Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Dec 11.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017 Oct;23(9-10):916–929. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000807

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Historical Neuropsychological Perspective

E Mark Mahone 1,2, Martha B Denckla 1,2
PMCID: PMC5724393  NIHMSID: NIHMS924361  PMID: 29198277

Abstract

The behavior patterns of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention that would ultimately become recognized as Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) have been described for centuries. Nevertheless, in the past 35 years, advances in diagnostic methods, identification of biomarkers, and treatments have advanced at an exponential rate. ADHD is now recognized as the most common behavioral disorder of childhood, with risks extending well into adulthood for both males and females, leading to its identification as a significant public health issue. This historical neuropsychological review of ADHD emphasizes scientific highlights in the past 35 years related to ADHD, including the evolution of the diagnosis (from Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood to ADHD), influential theories (executive functions, cognitive-energetic, delay aversion), landmark treatment studies (Multimodal Treatment of ADHD [MTA] and Preschool ADHD Treatment Study [PATS]), and advances in brain mapping techniques (anatomic, functional, and resting state magnetic resonance imaging, diffusion tensor imaging). The review concludes by highlighting the challenges of studying and treating a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder like ADHD, with emphasis on associated disorders and conditions (learning disabilities, sluggish cognitive tempo), special populations (girls, preschoolers, adults), and recommendations for scientific inquiry in the next 35 years. Neuropsychologists are well positioned to address the clinical and research challenges of the next generation of studies, especially involving advances in understanding the sexual dimor.phism, full developmental course, and dynamic risks associated with ADHD.

Keywords: Executive function, Neuroimaging, Childhood, Motor, Developmental disorder

INTRODUCTION

Although described over 200 years ago, the research and clinical literature characterizing the condition now known as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) has advanced substantially in the past 35 years. ADHD is now recognized as the most common behavioral disorder of childhood affecting nearly approximately 10% of U.S. children ages 4–17 years (Pastor, Reuben, Duran, & Hawkins, 2015), although worldwide estimates are lower (around 5%) and differ as a function of varying diagnostic methods, practice guidelines, and access to specialized clinical care (Polanczyk, de Lima, Horta, Biederman, & Rohde, 2007).

Symptoms of ADHD typically begin in early childhood, have a chronic course, and adversely affect the academics and social interactions of affected individuals (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2011). Despite advances in diagnostic processes, identification of biomarkers, and multimodal treatment, individuals with ADHD often struggle both socially and academically, even when treated. Approximately two-thirds of individuals have at least one co-existing developmental or psychiatric condition, such that “simple” ADHD is considered the exception rather than the rule (Larson, Russ, Kahn, & Halfon, 2011).

ADHD is now recognized a major public health issue (Hinshaw & Ellison, 2016). Recent estimates suggest that the annual incremental societal costs associated with ADHD total over 143 billion dollars in the United States alone (Doshi et al., 2012), with one-fourth of these costs borne by the U.S. education system. In school, children with ADHD are more likely to experience increased learning problems, school absences, troublesome relationships with peers, and a higher rate of ambulatory medical visits. As adults, these risks and associated problems result in 33% reduced earning and 15% increase in use of social assistance among those with ADHD (Fletcher, 2014). The costly toll that ADHD takes on individual adjustment, family life, schools, health care, and social services underscores the importance of understanding its etiology, developmental course, and outcomes, with the ultimate goal of earlier (accurate) identification, treatment, and the reduction of individual and societal burden.

ADHD AS A DISORDER: HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVES

ADHD is now recognized as a persistent neurodevelopmental disorder with known structural and functional brain anomalies. The present conceptualization of ADHD has evolved in parallel with the field of psychiatry more generally in the past 35 years, acknowledging the shift from psychological and environmental explanations of behavior to biological (or biopsychosocial) framework, based on the exponential advances in neuroimaging and genetic research (Faraone et al., 2015). The trend toward a biological explanation for ADHD was well established even before the term was coined in 1980, especially among psychiatrists who hypothesized a neurochemical basis (likely a dopamine deficiency) for the condition later described as Minimal Brain Dysfunction and ultimately ADHD.

Precursors to the formal adoption of the diagnosis of ADHD, going back to the nineteenth century, included the Hyperactive/Hyperkinetic Syndrome and ultimately the broad category of Minimal Brain Dysfunction. It is notable that the first use of a stimulant medication as a calming influence on hyperactivity was in children considered to have “brain damage,” rather than those with genetically based neurodevelopmental disorders such as Down syndrome (Bradley, 1937). The group of individuals defined as “brain damaged” was broadly defined by the combination of hyperactivity and motor impairments (but to a more minor degree falling short of the diagnosis of cerebral palsy).

The conference that introduced the term Minimal Brain Dysfunction built on the idea of “brain damage” as a combination of hyperactivity and minor motor dysfunction, and also added a heterogeneous set of learning and perceptual anomalies to the description (Clements, 1966). Initially, dyslexia was set aside as a very special category, but later for educational classification was subsumed under the category of learning disabilities.

In the years between 1966 and the 1975, implementation of the U.S. federal special education law (PL 94–142), some states had classes for individuals with “brain damage,” while others had classes for those with “neurological impairments”; however, admission requirements for both included symptoms of hyperactivity, minor motor anomalies, and perceptual or learning impairments, but with IQ in the normal range. Thus, before 1980, the bond between minor motor anomalies, often referred to as soft signs, and the behavioral precursor to ADHD was very strong, such that one publication (Denckla & Rudel, 1978) established that a quantitative motor examination could discriminate typically developing peers from hyperactive boys who were free of any learning disabilities.

DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA: HYPERKINETIC DISORDER, ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER, AND ADHD

Diagnostic and Statistical Manuals

By 1980, research by psychologists using continuous performance tests, most prominently Virginia Douglas (Douglas & Peters, 1979), became so influential that the mental health profession as a whole started to recognize the relationship between attention and the condition formerly known as the Hyperactive/Hyperkinetic Syndrome (or the broader version, Minimal Brain Dysfunction). The result was that attention became, and has remained, a central characterizing descriptor of the disorder. Eventually, the Hyperactive Reaction of Childhood diagnosis of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders Second Edition (DSM-II) (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1968) was renamed Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) with publication of the DSM-III (APA, 1980). Subtypes of the disorder were also first introduced in the DSM-III, and included Attention Deficit Disorder, with or without Hyperactivity. Subsequent revision (DSM-III-R; APA, 1987) renamed the condition again to its present label (Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder), but did away with subtypes in favor of a separate diagnosis, Undifferentiated ADHD, used for the individuals presenting without prominent hyperactivity. Subtypes were again introduced in the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), and included Inattentive, Hyperactive/Impulsive and Combined subtypes.

While there has been consistent support for the latent structure of symptom groups (inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity), the validity of the three subtypes as was called into question in the research literature, given the temporal instability of the categories, inconsistent practices for symptom counts between home versus school informants (Valo & Tannock, 2010), and sexual dimorphism in symptom manifestation (Willcutt et al., 2012). Also, the Hyperactive/Impulsive subtype was shown to be rare outside the preschool setting, and more appropriately considered a developmental precursor to the Combined subtype (Barkley, 1997). Concurrently, the DSM-IV age-of-onset criterion (requiring symptoms producing impairment by age 7 years) was also questioned, following observation of later symptom onset in girls (Todd, Huang, & Henderson, 2008), and the recognition of a potentially separate adult-onset manifestation (Barkley, Murphy, & Fischer, 2008).

The diagnostic criteria changed in several important ways with the publication of the (APA, 2013). Prior editions of the DSM classified ADD and ADHD among the Disorders Usually Diagnosed in Infancy Childhood, or Adolescence. With publication of the DSM-5, however, ADHD was characterized as a Neurodevelopmental Disorder, more appropriately recognizing its onset and chronic developmental course (Shaw & Polanczyk, 2017). While retaining most of the diagnostic criteria for ADHD that were previously included in the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), three important changes were introduced: (1) age-of-onset criterion was increased from 7 to 12 years; (2) symptoms of inattention and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity were required to be merely present (as opposed to causing “impairment”); (3) there was provision for using a reduced symptom threshold for diagnosis in adults (ages 17 years and older), requiring five (rather than six) symptoms of inattention and/or hyper-activity/impulsivity. Highlights of diagnostic changes in the DSM are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Progression of diagnostic criteria for ADHD since 1980

Version Year Features and unique criteria
DSM-III 1980 Attention Deficit Disorder, with Hyperactivity
Attention Deficit Disorder, without Hyperactivity
DSM-III-R 1987 Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
Undifferentiated ADHD (replaced ADD without Hyperactivity from DSM-III)
ICD-10 (European Description) 1992 F90: Hyperkinetic Disorders. Requires impaired attention, hyperactivity, early onset (before age 6 years), and long duration. Anxiety and mood disorders and ASD must be excluded.
F90.0: Disturbance of Activity and Attention. Includes ADHD without hyperactivity, and ADHD with associated features (learning, motor, ASD).
F90.1: Hyperkinetic Conduct Disorder. Used when criteria for ADHD and Conduct Disorder are met.
F90.8: Other Hyperkinetic Disorders
F90.9 Hyperkinetic Disorders, Unspecified
DSM-IV 1994 Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) with three subtypes:
ADHD, Predominantly Hyperactive/Impulsive Type
ADHD, Predominantly Inattentive Type
ADHD, Combined Type
ADHD, Not Otherwise Specified
DSM-IV-TR 2000 No substantial changes to primary diagnoses and subtypes
ADHD, Not Otherwise Specified allowed for onset >age 7 and described symptoms of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo
DSM-5 2013 ADHD with 3 “presentations”
Required “presence of”, rather than “impairment by” symptoms
Increased age of onset to <12 years
Included symptom criteria for adult diagnosis
ICD-10-CM 2017 F90: Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders. Symptoms must be present before age 7 years. Includes F90.0 (Inattentive Type), F90.1 (Hyperactive Type), F90.2 (Combined Type), F90.8 (Other type), F90.9 (Unspecified Type).

Note. DSM = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders; ICD = International Classification of Diseases

INTERNATIONAL CLASSIFICATION SYSTEMS

The World Health Organization (WHO) has long recognized ADHD in its criteria for the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-9, ICD-10; WHO, 1978, 1993), but has, until recently, kept the earlier name of Hyperkinetic Disorder. The criteria for the ICD-10-CM (WHO, 2017) are more in line with the DSM-5, and are outlined in Table 1. Historically, the ICD criteria follow many of the same diagnostic criteria as the earlier versions of the DSM; however, the requirement for earlier onset (before age 7 years), and the exclusions of ADHD diagnosis when accompanied by other disorders (including mood disorders) has contributed to lower diagnostic rates in studies using the ICD criteria (Hinshaw et al., 2011).

CONSENSUS STATEMENTS AND PRACTICE GUIDELINES

The progression and refinement of diagnostic methods for ADHD from the 1960s through the 1990s led to advancements in both identification and treatment. Despite this progress, the disorder remained controversial, leading to conflicting opinions in the public sector, and confusion for clinicians and policy-makers. One of the main controversies was the rapidly expanding use of stimulant medication. Also, by the late 1990s, there remained a belief by many that the disorder did not exist, and/or could not be reliably diagnosed. These controversies led professional researchers, clinicians, and other stakeholders to establish the first published consensus statements to address the controversies, and to outline considerations for care in children and adults. The NIH Consensus Statement on ADHD (NIH, 1998), and the International Consensus Statement on ADHD (Barkley, 2002), and the European Consensus Statement on Diagnosis and Treatment of Adult ADHD (Kooij et al., 2010), are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2.

Published consensus statements on ADHD

Statement Year Features and unique criteria
NIH Consensus Development Statement November 1998 Conference hosted by the National Institutes of Health, bringing together international experts on ADHD, as well as health care representatives from the public.
The consensus statement (NIH, 1998) concluded the following:
  • ADHD is valid disorder of childhood and a major public health problem.

  • Children with ADHD usually have pronounced difficulties and impairments resulting from the disorder across multiple settings, and can experience long-term adverse effects on academic performance, vocational success, and social-emotional development.

  • Evidence supporting the validity of ADHD includes the long-term developmental course over time, cross-national studies revealing similar risk factors, familial aggregation (recognized as either genetic or environmental), and, heritability.

International Consensus Statement on ADHD 2002 Prompted by continued inaccurate portrayal of ADHD in the media, and the stories of ADHD as a myth or as an otherwise benign condition.
Concern from international healthcare and research community that thousands would not seek treatment for the condition.
Supported by U.S. Surgeon General, American Medical Association, American Psychiatric Association, American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, American Psychological Association, and American Academy of Pediatrics.
The statement concluded, “as a matter of science, the notion that ADHD does not exist is simply wrong” (p. 89), and further charged the media to depict ADHD as realistically and accurately as it is depicted in the scientific community, as a valid disorder having substantial adverse impact on those who have the condition suffer from it through no fault of their own or their parents and teachers—a statement directly contradicting those in the popular media who had continued to portray ADHD as the result of bad parenting or teaching (Barkley, 2002).
European Consensus Statement on treatment of adult ADHD 2010 Adults with ADHD manifest anomalies in brain functioning, based on cognitive, electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies, and the effectiveness of pharmacological treatments with dopamine agonists support the neurobiological underpinnings of ADHD.
The high heritability and associated environmental risk factors suggest a primary role for genetic influences that are moderated by environmental factors in the majority of cases.
Adult ADHD can be reliably diagnosed with careful examination, and a cutoff of 4/9 DSM criteria for inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity are recommended for diagnosis of Adult ADHD.

During the same period, the leading professional practice organizations around the world also began to issue specific guidelines for the identification and treatment of ADHD in children and adolescents. A summary of the most recently published guidelines from the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP) (Pliskza & The AACAP Workgroup, 2007), the National Institute for Clinical Excellence (NICE) guidelines from the United Kingdom (2009), based in part on the earlier European Clinical Guidelines for Hyperkinetic Disorder (Taylor et al., 2004), the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP, 2011), and the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia (2012) is outlined in Table 3. Of these, the NICE guidelines specifically include treatment recommendations for ADHD in adults.

Table 3.

Current professional practice guidelines for ADHD

AACAP (2007) NICE (2009) AAP (2011) NHMRC Australian Guidelines (2012)
1. Screening for ADHD should be part of every mental health assessment. 1. Stipulates that children, adolescents and adults with ADHD require integrated care that addresses a wide range of personal, social, educational and occupational needs, – and that this care should be provided by adequately trained healthcare and education professionals. 1. Clinician should initiate evaluation for ADHD for any child 4–18 years of age who presents with academic or behavioral problems and symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity, or impulsivity. 1. Diagnosis is made using DSM-IV or ICD-10 guidelines.
2. Evaluation should include interview with child and parent(s), information from daycare or school. 2. Recommends treatment for individuals with ADHD is stratified by age and by impairment in school-aged children with ADHD. 2. For ADHD diagnosis, clinician should determine that current DSM criteria have been met. Information should be specifically obtained from parents/guardians, teachers, and other mental health clinicians. Alternative causes for behavior should be ruled out. 2. Assessment requires impairment across settings, and requires gathering of information from parents, health care providers and teachers.
3. If patient’s history is unremarkable, additional lab tests are not needed. 3. Evaluates the role of pharmacological and non- pharmacological treatments (including parent training) for ADHD. 3. Clinician should include assessment for other conditions that might coexist with ADHD (emotional, behavioral, developmental, physical). 3. Comprehensive medical, developmental and mental health history should be considered in assessment.
4. Psychological assessment not mandatory for diagnosis, but should be performed if patient has history of low cognitive ability or poor academic achievement. 4. Describes the role of specific systems of care (including educational systems) in treating ADHD. 4. Recognize patients with ADHD as having a chronic condition, with special health care needs. Management should follow the principles of the chronic care model and medical home. 4. Recommends holistic, child- centered approach to treatment, including treatment from a range of medical and non-medical service providers.
5. Evaluate for possible comorbidities. 5. Provides guidelines for clinician and patient in best practices through the developmental phases of the disorder. 5a. For preschool-aged children (4–5 years), clinicians should prescribe behavior therapy as the first line of treatment. 5. Psychological treatments are considered for all children, and used as front line therapy in children under age 7 years.
5a. For preschool-aged children, medication is not recommended; psycho-education is first line treatment; if psycho-education is effective in managing symptoms, child is monitored for recurrence once school begins. 5b. For elementary-aged children (6–11 years), clinician should prescribe FDA-approved medications and/or behavioral therapy.
5c. For adolescents (12–18 years), clinician should prescribe FDA-approved medications with assent of the adolescent and may prescribe behavioral therapy.
5b. For school-aged children (moderate ADHD), group-based psycho-education, cognitive- behavioral/social skills treatment, and educator training are first line of treatment; medications may be used for those not responding to non-pharmacological treatment.
5c. For school-aged children and adults (severe ADHD), medication is first line intervention, preceded by full physical and psychological exam; medication is always part of comprehensive treatment plan; non- pharmacological therapy is used for those who refuse medication.
6. Develop comprehensive treatment plan, recognize the chronicity of the condition, link treatment with school personnel, and community/family supports. 6. Describes the economic impacts of different treatments for ADHD. 6. Clinician should titrate medication to achieve maximum benefit with minimum adverse effects. 6. Most children and adolescents will benefit from stimulant medication, which should be titrated to maximum effective dose.
7. Initial trial of FDA-approved medications 7. Educational management should specifically be added to all treatment plans.
8. If initial treatment doesn’t improve symptoms, review diagnosis, add behavioral therapy or other medication.
9. Monitor side effects pharmacological treatments.
10. If patient has robust effect of pharmacotherapy and has normal academic and social functioning, then pharmacotherapy alone may be adequate.
11. If patient has less than optimal response to pharmacotherapy, or has comorbidities and/or psychosocial stressors, then add psychosocial therapy
12. Ongoing assessment of need for active treatment.
13. Monitor height and weight of patients treated with medications.

Note: AACAP = American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; NICE = National Institute for Clinical Excellence; AAP = American Academy of Pediatrics; NHRMC = National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia.

The AACAP, NICE, and AAP practice guidelines specifically address treatment issues for preschoolers with ADHD, recognizing the validity of the diagnosis and appropriateness of treatment in that age range. The AAP and NICE guidelines state that for preschool children with ADHD (ages 4–5 years), clinicians should prescribe behavior therapy, rather than medication, as the first line of treatment (AAP, 2011); however, the NICE guidelines go one step beyond to virtually preclude pharmacotherapy in the preschool years, and to use medication only as a second line treatment in school-age children with mild to moderate symptoms of ADHD. Of relevance to neuropsychologists, the AACAP 2007 guidelines state that psychological assessment is not mandatory for ADHD diagnosis, but is recommended in patients with suspected low IQ or poor academic achievement.

INFLUENTIAL THEORIES OF ADHD

ADHD and Executive Functions

The prevailing neuropsychological theories of ADHD in the past 25 years have emphasized the behavioral and cognitive disruptions associated with executive functions, broadly defined, secondary to presumed anomalies in (pre)frontalstriatal brain circuitry, and modulated by expression of catecholamines. The most influential of these was put forth by Barkley (1997) in an effort to extend prior explanations of behavioral dyscontrol more generally to those specific deficits observed in children with ADHD. Barkley’s theory built on the earlier writing of Quay (1988) and Gray (1982) who asserted that behavioral impulsiveness arises from the brain’s inhibitory control systems. The application of Barkley’s unifying theory was to interpret deficits in ADHD as a failure of self-control systems, with behavioral inhibition as the core. The theory specifically linked inhibition to four executive functions: working memory, self-regulation of affect-motivation-arousal, internalization of speech, and reconstruction (i.e., behavioral analysis and synthesis). With inhibition viewed as the primary deficit, these secondary executive function impairments ultimately led to deficits in motor control, including inefficient output (performance).

Beyond Executive Functions

Subsequent neuropsychological theories, including those based on growing awareness of frontostriatal functions as a result of brain mapping research, recognized that while deficient inhibitory control is a critical component of the ADHD phenotype, it is likely insufficient to characterize the range of functional behavioral problems observed. Heilman and colleagues (Heilman, Boeller, & Nadeau, 1991) emphasized the importance of the striatum in executive control, as it acts to gate sensation into two parallel behavioral systems: how (organization and praxis) and when (response inhibition). Neuropsychological research over the past 25 years has highlighted the wide range of neuropsychological differences (beyond inhibitory and executive control deficits), emphasizing the dynamic (Nigg & Casey, 2005) and heterogeneous nature of ADHD (Sonuga-Barke, 2010 Sonuga-Barke 2010 to the ref list.–>). At the group level, children with ADHD show inconsistent and often small reductions in performance on performance-based tests of executive function, with mean effect sizes in the small to moderate range (Sergeant, Willcutt, & Nigg, 2007), and expression moderated by other more contextual variables, such as IQ (Mahone et al., 2002). At the individual level, only a small proportion of children with ADHD manifest deficient performance across a range executive function tasks (Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga-Barke, 2005). Recognizing these limitations, recent theories have addressed a broader range of non-frontostriatal constructs, including state regulation, delay aversion, and performance variability.

Cognitive-Energetic Models

Based largely on the information processing models of Joseph Sergeant and colleagues, these state regulation models recognized the dynamic interplay between tonic versus phasic activation systems in the brain. The cognitive-energetic theory of ADHD (Sergeant, 2000) characterized the behaviors observed in ADHD as a product of the dynamic interplay between energetic (bottom–up) systems involving arousal, activation and effort, and cognitive (top–down) executive control systems that limit the expression of the energetic drive states, often in an inverted U manner allowing for other executive functions (e.g., working memory) to also moderate output (Sergeant, Guerts, Huijbregts, Sheres, & Oosterlaan, 2003). In contrast to Barkley’s (1997) executive function theory of ADHD, the cognitive-energetic model allowed for subcortical anomalies affecting arousal and activation to be primary in the manifestation of ADHD.

Motivation and Delay Aversion

The concept of delay aversion comes from the motivational model of ADHD, based on the influence of reward systems in the brain, and characterized behaviorally by the preference for “smaller sooner” (SS) over “larger later” (LL) rewards (Sonuga-Barke, Sergeant, Nigg, & Willcutt, 2008). Like the cognitive-energetic models, delay aversion theories incorporate executive control and dorsal-frontostriatal circuitry, represented behaviorally by inhibitory control problems. The dual pathway model, however, also recognizes the influence of a second (reward) circuit, represented by ventral frontostriatal dysfunction and reflected behaviorally in delay aversion (Sonuga-Barke, 2003), and anomalous sensitivity to reward (Luman, Van Meel, Oosterlaan, Sergeant, & Guerts, 2009).

More recently, the model has been expanded to consider a third potential pathway influencing behavioral output (i.e., a triple-pathway model), involving temporal processing deficits, manifested in difficulties with timing, time discrimination, reproduction, and motor synchronization (Sonuga-Barke, Paraskevi, & Thompson, 2010). The addition of these timing deficits to the model recognizes the influence of cerebellar functioning in the manifestation of cognitive and behavioral function in ADHD.

Response Variability

Intra-individual variability in response time (for both externally and self-paced tasks) is observed in groups of individuals with ADHD, both at the phenotypic and genetic levels, with explanations ranging from temporal processing deficits to arousal regulation and anomalies in the default-mode network (Kuntsi & Klein, 2012). Prior observation of slowed processing speed in ADHD, characterized by longer mean response times, is now recognized to be driven by significant skewing of the response-time distribution, and exacerbated by the presence of more frequent, very slow (“off task”) responses, measurable via the variable tau at the extreme tail of the distribution via ex-Gaussian analytic methods (Ghemlin et al., 2014).

Parallel to these findings, state regulation theories prompted investigations built on the notion that motor activation is facilitated by stimulus unpredictability. In a series of recent studies in children with ADHD, increased intra-individual variability (Ryan, Martin, Denckla, Mostofsky, & Mahone, 2010) and lapses from performance (manifest by increased tau), have been shown to be normalized by a introducing a moderate degree of unpredictability in the stimulus presentation, operationalized by “jittering” the inter-stimulus interval (Lee et al., 2015).

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo

During the field trials for the DSM-IV, investigators identified a set of symptoms, later termed sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT), that were observed in children with ADHD, but were also in many ways distinct, including lethargy, daydreaming, confusion, and drowsiness. The characterization of SCT was ultimately included in the examples provided for the ADHD, Not Otherwise Specified diagnosis in DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000). Initially, proposed revisions for DSM-5 included a separate Restrictive Inattentive ADHD presentation that overlapped with the SCT construct. Upon publication of DSM-5, however, neither the Restrictive Inattentive presentation, nor the diagnosis of SCT, was included. Most subsequent investigations of the SCT construct have identified three core symptoms, lethargy, underactivity, and slowness, that appear distinct from the inattention associated with ADHD and are hypothesized to be a function of earlier selective attention processes (Jacobson et al., 2012).

LANDMARK TREATMENT STUDIES

Multimodal Treatment Study of Children With ADHD (MTA)

This multi-site, study represented the largest controlled trial to of treatment for ADHD to date (Arnold et al., 1997), and enrolled 579 children with ADHD, ages 7–9, who were randomized to one of four treatment strategies: medication alone, psychosocial treatment alone, combined (medication + psychosocial treatment), and community control. Participants were treated actively for 14 months, with a follow-up 10 months after the conclusion of active treatment. At 14 months, all four groups showed sizeable reductions in symptoms, with those in medication-alone and combined strategies showing greatest improvement.

Behavioral therapy and community control strategy groups showed less improvement than the medication-alone and combined, but did not differ from one another (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999a). Medication management and behavioral therapies for this trial represented gold standard care, including careful titration of short-acting methylphenidate, dosed three times a day (for the mediation groups), and intensive behavioral therapy based on the summer program developed by Pelham and colleagues. In children with comorbid conditions, the medication groups showed less superiority to behavioral treatments (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999b), although admittedly, more intensive behavioral treatments (not offered in the MTA studies) may have been required to maintain greater effects.

At 24-month follow-up, the medication and combined groups continued to show persistence of improvement, but with notably diminished effects over time (MTA Cooperative Group, 2004). The initially superior results of the well-monitored medication condition were attenuated in long-term naturalistic follow-up, which showed not only the superiority of combination treatment, but also emergence of growth-related side effects in those treated with long-term medication, and the contribution of comorbidities (especially anxiety) and family-related processes (notably SES) that contributed significantly to functional outcome (Hinshaw, Arnold, & the MTA Cooperative Group, 2015).

Preschool ADHD Treatment Study (PATS)

The PATS was a large multi-site, double-blind, crossover, placebo-controlled clinical trial to study the effects of methylphenidate in carefully-diagnosed preschoolers with ADHD, ages 3 to 5.5 years (Abikoff et al., 2007). Like the MTA study, methylphenidate immediate release was titrated and administered three times per day. The medication led to improvement in symptoms with effect sizes smaller than those observed in the MTA trials (Greenhill et al., 2006). Most receiving medication maintained improvement over 10 months; however, side effects were more common (including slowed growth rate) than observed in older children. A 6-year naturalistic follow-up of those enrolled in the PATS study showed a remarkable stability of the symptoms (and disorder) over time, with continued high severity, despite ongoing medication treatment (Riddle et al., 2013).

Non-pharmacological Interventions

Long-term findings from the MTA and PATS studies highlight limitations of medication, and the importance of non-pharmacological treatments in overall care of individuals with ADHD. The types of non-pharmacological interventions that have been studied vary widely, and include, among others, behavioral treatments, dietary interventions, cognitive training, and neurofeedback, both in isolation and in combination with other treatments. A recent meta-analysis of these treatments identified challenges in study design around blinding of observers; however, among those well-blinded studies, interventions involving free fatty acid supplementation and exclusion of artificial food colors (among those with food sensitivities) showed small effects of unclear significance, with other treatments, including neurofeedback, cognitive training, and behavioral therapies produced inconsistent results with regard to core ADHD symptoms that require additional study using blinded assessments before conclusions can be drawn (Sonuga-Barke et al., 2013). Overall, these findings suggest that children need careful evaluations to identify comorbidities and that the most efficacious treatments continue to involve consideration of medication in conjunction with behavioral treatments and appropriate school interventions.

BIOMARKERS OF ADHD AND THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN BRAIN MAPPING RESEARCH

Genes, Environment, and ADHD

The recognition that ADHD runs in families prompted extensive research into the genetic etiology of the disorder. Based on observations from twin studies, the heritability of ADHD is as high as 70–80%, and first-degree relatives of probands with ADHD have a 5- to 10-fold increased risk in developing the disorder themselves (Faraone et al., 2015). Studies of candidate genes emphasized those associated with dopamine or other neurotransmitters (e.g., DAT1, DRD4, SNAP25; Wu, Yang, & Wang, 2014), although effect sizes have been small, and results have often not been replicated. The COMT gene has also been explored, given its role in degradation of prefrontal dopamine (Sun, Yuan, Shen, Ziong, & Wu, 2014). The heterogeneity of the phenotypic presentation, and variable patterns of lifetime symptom development, suggest that ADHD is associated with multiple genes of small effect, and gene–environment interactions, such that identification of a single genetic etiology is unlikely.

Neuroimaging and ADHD

A remarkable accumulation of evidence converging on the neurobiological nature of ADHD began with the availability of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Anatomic (aMRI) measurements, not clinical readings, focused on the basal ganglia, showing volume reductions (Aylward et al., 1996) and shape deformations (Qiu et al., 2009) in children with ADHD, with evidence that stimulant medication treatment may normalize morphological features of the basal ganglia (Sobel et al., 2010). Somewhat later, appreciation of the other regions participating in control circuits led to the finding of volume reductions in the cerebellum (Wyciszkiewicz, Pawlak, & Krawiec, 2017) and functionally defined frontal lobe sub-regions (Mahone, Ranta, et al., 2011).

Shaw and colleagues used longitudinal analyses of aMRI data to examine trajectories of development in children with ADHD (Shaw et al., 2007). Their landmark study demonstrated that in most regions of the cortex, children with ADHD showed a developmental lag, up to 3 years, in attaining peak cortical thickness, leading to the conclusion that ADHD may be best conceptualized as a delay in brain maturation, rather than as a discrete categorical entity. More recent longitudinal aMRI studies demonstrated that this rate of “thinning” is actually proportional to the level of symptoms, such that those with the most rapid thinning have the fewest ADHD symptoms, while those with the slowest (most delayed) cortical thinning have the most severe ADHD symptoms (Shaw et al., 2011), lending support to the dimensional (spectrum) view of ADHD.

In the past 15 years, multimodal neuroimaging methods (beyond aMRI) have added to the growing understanding of the neurobiology of ADHD. Using task-activated functional MRI (fMRI), a recent review of 55 studies of children with ADHD in terms of their task-responsive activations recorded a pattern of decreases in the executive/frontoparietal and, visual-attentional/ventral attentional networks, contrasted with increases in the default mode and somatomotor networks (Cortese et al., 2012). Resting state (non–task-activated/baseline) fMRI measurements of connectivity in children and adults with ADHD show atypicality of the default mode network, sometimes described as reduced coherence or decreased integrity thereof (Fair et al., 2010).

The burgeoning group of publications assessing functional connectivity in children with ADHD has added data showing interplay of the default mode, cognitive control, reward, attention, and amygdala-seeded networks (Castellanos & Aoki, 2016). At the same time, using of diffusion tensor/weighted imaging, a recent review revealed anomalies in frontostriatal white matter pathways, albeit not always in the same direction, in children and adolescents with ADHD (Tamm, Barnea-Goraly, & Reiss, 2012).

ADHD PHENOTYPES AND SPECIAL SUB-POPULATIONS

ADHD and Learning Disabilities

The history of learning disabilities (LD), first published as such in 1962, and the pre-1980 conceptualization of ADHD is one of over-inclusiveness under the Minimal Brain Dysfunction label (Clements, 1966). Two of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders, ADHD and dyslexia, co-occur more often than would be expected by chance (Cuoto et al., 2009), suggesting shared genetic risks. When starting with a sample of young children with ADHD, the observed emergence of LD is striking, such that one third struggle to learn basic reading. At later ages, when demands for “learning to read” give way the need for “reading to learn,” difficulties with both written expression and mathematics (associated with increased demands for executive control) are observed, raising the co-occurrence of between LD (all types) and ADHD to approximately two-thirds (Denckla, 2005; Hooper & Williams, 2005).

ADHD and Motor Control

Whether regarded as an associated or core feature of ADHD, anomalies of motor development have a long history of parallel description alongside attentional and executive attainments, suggesting a shared neural circuitry (Diamond, 2000). Overflow movements involve unconsciously produced concomitant movements of limbs or parts thereof (e.g., fingers) that are not the primarily requested movements, including feet-to-hands (with heel or toe walking) and mirroring of patterned hand or finger movements during repetitive and sequenced movements. Overflow movements are especially sensitive developmental biomarkers, diminishing to a point of disappearance during the first decade of life in typically developing children the functional outcome of maturation in the brain’s collateral inhibition (Gidley Larson et al., 2007).

Overflow movements persist, however, in children with neurological impairments, including ADHD (Cole et al., 2008). Mirror movements, an important subtype of overflow, have been long recognized as the most diagnostically discriminating motor sign for hyperactive and typically developing children (Denckla & Rudel, 1978), the physiology for which has been revealed recently by transcranial magnetic stimulation (Gilbert, Isaacs, Augusta, MacNeil, & Mostofsky, 2011).

Other aspects of anomalous motor development, including meeting balance and gait milestones and speed, rhythm, and patterning of foot and hand/finger movements, have also been found to be characteristic of children with ADHD, especially before adolescence, and more dramatically in young girls with ADHD (Cole et al., 2008). The motor endophenotype designation is meaningful in ADHD, as motor control matures earlier than cognitive or social-emotional control. As such, motor development may serve as a type of “canary in the coal mine” in diagnosis or prediction of ADHD. For example, the ability to hop on one foot at age 4 years was reported long ago to be the best predictor of hyperactivity at age 7 years (Nichols & Chen, 1981).

Sexual Dimorphism and Girls with ADHD

Available neuroimaging research examining brain development highlights a striking pattern in which girls reach maturation (i.e., peak cortical thickness) 2–5 years earlier than boys in most functional regions (Lenroot et al., 2007). At birth, it has been estimated that girls are 3 weeks ahead of boys in physical maturation, and by the time they enter school, girls are approximately 1 year ahead (Eme, 1992). These patterns of development in girls set the stage for sexually dimorphic manifestation of motor and executive dysfunction in children with ADHD, including risk genes for ADHD with different effects in males and females (Davies, 2014). While initial studies suggested that girls with ADHD had better adolescent and adult outcomes than boys with ADHD, more recent findings, most notably from the large-scale longitudinal studies, highlight considerable functional impairment in girls/women with ADHD, including persistent executive dysfunction (Miller, Loya, & Hinshaw, 2013), mood and externalizing disorders (Tung et al., 2016), and severe behavioral problems (i.e., suicide attempts, self-injury) into adulthood (Hinshaw et al., 2012).

Anatomic MRI studies of ADHD in girls also highlight patterns that are differently (rather than less) deviant in girls with ADHD, consistent with the observed later onset and greater social (vs. academic) impairment. Specifically, the medial and orbitofrontal regions (sometimes called the limbic frontal regions) are smaller to a greater degree than the dorsolateral and premotor regions, in contrast to the male ADHD frontal profile (Dirlikov et al., 2014). The breakdown of sub-domains of executive functions is also differently distributed among girls versus boys with ADHD (O’Brien, Dowell, Mostofsky, Denckla, & Mahone, 2010).

ADHD in Preschoolers

ADHD has become the most commonly diagnosed form of psychopathology in the preschool years, with CDC prevalence estimates at 2.7% of children ages 4–5 years. The increase in identification and treatment of preschool ADHD occurs in the context of parallel increases in enrollment in preprimary academic programs in the preschool years (Bosco & Bona, 2016), which has led some researchers to caution of an epidemic of preschoolers being wrongfully diagnosed (Hinshaw & Scheffler, 2014). Nevertheless, neuroanatomical changes in preschoolers with ADHD as young as 4 years are now identified, with rate of anatomic “delay” associated with symptom severity (Mahone, Crocetti, et al., 2011). The picture is concerning, because even when treated in preschool years, children with ADHD continue to have symptoms and significant functional impairment in elementary school (Riddle et al., 2013).

ADHD in Adults

The primary symptoms of ADHD most often begin in early childhood, and persist (in some form) into adulthood for as many of 85% of individuals with the disorder (van Lieshout et al., 2016). In the past 20 years, adult onset ADHD has also been recognized, and (Faraone et al., 2015), and is now acknowledged as valid condition with genetic associations that are potentially unique from the child-onset variation (Agnew-Blais et al., 2016). The DSM-5 criteria for ADHD also allow for an adjusted diagnostic threshold in individuals ages 17 years and older, such that only five of nine symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention are required (APA, 2013). Adult diagnosis is acknowledged to be more difficult than in the early years due to multiple comorbidities accruing by adult life and variable access to childhood records or histories.

SUMMARY AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

In the past 35 years, ADHD has emerged as one of the most diagnosed, treated, and researched neurologically based behavioral conditions. Nevertheless, outcomes remain poor. The preponderance of research involving behavioral and phenotypic expression of ADHD has suffered from the population bias, such that most samples are biased toward a population of boys aged 6–12 years. Moreover, diagnostic methods and societal gender role beliefs have contributed to a male bias in ADHD diagnosis, where boys are at risk for over-diagnosis, and girls are at risk for under-diagnosis (Bruchmuller, Margraf, & Schneider, 2012). Neuropsychologists working with individuals with ADHD (whether or not it is the primary reason for referral) are encouraged to recognize that the developmental course is often lifelong, necessitating a “family practice” approach to consultation and intervention, in which caregivers provide assistance in different contexts and in different formats throughout the life of the individual with the condition.

At the time of this writing, the psychiatric conceptualization of ADHD remains categorical, while neuropsychological theories of ADHD emphasize functions as dynamic, dimensional constructs. In the next 35 years, research using dimensional frameworks such as Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; Insel et al., 2010), which also call for integration of multiple units of analysis (physiology, behavior), may be better suited to study the individual with ADHD, without the constraints of categorical diagnoses, especially for those individuals whose behaviors and cognitive skill patterns fall sub-threshold for diagnosis.

In conclusion, several prominent clinical and research questions remain, and will likely be the focus of the next generation of investigation into ADHD. Our (admittedly incomplete) list of suggested future investigations would include: (i) Links between multimodal brain mapping and phenotypic dimensions associated with ADHD, with emphasis on accessing international large datasets of bio-markers (e.g., enigma.io). (ii) Clarification of the neurobiological and phenotypic presentation of adult onset ADHD as a disorder potentially distinct from the more common developmental onset variety. (iii) Tracking the epidemiology of ADHD following implementation of DSM-5, with its more liberal diagnostic requirements compared to its predecessors. (iv) Investigations into the Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) construct, and its relationship with ADHD, emphasizing objectively measurable signs of the condition. (v) Continued neurobiological investigation of those individuals with attention deficits, who have never manifested developmentally inappropriate symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity. Do such individuals exist, and if so, do they represent a novel condition separable from ADHD or other neurodevelopmental disorders?

Acknowledgments

Supported by U54 HD079123, R01 HD068425.

References

  1. Abikoff HB, Vitiello B, Riddle MA, Cunningham C, Greenhill LL, Swanson JM, … Wigal T. Methylphenidate effects on functional outcomes in the preschoolers with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder treatment study (PATS) Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2007;17:581–592. doi: 10.1089/cap.2007.0068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agnew-Blais JC, Polanczyk GV, Danese A, Wertz J, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L. Evaluation of the persistence, remission, and emergence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:713–720. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Academy of Pediatrics. ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 2011;128(5):1–16. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders. 2. Washington, DC: Author; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders. 3. Washington, DC: Author; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders. 3. Washington, DC: Author; 1987. rev. [Google Scholar]
  7. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  8. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 2000. Text Revision. [Google Scholar]
  9. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders. 5. Washington, DC: Author; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  10. Arnold LE, Abikoff HB, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, Elliott G, Greenhill LL, … Wells KC. The National Institute of Mental Health collaborative multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD (the MTA): Design challenges and choices. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1997;54:865–870. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830210113015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Aylward EH, Reiss AL, Reader MJ, Singer HS, Brown JE, Denckla MB. Basal ganglia volumes in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology. 1996;11:112–115. doi: 10.1177/088307389601100210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barkley RA. International consensus statement on ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2002;5(2):89–111. doi: 10.1023/a:1017494719205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Fischer M. Identifying new symptoms of ADHD in adulthood. In: Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Fischer M, editors. ADHD in adults. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 170–204. [Google Scholar]
  15. Bosco BP, Bona A. Changes in academic demands and ADHD in young children. JAMA Pediatrics. 2016 doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4132. Published online February 22, 2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bradley C. The behavior of children receiving Benzedrine. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1937;3:577–585. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bruchmuller K, Margraf J, Schneider S. Is ADHD diagnosed in accord with diagnostic criteria? Overdiagnosis and influence of client gender on diagnosis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80:128–138. doi: 10.1037/a0026582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Castellanos FX, Aoki Y. Intrinsic functional connectivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A science in development. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2016;1:253–261. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Clements SD. NINDS Monographs. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare; 1966. Minimal brain dysfunction in children: Terminology and identification: Phase one of a three-phase project. 9. Public Health Service Bulletin No.1415) [Google Scholar]
  20. Cole W, Mostofsky SH, Gidley Larson JC, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Age related change in motor subtle signs among girls and boys with ADHD. Neurology. 2008;71(19):1514–1520. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000334275.57734.5f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cortese S, Kelly C, Chabernaud C, Proal E, Di Martino A, Milham MP, Castellanos FX. Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: A meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169(10):1038–1055. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11101521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Couto JM, Gomex L, Wigg K, Ickowicz K, Pathare T, Malone M, … Barr CL. Association of ADHD with a candidate region for reading disabilities on chromosome 6p. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:368–375. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Davies W. Sex differences in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Candidate genetic and endocrine mechanisms. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2014;35:331–346. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Denckla MB. Executive function. In: Gozal D, Molfese DL, editors. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: From genes to patients. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; 2005. pp. 165–184. [Google Scholar]
  25. Denckla MB, Rudel RG. Anomalies of motor development in hyperactive boys. Annals of Neurology. 1978;3:231–233. doi: 10.1002/ana.410030308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development. 2000;71:44–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dirlikov B, Rosch KS, Crocetti D, Denckla MB, Mahone EM, Mostofsky SH. Distinct frontal lobe morphology in girls and boys with ADHD. Neuroimage: Clinical. 2014;10:222–229. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Doshi JA, Hodgkins P, Kahle J, Sikirica V, Cangelosi MJ, Setyawan J, … Neumann PJ. Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51:990–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Douglas VI, Peters KG. Toward a clearer definition of the attentional deficits of hyperactive children. In: Hale GA, Lewis M, editors. Attention and the developments of cognitive skills. New York: Plenum Press; 1979. pp. 173–248. [Google Scholar]
  30. Eme RF. Selective female affliction in the developmental disorders of childhood: A review. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1992;21:354–364. [Google Scholar]
  31. Fair DA, Posner J, Nagel BJ, Bathula D, Costa Dias TG, Mills KL, … Nigg JT. Atypical default network connectivity in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68:1084–1091. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Faraone SV, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Biederman J, Buitelaar JK, Ramos-Quiroga JA, … Franke B. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Reviews: Disease Primers. 2015;1:15020. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fletcher JM. The effects of childhood ADHD on adult labor market outcomes. Health Economics. 2014;23:159–181. doi: 10.1002/hec.2907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ghemlin D, Fuermaier ABM, Walther S, Debelak R, Rentrop M, Westermann C, … Weisbrod M. Intraindividual variability in inhibitory function in adults with ADHD – An ex-Gaussian approach. PLoS One. 2014 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gidley Larson JC, Mostofsky SH, Goldberg M, Cutting L, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Effects of gender and age on motor exam in typically developing children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2007;32(1):543–562. doi: 10.1080/87565640701361013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gilbert DL, Isaacs KM, Augusta M, MacNeil LK, Mostofsky SH. Motor cortex inhibition: A marker of ADHD behavior and motor development in children. Neurology. 2011;76:615–621. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820c2ebd. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gray JA. The neuropsychology of anxiety. New York: Oxford University Press; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  38. Greenhill L, Kollins S, Abikoff H, McCracken J, Riddle M, Swanson J, … Cooper T. Efficacy and safety of immediate-release methylphenidate treatment for preschoolers with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:1284–1293. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000235077.32661.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Heilman KM, Voeller KKS, Nadeau SE. A possible pathophysiologic substrate of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology. 1991;6(Suppl):S76–S81. doi: 10.1177/0883073891006001s09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE the MTA Cooperative Group. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, multimodal treatment, and longitudinal outcome: Evidence, paradox, and challenge. WIREs Cognitive Science. 2015;6:39–52. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hinshaw SP, Ellison K. ADHD: What everyone needs to know. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hinshaw SP, Owens EB, Zalecki C, Huggins SP, Montenegro-Nevado AJ, Schrodek E, Swanson EN. Prospective follow-up of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into early adulthood: Continuing impairment includes elevated risk for suicide attempts and self-injury. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80:1041–1051. doi: 10.1037/a0029451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hinshaw SP, Scheffler RM. NY Times. 2014. Feb 23, Expand pre-K, not ADHD. Op-Ed. [Google Scholar]
  44. Hinshaw SP, Scheffler RM, Fulton BD, Aase H, Banaschewski T, Cheng W, … Weiss MD. International variation in treatment procedures for ADHD: Social context and recent trends. Psychiatric Services. 2011;62:459–464. doi: 10.1176/ps.62.5.pss6205_0459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hooper SR, Williams EA. Attention deficit hyper-activity disorder and learning disabilities. In: Gozal D, Molfese DL, editors. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: From genes to patients. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; 2005. pp. 215–254. [Google Scholar]
  46. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine D, Quinn K, … Wang P. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;167:348–351. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jacobson LA, Murphy-Bowman SC, Pritchard AE, Tart-Zelvin A, Zabel TA, Mahone EM. Factor structure of a sluggish cognitive tempo scale in clinically referred children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40(8):1327–1337. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9643-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kooij SJJ, Bejerot S, Blackwell A, Caci H, Casas-Brugué M, Carpentier PJ, … Asherson P. European consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: The European Network Adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10(67):1–24. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-10-67. http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/10/67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kuntsi J, Klein C. Intraindividual variability in ADHD and its implications for research of causal links. Current Topics in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;9:67–91. doi: 10.1007/7854_2011_145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Larson K, Russ SA, Kahn RS, Halfon N. Patterns of comorbidity, functioning, and service use for US children with ADHD. Pediatrics. 2011;127:462–467. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lee RWY, Jacobson LA, Pritchard AE, Ryan M, Yu Q, Denckla MB, … Mahone EM. Jitter reduces response time variability in ADHD: An ex-Gaussian analysis. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2015;19:794–804. doi: 10.1177/1087054712464269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lenroot RK, Gogtay N, Greenstein DK, Wells EM, Wallace GL, Clasen LS, et al. Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage. 2007;36:1065–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Luman M, Van Meel CS, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA, Guerts HM. Does reward frequency or magnitude drive reinforcement-learning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Psychiatry Research. 2009;168:222–229. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mahone EM, Crocetti D, Ranta ME, Gaddis A, Cataldo M, Slifer KJ, … Mostofsky SH. A preliminary neuroimaging study of preschool children with ADHD. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2011;25:1009–1028. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2011.580784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mahone EM, Hagelthorn KM, Cutting LE, Schuerholz LJ, Pelletier SF, Rawlins C, … Denckla MB. Effects of IQ on executive function measures in children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology. 2002;8:52–65. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.1.52.8719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mahone EM, Ranta ME, Crocetti D, O’Brien JW, Kaufmann WE, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH. Comprehensive examination of frontal regions in boys and girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2011;17:1047–1057. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711001056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Miller M, Loya F, Hinshaw SP. Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD: Developmental trajectories and associations with symptom change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2013;54:1005–1015. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. MTA Cooperative Group. A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999a;56:1073–1087. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. MTA Cooperative Group. Moderators and mediators of treatment response for children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999b;56:1088–1096. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. MTA Cooperative Group. National Institute of Mental Health multimodal treatment study of ADHD follow-up: 24-month outcomes of treatment strategies for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2004;113:754–761. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.4.754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. National Health and Medical Research Council. Clinical practice points on the diagnosis, assessment, and management of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Australia: Australian Government National Health and Medical Research Council; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  62. National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis and management of ADHD in children, young people and adults. London: British Psychological Society and Royal College of Psychiatrists; 2009. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Nichols P, Chen T. Minimal brain dysfunction: A prospective study. Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum Associates; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  64. Nigg JT, Casey BJ. An integrative theory of attention–deficit hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:785–806. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050376. doi:10.10170S0954579405050376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Nigg JT, Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Sonuga-Barke EJS. Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1224–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. NIH Consensus Statement. Diagnosis and treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. 1998;16(2):1–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. O’Brien JW, Dowell LR, Mostofsky SH, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Neuropsychological profile of executive function in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2010;25(7):656–670. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pastor P, Reuben C, Duran C, Hawkins L. Association between diagnosed ADHD and selected characteristics among children aged 4–17 years: United States, 2011–2013. NCHS Data Brief. 2015;201:1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pliszka S the AACAP Workgroup on Quality Issues. Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46:894–921. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318054e724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:942–948. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Qiu A, Crocetti D, Adler M, Mahone EM, Denckla MB, Miller MI, Mostofsky SH. Basal ganglia volume and shape in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:74–82. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Quay HC. The behavioral reward and inhibition systems in childhood behavioral disorder. In: Bloomingdale LM, editor. Attention deficit disorder III: New research in treatment, psychopharmacology, and attention. New York: Pergamon; 1988. pp. 176–186. [Google Scholar]
  73. Riddle MA, Yershova K, Lazzaretto D, Paykina N, Yenokyan G, Greenhill L, … Posner K. The preschool attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder treatment study (PATS) 6-year follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52:264–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Ryan M, Martin RB, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH, Mahone EM. Inter-stimulus jitter facilitates response control in children with ADHD. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2010;16:388–393. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709991305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sergeant JA. The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2000;24:7–12. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00060-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sergeant J, Guerts H, Huijbregts S, Sheres A, Oosterlaan J. The top and bottom of ADHD: A neuropsychological perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2003;27:583–592. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sergeant JA, Willcutt E, Nigg J. How clinically functional are executive function measures of ADHD? In: Shaffer D, Leibenluft E, Rohde LA, Sirovatka P, Regier DA, editors. Externalizing disorders of childhood: Refining the research agenda for DSM-V. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  78. Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Greenstein D, … Rapoport JL. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104:19649–19654. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Shaw P, Gilliam M, Liverpool M, Weddle C, Malek M, Sharp W, … Giedd J. Cortical development in typically developing children with symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity: Support for a dimensional view of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168:143–151. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Shaw P, Polanczyk GV. Combining epidemiological and neurobiological perspectives to characterize the lifetime trajectories of ADHD. European Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2017;26:139–141. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0944-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Sobel LJ, Bansal R, Maia TV, Sanchez J, Mazzone L, Durkin K, … Peterson BS. Basal ganglia surface morphology and the effects of stimulant medications in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;67:977–986. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sonuga-Barke E. The dual pathway model of AD/HD: An elaboration of neurodevelopmental characteristics. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2003;27:593–604. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Sonuga-Barke EJ. Disambiguating inhibitory dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: toward the decomposition of developmental brain phenotypes. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:599–601. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sonuga-Barke EJ, Brandeis D, Cortese S, Daley D, Ferrin M, Holtmann M … the European ADHD Guidelines Group. Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: Systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;170:275–289. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sonuga-Barke EJ, Sergeant JA, Nigg J, Willcutt E. Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Nosologic and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2008;17:367–384. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Sonuga-Barke E, Paraskevi B, Thompson M. Beyond the dual pathway model: Evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sun H, Yuan F, Shen X, Xiong G, Wu J. Role of COMT in ADHD: A systematic meta-analysis. Molecular Neurobiology. 2014;49:251–261. doi: 10.1007/s12035-013-8516-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Tamm L, Barnea-Goraly N, Reiss AL. Diffusion tensor imaging reveals white matter abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2012;202(3):150–154. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Taylor E, Dopfner M, Sergeant J, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, … Zuddas A. European clinical guidelines for hyperkinetic disorder-first upgrade. European Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;13(Suppl 1):1/7–1/30. doi: 10.1007/s00787-004-1002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Todd RD, Huang H, Henderson CA. Poor utility of the age of onset criterion for DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Recommendations for DSM-V and ICD-11. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:942–949. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01892.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Tung I, Li JJ, Meza JI, Jezior KL, Kianmahd JSV, Hentschel PG, O’Neil PM, Lee SS. Patterns of comorbidity among girls with ADHD: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2016;138(4):e20160430. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-0430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Valo S, Tannock R. Diagnostic instability of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes: Effects of informant source, instrumentation, and methods for combining symptom reports. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39:749–760. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Van Lieshout M, Luman M, Twisk JW, van Ewijk H, Groenman AP, Thissen AJ, … Oosterlaan J. A 6-year follow-up of a large European cohort of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder combined subtype: Outcomes in late adolescence and young adulthood. European Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2016;25:1007–1017. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0820-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Willcutt EG, Nigg JT, Pennington BF, Solanto MV, Rohde LA, Tannock R, … Lahey BB. Validity of the DSM-IV Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:991–1010. doi: 10.1037/a0027347991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. World Health Organization. The ICD-9 Classification Diseases. 9. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  96. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  97. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases. 10. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO; 2017. Clinical Modifications (ICD-10-CM) [Google Scholar]
  98. Wu Z, Yang L, Wang Y. Applying imaging genetics to ADHD: The promises and the challenges. Molecular Neurobiology. 2014;15:449–462. doi: 10.1007/s12035-014-8683-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wyciszkiewicz A, Pawlak MA, Krawiec K. Cerebellar volume in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Replication study. Journal of Child Neurology. 2017;32:215–221. doi: 10.1177/0883073816678550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES