Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2017 Dec 13;12(12):e0189148. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189148

Does a decision aid improve informed choice in mammography screening? Results from a randomised controlled trial

Maren Reder 1,2,*, Petra Kolip 1
Editor: Jacobus P van Wouwe3
PMCID: PMC5728514  PMID: 29236722

Abstract

Background

Decision aids can support informed choice in mammography screening, but for the German mammography screening programme no systematically evaluated decision aid exists to date. We developed a decision aid for women invited to this programme for the first time based on the criteria of the International Patient Decision Aids Standards Collaboration.

Objective

To determine whether a decision aid increases informed choice about mammography screening programme participation.

Methods

A representative sample of 7,400 women aged 50 was drawn from registration offices in Westphalia-Lippe, Germany. Women were randomised to receive usual care (i.e., the standard information brochure sent with the programme’s invitation letter) or the decision aid. Data were collected online at baseline, post-intervention, and 3 months follow-up. The primary outcome was informed choice. Secondary outcomes were the constituents of informed choice (knowledge, attitude, intention/uptake), decisional conflict, decision regret, and decision stage. Outcomes were analysed using latent structural equation models and χ2-tests.

Results

1,206 women participated (response rate of 16.3%). The decision aid increased informed choice. Women in the control group had lower odds to make an informed choice at post-intervention (OR 0.26, 95% CI 0.18-0.37) and at follow-up (OR 0.66, 95% CI 0.46-0.94); informed choices remained constant at 30%. This was also reflected in lower knowledge and more decisional conflict. Post-intervention, the uptake intention was higher in the control group, whereas the uptake rate at follow-up was similar. Women in the control group had a more positive attitude at follow-up than women receiving the decision aid. Decision regret and decision stage were not influenced by the intervention.

Conclusion

This paper describes the first systematic evaluation of a newly developed decision aid for the German mammography screening programme in a randomised controlled trial. Our decision aid proved to be an effective tool to enhance the rate of informed choice and was made accessible to the public.

Trial registration

German Clinical Trials Register DRKS00005176.

Introduction

Women aged 50 to 69 are invited to participate in the German mammography screening programme (MSP). However, informed choice is only achieved in a small proportion of decisions made about participation in the MSP [1]. This uninformed compliance [2] is a major public health problem, mainly because it is unclear whether mammography screening is beneficial [3]. Many women expect unrealistic benefits of this screening [4]. A decision is classified as informed, if the decision maker has good knowledge about the options, her/his attitude is congruent with the decision, and she/he then implements this decision [5]. To enable women invited to the MSP to such an informed choice, they need to be informed about the existing benefits and harms including their probability and to be supported in clarifying the meanings of those benefits and harms for themselves [6]. Importantly, no correct course of action can be determined [7]—only a personally preferred course of action.

Decision aids (DAs) are an effective way to support informed choices: they improve knowledge about options, increase active engagement in decision making, lead to a higher proportion of choices being in congruence with the decision maker‘s values, and lower decisional conflict due to feeling uninformed or unclear about ones values [8]. A typical DA presents risk information both numerically and graphically and includes a type of values clarification exercise. The International Patient Decision Aid Standards Collaboration (IPDAS) [9] developed very comprehensive and widely used standards for DAs. In the context of mammography screening, DAs have the potential to increase the proportion of women making informed choices [10].

Unfortunately, the magnitude of information materials for all kinds of health decisions is contrasted by a paucity of high-quality decision aids [11]. Lenz et al., taking an inventory of German language DAs, found 12 DAs that had been evaluated in a RCT [11] but none of these was on mammography screening. Overall, two types of information materials on the MSP are available: (1) materials published by the Kooperationsgemeinschaft Mammographie which also offers the screening itself; and (2) materials published by health insurances. None of the materials available for the German MSP at the time our study were sufficiently in line with IPDAS criteria [9] or systematically evaluated in a RCT. We therefore developed an evidence-based online interactive decision aid for the German MSP. Recently and after the end of our data collection, a printed DA has been developed by the IQWiQ [12] but it has not been systematically evaluated in a RCT yet.

Internationally, DAs on mammography screening have been systematically evaluated. The Cochrane Review [8] reports two DAs on mammography screening [10, 13] (see DAs 1 and 2 described below). Completively, the Inventory of Decision Aids of the Ottawa Hospital Research Institute [14] yields 5 DAs (other than our DA) on mammography screening: (1) a DA for 40-year old women [13], (2) a DA for 70-year old women [10], (3) a DA for women ages 40-49 for deciding screening start age and interval [15], (4) a DA for women with dense breasts [16], and (5) a DA with the options of screening start at 40 or 50 [17]. None of these DAs is targeted at an average risk population ages 50-69 and three did not present not having a mammography as an option but instead compared different starting ages, screening intervals or additional screening tests.

The DA for women aged 40 improved knowledge and increased the proportion of women who had made a choice but did not affect the proportion of informed choices [13]. The DA for women aged 70 increased knowledge and proportion of informed choices [10]. In a RCT comparing a DA for women aged 50 with information on overdetection to a DA without such information [18], more women in the intervention group had adequate knowledge, fewer had a positive attitude, and fewer intended to be screened. In a pre-post study with 75 women ages 40 to 49 [19], the DA reduced decisional conflict and had no effect on intention. Therefore, we expect positive effects of our DA on informed choice, knowledge and decisional conflict.

Study objectives

The present study aimed to assess for the first time in Germany the effect of an interactive online DA on informed choice in a RCT with 3 months follow-up. This RCT compared women receiving a DA additional to usual care (i.e., the brochure of the MSP [20]) to women only receiving usual care. The DA included both additional information (all cause mortality) as well as a different presentation (crowd figure pictograms and values clarification exercise). The primary objective was to assess whether the DA increases the proportion of women making an informed choice. The secondary objectives were to evaluate whether the DA (1) increases knowledge about the MSP, (2) changes attitudes on the MSP, (3) changes participation intentions, (4) reduces decisional conflict, and (5) reduces decision regret.

Methods

This study was conducted in Wesphalia-Lippe, Germany. In our non-blinded two-armed RCT (German Clinical Trials Register DRKS00005176), the participating women were randomised to receive either the DA (intervention group) or usual care (control group). The usual care for women aged 50 in Germany involved, at the time of the study, an invitation to the MSP accompanied by an information brochure (see [20]). This brochure contained written and numerical information about the MSP. Both study groups received these standard materials; the intervention group additionally received the DA. The online assessments were conducted at baseline (T1), post-intervention (T2, two weeks after T1), and 3 months follow-up (T3).

The Ethics Commission of the Medical Association Westphalia-Lippe and the Medical Faculty of the University of Münster approved our study protocol. It was originally planned that women would receive the baseline questionnaire and the DA as well as the post questionnaire in one session. The baseline assessment was moved to two weeks before the intervention and post questionnaire to keep the time needed to work through the DA and respond to the questionnaire to an acceptable level.

Our study invitation contained information about the content, purpose, and procedure of the study including the information that our trial was conducted independent of the MSP. Once written informed consent was obtained, the participating women were randomised to the intervention or control group by the researchers through an allocation sequence generated by a random number generator (Random.org). Women were only informed about their study group at the second assessment, when they either received the DA or only the questionnaire.

The invitation to the study was sent by post 3 weeks before the estimated arrival of the MSP invitation (for more details, see the study protocol [6]). Three weeks after the postal invitation, women consenting to study participation and providing their e-mail address received the link to the baseline questionnaire (T1). Women were e-mailed the link to the second assessment (T2) 2 weeks after the baseline assessment. At T2, women in the intervention group received a link to the DA and the second assessment whereas women in the control group received a link only to the second assessment. The link to the third assessment (T3) was e-mailed to the women 3 months after T2. The screening appointment was assumed to have passed at this time. A reminder was e-mailed 10 days after each survey. Participating women received all 3 e-mails irrespective of their response to the previous questionnaire. Data were collected between April and November 2014. All questionnaires were based on the questionnaire of the study ‘Informed Choice of German and Turkish Women for Participation in the MSP (InEMa)’ [21]. Modifications were made to make it suitable for evaluating an intervention and to be used Online (for the original questionnaire see [1]). All assessments were linked to each other through a self-generated code.

Participants and recruitment

The sample of 7,400 women was randomly drawn from registration offices in Westphalia-Lippe, North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany. Women were eligible for the trial if they were aged 50 (birth months of March to May 1964). Women with a potential Turkish migration background (according to a name algorithm [22]) were assigned to the InEMa study and accordingly, our sample comprised only women without Turkish migration background.

Decision aid

The intervention group received an online DA which was designed to comply with IPDAS criteria (see the BARMER website where our DA (the DA is in German) was made available after the end of our study (https://www.barmer.de/gesundheit/praevention/krebspraevention/krebsfrueherkennung/mammographie-13876) and the Decision Aid Library Inventory where it was registered (https://decisionaid.ohri.ca/AZsumm.php?ID=1673)).

Our Online DA consisted of a static information part and an interactive part (see study protocol for a more detailed description [6]). Mathieu et al.’s DA [13] provided the basis for developing the structure of our DA. To meet IPDAS criteria [23], our DA presented the decision options of participation or non-participation in the MSP in their relevant context.

In the information part, the chance of each outcome was expressed as event rate per 200 women screened every 2 years for 20 years using absolute numbers accompanied by crowd figure pictograms (see additional file of the protocol [6]). The advantages and disadvantages of the MSP and their probabilities were described. This was compared to the option of no screening. The information women receive with the brochure in the MSP invitation [20] was included in the information part of our DA (see quantitative information in Table 1).

Table 1. Comparison between decision aid and information brochure.

Information brochure Decision aid
General description Paper based booklet / PDF, 12 pages Online decision aid, information part and interactive part with 3 steps (assigning the information items to categories, rating the importance of each information item, making a choice)
Visual aspects Short texts, no graphics or pictures, use of arrows as bullet points, questions as headings Short texts, bullet points, questions as headings, coloured text boxes, crowd figure pictograms, rating scales, graphical summary of personal responses, downloadable personal PDF at the end
Key factual content General information about the MSP, quality of the MSP, breast cancer and its risk factors, screening procedure, interval cancers and symptoms, follow-up diagnostics, advantages and disadvantages of the MSP Target group of the DA, breast cancer mortality, overall mortality, true positives, false positives, interval cancers, overdiagnoses, screening procedure, symptoms of breast cancer
Quantitative information Number of:
  • positive and negative screening results

  • follow-up diagnostics and biopsies

  • breast cancer diagnoses

  • interval cancers

  • breast cancer deaths with mammography screening

  • additional deaths without mammography screening

  • overdiagnoses

Number of:
  • breast cancer deaths with and without mammography screening

  • all-cause deaths with and without mammography screening

  • negative screening results

  • positive screening results/ follow-up diagnostics

  • breast cancer diagnoses

  • interval cancers

  • overdiagnoses

Presentation of quantitative information Absolute numbers presented in text (200 women with biannual mammography screening over 20 years) Absolute numbers supported by 3 crowd figure pictograms consisting of 200 female pictograms (200 women over 20 years): (1) breast cancer mortality with biannual mammography screening, (2) breast cancer mortality without mammography screening, (3) false positives, breast cancer diagnoses, and interval cancers with biannual mammography screening
Values clarification exercise None Interactive personal work sheet, evaluating information as in favour of or against mammography screening, evaluating importance of information, making a decision about participation in the MSP, input window for remaining questions, downloadable PDF summarising information and personal responses

The interactive part of the DA summarised the main points of the information part and encouraged engagement with the information. It consisted of three steps. (1) The women assigned the information items to the categories ‘in favour of mammography screening’, ‘neither for nor against such screening’, or ‘against the screening’. (2) They rated the importance of each information item for the decision. (3) They made a choice. At the end, the participants received a tailored summary based on their responses.

The content, design, and layout of the DA was informed by qualitative interviews with women and by consulting experts for women‘s health and prevention (see the study protocol [6]). The findings of pre-testing the DA resulted in additionally including the likelihood of all cause mortality. Information on all cause mortality was not part of the brochure and the first draft of our DA. Since both experts and women in the qualitative pre-test, thought information on this desirable, we included this information in our DA. As a result, the information content of the DA differed from that of the brochure.

Outcome measures

Primary outcome measure

According to the three-dimensional classification framework of Marteau et al. [5], the following dimensions were assessed and the continuous scales were then dichotomously coded: (1) knowledge about the screening, (2) attitude towards the screening, and (3) intention/uptake (depending on measurement point). A choice to take part in the MSP was considered to be informed, if a woman had adequate knowledge, positive attitude and positive intention/uptake [5]. A choice not to participate in the MSP was considered to be informed, if a woman had adequate knowledge, negative attitude and no intention/uptake [5]. Therefore, a dichotomous primary outcome resulted (informed/uninformed). Informed choice is thus dependent on both the dimensions used as well as the cut-points employed to dichotomise the continuous dimensions knowledge and attitude.

Secondary outcome measures

We used a similar approach to assess knowledge as in a previous study on screening decisions [1] using seven multiple choice items: (1) target group of the MSP, (2) number of women receiving a positive result, (3) whether a positive screening result equals a diagnosis, (4) existence of false negatives, (5) number of diagnoses in screened vs unscreened populations, (6) number of breast cancer deaths in screened vs unscreened populations, and (7) existence of overtreatment. Questions 1 and 3 to 7 assessed conceptual knowledge. Only Question 2 assessed numerical knowledge with four value ranges as response options. This numerical information was considered to be especially important as the number of women receiving a positive mammography screening result (as opposed to a negative screening result) is the most proximal screening outcome. Each correct answer was scored 1 point; items were then summed to calculate the knowledge index (range 0 to 7). To calculate informed choice, the knowledge index was dichotomised. We used a marking scheme for the knowledge index that is similar to previous research [10, 24] and decided a priori that a mark of 50% or above (score > 3) would be considered as adequate knowledge (see study protocol for a more detailed description of all outcome measures [6]).

Attitude was measured according to the reasoned action approach of Fishbein and Ajzen [25]. The four items were adapted from Marteau et al. [5] for use with mammography screening and rated on a five-point scale. To calculate informed choice, the scale ranging from -8 to +8 was dichotomised: a scale score of ≥ 0 represented a positive attitude.

Intention to participate in the MSP and self-reported uptake were each measured using one item: intention to participate in the MSP in the next 3 months (yes/no/undecided) and uptake of the MSP in the last 3 months (MSP/opportunistic screening/none). To calculate informed choice, intention was dichotomised as ‘participation in the MSP in the next 3 months’ and ‘no participation in the MSP in the next 3 months’ and undecided women were excluded. In addition, women who reported at T2 to want to participate in opportunistic screening were excluded. Consequently, only those were categorised as women with a positive intention, who intended to participate in the MSP in the next 3 months (this category did not include women intending to participate in opportunistic screening or intending to participate in the MSP at some time beyond the three months), and only those were categorised as women with a negative intention, who neither intended to participate in the MSP nor in opportunistic screening in the next 3 months (this category also did not include undecided women). At T3, behaviour was dichotomised as ‘participation in the MSP’ and ‘no participation in the MSP’. Women who had taken part in opportunistic screening were excluded.

Decisional conflict was measured using the 4-item SURE (Sure of myself; Understand information; Risk-benefit ratio; Encouragement) test [26]. To calculate the total score ‘uncertainty’ all items were summed (score range 0 to 4) with a high value indicating high decisional conflict. Decision regret was measured at T3 using the Decision Regret Scale [27]. Decision stage was measured with one item with the response options ‘not thought about’, ‘contemplating it’, ‘close to deciding’, and ‘choice already made’ [28].

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed with SPSS version 23.0 (IBM, Corp., Armonk, NY) and MPlus version 7.0 (Muthen & Muthen, Los Angeles, CA). Possible baseline differences between trial arms were statistically tested with an α of.15. The impact of the DA on the primary outcome informed choice was analysed using cross-sectional χ2-tests for all measurement points according to the classification model of Marteau et al. [5].

All secondary outcomes with several indicators (i.e., knowledge, attitude, decisional conflict, decision regret) were modelled as latent variables which allowed to account for measurement error and to test measurement invariance. Since measurement invariance is necessary to conduct and interpret analyses on longitudinal multigroup data [29], measurement invariance levels were tested across time and group before analyses of intervention effects were performed. Partial invariance (i.e., invariance of the majority of indicators, with some parameters being freely estimated) was tested if full measurement invariance was not tenable. Partial measurement invariance is unproblematic when only few loadings or intercepts are variant [29]. Additionally, modelling outcomes as latent variables allowed us to apply full information maximum likelihood estimation enabling us to include individuals with missing values in the analysis. To test the intervention effect, autoregressive latent models [29] were applied with group (0 = control, 1 = DA) predicting T2 and T3 latent outcomes. A first order autoregressive effect of T1 on T2 and a second order autoregressive effect of T1 on T3 were specified. The autoregressive path between T2 and T3 was fixed to 0 to allow a comparison to the baseline measurement. For scale setting, all models were calculated using the Fixed-Factor-Method [29].

Two types of latent analyses were conducted: (1) Numeric secondary outcome items forming a continuous latent factor (i.e., attitude and decision regret) were analysed using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). For these models, model fit was evaluated using the following goodness-of-fit indices: Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). A model with acceptable fit should yield a CFI ≥ 0.90, TLI ≥ 0.90 and a RMSEA ≤ 0.08 [29]. For the CFA models, the assumption of the respective invariance level held, if the CFI difference to the model of the previous invariance level was ≤ .01 [30]. For our analyses, data had to be strong-factorially invariant (loadings and intercepts of the indicators equal over time and group) [29].

(2) Categorical secondary outcomes forming a continuous latent factor (i.e., decisional conflict and knowledge) were analysed using 2-parameter-logistic item factor analysis (IFA). For these models, the assumption of invariance held, if the loglikelihood test was not significant [31].

Single-indictor secondary outcomes were analysed with χ2-tests (intention and uptake) or a Mann-Whitney-U-test (decision stage) according to their level of measurement.

Results

7,400 women were invited to take part in this study (see Fig 1). 1,206 women consented to take part in the study and provided their e-mail address, through which they were contacted at the three assessments. In our sample size calculation, we had determined a sample of 740 women [6]. Compared to our estimated response rate of 15%, the actual response rate was 16.3%. At T3 41.4% of women randomised responded. After code matching the data of all measurement points, 1,052 datasets resulted (this includes women responding at any one measurement point). Women who ever had breast cancer (n = 29), and women, who did not respond to this question (n = 26), were excluded from the analyses. Women, who self-reported at T2 that the appointment proposed in the MSP invitation already had passed, were also excluded (n = 84) because they either would already have attended the screening or decided to not attend it. Accordingly, the data of 913 women were analysed.

Fig 1. Consort flow diagram.

Fig 1

Baseline characteristics

Background and outcome variables were similar between groups (Table 2). Most participating women had received at least 10 years of school education. For almost all German was their main language. More than 60% had had a mammogram in the past; of those, more than half had been conducted for screening purposes. Just under 60% had received the invitation to the MSP and the associated brochure. About 90% had a statutory health insurance covering the MSP.

Table 2. Baseline characteristics, n (%).

Control Decision aid
General
Education
 9 years 43 (9.7) 50 (11.2)
 10 years 189 (42.6) 194 (43.4)
 11 years 74 (16.7) 57 (12.8)
 ≥12 years 133 (30.0) 141 (31.5)
 Other 5 (1.1) 5 (1.1)
Main language
 German* 444 (99.8) 466 (100.0)
Internet information search per week
 <1h 73 (16.0) 83 (18.1)
 1h to <2h 157 (34.5) 146 (31.9)
 2h to <5h 150 (33.0) 139 (30.3)
 5h to <10h 52 (11.4) 55 (12.0)
 >10h 23 (5.1) 35 (7.6)
Internet importance
 important 264 (58.3) 263 (57.8)
 neither nor 123 (27.2) 110 (24.2)
 unimportant 66 (14.6) 82 (18.0)
Self-rated health
 very good 90 (20.5) 87 (19.7)
 good 254 (57.7) 243 (55.0)
 neither nor 82 (18.6) 98 (22.2)
 bad/very bad 14 (3.2) 14 (3.2)
Mother or sister with breast cancer 70 (15.8) 63 (14.1)
Mammography
Ever mammogram 284 (63.7) 289 (64.2)
 If yes, reason for last mammogram:
  - screening 160 (56.7) 144 (50.2)
  - diagnostic 116 (41.1) 140 (48.8)
  - don’t know 6 (2.1) 3 (1.0)
Invitation to MSP received 214 (55.4) 166 (59.7)
Health insurance
Health insurance:
- Statutory health insurance** 340 (76.6) 319 (72.8)
- Statutory & complementary private health insurance 57 (12.8) 79 (18.0)
- Private health insurance*** 37 (8.3) 30 (6.8)
- Other 10 (2.3) 10 (2.3)

Note.

*Including women providing more than 1 main language.

**Covers the MSP.

***Coverage of the MSP depends on the insurance.

Primary analysis

Most women made no informed choice irrespective of time or group (see Table 3). In the control group the proportion of uninformed choices was constant at about 70%. The proportion of informed choices at T1 was similar between the groups (χ2 = 0.43, df = 1, p = .542). At T2 (χ2 = 57.20, df = 1, p < .001) and T3 (χ2 = 5.24, df = 1, p = .024) the proportion of informed choices in the DA group was significantly higher. Thus, women in the control group had at T2 (OR 0.26, 95% CI 0.18-0.37) and T3 (OR 0.66, 95% CI 0.46-0.94) lower odds, to make an informed choice than women in the DA group.

Table 3. Informed choice and its three dimensions, n (%).

Percentage of… Group T1 T2 T3
adequate knowledge DA 129 (28.6) 189 (66.8) 141 (51.3)
control 133 (29.8) 122 (31.4) 131 (40.2)
positive attitude DA 407 (90.0) 235 (83.0) 229 (84.2)
control 399 (88.7) 342 (87.5) 275 (85.1)
positive intention/completed screening DA 295 (87.3) 190 (81.9) 168 (65.4)
control 194 (87.5) 280 (90.0) 203 (67.4)
informed choice DA 87 (26.0) 142 (61.5) 99 (39.8)
control 94 (28.3) 89 (28.9) 88 (30.3)

Secondary analysis

Knowledge

The proportion of women with adequate knowledge was similar between the groups at T1, and being less than one-third, this proportion must be seen as low (Table 4). Table 5 shows the model fit indices of the measurement invariance models and of the predictor model. Since item 6 (Who is more likely to die of breast Cancer? Women participating in the MSP/ Women not participating in the MSP/ Both the same) showed negative loadings (women with a higher level of knowledge were less likely to answer the item correctly), it was excluded. Since we assume in knowledge items that a correct response correspond with a higher knowledge level, negative loading or loadings close to zero are not adequate to differentiate between knowledge levels.

Table 4. Decisional conflict, decision regret, and decision stage, M (SD).
Outcome Group T1 T2 T3
Knowledge DA 2.73 (1.41) 3.96 (1.33) 3.57 (1.16)
Control 2.79 (1.34) 2.92 (1.40) 3.21 (1.28)
Attitude DA 3.39 (2.91) 2.96 (3.41) 2.84 (3.51)
Control 3.33 (2.88) 3.20 (2.94) 3.39 (3.48)
Decisional conflict DA 1.49 (1.62) 0.52 (0.86) 0.62 (1.09)
Control 1.55 (1.59) 0.99 (1.37) 0.86 (1.21)
n (%) of Yes-responses
Understand information DA 294 (65.9) 272 (97.8) 246 (88.8)
Control 284 (63.7) 308 (80.4) 275 (84.1)
Risk-benefit ratio DA 292 (65.3) 257 (92.1) 245 (88.8)
Control 287 (64.3) 295 (77.0) 264 (81.0)
Encouragement DA 260 (58.6) 233 (83.8) 218 (79.6)
Control 259 (58.5) 277 (73.1) 235 (71.9)
Sure of myself DA 264 (58.7) 207 (74.2) 222 (80.7)
Control 261 (58.3) 274 (70.8) 250 (77.2)
Decision regret DA - - 12.18 (15.31)
Control - - 12.69 (15.92)
Decision stage DA 3.33 (1.05) 3.67 (.71) -
Control 3.36 (1.019) 3.58 (.811) -
n (%) of categories
Not thought about it DA 39 (8.5) 3 (1.1) -
Control 37 (8.2) 9 (2.3) -
Thinking about both options DA 82 (17.9) 30 (10.7) -
Control 70 (15.6) 54 (13.8) -
Close to making a decision DA 25 (5.5) 23 (8.2) -
Control 35 (7.8) 29 (7.4) -
Made a decision DA 311 (68.1) 224 (80.0) -
Control 308 (68.4) 300 (76.5) -
Table 5. Fit-information of 2-parameter-logistic item factor models.
Outcome Tested model Loglikelihood df AIC BIC ΔLoglikelihood Δdf p Pass?
Decisional conflict Configural invariant -4174.26 55 8458.53 8723.24 - - - -
Full invariant -4212.97 25 8475.94 8596.27 77.41 30 < .001 no
Partial invariant* -4193.85 27 8441.70 8571.66 39.17 28 .078 yes
DA as predictor -3611.88 18 7259.76 7346.40 - - - -
Knowledge Configural invariant** -6539.91 115 13309.83 13862.36 - - - -
Full invariant** -6630.46 65 13390.91 13703.21 181.09 50 < .001 no
Partial invariant** -6556.33 89 13290.66 13718.27 32.83 26 .167 yes
DA as predictor -6073.74 47 12241.48 12467.30 - - - -

Note. - Parameter not possible.

*In the DA group, the thresholds of 2 items at T2 were estimated freely.

** Convergence criterion .02.

Partially strong measurement invariance was accepted. The paths from group to the latent factor knowledge T2 (γ = 0.151, p = .010) and to the latent factor knowledge T3 (γ = 0.103, p = .034) were significant indicating that at T2 and T3, the DA group had a higher knowledge level than the control compared to T1.

Attitude

Both groups had, at all measurement points, a very positive attitude towards participation in the MSP. Strong measurement invariance was accepted (Table 6). The path (see Fig 2) from group to the latent factor attitude T2 was not significant (γ = −0.083, p = .170). At T2, the DA group and control group had a similar attitude compared to T1. The path from group to the latent factor attitude T3 was significant (γ = −0.229, p = .004). At T3, the DA group had a more negative attitude than the control compared to T1.

Table 6. Fit-information of confirmatory factor models.
Outcome Tested model χ2 df p RMSEA (90%CI) CFI ΔCFI TLI ΔTLI Pass?
Attitude Configural invariant 105.66 78 .020 .028 (.012-.041) .996 - .994 - yes
Weak invariant 118.62 93 .038 .025 (.006-.037) .996 .000 .995 + yes
Strong invariant 143.40 108 .013 .027 (.013-.038) .995 .001 .994 .001 yes
DA as predictor 176.31 62 < .001 .045 (.037-.053) .984 - .980 - -
Decision regret Configural invariant 65.47 10 < .001 .137 (.106-.169) .952 - .904 - yes
Weak invariant 74.27 14 < .001 .120 (.094-.148) .948 .004 .926 + yes
Strong invariant 80.03 18 < .001 .108 (.084-.132) .946 .002 .941 + yes
DA as predictor 61.795 9 < .001 .099 (.077-.124) .954 - .923 - -

Note. - Parameter not possible. + Improvement of parameter.

Fig 2. Predictor model of attitude.

Fig 2

Unstandardised parameters; a to d: constraints over time; intercepts constrained over time and not shown; residual covariances estimated freely and not shown; A: important/unimportant, B: a good thing/a bad thing, C: pleasant/unpleasant, D: beneficial/harmful; group: 0 = control, 1 = DA.

Intention and uptake

At T1 and T2, the majority of women responded that they would participate in the MSP within the next 3 months (see Table 7). At T3, in both groups, the majority had participated in the MSP (additionally, more than 5% had conducted an opportunistic mammography). At T1, a quarter of women was undecided. For intention at T1 (χ2 = 0.01, df = 1, p = 1.000) and uptake at T3 (χ2 = 0.27, df = 1, p = .653), there were no significant differences between the groups. At T2 (χ2 = 7.56, df = 1, p = .007), there was a significant difference: The proportion of those, who did not want to participate in the screening, was higher in the DA group (18.1%) than in the control group (10.0%). Women in the control group had higher odds to have a positive intention (OR 2.00, 95% CI 1.21-3.29) than women in the DA group.

Table 7. Intention and uptake for DA and control, n (%).
Group Intention T1 T2 Uptake T3
DA MSP 295 (65.3) 190 (68.3) MSP 168 (62.0)
no MSP* 43 (9.5) 44 (15.8) no screening 89 (32.8)
undecided 114 (25.2) 44 (15.8) opportunistic 14 (5.2)
Control MSP 294 (66.1) 280 (71.8) MSP 203 (61.9)
no MSP* 42 (9.4) 38 (9.7) no screening 98 (29.9)
undecided 109 (24.5) 72 (18.5) opportunistic 27 (8.2)

Note.

* Including women intending an opportunistic mammogram.

Decisional conflict

Decisional conflict was similar in the DA and control groups at T1 with average values around 1.5 on a scale of 0 to 4 (see Table 3). Partially strong measurement invariance was accepted (Table 5). The paths from group to the latent factor decisional conflict T2 (γ = 0.335, p < .001) and to the latent factor decisional conflict T3 were significant (γ = 0.149, p = .042). At T2 and T3, the DA group had lower decisional conflict than the control compared to T1.

Decision regret

At T3, most women experienced no regret (control: 40.8%; DA: 42.9%). Strong measurement invariance was accepted (Table 6). The path from group to T3 was not significant (γ = −0.060, p = .498). At T3, the DA group had a similar level of decision regret as the control group.

Decision stage

At T1 in both groups, 68% indicated they had already made a decision. This percentage increased to 76.5% in the control at T2 and to 80.0% in the DA group. For decision stage, there was neither a significant difference between groups at T1 (U = 101794.00, z = −.32, p = .755) nor at T2 (U = 52676.00, z = −1.23, p = .220).

Discussion

This was the first study that evaluated the impact of a newly developed online DA for the German MSP in a randomised controlled trial. As hypothesised, the DA resulted in a greater proportion of informed choices, a higher knowledge level, and less decisional conflict. Contrary to our hypothesis, decision regret was not reduced by the DA. For attitude and intention/uptake, we did not formulate specific hypotheses. Our results for these outcomes were mixed: depending on the measurement point, we either found a decrease or no effect of the DA on these measures.

In the control group the proportion of uninformed choices was constant at about 70%. This confirms the results of a previous German study, according to which very few decisions about the MSP are made informed [1]. Our results at both post intervention and follow-up show that our DA led to more informed decisions than the existing information materials, that were provided with the invitation, were able to achieve alone. Previous research on levels of informed choice after exposure to a DA on mammography screening shows mixed results. Mathieu et al. (2007) evaluating a DA for women aged 70 reported a greater proportion of women making an informed choice [10] while Mathieu et al. (2010) in their cross-sectional study on women aged 40 found no difference in the proportion of women making an informed choice [13].

Regarding our secondary outcomes, the DA showed different effects. The proportion of adequate knowledge at baseline with less than one-third must be seen as low. Contrastingly, Hersch et al. reported correct responses to knowledge items of over 70% at baseline in an Australian study [18]. In a cross-sectional survey in the Netherlands, Agt et al. assessed only conceptual knowledge and reported 95% of women to have sufficient knowledge [32]. These differences may in part be explained by differences between mammography screening information materials and programmes in the different countries. Another possible explanation is that the different knowledge measures account for these wide discrepancies. Existing knowledge measures differ in content, difficulty, number of items and response formats. Our DA group had a higher knowledge level than the control group at post-intervention and follow-up. This is in line with the Cochrane review according to which DAs in general lead to higher knowledge than usual care [8]. Regarding a DA for mammography screening, Mathieu et al. (2007) found a significant increase in knowledge [10]. Similarly, Mathieu et al. (2010) also reported a significant effect of their DA on knowledge [13].

It remains unclear which component of our DA or which combination of components is responsible for the knowledge increase. Crowd figures using circles or pictograms to indicate the number of affected and unaffected people have been shown to increase accuracy of risk perception [33, 34]. DAs always interact with other factors. Petrova et al. conducted an experiment on 3 presentation formats for statistical information about breast cancer (text, fact box, visual aid) and found no main effect of information format on number of correct comprehension questions [35]. Presenting numerical information accompanied by a visual aid improved knowledge compared to alternative formats only when perceived severity of the disease was not extremely high [35]. Previous beliefs about mammography screening as well as a high level of fear of breast cancer interfere with comprehension of evidence based information and thus hinder knowledge increase and informed choices [35].

Both groups had, at all measurement points, a very positive attitude towards participation in the MSP. Hersch et al. and Agt et al. similarly reported that most women had a positive attitude [18, 32]. Post-intervention, we found no differences between groups. In contrast, at follow-up, the DA group had a more negative attitude than the control group. Our DA may have made the women feel less positive about screening through increasing their knowledge about the low personal benefit of screening, but it remains questionable why this effect did not occur at post-intervention. Similarly, Mathieu et al. reported no difference in attitude in their DA group [13].

At baseline and post-intervention, the majority of participating women responded that they would participate in the MSP within the next 3 months. At T3, in both groups, the majority had participated in the MSP. Intention to participate was lower in the DA group post-intervention. The uptake rate was unaffected by our DA regardless of a lower post-intervention intention to participate. Regarding intention and uptake, the effect of DAs is not clear. One study reported no effect on intention following a DA [36] whereas others did find a lower intention following a DA [37] or in the DA group [13]. The situation is similar for uptake with research showing no effect on uptake [10] as well as lower uptake in the DA group [24]. According to the Cochrane review, DAs with an explicit values clarification exercise were more likely to lead to a value congruent choice [8] but there is no clear effect on intention per se. Depending on the type of screening or treatment offered, more information will have a positive, no, or a negative effect. This argument similarly applies to attitude.

The proportion of women at follow-up not having participated in the MSP was significantly higher than the intention to not participate in the MSP at post-intervention. Apart from a true change of mind, other explanations have to be taken into account. We assessed intention to participate within the next 3 months. Accordingly, some women will have had the intention to take part in the next three months but either received the invitation to the MSP so late that the appointment at our third measurement point had not yet passed orsome barrier hindered an intended attendanceand the appointment was postponed. Potentially, social desirability may have been a factor influencing intention responses, since in a context where early detection is seen as something good by society, an intention to not participate may be perceived as deviant and thus not reported. Retrospective uptake responses may not be biased as strongly as screening attendance either has or has not occurred.

The DA group had lower decisional conflict than the control at both post-intervention and follow-up. This is in congruence withthe Cochrane review reporting overall, a reduction in decisional conflict following a DA [8]. Contrastingly, two randomised controlled studies on mammography screening reported no effect of a DA on decisional conflict [10, 37] whereas one study found lower decisional conflict [18]. However, this may be different for various subgroups of women resulting in opposing effects. Many women have decided before contact with a DA [38]. This then may cause more decisional conflict if personal preference and scientific evidence do not match; i.e., a DA can increase decisional conflict [38]. DAs are also seen most positive by women who merely verify their previous decision with the DA [38]. At present it is uncertain whether high decisional conflict is a good or a bad thing [24], since high decisional conflict could be an indicator for an active deliberation process [39] as well as for insufficient decision support.

Most women experienced no decision regret and this was not affected by the DA. This is in line with other research. According to the Cochrane review, only 7 studies assessed decision regret and none reported statistically significant differences [8].

At baseline, the majority of women indicated they had already made a decision which is similar to a RCT by Hersch et al. [18]. Our study groups were similar in decision stage at post-intervention and follow-up. This may be explained by our DA only being relevant for women who have not yet formed a decision. Thus, decision stage in our study would not have been expected to alter since the majority had already made the decision leaving little room for decision process progression.

Strengths and limitations

Major strengths of this study were its design as a RCT and as one of the first the follow-up at 3 months. This allowed the evaluation of long-term effects of the DA (e.g., the persistence of increased knowledge) and also of screening uptake. Importantly, another strength of this research is that we developed and evaluated the DA in accordance with IPDAS criteria which are designed to ensure a minimum level of quality for DAs. Our DA was systematically developed and evidence based. The brochure that was in use during our study and was thus the usual care, had been in use since September 2010 and openly discussed benefits and harms of the MSP [40]. Only some but not all statistics were presented in absolute numbers [41]. Gummersbach and Abholz indicate that a graphical presentation of the numbers may have been useful and that the probable lack of effect on overall mortality should be mentioned [41]. All 3 points were all fulfilled by our DA but not by the brochure.

Most respondents received the questionnaire at about the time of receipt of the official invitation materials. This ensured a high relevance of the decision. Some women had not yet received the invitation to the MSP and the associated brochure. Thus, the timing of the decision and the availability of usual care information materials was heterogeneous. Since the information brochure is also available online, it remains speculative how whether the women not yet having received their invitation had access to it.

To avoid the potential disruptive influence of previous screening experiences or habits, only women aged 50 who are invited for the first time to the MSP were included in the study. Our study achieved acceptable response and attrition rates but it has to be noted that women participating in our study will likely have been different from those not participating: in particular, it can be assumed that they may have been more interested in mammography screening as a topic and thus may probably have informed themselves about this topic more than the general population.

Since the sample we drew from the population registries was representative except for women with Turkish migration background, our results cannot be applied to this group. We had almost no women in our sample whose main language did not include German. Women whose mother tongue was not German may have been less likely to participate in our study.

In calculating informed choice, intention to participate in the MSP within the next three months was used as one of 3 dimensions. Consequently, women whose appointment was at a later date were classified as non-intenders and an artificial mismatch (or match) between attitude and intention would be created due to the time frame of the intention question. This time limitation may explain some of the inconsistencies between attitude and intention that have been categorised as uninformed decision [1]. Nevertheless, a limitation of the period was important for the validity of the question [25]. Assessed uptake at T3, since it was self-reported, will have deviated to some degree from actual uptake.

Using latent structural equation models for our analyses had several advantages. (1) We could explicitly account for measurement error. (2) Testing measurement invariance across time and groups enabled us to establish a crucial prerequisite for testing our intervention effect. Measurement invariance entails that the same construct is measured regardless of time or group. Thus, the item specific information is unaffected by these contextual factors [29]. (3) In order to deal with missing values, we could apply full information maximum likelihood estimation allowing us to include individuals with missing values in the analysis.

The knowledge index contained only some facts about the MSP. These have been carefully selected, however, by no means cover the entire spectrum of possibly decision relevant facts. Additionally, we had to exclude one item, which did not differentiate women with a high or low knowledge level. It remains speculative, what caused this problem in this item.

Practice implications and future research

An important aspect for future research is the influence of affect on information processing, especially since decisions about cancer screening involve counterintuitive evidence [35] like overdiagnoses. When cancer is perceived as a severe condition, understanding of screening statistics can be reduced [35]. Therefore, when the consequences of a decision are affect-rich (e.g., when women experience fear of breast cancer) heuristic processes are engaged, which neglect this probabilistic information [42]. Hence, for women in our study with strong previous beliefs, the effect of our DA may have been limited. Previous research showed that visual aids have no benefits for women regarding breast cancer as highly severe condition [35]. These women might have rather relied on established beliefs being less motivated to engage with the visual aids than women experiencing less severe affect regarding breast cancer [35]. These moderating and mediating factors will be interesting to assess in future research.

Since almost all trials on DAs fail to assess potential long-term effects on health or quality of life [24]—and our study is no exception to this—it would be interesting not only to include these measures but also to see how the different outcome groups of screening (false-positives, negatives, true positives) affect these measures. We assessed screening outcomes, but in this time span, to few events for any calculations occurred.

It can be hypothesised that the effect of the DA will be larger in women with higher health literacy. It remains questionable in how far a one size fits all approach will ever be feasible for a DA on the MSP. Not only should a DA be available in different languages and for different levels of health literacy but also for women with differing existing knowledge levels and finally with differing motivations to engage with this decision. A tailored approach therefore, may be indicated in future DA developments. Alternatively, at least regarding health literacy levels, a universal precautions approach may be beneficial although it may be difficult to keep the DA to an acceptable length. A possibility may be to further develop the DA to include more explanatory information sections that can be accessed if desired (e.g., as mouse hover boxes).

After the end of our study, the information brochure of the MSP has been updated [43] and, additionally, was further developed into a DA and a concept for a future online version [12]. The new materials have not yet been evaluated in an RCT and a comparison with our DA would be interesting.

Conclusion

This is the first study to assess the impact of an online DA for the German MSP in a RCT. The DA developed in this project is a valuable tool to support decision making and has been implemented through making it publicly available on the website of the BARMER since December 2015. Since the DA has proven to be effective in supporting women to make an informed choice, the results are relevant to practice and the DA can be widely used to support women in decision making. The DA also significantly increased women’s knowledge about the screening and decreased decisional conflict.

Supporting information

S1 File. Original application to the ethics committee in German.

(PDF)

S2 File. English translation of the application to the ethics committee.

(PDF)

S3 File. CONSORT checklist.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Emily Finne and Dr. Johanna Ute Frisch for their valuable comments to the manuscript. We acknowledge the financial support of the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the Open Access Publication Fund of Bielefeld University for the article processing charge.

Data Availability

Data are available upon request due to ethical restrictions. Our data contain information that may allow identification of study participants and our consent form did not cover publication of the raw data. For these data protection reasons, data can only be made available to interested researchers without socio-demographic variables or in aggregated form. Interested researchers may submit requests to the Data Protection Officer of Bielefeld University, Ms. Anja Schmid. Contact: Bielefeld University, Postfach 10 01 31, D-33501 Bielefeld, Germany. E-mail: datenschutzbeauftragte@uni-bielefeld.de.

Funding Statement

This study was funded by BARMER, a provider of statutory health insurance and a federal, self-governing corporation under public law (https://www.barmer.de/ueberuns/barmer/unternehmen-26960). We acknowledge the financial support of the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the Open Access Publication Fund of Bielefeld University for the article processing charge. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 File. Original application to the ethics committee in German.

(PDF)

S2 File. English translation of the application to the ethics committee.

(PDF)

S3 File. CONSORT checklist.

(PDF)

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon request due to ethical restrictions. Our data contain information that may allow identification of study participants and our consent form did not cover publication of the raw data. For these data protection reasons, data can only be made available to interested researchers without socio-demographic variables or in aggregated form. Interested researchers may submit requests to the Data Protection Officer of Bielefeld University, Ms. Anja Schmid. Contact: Bielefeld University, Postfach 10 01 31, D-33501 Bielefeld, Germany. E-mail: datenschutzbeauftragte@uni-bielefeld.de.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES