cortez, Lauren Cline, Arshjot Khokhar, Rebecca Mason, and Melissa Ling for assistance in the production of this data. This work was supported by grants MH100879, GM102227, and HG002790.
Signor SA, Abbasi M, Marjoram P, Nuzhdin SV. Conservation of social effects (Ψ) between two species of Drosophila despite reversal of sexual dimorphism. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:10031–10041. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.3523
REFERENCES
- Bailey, J. K. , Genung, M. A. , Ware, I. , Gorman, C. , Van Nuland, M. E. , Long, H. , et al. (2013). Indirect genetic effects: an evolutionary mechanism linking feedbacks, genotypic diversity and coadaptation in a climate change context. Funct Ecol, 28, 87–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12154. [Google Scholar]
- Bailey, N. W. , & Hoskins, J. L. (2014). Detecting cryptic indirect genetic effects. Evolution, 68, 1871–1882. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.2014.68.issue-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey, N. W. , & Zuk, M. (2012). Socially flexible female choice differs among populations of the Pacific field cricket: Geographical variation in the interaction coefficient psi (Ψ). Genetics, 279, 3589–3596. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.0631 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bateman, A. J. (1948). Intra‐sexual selection in Drosophila . Heredity, 2, 349–368. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1948.21 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijma, P. (2014). The quantitative genetics of indirect genetic effects: A selective review of modelling issues. Heredity, 112, 61–69. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.2013.15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bleakley, B. H. , & Brodie, E. D. (2009). Indirect genetic effects influence antipredator behavior in guppies: Estimates of the coefficient of interaction psi and the inheritance of reciprocity. Evolution, 63, 1796–1806. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.2009.63.issue-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brakefield, P. M. (2003). Artificial selection and the development of ecologically relevant phenotypes. Ecology, 84, 1661–1671. https://doi.org/10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[1661:ASATDO]2.0.CO;2 [Google Scholar]
- Brinker, T. , Bijma, P. , Visscher, J. , Rodenburg, T. B. , & Ellen, E. D. (2014). Plumage condition in laying hens: Genetic parameters for direct and indirect effects in two purebred layer lines. Genetics, Selection, Evolution, 46, 33 https://doi.org/10.1186/1297-9686-46-33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camerlink, I. , Turner, S. P. , Bijma, P. , & Bolhuis, J. E. (2013). Indirect genetic effects and housing conditions in relation to aggressive behaviour in pigs. PLoS ONE, 8, e65136 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camerlink, I. , Ursinus, W. W. , Bijma, P. , Kemp, B. , & Bolhuis, J. E. (2015). Indirect genetic effects for growth rate in domestic pigs alter aggressive and manipulative biting behaviour. Behavior Genetics, 45, 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-014-9671-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campo, D. , Lehmann, K. , Fjeldsted, C. , Souaiaia, T. , Kao, J. , & Nuzhdin, S. V. (2013). Whole‐genome sequencing of two North American Drosophila melanogaster populations reveals genetic differentiation and positive selection. Molecular Ecology, 22, 5084–5097. https://doi.org/10.1111/mec.12468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chakir, M. , Peridy, O. , Capy, P. , Pla, E. , & David, J. R. (1993). Adaptation to alcoholic fermentation in Drosophila: A parallel selection imposed by environmental ethanol and acetic acid. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90, 3621–3625. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.90.8.3621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chakraborty, M. , & Fry, J. D. (2016). Evidence that environmental heterogeneity maintains a detoxifying enzyme polymorphism in Drosophila melanogaster . Current Biology, 26, 219–223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman, T. , Liddle, L. F. , Kalb, J. M. , & Wolfner, M. F. (1995). Cost of mating in Drosophila melanogaster females is mediated by male accessory gland products. Nature, 373, 241–244. https://doi.org/10.1038/373241a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chenoweth, S. F. , Rundle, H. D. , & Blows, M. W. (2010). Experimental evidence for the evolution of indirect genetic effects: Changes in the interaction effect coefficient, psi, due to sexual selection. Evolution, 64, 1849–1856. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.00952.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho, W. , Heberlein, U. , & Wolf, F. W. (2004). Habituation of an odorant‐induced startle response in Drosophila . Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 3, 127–137. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.2004.3.issue-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cobb, M. , Burnet, B. , & Connolly, K. (1986). The structure of courtship in the Drosophila melanogaster species sub‐group. Behaviour, 97, 182–211. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853986X00379 [Google Scholar]
- Cobb, M. , Connolly, K. , & Burnet, B. (1985). Courtship behaviour in the melanogaster species sub‐group of Drosophila . Behaviour, 95, 203–230. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853985X00136 [Google Scholar]
- Cobb, M. , Connolly, K. , & Burnet, B. (1987). The relationship between locomotor activity and courtship in the melanogaster species sub‐group of Drosophila . Animal Behavior, 35, 705–713. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80106-9 [Google Scholar]
- Colomb, J. , & Brembs, B. (2014). Sub‐strains of Drosophila Canton‐S differ markedly in their locomotor behavior. F1000Research, 3, 176 https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.4263.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David, J. , & Bocquet, C. (1976). Compared toxicities of different alcohols for two Drosophila sibling species: D. melanogaster and D. simulans . Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology. C: Comparative Pharmacology, 54, 71–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4492(76)90066-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson, W. J. , Rowan, R. G. , & Brennan, M. D. (1984). Regulatory gene evolution: Adaptive differences in expression of alcohol dehydrogenase in Drosophila melanogaster and Drosophila simulans . Heredity, 52(Pt 2), 215–225. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1984.23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dingemanse, N. J. , & Araya‐Ajoy, Y. G. (2015). Interacting personalities: Behavioural ecology meets quantitative genetics. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 30, 88–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2014.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dorado, G. , & Barbancho, M. (1984). Differential responses in Drosophila melanogaster to environmental ethanol: Modification of fitness components at the Adh locus. Heredity, 53, 309–320. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1984.90 [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson, C. T. J. , O'Neill, T. L. , Audsley, N. , & Isaac, R. E. (2015). The sexually dimorphic behaviour of adult Drosophila suzukii: Elevated female locomotor activity and loss of siesta is a post‐mating response. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218, 3855–3861. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.125468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiumera, A. C. , & Dumont, B. L. (2006). Natural variation in male‐induced “cost‐of‐mating”and allele‐specific association with male reproductive genes in Drosophila melanogaster . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 361, 355–361. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friberg, U. , & Arnqvist, G. (2003). Fitness effects of female mate choice: Preferred males are detrimental for Drosophila melanogaster females. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 16, 797–811. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00597.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fry, J. D. (2014). Mechanisms of naturally evolved ethanol resistance in Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Experimental Biology, 217, 3996–4003. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.110510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fry, J. D. , Bahnck, C. M. , Mikucki, M. , Phadnis, N. , & Slattery, W. C. (2004). Dietary ethanol mediates selection on Aldehyde dehydrogenase activity in Drosophila melanogaster . Integrative and Comparative Biology, 44, 275–283. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/44.4.275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fry, J. D. , Donlon, K. , & Saweikis, M. (2008). A worldwide polymorphism in aldehyde dehydrogenase in Drosophila melanogaster: Evidence for selection mediated by dietary ethanol. Evolution, 62, 66–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00288.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcin, F. , Cote, J. , Lau You Hin, G. , Chawla, S. , & Radouco‐Thomas, S. (1986). Ethanol metabolizing system in Drosophila. Aldehyde dehydrogenase: Functional aspects in adult and during development. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 10, 167–176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-5846(86)90071-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J. B. , May, T. W. , & Wilks, A. V. (1981). Genetic variation at the alcohol dehydrogenase locus in Drosophila melanogaster in relation to environmental variation: Ethanol levels in breeding sites and allozyme frequencies. Oecologia, 51, 191–198. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00540600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J. B. , & Wilks, A. V. (1988). The alcohol dehydrogenase polymorphism of Drosophila melanogaster in relation to environmental ethanol, ethanol tolerance and alcohol dehydrogenase activity. Heredity, 60(Pt 3), 403–414. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1988.58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosjean, Y. , Rytz, R. , Farine, J.‐P. , Abuin, L. , Cortot, J. , Jefferis, G. S. X. E. , & Benton, R. (2011). An olfactory receptor for food‐derived odours promotes male courtship in Drosophila . Nature, 478, 236–240. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodges, T. K. , Laskowski, K. L. , Squadrito, G. L. , De Luca, M. , & Leips, J. (2013). Defense traits of larval Drosophila melanogaster exhibit genetically based trade‐offs against different species of parasitoids. Evolution, 67, 749–760. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.2013.67.issue-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann, A. A. , & McKechnie, S. W. (1991). Heritable variation in resource utilization and response in a winery population of Drosophila melanogaster . Evolution, 45, 1000–1015. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1991.tb04366.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Husak, J. F. , & Fox, S. F. (2008). Sexual selection on locomotor performance. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 10, 213–228. [Google Scholar]
- Joshi, A. , & Thompson, J. N. (1997). Adaptation and specialization in a two‐resource environment in Drosophila species. Evolution, 51, 846–855. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.1997.51.issue-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazancıoğlu, E. , Klug, H. , & Alonzo, S. H. (2012). The evolution of social interactions changes predictions about interacting phenotypes. Evolution, 66, 2056–2064. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.2012.66.issue-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kent, C. , Azanchi, R. , Smith, B. , Formosa, A. , & Levine, J. D. (2008). Social context influences chemical communication in D. melanogaster males . Current Biology, 18, 1384–1389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerver, W. , & Rotman, G. (1987). Development of ethanol tolerance in relation to the alcohol dehydrogenase locus in Drosophila melanogaster. II. The influence of phenotypic adaptation and maternal. Heredity, 58, 239–248. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1987.38 [Google Scholar]
- Kuijper, B. , Stewart, A. D. , & Rice, W. R. (2006). The cost of mating rises nonlinearly with copulation frequency in a laboratory population of Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 19, 1795–1802. https://doi.org/10.1111/jeb.2006.19.issue-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lailvaux, S. P. , Alexander, G. J. , & Whiting, M. J. (2003). Sex‐based differences and similarities in locomotor performance, thermal preferences, and escape behaviour in the lizard Platysaurus intermedius wilhelmi . Physiological and Biochemical Zoology, 76, 511–521. https://doi.org/10.1086/376423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lefèvre, T. , de Roode, J. C. , Kacsoh, B. Z. , & Schlenke, T. A. (2012). Defence strategies against a parasitoid wasp in Drosophila: Fight or flight? Biology Letters, 8, 230–233. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.0725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, Y. , Fink, C. , El‐Kholy, S. , & Roeder, T. (2015). The octopamine receptor octß2R is essential for ovulation and fertilization in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster . Archives of Insect Biochemistry and Physiology, 88, 168–178. https://doi.org/10.1002/arch.v88.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long, T. A. F. , & Rice, W. R. (2007). Adult locomotory activity mediates intralocus sexual conflict in a laboratory‐adapted population of Drosophila melanogaster . Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274, 3105–3112. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maklakov, A. A. , & Arnqvist, G. (2009). Testing for direct and indirect effects of mate choice by manipulating female choosiness. Current Biology, 19, 1903–1906. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marie‐Orleach, L. , Vogt‐Burri, N. , Mouginot, P. , Schlatter, A. , Vizoso, D. B. , Bailey, N. W. , & Schärer, L. (2017). Indirect genetic effects and sexual conflicts: Partner genotype influences multiple morphological and behavioral reproductive traits in a flatworm. Evolution, 71, 1232–1245. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.13218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKechnie, S. W. , & Morgan, P. (1982). Alcohol dehydrogenase polymorphism of Drosophila melanogaster: Aspects of alcohol and temperature variation in the larval environment. Australian Journal of Biological Sciences, 35, 85–93. https://doi.org/10.1071/BI9820085 [Google Scholar]
- McKenzie, J. A. , & McKechnie, S. W. (1979). A comparative study of resource utilization in natural populations of Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans . Oecologia, 40, 299–309. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00345326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mercot, H. , Defaye, D. , Capy, P. , Pla, E. , & David, J. R. (1994). Alcohol tolerance, ADH activity, and ecological niche of Drosophila species. Evolution, 3, 746–757. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.1994.48.issue-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milan, N. F. , Kacsoh, B. Z. , & Schlenke, T. A. (2012). Alcohol consumption as self‐medication against blood‐borne parasites in the fruit fly. Current Biology, 22, 488–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A. J. , Brodie III, E. D. , & Wolf, J. B. (1997). Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process: I. Direct and indirect genetic effects of social interactions. Evolution, 51, 1352–1362. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.1997.51.issue-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A. J. , & Pizzari, T. (2005). Quantitative genetic models of sexual conflict based on interacting phenotypes. American Naturalist, 165, S88–S97. https://doi.org/10.1086/429354 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muhammed‐Ali, A. , & Burnet, B. (1995). Ethanol tolerance and variation at the alcohol dehydrogenase (Adh) locus of Drosophila mauritiana . Heredity, 74, 438–444. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1995.61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuzhdin, S. V. , Friesen, M. L. , & McIntyre, L. M. (2012). Genotype–phenotype mapping in a post‐GWAS world. Trends in Genetics, 28, 421–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tig.2012.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson‐Manning, C. F. , Wagner, M. R. , & Mitchell‐Olds, T. (2012). Adaptive evolution: Evaluating empirical support for theoretical predictions. Nature Reviews Genetics, 13, 867–877. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg3322 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons, P. A. (1977). Larval reaction to alcohol as an indicator of resource utilization differences between Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans . Oecologia, 30, 141–146. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00345417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons, P. A. , & King, S. B. (1977). Ethanol: Larval discrimination between two Drosophila sibling species. Experientia, 33, 898–899. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01951269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons, P. A. , & Spence, G. E. (1981). Ethanol utilization: Threshold differences among three Drosophila species. American Naturalist, 117, 568–571. https://doi.org/10.1086/283740 [Google Scholar]
- Partridge, L. , Green, A. , & Fowler, K. (1987). Effects of egg‐production and of exposure to males on female survival in Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Insect Physiology, 33, 745–749. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1910(87)90060-6 [Google Scholar]
- Peeters, K. , Eppink, T. T. , Ellen, E. D. , & Visscher, J. (2012). Indirect genetic effects for survival in domestic chickens (Gallus gallus) are magnified in crossbred genotypes and show a parent‐of‐origin effect. Genetics, 192, 705–713. https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.112.142554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry, G. (1996). The evolution of sexual dimorphism in the lizard Anolis polylepis (Iguania): Evidence from intraspecific variation in foraging behavior and diet. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 74, 1238–1245. https://doi.org/10.1139/z96-137 [Google Scholar]
- Peterson, C. C. , & Husak, J. F. (2006). Locomotor performance and sexual selection: Individual variation in sprint speed of collared lizards (Crotaphytus collaris). Copeia, 2, 216–224. https://doi.org/10.1643/0045-8511(2006)6[216:LPASSI]2.0.CO;2 [Google Scholar]
- Pitnick, S. (1991). Male size influences mate fecundity and remating interval in Drosophila melanogaster . Animal Behavior, 41, 735–745. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80340-9 [Google Scholar]
- Pitnick, S. , & Garcia‐Gonzalez, F. (2002). Harm to females increases with male body size in Drosophila melanogaster . Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 269, 1821–1828. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saltz, J. B. (2013). Genetic composition of social groups influences male aggressive behaviour and fitness in natural genotypes of Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics, 280, 20131926 https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.1926 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Signor, S. A. , Abbasi, M. , Marjoram, P. , & Nuzhdin, S. V. (2017). Indirect genetic effects mediate a conflict in locomotion among Drosophila melanogaster males and females. Evolution, 71, 1765–1775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, M. L. , Wedell, N. , & Hosken, D. J. (2007). Sexual selection and female fitness in Drosophila simulans . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62, 721–728. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, M. L. , Wigmore, C. , Hodgson, D. J. , Wedell, N. , & Hosken, D. J. (2008). Multiple mating increases female fitness in Drosophila simulans . Animal Behavior, 76, 963–970. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.015 [Google Scholar]
- Thomson, M. S. , Jacobson, J. W. , & Laurie, C. C. (1991). Comparison of alcohol dehydrogenase expression in Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans . Molecular Biology and Evolution, 8, 31–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tompkins, L. , Gross, A. C. , Hall, J. C. , Gailey, D. A. , & Siegel, R. W. (1982). The role of female movement in the sexual behavior of Drosophila melanogaster . Behavior Genetics, 12, 295–307. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wahlsten, D. (2001). Standardizing tests of mouse behavior: Reasons, recommendations, and reality. Physiology & Behavior, 73, 695–704. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00527-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wigby, S. , & Chapman, T. (2004). Female resistance to male harm evolves in response to manipulation of sexual conflict. Evolution, 58, 1028–1037. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.2004.58.issue-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, J. B. , Brodie III, E. D. , Cheverud, J. M. , Moore, A. J. , & Wade, M. J. (1998). Evolutionary consequences of indirect genetic effects. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, 64–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(97)01233-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang, H.‐P. , & Nuzhdin, S. V. (2003). Fitness costs of Doc expression are insufficient to stabilize its copy number in Drosophila melanogaster . Molecular Biology and Evolution, 20, 800–804. https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msg087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu, J. , & Fry, J. D. (2015). Preference for ethanol in feeding and oviposition in temperate and tropical populations of Drosophila melanogaster . Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 155, 64–70. https://doi.org/10.1111/eea.12285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziolo, L. K. , & Parsons, P. A. (1982). Ethanol tolerance, alcohol‐dehydrogenase activity and Adh allozymes in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetica, 57, 231–237. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00056488 [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data deposited in the dryad repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.p3215 and https://doi.org/datadryad.org/resource/10.5061/dryad.bf278.