Skip to main content
BMJ Open logoLink to BMJ Open
. 2017 Jun 23;7(6):e016244. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-016244

The 2015 Middle Childhood Survey (MCS) of mental health and well-being at age 11 years in an Australian population cohort

Kristin R Laurens 1,2,3, Stacy Tzoumakis 4, Kimberlie Dean 1,2,5, Sally A Brinkman 6,7, Miles Bore 8, Rhoshel K Lenroot 1,2, Maxwell Smith 9, Allyson Holbrook 9, Kim M Robinson 9, Robert Stevens 10, Felicity Harris 1,2, Vaughan J Carr 1,2,11, Melissa J Green 1,2
PMCID: PMC5726143  PMID: 28645979

Abstract

Purpose

The Middle Childhood Survey (MCS) was designed as a computerised self-report assessment of children’s mental health and well-being at approximately 11 years of age, conducted with a population cohort of 87 026 children being studied longitudinally within the New South Wales (NSW) Child Development Study.

Participants

School Principals provided written consent for teachers to administer the MCS in class to year 6 students at 829 NSW schools (35.0% of eligible schools). Parent or child opt-outs from participation were received for 4.3% of children, and MCS data obtained from 27 808 children (mean age 11.5 years, SD 0.5; 49.5% female), representing 85.9% of students at participating schools.

Findings to date

Demographic characteristics of participating schools and children are representative of the NSW population. Children completed items measuring Social Integration, Prosocial Behaviour, Peer Relationship Problems, Supportive Relationships (at Home, School and in the Community), Empathy, Emotional Symptoms, Conduct Problems, Aggression, Attention, Inhibitory Control, Hyperactivity-Inattention, Total Difficulties (internalising and externalising psychopathology), Perceptual Sensitivity, Psychotic-Like Experiences, Personality, Self-esteem, Daytime Sleepiness and Connection to Nature. Distributions of responses on each item and construct demarcate competencies and vulnerabilities within the population: most children report mental health and well-being, but the population distribution spanned the full range of possible scores on every construct.

Future plans

Multiagency, intergenerational linkage of the MCS data with health, education, child protection, justice and early childhood development records took place late in 2016. Linked data were used to elucidate patterns of risk and protection across early and middle child development, and these data will provide a foundation for future record linkages in the cohort that will track mental and physical health, social and educational/occupational outcomes into adolescence and early adulthood.

Keywords: social-emotional function, behaviour, psychopathology, personality, child development, record linkage, data linkage, epidemiology


Strengths and limitations of this study.

  • The Middle Childhood Survey (MCS) assessed psychosocial and behavioural constructs reflecting mental health and well-being by self-report in a large sample of 27 808 children aged approximately 11 years (31.4% of eligible children), which is representative of the New South Wales population.

  • Constructs were assessed using items selected from measures with established reliability and validity for assessment of children aged 11 years, but item reduction and modifications made to item wording, response options and scale scoring limits direct comparison with published data on some measures.

  • The depth of information obtained was constrained by the time available within schools for survey administration, lack of accompanying parent and/or teacher reports and sensitivities associated with assessing psychosocial and behavioural constructs in children by self-report.

  • The MCS measured the full spectrum of personal competencies and vulnerabilities in the population, providing capacity to guide the development and implementation of universal mental health promotion programmes alongside targeted approaches for vulnerable children.

  • The MCS is embedded within an intergenerational, multiagency record linkage study, the New South Wales Child Development Study, which permits MCS data to be interpreted in the context of longitudinal data that is subject to minimal selection and participation bias.

Introduction

Middle childhood (age 6–12 years) is a critical period in which to establish social, emotional-behavioural, cognitive and physical competencies that support successful transition to adolescence.1 2 Children are increasingly exposed to influences beyond the home, and encounter various new challenges, particularly at school. During this time, mental health problems emerge for some children, causing impairments in functioning and increasing risk for future adverse health, social and educational outcomes.3 4 Thus, middle childhood represents an important period for establishing strong psychosocial foundations to support future mental health and well-being. Here, we introduce the 2015 Middle Childhood Survey (MCS), designed as a self-report measure of children’s psychosocial experiences in middle childhood (at approximately 11 years of age) administered online during the final year of primary (elementary) school for a population cohort of children being studied longitudinally within the New South Wales Child Development Study5 (NSW-CDS; http://nsw-cds.com.au/).

The NSW-CDS is a multigenerational record linkage study that combines administrative health, education, child protection and justice records for an Australian state-based population cohort of children (n=87 026) and their parents. The cohort was defined as those children who entered their first year of full-time schooling (Kindergarten) in NSW in 2009 at approximately 5 years of age and for whom class teachers completed the Australian Early Development Census6 (AEDC) on each child (99.7% coverage). The AEDC data on early childhood social, emotional-behavioural, cognitive, communication and physical development were linked with child and parent administrative records in a first record linkage conducted in 20135; a second record linkage that included MCS data and updated administrative records to the age of 12 years was undertaken in late 2016.

Reflecting the primary interest of the NSW-CDS in identifying childhood predictors of later mental health and related outcomes,5 the MCS items focused on the assessment of social and emotional-behavioural competencies that are typically attained during middle childhood1 2 and which have been demonstrated as predictive of various adolescent and adulthood health and social outcomes.3 4 7 These competencies include establishing and maintaining positive social relationships, understanding and appreciating the perspectives of others, recognising and managing emotions and behaviours and the development of personality and self-esteem. Other aspects of childhood mental health and well-being that are associated with health, social and educational outcomes, such as psychotic-like experiences,7 8 daytime sleepiness9 and engagement with the natural environment10 were also included. Like the AEDC, the MCS was designed as a population measurement tool rather than a diagnostic instrument for the identification of children presenting needs that require specialist support services or therapeutic intervention.11Thus, the MCS measured both successful attainment of these competencies as well as vulnerabilities or immaturity of these skills relative to age peers. This paper describes the content and administration of the MCS, and presents the mental health and well-being profiles of children in the MCS sample.

Cohort description

Eligible sample

The target sample for the study included all year 6 students enrolled at government (public) and non-government (private) schools in the Australian state of New South Wales (NSW) during 2015 (88 572 children enrolled in 2371 schools), in order to capture the same cohort of children assessed within the AEDC in 2009. A two-stage recruitment procedure was used (figure 1) to ensure that students remained anonymous to researchers for future record linkage purposes: Principals (Head Teachers) provided active consent for their school to participate; subsequent child recruitment within participating schools was managed by school personnel using an opt-out consent procedure for parents and/or children.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow diagram illustrating derivation of the final sample of 829 schools, and 27 808 children, who participated in the Middle Childhood Survey 2015 (MCS). [NSW, New South Wales.]

Procedures

Pilot testing

Commencing in October 2012, school sector representatives and stakeholders representing various education and parent and communities groups (see Acknowledgements) were consulted regarding the method of MCS administration in schools. During 2014, the feasibility of administration procedures (and acceptability of the MCS items) was tested with year 6 students (n=645) enrolled at 11 schools spanning the government and non-government sectors, and metropolitan and rural regions of NSW. Minor adaptations to administration procedures and MCS items were made on the basis of feedback received from participating schools, and on psychometric analysis of the pilot data (including factor and item response theory analyses).

Data management

The MCS data collection was managed by a third party information technology (IT) contractor that delivered the online student survey and the automated email correspondence with schools on behalf of the researchers. The IT contractor was provided with all Principal/school email addresses by the school sector representatives, and received all NSW year 6 students’ identifying information (eg, name, date of birth), based on 2014 (year 5) enrolment records, directly from the NSW Board of Studies, Teaching and Educational Standards, under a confidential data usage agreement. Identifying information for these eligible students was prepopulated into an online administration portal that was accessible only to school teachers assisting with MCS administration. To account for new enrolments in 2015, teachers were able to update the personal identifiers to include new students. A unique access code was generated by the IT contractor for each child to ensure that the survey responses were associated with the correct personal identifiers for later linkage processes.

School recruitment

From March 2015, the school sector representatives and study stakeholders used their established avenues for communicating with school personnel and/or parents to seek their support and participation in the study. In April 2015, Principals of NSW schools with an enrolment of year 6 students were sent an electronic study information leaflet by email, inviting the school to participate in the study. Principals (or an authorised representative) provided written informed consent for their school to participate, or declined participation, using a unique web-link for each school. Where no responses were received from schools during a 4-month school recruitment period, telephone contact was made by researchers and supplemented by automated reminder emails. Principals of participating schools were able to nominate a preferred 2-week window during July–September 2015 to administer the MCS, and a dedicated coordinator (ie, teacher or support person) to supervise MCS administration at their school.

Child recruitment

Both printed and electronic copies of study information leaflets were sent to participating schools for distribution to parents/carers of year 6 students at least a fortnight prior to the scheduled MCS administration. Electronic copies of these leaflets were also available on the study website in English and the 10 most common languages spoken by families of children enrolled in NSW schools; an audio version in English was also available at this site. Parents/carers could opt-out their child from participation using online forms, or by written or verbal instruction to class teachers. Children could opt-out either online or by verbal instruction to class teachers. Teachers recorded online any written or verbal opt-outs received from parents or children prior to administration of the MCS. Opting out of the study was also possible after MCS administration; capacity to withdraw MCS data remained available until the closure of the survey portal to data collection on 16 October 2015. MCS data were then deidentified by the IT contractor for provision to the researchers, at which point removal of a specific child’s responses was no longer possible.

Survey administration

The MCS was administered within participating schools during class time over a 3-month period commencing July 2015. Classroom teachers supervised the survey administration according to instructions provided in an online administration guide. Schools determined the setting of survey administration depending on availability of computing resources, while maintaining confidentiality for participants. Children could complete the survey over multiple sessions, using the unique access code provided to the child by their teacher. Children with special needs could complete the survey with the assistance of their normal classroom support (eg, adult helper) and/or an audio-recording of the survey. Researchers monitored the administration of MCS in schools via an online portal (which held school-level information only), and arranged alternative administration times for any school that had not administered the survey within their nominated 2-week window.

Data provision

During the administration process, participating students’ personal identifiers were stored by the IT contractor separately from MCS responses. Only deidentified survey data (coded by unique identification number) was provided to the researchers in December 2015. A separate dataset containing only the minimum identifying information for the cohort of participating students (ie, without the survey response data) was provided to a third party linkage provider—the Centre for Health and Record Linkage (http://www.cherel.org.au/)—to be retained under a confidential data usage agreement that enables linkage of MCS data with administrative data collections in the NSW-CDS; at no time during the study execution were personal identifiers available to researchers.

Measures

The content of the MCS was established via consensus among a working group comprising NSW-CDS Scientific Committee members who are coauthors on this manuscript. Members represented expertise in child development, developmental psychopathology, education, psychology, psychiatry and population health. The group reviewed measures with established reliability and validity for assessment of children aged 11 years, and incorporated measures both of competencies and vulnerabilities in social and emotional-behavioural development. Each construct of interest was assessed by multiple items; in some instances, only a subset of the items from the original scales was included due to constraints on the number of items that could be administered to children during class time. In such cases, the subset of items demonstrated in previous studies as providing the most coherent but comprehensive assessment of the construct was selected. Minor wording changes were made to several MCS items to increase their acceptability to Australian children (modified items are indicated by * in table 1 and online supplementary table 1-X). Furthermore, to avoid children having to adapt their responses to the different response formats used in the original scales, a standardised response format was adopted for all items, modelled on the three-choice format of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ),12 13 namely: not true (scored 0); somewhat true (1) and certainly true (2). A standard approach of summing items on all scales (after reverse scoring of some items, as indicated in table 1 and online supplementary table 1-X) to compute total scale scores was also adopted.

Table 1.

Summary of items measuring each mental health and well-being domain assessed within the Middle Childhood Survey (MCS) and, for each item, the number of children providing data (of the 27 808 who commenced the survey) and the distributions of the three response options

MCS domain (and source measure) Item Sample Not True Somewhat True Certainly True
(n) % (n) % (n) % (n)
Social Integration (QSL) My school is a place where…
… I learn to get along with other people 26 859 3.0 (806) 26.8 (7189) 70.2 (18 864)
… Other students accept me as I am 26 856 6.6 (1769) 32.6 (8760) 60.8 (16 327)
… People trust me 26 856 4.1 (1100) 34.0 (9136) 61.9 (16 620)
… I am popular with other students 26 856 17.0 (4574) 45.1 (12 123) 37.8 (10 159)
… I know people think a lot of me 26 856 20.1 (5393) 50.5 (13 566) 29.4 (7897)
… I get on well with the other students in my class 26 855 3.4 (920) 33.3 (8933) 63.3 (17 002)
… People can depend on me 26 854 5.1 (1380) 35.5 (9542) 59.3 (15 932)
… Other students are very friendly 26 854 4.8 (1281) 37.5 (10 075) 57.7 (15 498)
Prosocial Behaviours (SDQ) I try to be nice to other people. I care about their feelings 27 494 1.3 (359) 22.6 (6224) 76.1 (20 911)
I usually share with others (eg, CDs, games, food) 27 486 7.9 (2180) 45.9 (12 609) 46.2 (12 697)
I am helpful if someone is hurt, upset or feeling ill 27 482 2.6 (728) 29.7 (8174) 67.6 (18 580)
I am kind to younger children 27 478 2.1 (583) 15.2 (4177) 82.7 (22 718)
I often volunteer to help others (parents, teachers, children) 27 474 6.0 (1653) 44.0 (12 096) 50.0 (13 725)
Peer Relationship Problems (SDQ) I would rather be alone than with people of my age 27 484 71.6 (19 667) 20.6 (5650) 7.9 (2167)
I have one good friend or more (R) 27 480 2.0 (544) 7.6 (2077) 90.5 (24 859)
Other people my age generally like me (R) 27 480 6.4 (1745) 42.1 (11 576) 51.5 (14 159)
Other children or young people pick on me or bully me 27 477 66.9 (18 387) 23.7 (6517) 9.4 (2573)
I get along better with adults than with people my own age 27 474 52.8 (14 518) 36.7 (10 075) 10.5 (2 881)
Supportive Home Relationships (HKS/MDI) In my home, there is a parent or another adult …
… who listens to me when I have something to say 26 924 4.4 (1181) 30.3 (8147) 65.4 (17 596)
… who I can talk to about my problems 26 922 8.2 (2212) 24.3 (6553) 67.4 (18 157)
… who wants me to do my best 26 928 1.6 (435) 12.0 (3220) 86.4 (23 273)
… who believes that I will be a success 26 922 3.4 (906) 20.0 (5382) 76.6 (20 634)
Supportive School Relationships (HKS/MDI) At my school, there is a teacher or another adult…
… who really cares about me 26 918 7.1 (1920) 34.4 (9265) 58.4 (15 733)
… who listens to me when I have something to say 26 916 5.7 (1528) 31.5 (8466) 62.9 (16 922)
… who believes that I will be a success 26 917 5.7 (1536) 32.8 (8831) 61.5 (16 550)
… who tells me when I've done a good job 26 915 3.8 (1017) 23.0 (6177) 73.3 (19 721)
Supportive Community Relationships (HKS/MDI) In my neighbourhood/community (NOT from your school or family), there is an adult…
… who really cares about me 26 910 19.0 (5101) 35.5 (9540) 45.6 (12 269)
… who listens to me when I have something to say 26 910 20.5 (5522) 37.2 (10 005) 42.3 (11 383)
… who believes that I will be a success 26 909 20.4 (5477) 35.9 (9673) 43.7 (11 759)
… who tells me when I've done a good job 26 909 19.3 (5186) 30.9 (8308) 49.9 (13 415)
Empathy (FTI) I want to help people who get treated badly 27 117 3.5 (957) 27.7 (7500) 68.8 (18 660)
I often feel worried about people that are not as lucky as me, and feel sorry for them 27 113 5.3 (1425) 34.4 (9337) 60.3 (16 351)
I sometimes try to understand my friends better by pretending I am them† 27 111 40.6 (11 003) 39.1 (10 591) 20.3 (5517)
I think people can have different opinions about the same thing 27 108 2.1 (567) 21.8 (5919) 76.1 (20 622)
Emotional Symptoms (SDQ) I get a lot of headaches, stomach-aches or sickness 27 489 56.6 (15 552) 33.0 (9075) 10.4 (2862)
I worry a lot 27 484 40.4 (11 097) 41.1 (11 304) 18.5 (5083)
I am often unhappy, depressed or tearful 27 480 72.8 (20 017) 21.2 (5817) 6.0 (1646)
I am nervous in new situations. I easily lose confidence 27 479 36.9 (10 128) 44.4 (12 191) 18.8 (5160)
I have many fears, I am easily scared 27 473 56.5 (15 517) 32.0 (8803) 11.5 (3153)
Conduct Problems (SDQ) I get very angry and often lose my temper 27 485 62.1 (17 079) 26.7 (7350) 11.1 (3056)
I usually do as I am told (R) 27 484 3.1 (863) 43.7 (12 011) 53.2 (14 610)
I fight a lot. I can make other people do what I want 27 480 83.3 (22 896) 14.0 (3846) 2.7 (738)
I am often accused of lying or cheating 27 478 62.6 (17 207) 26.5 (7278) 10.9 (2993)
I take things that are not mine from home, school or elsewhere 27 474 88.3 (24 267) 9.6 (2636) 2.1 (571)
Aggression (EATQ-R) If I get mad at someone, I might hit them* 27 484 65.6 (18 020) 25.4 (6975) 9.1 (2489)
When I am angry, I throw or break things 27 472 79.8 (21 916) 14.6 (4022) 5.6 (1534)
Attention (EATQ-R) I pay close attention when someone asks me to do something* 27 165 4.2 (1133) 46.3 (12 588) 49.5 (13 444)
It is easy for me to really concentrate on homework problems 27 144 18.3 (4969) 47.4 (12 854) 34.3 (9321)
When trying to study, I have difficulty tuning out background noise and concentrating (R) 27 120 26.3 (7146) 45.2 (12 260) 28.4 (7714)
Inhibitory Control (EATQ-R) When I am excited, it’s hard for me to wait my turn to speak* (R) 27 162 25.7 (6970) 44.2 (11 993) 30.2 (8199)
When someone tells me to stop doing something, it is easy for me to stop‡ 27 157 9.4 (2540) 47.6 (12 926) 43.0 (11 691)
I often say the first thing that comes to mind* (R) 27 155 24.3 (6586) 52.0 (14 118) 23.8 (6451)
It’s hard for me not to open presents before I’m supposed to (R) 27 149 43.6 (11 827) 30.6 (8302) 25.9 (7020)
When I am having a good time I find it hard to go home* (R) 27 144 14.6 (3955) 33.5 (9086) 52.0 (14 103)
I often call out answers before the teacher calls my name* (R) 27 133 54.8 (14 873) 33.4 (9059) 11.8 (3201)
The more I try to stop myself from doing something I shouldn’t, the more likely I am to do it (R) 27 127 51.4 (13 938) 36.8 (9994) 11.8 (3195)
Hyperactivity/Inattention (SDQ) I am restless, I cannot stay still for long 27 490 32.8 (9024) 41.2 (11 317) 26.0 (7149)
I am constantly fidgeting or squirming 27 481 52.7 (14 478) 34.0 (9335) 13.3 (3668)
I am easily distracted, I find it difficult to concentrate 27 479 39.5 (10 846) 42.5 (11 676) 18.0 (4957)
I think before I do things (R) 27 474 7.6 (2087) 52.7 (14 466) 39.8 (10 921)
I finish the work I'm doing. My attention is good (R) 27 472 5.9 (1610) 48.2 (13 232) 46.0 (12 630)
Perceptual Sensitivity (EATQ-R) I am very aware of noises 27 157 11.0 (2988) 40.8 (11 071) 48.2 (13 098)
I notice even little changes taking place around me, like lights getting brighter in a room 27 153 14.2 (3853) 39.4 (10 696) 46.4 (12 604)
I tend to notice little changes that other people do not notice 27 140 13.0 (3533) 49.2 (13 360) 37.8 (10 247)
I can tell if another person is angry by their expression 27 138 2.9 (774) 28.2 (7644) 69.0 (18 720)
Psychotic-Like Experiences (PLEQ-C) Have you ever…
… believed that other people could read your thoughts?* 27 000 54.2 (14 642) 33.5 (9048) 12.3 (3310)
… believed that you were being sent special messages through the television? 26 993 69.5 (18 773) 22.0 (5947) 8.4 (2273)
… thought that you were being followed or spied on? 26 992 43.0 (11 601) 34.2 (9229) 22.8 (6162)
… heard voices that other people could not hear? 26 990 42.9 (11 567) 30.5 (8227) 26.7 (7196)
… felt that you were under the control of some special power? 26 990 70.4 (19 012) 19.3 (5221) 10.2 (2757)
… known what another person was thinking even though that person wasn't speaking? 26 983 42.0 (11 345) 38.3 (10 330) 19.7 (5308)
… felt as though your body had been changed in some way that you could not understand? 26 976 55.2 (14 879) 30.7 (8290) 14.1 (3807)
… felt that you had special powers that other people don't have?* 26 976 62.0 (16 732) 23.4 (6314) 14.6 (3930)
… seen something or someone that other people could not see? 26 976 48.0 (12 949) 27.8 (7489) 24.2 (6538)
Agreeableness (BFQ-C) I am friendly to others in my school* 27 735 0.7 (185) 22.5 (6238) 76.8 (21 312)
I forgive others when they do something wrong* 27 734 3.2 (898) 38.3 (10 619) 58.5 (16 217)
I am kind even to people I don’t like* 27 422 10.4 (2853) 49.6 (13 615) 39.9 (10 954)
I think other people are good and honest 27 416 4.9 (1354) 55.7 (15 269) 39.4 (10 793)
I like to let other people use my things* 27 415 7.7 (2121) 49.7 (13 614) 42.6 (11 680)
Conscientiousness (BFQ-C) I check my work to make sure it is right* 27 734 7.6 (2118) 48.3 (13 395) 44.1 (12 221)
I like to be on time* 27 733 4.7 (1301) 29.0 (8051) 66.3 (18 381)
I keep my room neat and tidy* 27 427 16.8 (4596) 49.1 (13 476) 34.1 (9355)
I like to keep my things in order* 27 426 11.5 (3153) 40.7 (11 151) 47.8 (13 122)
I am messy* (R) 27 421 49.6 (13 591) 39.0 (10 687) 11.5 (3143)
Neuroticism (BFQ-C) I get nervous about many things* 27 734 24.0 (6649) 53.7 (14 895) 22.3 (6190)
I have bad moods* 27 734 24.0 (6662) 51.1 (14 177) 24.9 (6895)
I get angry easily* 27 734 51.6 (14 314) 34.1 (9446) 14.3 (3974)
I get upset easily* 27 733 54.6 (15 134) 34.2 (9473) 11.3 (3126)
I cry a lot* 27 733 75.4 (20 901) 19.7 (5469) 4.9 (1363)
Extraversion (BFQ-C) I am happy and active* 27 735 1.0 (289) 25.2 (6999) 73.7 (20 447)
I like to be with other people* 27 734 2.0 (556) 20.8 (5772) 77.2 (21 406)
I like to talk with others 27 428 1.6 (443) 19.5 (5358) 78.9 (21 627)
I make friends easily* 27 419 8.5 (2325) 38.0 (10 426) 53.5 (14 668)
I am a shy person* (R) 27 419 47.1 (12 928) 39.0 (10 696) 13.8 (3795)
Intellect/Openness (BFQ-C) I easily learn my school work* 27 735 4.2 (1171) 51.1 (14 177) 44.7 (12 387)
I know many things 27 735 3.3 (919) 46.2 (12 812) 50.5 (14 004)
I know the answers to questions my teacher asks* 27 734 3.3 (907) 71.4 (19 808) 25.3 (7019)
I understand my school work* 27 733 2.7 (762) 45.4 (12 588) 51.9 (14 383)
I like learning new things 27 415 3.2 (865) 26.3 (7223) 70.5 (19 327)
Self-esteem (MSLSS) There are lots of things I can do well 27 174 2.3 (629) 32.1 (8726) 65.6 (17 819)
I like myself 27 171 6.0 (1622) 28.5 (7745) 65.5 (17 804)
I am a nice person 27 169 1.4 (369) 29.6 (8033) 69.1 (18 767)
Daytime Sleepiness (PDSS) I fall asleep or get drowsy during class* 27 106 59.5 (16 120) 30.4 (8248) 10.1 (2738)
I am tired and grumpy during the day* 27 105 65.8 (17 833) 28.6 (7754) 5.6 (1518)
I am usually alert most of the day* (R) 27 104 9.1 (2475) 45.3 (12 282) 45.6 (12 347)
Connection to Nature (CTNI/CTNS) When I feel sad, I like to go outside and enjoy nature 27 103 19.8 (5361) 41.9 (11 345) 38.4 (10 397)
Being in nature makes me feel peaceful* 27 102 10.1 (2729) 38.1 (10 335) 51.8 (14 038)
I feel strongly connected with nature* 27 101 21.5 (5825) 45.0 (12 207) 33.5 (9069)

R denotes an item that was subsequently reversed in the computation of domain scores.

*Denotes item with minor wording change from original scale.

†Denotes item removed from the modified Empathy scale (three items).

‡Denotes item reassigned from the modified Inhibitory Control (items) to the modified Attention scale (items).

QSL, Quality of School Life; BFQ-C, short form of the Big Five Questionnaire for Children; CTNI/CTNS, Connection to Nature Index/Connectedness to Nature Scale; EATQ-R, Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire—Revised; FTI, Feeling and Thinking Index; HKS/MDI, Healthy Kids Scale/Middle Years Development Index; MSLSS, Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale; PDSS, Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale; PLEQ-C, Psychotic-Like Experiences Questionnaire for Children; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.

Supplementary material 1

bmjopen-2017-016244supp001.pdf (230.4KB, pdf)

In total, the MCS comprised 116 items with specific forced-choice response options. The first eight items assessed demographic information: age, sex, month of birth, residential postcode, number of people living in the child’s usual residence, main language spoken at home and whether the child used the audio-recording or received assistance from an adult to complete the survey (table 2). The remaining 108 items assessed a range of child mental health and well-being constructs, including: Social Integration, Prosocial Behaviour, Peer Relationship Problems, Supportive Relationships (at home, school and in the community), Empathy, Emotional Symptoms, Conduct Problems, Aggression, Attention, Inhibitory Control, Hyperactivity-Inattention, Total Difficulties (internalising and externalising psychopathology), Perceptual Sensitivity, Psychotic-Like Experiences, Personality, Self-esteem, Daytime Sleepiness and Connection to Nature (engagement with natural environment). The source measure for each of these constructs is described below; for brevity, these are presented according to their questionnaire of derivation:

  1. Social Integration at school was assessed using the full, unmodified 8-item Social Integration subscale of the Quality of School Life questionnaire.14 Response options were reduced from the original 4-choice to the standard 3-choice response format, and the total sum of items derived in place of an average of items used in previous research.

  2. Prosocial Behaviour and Psychopathology were assessed using the 25-item SDQ,12 13 which comprises four psychopathology subscales (Emotional Symptoms, Peer Relationship Problems, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity-Inattention), and a Prosocial Behaviour subscale. Items and response options were unmodified from the original scale, and the standard scoring metric applied: five items assessed each of the subscales, and Total Difficulties was computed by summing the 20 items from the four psychopathology subscales.

  3. Supportive Relationships at home, at school and in the neighbourhood/community were assessed using 12 items (four per subscale) selected from the Healthy Kids Survey.15 These items included those (three per subscale) used in the Middle Years Development Index 16 (MDI) plus an additional item for each subscale. Item wordings were unmodified from the MDI, but the 4-choice rating scale and averaged total score were replaced.

  4. Sixteen items from four subscales in the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire—Revised (EATQ-R) 17 assessed Attention (four items; selected from seven), Inhibitory Control (seven items; selected from 11), Perceptual Sensitivity (four items; selected from six) and Aggression (two items; selected from 11). The first three of these subscales comprise part of a measure of Effortful Control within the EATQ-R. Minor modifications to the wording of several items were made, and the original 5-point rating response scale and averaged total score replaced.

  5. Empathy was assessed using four items from the 12-item Feeling and Thinking Instrument 18; item wording was unmodified, but the original 5-point rating response scale replaced.

  6. Psychotic-like experiences were assessed with nine items from the Psychotic-Like Experiences Questionnaire for Children 8 19 (two with minor rewording from the original), with the original 3-choice response format retained.

  7. Dimensions of personality (Extraversion, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness and Intellect/Openness) were assessed using 25 items (5 per dimension) modified from an unpublished 30-item short-form of the 65-item Big Five Questionnaire for Children (BFQ-C)20 supplied by the author (Barbaranelli, personal communication). Items were reworded to simplify the translation from Italian to English. Following pilot testing in 2014, 5 of the 25 items were replaced with other candidates, adapted from the full BFQ-C, to improve psychometric properties. The original 5-point rating response scale was replaced.

  8. Self-esteem was measured with three unmodified items from the 7-item Self-Satisfaction subscale of the Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale.21 The original 4-choice response scale and averaged total score were replaced.

  9. Daytime sleepiness was assessed with three items selected from the 8-item Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale,9 with minor rewording of items and replacement of the original 5-point response scale.

  10. Connection to Nature (or, children’s engagement with the natural environment) was measured with three items; two were modified from the 7-item Enjoyment of Nature subscale of the Connection to Nature Index 22 and one modified from the 14-item Connectedness to Nature Scale.23 The original 5-point rating scales of both measures were replaced.

Table 2.

Summary of selected demographic characteristics self-reported by the 27 808 children completing the Middle Childhood Survey (MCS)

Demographic item Sample Prevalence
(n) % (n)
Age of child 27 808
  10 years or younger 0.5 (135)
  11 years 54.7 (15 198)
  12 years 44.1 (12 259)
  13 years or older 0.8 (216)
Sex of child 27 808
  Female 49.5 (13 754)
  Male 50.5 (14 054)
Number of people living in child’s home (main residence) 27 803
  3 or less 15.1 (4187)
  4 35.8 (9948)
  5 27.8 (7718)
  6 or more 21.4 (5950)
Main language spoken at home 27 803
  English 87.3 (24 272)
  Arabic 1.9 (525)
  Vietnamese 1.3 (365)
  Cantonese 1.1 (296)
  Mandarin 1.0 (278)
  Hindi 0.8 (211)
  Tagalog 0.5 (141)
  Spanish 0.4 (99)
  Greek 0.2 (49)
  Italian 0.1 (35)
  Other 5.5 (1532)
Child made use of MCS audio recording 27 803 2.5 (695)
Child received assistance from an adult to complete survey 27 802 5.0 (1398)

Findings to date

Sample characteristics

A flow diagram summarising the stages of school and child recruitment is provided in figure 1; this also details the reasons for non-participation of schools i and/or children in the MCS. Of the 2371 NSW schools with an eligible year 6 student enrolment, 829 (35.0%) administered the MCS. These schools provided a total enrolment of 32 389 children who were invited to complete the MCS (representing 36.6% of year 6 enrolments in NSW schools). Among these, 27 808 participated in the MCS (85.9% of invited children). Parent and child opt-outs totalled 4.3% of eligible children (the remaining 9.9% did not participate for other reasons detailed in figure 1). The mean age of participating children was 11.5 years (SD 0.5); other demographic information on participants is summarised in table 2. Average survey completion time was 16.5 min, with 90% of children completing within 7–50 min.

The representativeness of participating schools and children relative to the respective state population was estimated using publicly accessible national school-level data on enrolment and sociodemographic indices. Table 3 compares the demographic characteristics of all NSW schools and MCS participating schools, first as distributions of unweighted data, and second as distributions after weighting by year 6 enrolment and number of MCS participants per school. The 829 schools that participated in the MCS were comparable on a range of demographic indices to the total population of NSW schools with a year 6 enrolment; all figures reported for the MCS participating schools (both unweighted data and weighted estimates) lie within ~2% of NSW rates.

Table 3.

Demographic characteristics of MCS participating schools relative to all NSW schools with a year 6 student enrolment (unweighted and weighted by enrolment)

Demographic measure Unweighted averages Weighted averages*
NSW schools (n=2371) MCS schools (n=829) NSW schools (weighted) MCS schools (weighted)
% (n) % (n) % (n) % (n)
School sector:
  Government 67.9 (1609) 67.1 (556) 67.4 66.6
  Non-government 32.1 (762) 32.9 (273) 32.6 33.4
Geographical location:
  Metropolitan 59.9 (1421) 62.4 (517) 76.3 76.2
  Rural 37.7 (894) 35.8 (297) 23.1 23.3
  Remote 1.8 (43) 1.4 (12) 0.4 0.5
  Very remote 0.5 (13) 0.4 (3) 0.1 0.1
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
ICSEA score 1007.7 (93.5) 1002.8 (92.4) 1033.2 (87.1) 1026.5 (84.1)
Socioeducational quartiles based on ICSEA (%):
  Lowest 28.8 (22.3) 29.6 (22.3) 23.5 (20.3) 24.6 (20.5)
  Lower-Middle 24.3 (9.3) 24.6 (8.4) 22.9 (9.3) 23.6 (8.7)
  Higher-Middle 23.4 (8.8) 23.5 (8.7) 24.7 (7.8) 24.9 (7.8)
  Highest 23.5 (21.7) 22.4 (20.5) 29.0 (23.4) 26.9 (21.7)
Proportion LBOTE (%) 23.3 (27.3) 23.7 (27.4) 31.1 (30.3) 30.2 (30.1)
Proportion Indigenous (%) 9.1 (13.7) 9.5 (13.4) 6.0 (9.2) 6.3 (9.1)
Proportion female (%) 48.6 (9.3) 48.8 (7.1) 48.5 (10.3) 48.7 (7.0)

*To estimate the proportions of children in NSW and MCS schools described by each demographic measure, weighting was applied based on the number of year 6 students (NSW schools) and MCS participants in each school (MCS schools); see Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority [2015], ICSEA 2014: Technical Report. http//www.acara.edu.au/_resources/ICSEA_2014_technical_report.pdf).

ICSEA, Index of Community Socio-Educational Advantage 2014 (this score is derived from a number of variables, including parental school and non-school education and occupation, the school’s geographical location and proportion of Indigenous students); LBOTE, Language Background Other Than English; MCS, Middle Childhood Survey; NSW, New South Wales.

Item responses and scale distributions

Table 1 summarises the distribution of children’s responses on all MCS items, grouped according to the constructs they measured. Similar data, reported separately for girls and boys, are provided in online supplementary table 1-X. The total number of children reporting each item ranged from a minimum of 26 853 (3.4% missing) to 27 735 (0.3% missing). An unknown portion of these missing responses related to data server capacity issues encountered early in the MCS administration period and resolved promptly by the IT contractor.

For each MCS construct, table 4 (and online supplementary table 2-X) provides descriptive statistics (including number of children providing complete data on the scale, means, SD, minima and maxima), internal consistency coefficients (ordinal α-coefficients24) and scores corresponding to a range of percentiles in the sample distribution (ie, 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, 90th). These percentiles were adapted from those reported for the AEDC6 (where scores in the lowest 10th percentile were described as ‘developmentally vulnerable’, between the 10th and 25th percentiles as ‘developmentally at risk' and between the 25th–50th and >50th percentiles as two bands of ‘developmentally on track’ scores), with the 75th and 90th percentiles added to accommodate the bidirectional orientation of MCS scales.

Table 4.

Descriptive statistics (number of children providing complete data on the subscale, means, SD, minima and maxima), internal consistency coefficients (ordinal α) and scores corresponding to a range of percentiles in the sample distribution for each mental health and well-being domain assessed within the Middle Childhood Survey (MCS)

MCS domain (number of items in subscale) Source Measure Sample (n) Mean SD Minima Maxima Ordinal α Scores corresponding to percentiles:
10th 25th 50th 75th 90th
Social Integration (8 items) QSL 26 853 11.76 3.38 0 16 0.91 7 9 12 14 16
Prosocial Behaviour (5 items) SDQ 27 474 8.03 1.73 0 10 0.78 6 7# 8 9 10
Peer Relationship Problems (5 items) SDQ 27 474 2.03 1.78 0 10 0.69 0 1 2 3# 4
Supportive Home Relationships (4 items) HKS/MDI 26 922 6.78 1.64 0  8 0.88 4 6 8 8 8
Supportive School Relationships (4 items) HKS/MDI 26 915 6.34 1.92 0  8 0.91 4 5 7 8 8
Supportive Community Relationships (4 items) HKS/MDI 26 909 5.02 2.76 0  8 0.96 0 4 5 8 8
Empathy (4 items) FTI 27 108 5.74 1.48 0  8 0.60 4 5 6 7 8
Empathy (3 items)* FTI 27 108 4.94 1.20 0  6 0.70 3 4 5 6 6
Emotional Symptoms (5 items) SDQ 27 473 3.02 2.31 0 10 0.79 0 1 3 4# 6
Conduct Problems (5 items) SDQ 27 474 1.80 1.80 0 10 0.80 0 0 1 3# 4
Aggression (2 items) EATQ-R 27 472 0.69 1.04 0  4 0.81 0 0 0 1 2
Attention (3 items) EATQ-R 27 120 3.59 1.42 0  6 0.57 2 3 4 5 6
Attention (4 items)* EATQ-R 27 120 4.93 1.78 0  8 0.67 3 4 5 6 7
Inhibitory Control (7 items) EATQ-R 27 127 7.93 2.94 0 14 0.76 4 6 8 10 12
Inhibitory Control (6 items)* EATQ-R 27 127 6.59 2.68 0 12 0.75 3 5 7 9 10
Hyperactivity-Inattention (5 items) SDQ 27 472 3.60 2.37 0 10 0.77 0 2 3 5# 7
Total Difficulties (Psychopathology) (20 items) SDQ 27 472 10.45 6.07 0 40 0.88 3 6 10 14# 19
Perceptual Sensitivity (4 items) EATQ-R 27 138 5.60 1.77 0  8 0.71 3 4 6 7 8
Psychotic-like experiences (9 items) PLEQ-C 26 976 5.66 4.46 0 18 0.90 0 2 5 9 12
Extraversion (5 items) BFQ-C 27 419 8.03 1.76 0 10 0.76 6 7 8 9 10
Neuroticism (5 items) BFQ-C 27 733 3.48 2.28 0 10 0.80 1 2 3 5 7
Conscientiousness (5 items) BFQ-C 27 421 6.90 2.20 0 10 0.78 4 5 7 9 10
Agreeableness (5 items) BFQ-C 27 415 7.30 1.86 0 10 0.77 5 6 7 9 10
Intellect/Openness (5 items) BFQ-C 27 415 7.27 1.91 0 10 0.85 5 6 7 9 10
Self-esteem (3 items) MSLSS 27 169 4.91 1.19 0  6 0.70 3 4 5 6 6
Daytime Sleepiness (3 items) PDSS 27 104 1.54 1.37 0  6 0.64 0 0 1 2 3
Connection to Nature (3 items) CTNI/CTNS 27 101 3.72 1.81 0  6 0.88 1 3 4 5 6

#For the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire subscales, scores corresponding to the 80th percentile (ie, equating to the cut-off describing a ‘Borderline’ rating) were: Emotional Symptoms=5, Peer Relationship Problems=3, Conduct Problems=3, Hyperactivity-Inattention=6, Prosocial Behaviour (20th percentile)=7 and Total Difficulties=16. The Total Difficulties scale represents the sum of items on the four psychopathology scales (Emotional Symptoms, Peer Relationship Problems, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity-Inattention); BFQ-C, short form of the Big Five Questionnaire for Children; CTNI/CTNS, Connection to Nature Index/Connectedness to Nature Scale. * Indicates the revised version of the scale with modified number of items (see Footnote ii); EATQ-R, Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire—Revised; FTI, Feeling and Thinking Index; HKS/MDI, Healthy Kids Scale/Middle Years Development Index; MSLSS, Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale; QSL, Quality of School Life; PDSS, Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale; PLEQ-C, Psychotic-Like Experiences Questionnaire for Children; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.

The total number of children providing complete scale data ranged from a minimum of 26 853 (3.4% missing) to a maximum of 27 733 (0.3% missing). On average, children in the sample scored in the range reflecting healthier or more developmentally mature functioning on each construct, but the population distribution spanned the full range of possible scores on every scale. For most scales, each of the specified percentiles was associated with a unique score on the scale even at the extremes (10th and 90th percentiles), indicating a lack of ceiling/floor effects in measurement. The ordinal α-coefficients indicated adequate reliability for all MCS domains; for the two scales with the lowest α-coefficients (Attention and Empathy), minor modifications to these scales ii improved the coefficients and these revised scales are also summarised in the Tables.

Profile of mental health and well-being in the MCS cohort

High mean total scores on Social Integration, Prosocial Behaviour, Empathy, Attention, Inhibitory Control and Self-esteem were indicative of healthier functioning or developmentally more mature capacities for the majority of children in the sample. High mean scores also indicated most children’s access to Supportive Relationships at Home, School and in the Community, and engagement with the natural environment (Connection to Nature). Low mean total scores on Peer Relationship Problems, Emotional Symptoms, Conduct Problems, Aggression, Hyperactivity-Inattention, Total Difficulties (psychopathology) and Daytime Sleepiness were further indicative of healthy functioning among the majority of children in the MCS cohort. Nonetheless, on all scales, there were children who displayed less healthy or developed functioning or lacked access to supports (eg, 13.2% of children reported a lack of any supportive relationship with an adult in their community or neighbourhood).

Other scales in the MCS measured unusual thoughts or perceptual experiences that, although more prevalent in children with neurodevelopmental disorders and those who later develop adult psychiatric illness, are nonetheless common in child populations25: a majority of children (52.2%) responded ‘Certainly True’ to at least one of the nine PLE items, and the high mean total scores on Perceptual Sensitivity indicated that most children also reported sensitivity to slight, low-intensity stimulation in the environment. With respect to personality dimensions, on average, children produced higher scores on Extraversion, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness and Openness/Intellect scales (reflecting a tendency to avoid endorsement of the ‘Not True’ response), and lower scores on Neuroticism, relative to the scale range of each construct.

Pearson’s correlation coefficients indicating the pattern, direction and strength of associations (small 0.1; medium 0.3; large 0.5)26 between the MCS scales are provided in online supplementary table 3-X (with associations for girls and boys provided in online supplementary table 4-X). Almost all constructs related significantly in this large sample, with almost half (45%) of the associations of medium or large magnitude.

Comparison with published data

Direct comparison of MCS responses with published data on the SDQ and PLE scales from general population samples was afforded by use of the original items, response options and scoring methods for these scales. Mean scores on Prosocial Behaviour and Conduct Problems aligned closely with Australian self-report SDQ norms published in 2005 by age and sex (based on a Victorian community sample of 553 children aged 11–17 years, including 292 children aged 11–13 years)27, and were slightly greater in our sample for Total Difficulties, Emotional Symptoms, Peer Relationship Problems and Hyperactivity-Inattention. This pattern of change in means over the decade between the 2005 study and ours appears consistent with the small, but significant, increases observed between 2007 and 2012 in the self-report subscale means for Total Difficulties, Emotional Symptoms, Peer Relationship Problems and Hyperactivity-Inattention (but a decrease in Conduct Problems) in nationally representative New Zealand samples of children aged 12–15 years,28 and with a similar increase in Emotional Symptoms and decrease in Conduct Problems between 2009 and 2014 in English community samples of children aged 11–13 years.29 The mean PLE score in the MCS sample aligned closely with that reported previously for a relatively deprived inner-city London, UK, community sample aged 9–12 years19 using these same nine items, although the overall prevalence of a ‘Certainly True’ to at least one of the nine items in the MCS (52.2%) was lower than that obtained in the London sample (66.0%).8

For the SDQ psychopathology scales, table 5 (and online supplementary table 5-X) indicates the proportions of children falling within the normal (defined as ~80%), borderline (~10%) and abnormal (~10%) categories defined for the SDQ based on the UK population norms, as well as the proportions of children scoring in each category of the more recent four-level solution (close to average ~80%, slightly raised ~10%, high ~5%, very high ~5%). Several departures from these figures are notable (eg, 91% of children scored in the normal range of the Prosocial Behaviour scale, and only 67% of children scored ‘close to average’ on the Peer Relationship Problems scale); the application of the established scoring metrics derived on the UK population samples may overestimate the prevalence of problems with peers and underestimate vulnerability on Prosocial Behaviour among Australian children aged approximately 11 years.

Table 5.

Distribution of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) categories on each subscale as defined by the traditional three-level and more recent four-level solutions

SDQ subscale Sample Normal Borderline Abnormal
(n) % (n) % (n) % (n)
 Emotional Symptoms 27 473 84.6 (23 233) 6.5 (1778) 9.0 (2462)
 Peer Relationship Problems 27 474 81.2 (22 318) 13.8 (3789) 5.0 (1367)
 Conduct Problems 27 474 83.2 (22 870) 7.7 (2125) 9.0 (2479)
 Hyperactivity-Inattention 27 472 78.0 (21 416) 9.5 (2613) 12.5 (3443)
 Prosocial Behaviour 27 474 90.7 (24 908) 5.6 (1543) 3.7 (1023)
 Total Difficulties 27 472 79.9 (21 943) 11.6 (3180) 8.6 (2349)
SDQ subscale Sample Close to average Slightly raised High Very high
(n) % (n) % (n) % (n) % (n)
 Emotional Symptoms 27 473 75.2 (20 659) 9.4 (2574) 6.5 (1778) 9.0 (2462)
 Peer Relationship Problems 27 474 66.9 (18 368) 14.4 (3950) 8.9 (2445) 9.9 (2711)
 Conduct Problems 27 474 83.2 (22 870) 7.7 (2125) 4.6 (1258) 4.4 (1221)
 Hyperactivity-Inattention 27 472 78.0 (21 416) 9.5 (2613) 6.2 (1713) 6.3 (1730)
 Prosocial Behaviour* 27 474 90.7 (24 908) 5.6 (1543) 2.3 (633) 1.4 (390)
 Total difficulties 27 472 75.8 (20 815) 10.6 (2923) 5.0 (1385) 8.6 (2349)

*For the Prosocial Behaviour subscale, the four-level classification labels are instead ‘close to average’, ‘slightly lowered’, ‘low’ and ‘very low’.

Capacity for direct comparison of MCS data with published data from similar large, general population samples was limited for the other scales owing to modification from the original response formats to a standard three-choice format, adoption of a standard method of summed total scores for all scales and by minor alterations to the wording of some items. Despite these modifications, consistencies with data from other developed nations were apparent: children’s reports of Social Integration at school were similar to those reported previously in primary school samples in Australia14 and Hong Kong30 31; response patterns on the EATQ-R scales (Attention, Inhibitory Control, Perceptual Sensitivity and Aggression) aligned with data from a community sample of 1055 Dutch32 school students of similar age and access to Supportive Relationships at Home, School and in the Community was similar to that reported for a community sample of Canadian fourth-grade school children (~2 years younger than our sample).16 The pattern of responses on the Big Five personality constructs was also consistent with that reported for an Australian sample of 268 children aged 10–12 years33 using the full 65-item version of the BFQ-C.20

Sex differences

Supplementary table 2-X provides the item responses and scale distributions separately for girls and boys, and the eta squared (η2) estimate of the effect size of sex differences for each scale. Statistically significant differences between the scores of girls and boys were apparent on all scales, although the magnitude of these differences was small (sex effects on all scales accounted for ≤2% of total variance, except for the small-to-medium effects, explaining 4% of total variance, on Prosocial Behaviour and Aggression). Across the domains, girls’ mean scores were greater than those of boys’ on Social Integration, Prosocial Behaviour, Supportive Relationships at Home, School and in the Community, Empathy, Emotional Symptoms, Attention and Inhibitory Control, Perceptual Sensitivity and Psychotic-Like Experiences, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, Self-esteem and Connection to Nature. Conversely, boys’ mean scores were greater on Peer Relationship Problems, Conduct Problems and Aggression, Hyperactivity-Inattention, Total Difficulties (psychopathology), Extraversion, Openness/Intellect and Daytime Sleepiness.

Strengths and limitations

The major strengths of the MCS are twofold. First, the MCS provides a comprehensive assessment of psychosocial and behavioural constructs reflecting mental health and well-being in a large sample of 27 808 children aged approximately 11 years (representing 31.4% of eligible NSW students), which is representative of the NSW population on a range of demographic variables (table 3). Second, the MCS incorporated measures of both personal competencies and vulnerabilities, and the scores on every scale spanned the entire range of possible scores, providing capacity to examine patterns of both strength and vulnerability in the population. This also facilitates the identification of determinants of average mental health in the population (rather than focusing on the extreme ends of the distribution), which will provide important information to guide the development and implementation of universal mental health promotion programmes alongside targeted approaches for vulnerable children.34 Data were collected by self-report, providing access to the child’s own perspective on their experiences, which may be particularly important for phenomena that are less readily judged by other informants. Finally, an important strength of the MCS lies in being embedded within planned record linkages of the NSW-CDS,5 incorporating intergenerational records on health, education, child protection and justice contacts, and with the AEDC6 assessment of early childhood development at age 5 years. This will allow responses on the MCS to be interpreted in the context of longitudinal data that is subject to minimal selection bias and will permit investigation of multiple factors associated with outcomes of low prevalence, and/or of relevance to cultural, geographic, socioeconomic or other subgroups within the population.

A number of limitations of the MCS must be acknowledged. Despite the large sample obtained being representative of the population from which it was drawn, failure to obtain data from all individuals will have the consequence of limiting data available to the current and future record linkages conducted within the NSW-CDS framework. The MCS is further limited by a lack of parent and/or teacher reports to supplement children’s self-report. Only moderate agreement is typical between child, parent and teacher ratings of children’s mental health and well-being, indicating that the ratings of informants are not interchangeable.35 Furthermore, the MCS was limited in coverage both in terms of domains assessed and the number of items assessing each domain; these were constrained by the limited time available within schools for survey administration, lack of parent and/or teacher reports on additional aspects of children’s experiences and by the sensitivities associated with assessing domains perceived as potentially distressing for the child. For example, information on potentially important constructs such as bullying/victimisation experiences or physical health (including participation in health/leisure activities and nutrition) was not obtained. Similarly, our assessment of Aggression was limited to only two items, which do not capture the full complexity and multidimensional nature of this construct. And, while aspects of the cognitive control of emotions and behaviours were measured, no assessment of cognitive capacities was obtained; linkage of the MCS with education records on academic progress within the NSW-CDS will provide some index of these capacities. The lack of capacity to compare MCS data directly with published data from similar large, general population samples was limited for most scales owing to modification from the original response formats to a standard three-choice format, adoption of a standard method of summed total scores for all scales and by minor alterations to the wording of some items. On several scales, including the personality dimensions, the restriction of responses to three categories may have artificially reduced variability among participants, with <10% of children electing one of the three options on several items. Prior to MCS administration, psychometric testing of our English translation of the short-form Italian BFQ-C20 measure of personality dimensions was conducted using the data obtained from 645 children during pilot testing of the survey in 2014, with subsequent revision of 5 of the 25 items assessing these dimensions in the MCS. A manuscript reporting the validity and reliability of this revised measure is currently being drafted for publication.

Future plans

Further structural analysis of the MCS data is underway to derive the most psychometrically robust measures of each mental health and well-being domain. The multiagency, intergenerational linkage of the MCS data with other health, education, child protection, justice and AEDC records took place late in 2016. This will be used to elucidate patterns of risk and protection across early and middle child development, and also provide a foundation for future record linkages in the cohort that will track mental and physical health, social and educational/occupational outcomes into adolescence and early adulthood. The record linkage will also incorporate data on the quality and extent of implementation of mental health promotion and early intervention programmes in NSW schools, affording an opportunity to examine how delivery of such programmes may modify individual pathways of social, emotional and behavioural function between early and middle childhood. This work will assist in determining appropriate universal mental health promotion and targeted early intervention programmes that can bolster strengths and mitigate risks in order to maximise healthy development.

i

The reasons for principal opt-outs were not assessed systematically, but among those who volunteered this information, these were predominantly that the school was too busy to participate or already committed to other research participation.

ii

For the Empathy construct, the ‘alpha if item removed’ value indicated improvement of the α-coefficient following removal of one of the four items. For the Attention construct, alpha was improved by relocating an item from the Inhibitory Control scale that has been previously demonstrated to load with the Attention items in published factor analysis of the full scale.32 These modifications are indicated by ‡ in table 1 (and online supplementary table 1-X), and detail on the revised scales included in table 4 (and online supplementary table 2-X).

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support provided for the MCS administration by the NSW Department of Education (represented by Robert Stevens, Susan Harriman and Liliana Ructtinger), the Catholic Education Commission NSW (represented by Peter Grace), the Association of Independent Schools of NSW (represented by Robyn Yates and Susan Wright) and other stakeholder organisations including the NSW Primary Principals’ Association, NSW Teachers Federation, Independent Education Union NSW/ACT, Federation of Parents and Citizens Associations of NSW, Council of Catholic School Parents, NSW Parents’ Council, Isolated Children’s Parents’ Association and the NSW Aboriginal Education Consultative Group. The authors also acknowledge the contributions of all members of the NSW-CDS Scientific Committee who oversaw the project: Vaughan Carr (Chairman), Miles Bore, Sally Brinkman, Marilyn Chilvers, Kimberlie Dean, Katherine Dix, Luke Duffy, Melissa Green, Felicity Harris, Allyson Holbrook, Maina Kariuki, Kristin Laurens, Rhoshel Lenroot, Kim Robinson, Maxwell Smith, Titia Sprague, Robert Stevens, Michael Tarren-Sweeney, Stacy Tzoumakis and Megan Williams; as well as the support for MCS administration provided by former members of the Committee: Stephanie Dick, Philip Hull, Luming Luo, Stephen Lynn, Brooke McIntyre and Alessandra Raudino. The authors thank the IT contractor, Multi-Media Concepts (Cary Redstone and Jarratt Holliday), for their provision of the online survey. The information and perspectives expressed in the manuscript do not necessarily reflect the views of these contributing organisations or individuals.

Footnotes

Collaboration: Initial data analyses and publications on Middle Childhood Survey and linked data were generated primarily by the authors of this paper and other members of the Scientific Committee, named in the Acknowledgements section, who oversaw the New South Wales Child Development Study (NSW-CDS). The research team is open to research collaborations with other scientists, within restrictions placed on the use of linked data according to strict privacy legislation; interested parties should contact the Lead Investigator of the NSW-CDS (Vaughan Carr: v.carr@unsw.edu.au) with their expressions of interest.

Contributors: In line with the ICMJE authorship guidelines, KRL, ST, KD, SAB, MB, RKL, MS, AH, KMR, RS, FH, VJC and MJG made substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work. KRL, ST, KD, FH, VJC and MJG made substantial contributions to the acquisition, analysis and interpretation of the data. KRL, ST, KD, SAB, MB, RKL, MS, AH, KMR, RS, FH, VJC and MJG contributed to the drafting of the manuscript and/or revising of the manuscript. KRL, ST, KD, SAB, MB, RKL, MS, AH, KMR, RS, FH, VJC and MJG have given final approval of the version to be published, and agree to its accuracy.

Funding: This work was supported by funding from a National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (NHMRC) Project Grant (1058652); an Australian Research Council Linkage Project (LP110100150), with the NSW Ministry of Health, NSW Department of Education and the NSW Department of Family and Community Services representing the Linkage Project Partners; and an Australian Rotary Health Research Grant (RG104090). KRL, FH and VJC were supported by funding from the Schizophrenia Research Institute, Australia, using infrastructure funding from the NSW Ministry of Health. MJG was supported by an NHMRC R.D. Wright Biomedical Career Development Fellowship (1061875). KD was supported by the Justice Health & Forensic Mental Health Network, NSW. SAB was supported by an NHMRC Early Career Fellowship (1090146). KMR received scholarship funding from the Australian Postgraduate Award scheme, the Faculty of Education and Arts at the University of Newcastle and the Australian Research Council Linkage Project (LP110100150).

Competing interests: None declared.

Ethics approval: Ethical approval was obtained from the University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Committee (UNSW HREC reference HC14307) and the NSW Department of Education State Education Research Applications Process (reference 2015082); the use of opt-out consent procedures (for parents and children) was guided by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council National Statement of Ethical Conduct in Human Research (chapter 2.3), which specifies conditions under which these procedures are appropriate. Access to publicly available school-level data on enrolment and demographic indices for the 2371 eligible NSW schools (used to estimate the representativeness of participating schools and children) was acquired from the Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority under UNSW HREC approval (reference HC14348).

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Data sharing statement: Strict privacy legislation places restrictions on the use of linked data; scientists interested in using the data in collaboration with the NSW-CDS Investigators should contact the Lead Investigator of the NSW-CDS (Vaughan Carr: v.carr@unsw.edu.au) with their expressions of interest.

Correction notice: This paper has been amended since it was published Online First. Owing to a scripting error, some of the publisher names in the references were replaced with 'BMJ Publishing Group'. This only affected the full text version, not the PDF. We have since corrected these errors and the correct publishers have been inserted into the references.

References

  • 1. Collins WA. Development during middle childhood: the years from six to twelve. Washington, D.C.: National Academies, 1984. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Zembar MJ, Blume LB. Middle Childhood Development: a Contextual Approach. Upper Saddle River, N.J: Merrill/Pearson, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Costello EJ, Copeland W, Angold A. The Great Smoky Mountains Study: developmental epidemiology in the southeastern United States. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2016;51:639–46. 10.1007/s00127-015-1168-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Poulton R, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study: overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2015;50:679–93. 10.1007/s00127-015-1048-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Carr VJ, Harris F, Raudino A, et al. New South Wales Child Development Study (NSW-CDS): an australian multiagency, multigenerational, longitudinal record linkage study. BMJ Open 2016;6:e009023 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Brinkman SA, Gregory TA, Goldfeld S, et al. Data resource profile: the australian early development index (AEDI). Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:1089–96. 10.1093/ije/dyu085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Laurens KR, Luo L, Matheson SL, et al. Common or distinct pathways to psychosis? A systematic review of evidence from prospective studies for developmental risk factors and antecedents of the schizophrenia spectrum disorders and affective psychoses. BMC Psychiatry 2015;15:1–20. 10.1186/s12888-015-0562-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Laurens KR, Hobbs MJ, Sunderland M, et al. Psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of 8000 children aged 9 to 11 years: an item response theory analysis. Psychol Med 2012;42:1495–506. 10.1017/S0033291711002108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Drake C, Nickel C, Burduvali E, et al. The paediatric daytime sleepiness scale (PDSS): sleep habits and school outcomes in middle-school children. Sleep 2003;26:455–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Tim Gill. The Benefits of Children's Engagement with Nature: A Systematic Literature Review. Child Youth Environ 2014;24:10–34. 10.7721/chilyoutenvi.24.2.0010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Brinkman S, Sayers M, Goldfeld S, et al. Population monitoring of language and cognitive development in Australia: the australian Early Development Index. Int J Speech Lang Pathol 2009;11:419–30. 10.1080/17549500903147552 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997;38:581–6. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40:1337–45. 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Ainley J, Bourke S. Student views of primary schooling. Research Papers in Education 1992;7:107–28. 10.1080/0267152920070202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Hanson TL, Kim J. West REL, Measuring resilience and youth development: the psychometric properties of the healthy kids survey. Issues & answers. REL 2007-No. 34. San Francisco, CA: IES, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Schonert-Reichl KA, Guhn M, Gadermann AM, et al. Development and Validation of the Middle years Development Instrument (MDI): Assessing Children's Well-Being and Assets across Multiple Contexts. Soc Indic Res 2013;114:345–69. 10.1007/s11205-012-0149-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Ellis LK, Rothbart MK. Revision of the early adolescent temperament questionnaire biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Minneapolis, MN: Poster reprint, 2001. http://www.bowdoin.edu/~sputnam/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/pdf/lesa-ellis-srcd-poster-reprint.pdf (accessed 4 Aug 2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Garton AF, Gringart E. The development of a scale to measure empathy in 8- and 9-year-old children. Aust J Educ & Dev Psychol 2005;5:17–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Laurens KR, Hodgins S, Maughan B, et al. Community screening for psychotic-like experiences and other putative antecedents of schizophrenia in children aged 9-12 years. Schizophr Res 2007;90(1-3):130–46. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Barbaranelli C, Caprara GV, Rabasca A, et al. A questionnaire for measuring the big five in late childhood. Pers Individ Dif 2003;34:645–64. 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00051-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Huebner ES. Preliminary development and validation of a multidimensional life satisfaction scale for children. Psychol Assess 1994;6:149–58. 10.1037/1040-3590.6.2.149 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Cheng JC-H, Monroe MC. Connection to Nature: children's Affective Attitude Toward Nature. Environ Behav 2012;44:31–49. 10.1177/0013916510385082 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Mayer FS, Frantz CM. The connectedness to nature scale: a measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J Environ Psychol 2004;24:503–15. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Zumbo BD, Gadermann AM, Zeisser C. Ordinal versions of coefficients alpha and theta for Likert rating scales. J Modern Appl Stat Method 2007;6:21–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Kelleher I, Connor D, Clarke MC, et al. Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychol Med 2012;42:1857–63. 10.1017/S0033291711002960 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull 1992;112:155–9. 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Mellor D. Normative data for the strengths and difficulties questionnaire in Australia. Aust Psychol 2005;40:215–22. 10.1080/00050060500243475 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Fleming TM, Clark T, Denny S, et al. Stability and change in the mental health of New Zealand secondary school students 2007-2012: results from the national adolescent health surveys. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2014;48:472–80. 10.1177/0004867413514489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Fink E, Patalay P, Sharpe H, et al. Mental Health Difficulties in early adolescence: a comparison of two Cross-Sectional studies in England from 2009 to 2014. J Adolesc Health 2015;56:502–7. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.01.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Kong C-K. Classroom learning experiences and students’ perceptions of quality of school life. Learn Environ Res 2008;11:111–29. 10.1007/s10984-008-9040-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Sun-Keung Pang N. Students' Perceptions of Quality of School Life in Hong Kong primary schools. J Educ Res 1999;14:1–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Muris P, Meesters C. Reactive and regulative temperament in Youths: psychometric evaluation of the early adolescent temperament Questionnaire-Revised. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2009;31:7–19. 10.1007/s10862-008-9089-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Gresham D, Gullone E. Emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: the explanatory roles of personality and attachment. Pers Individ Dif 2012;52:616–21. 10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Goodman A, Goodman R. Population mean scores predict child mental disorder rates: validating SDQ prevalence estimators in Britain. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011;52:100–8. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02278.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. van der Ende J, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H. Agreement of informants on emotional and behavioral problems from childhood to adulthood. Psychol Assess 2012;24:293–300. 10.1037/a0025500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material 1

bmjopen-2017-016244supp001.pdf (230.4KB, pdf)


Articles from BMJ Open are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES