Abstract
The goal of this research was to expand theoretical models of adolescent depression to determine whether individual differences in cognitive processing—specifically attentional control deficits—help to explain increased risk for depression during adolescence. We also examined whether this pathway was stronger in girls than in boys. A longitudinal design was used to examine whether poor attentional control in everyday life (i.e., difficulties shifting between ideas, tasks, and activities) contributes to depression over time by fostering higher levels of stress reactivity. Youth (298 boys, 338 girls) completed questionnaires assessing stress reactivity (6th and 7th grades) and depressive symptoms (6th, 7th and, 8th grades); teachers completed the shifting subscale of the Behavior Rating Scale of Executive Function (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000a) to assess attentional control (6th and 7th grades). Structural equation modeling analyses provided support for the predicted pathway in girls but not boys, yielding a significant indirect effect from 6th grade shifting deficits to 8th grade depressive symptoms via 7th grade stress reactivity. These results suggest that attentional control deficits in early adolescence heighten girls’ sensitivity to stress and consequent depressive symptoms, providing a critical direction for efforts to decrease adolescent girls’ risk for depression.
Keywords: attentional control, stress reactivity, depression, gender
Adolescence is a stage of heightened risk for depression, particularly in girls (Hankin & Abramson, 2001; Hyde, Mezulis, & Abramson, 2008; Rudolph, 2009). Why adolescents in general, and girls in particular, show a dramatic rise in depression is a focus of much attention and concern, with significant implications for efforts to identify, prevent, and treat depression before it exerts debilitating effects on youths’ development and sets the stage for lifelong impairment. Prior research attempting to solve this puzzle has considered a range of risk factors, many of which are more common or salient in girls, including heightened biological (Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long, & Griggs, 2009) and neural (Guyer, McClure-Tone, Shiffrin, Pine, & Nelson, 2009) sensitivity, puberty-related risks (Rudolph, 2014), emotional vulnerability (Charbonneau, Mezulis, & Hyde, 2009; for a review, see Hyde et al., 2008), and exposure and reactivity to social stress (Hankin, Mermelstein, & Roesch, 2007; Rudolph, 2002, 2009; Shih, Eberhart, Hammen, & Brennan, 2006). In this research, we sought to build on existing theoretical models and a growing body of empirical data to determine the role of cognitive processing, specifically poor attentional control, as a domain of risk. In particular, we examined whether attentional control deficits, as reflected in everyday manifestations of cognitive inflexibility, contribute to elevated risk for depression during early adolescence. Moreover, we attempted to identify one potential pathway—namely heightened reactivity to interpersonal stress—through which attentional control deficits prospectively contribute to depressive symptoms and we examined whether this pathway is more robust in girls.
Conceptualizing Attentional Control and its Link to Depression
Attentional control is one dimension of executive function, a set of cognitive processes that regulate thoughts, feelings, and actions (Barkley, 1997; Miyake et al., 2000). In particular, attentional control involves the ability to focus and shift attention as needed, flexibly attend to particular tasks or goals, and shift between mental sets (i.e., disengage from irrelevant task sets; engage in relevant task sets; Banich, 2009; Miyake, et al., 2000; Muris, van der Pennen, Sigmond, & Mayer, 2008; Whitmer & Gotlib, 2012). Within the context of performance-based tasks, attentional control involves switching one’s attentional set to respond to new information (e.g., changes in instructions or task demands; Banich, 2009). Within the context of everyday life, attentional control may play a role in youths’ broad capacity to control thoughts, emotions, and behaviors (Rueda, Purificacion, & Rothbart, 2010) by helping maintain attention to, or switch attention between, tasks and activities (Miyake et al., 2000; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000) in order to self-regulate and adapt to current contextual demands. Thus, deficits in attentional control may undermine youths’ ability to effectively regulate cognition and emotion, thereby increasing risk for depression (Wilkins0n & Goodyear, 2006). Indeed, a growing body of research using diverse conceptualizations and assessment approaches suggests that individuals with attentional control deficits manifest higher levels of depression and associated internalizing symptoms, although research with adolescents is in its early stages.
One relevant line of investigation focuses on laboratory tasks of attentional control. This research reveals that depressed youth demonstrate deficits in their ability to switch attention relative to nondepressed youth (Gunther, Konrad, De Brito, Herpertz-Dahlmann, & Vloet, 2011; Wilkinson & Goodyer, 2006), particularly under emotionally valenced conditions such as switching attentional set away from a sad focus (Kyte, Goodyer, & Sahakian, 2005). A recent meta-analysis confirms that adolescents with major depressive disorder (MDD) show significantly worse performance on shifting tasks than those without MDD (Wagner, Muller, Helmreich, Huss, & Tadic, 2015). Research with depressed adults reveals similar difficulties in set-shifting and associated attentional biases (for a review, see Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014), with some research also suggesting specificity to attentional processes in the context of negative stimuli (Sanchez, Vazquez, Marker, LeMoult, & Joormann, 2013). Moreover, a few training studies suggest that improving cognitive control (Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2008; Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer, & Thase, 2007), including attentional switching abilities, reduces depressive symptoms in adults.
A second relevant line of investigation focuses on informant reports of attentional control as well as everyday executive function and related dimensions of temperament such as effortful control, typically measured as a composite of attentional control and inhibitory control (i.e., the ability to intentionally direct internal resources towards goals or to inhibit inappropriate behaviors; Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Several studies have revealed that self-reports of attentional control deficits (e.g., focusing, shifting; Melendez, Bechor, Rey, Pettit, & Silverman, 2016; Muris, Mayer, van Lint, & Hofman, 2008; Verstraeten, Bijttebier, Vasey, & Raes, 2011) are concurrently associated with depressive symptoms. Research using broader self-, parent-, or teacher-report measures of executive function and effortful control that include items reflecting attentional control (e.g., shifting, distractibility, focus) also establishes concurrent (Agoston & Rudolph, 2015; Hocking et al., 2011; Lengua, 2002; Verstraeten, Vasey, Raes, & Bijttebier, 2009) and prospective (Lengua, 2006; Letkiewicz et al., 2014; Snyder & Hankin, 2016) associations with depressive and internalizing symptoms.
Despite this preliminary evidence for linkages between attentional control deficits and depression, few longitudinal studies have examined whether poor attentional control predicts subsequent depression across early adolescence. Moreover, empirical studies of attentional control have yet to identify specific processes through which cognitive inflexibility conveys risk for depression (for a relevant study examining effortful control, see Snyder & Hankin, 2016). Finally, minimal attention has been paid to whether there are gender differences in the contribution of attentional control deficits to depression. The present study used a prospective longitudinal design to (a) examine the role of stress reactivity as one explanatory mechanism linking attentional control (in the form of shifting attention) deficits to depression across adolescence: and (b) examine gender differences in this process.
Attentional Control Deficits as a Predictor of Heightened Stress Reactivity
Cognitive inflexibility or difficulty shifting attention may foster rigid thought patterns and perseveration on ineffective responses (Miyake, 2000), particularly within demanding or stressful situations (De Lissnyder et al., 2012). Being able to shift attention away from stressors and accompanying negative emotions and toward coping efforts is vital to responding in a purposeful and effective manner (Compas, Connor-Smith, & Jaser, 2004). Thus, youth who have difficulty refocusing their attention may fail to engage in constructive efforts to resolve stressors (e.g., problem solving and emotion regulation), instead showing heightened involuntary stress reactivity marked by cognitive perseveration (e.g., dwelling on unpleasant thoughts and events) along with more intense and prolonged emotional and physiological arousal (Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014). Indeed, several theoretical conceptualizations of coping and emotion regulation consider the role of attentional control processes in determining how individuals respond to stress and negative emotions.
Whitmer and Gotlib’s (2012) attentional scope model of depressive rumination proposes that dysfunctional attentional control processes (i.e., a narrowing in attentional focus that results in a constricted array of thoughts, perceptions, and actions) contribute to individual differences in the tendency to engage in negative and repetitive thinking. In support of this model, they review evidence from several lines of research with adults demonstrating that poorer intentional suppression of irrelevant information, difficulty disengaging from no-longer relevant negative information, and impaired ability to shift attention are associated with depressive rumination, as reflected in repetitive brooding about negative personal concerns and/or about the implications, causes, and meanings of negative emotions (Altamirano, Miyake, & Whitmer, 2010; Davis & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; cf. Wilkinson & Goodyer, 2006, in adolescents).
Some theory and research focus more specifically on the role of attentional control over negative emotionally valenced stimuli and within stressful contexts (for a review, see Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014). For example, Joormann and Vanderlind (2014) propose that deficits in cognitive control influence attention to emotion-eliciting aspects of events, which then undermine effective emotion regulation and responses to stress (see also Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan, & De Raedt, 2011). Consistent with this idea, difficulty shifting attention away from negative emotional (but not non-emotional) stimuli predicts rumination (Demeyer, De Lissnyder, Koster, & De Raedt, 2012), particularly in response to stress (De Lissnyder et al., 2012), demonstrating the importance of considering the role of attentional control in stress reactivity. Moreover, difficulties disengaging attention from negative stimuli are associated with heightened emotional reactivity in response to negative mood and stress induction in adults (Compton, Heller, Banich, Palmieri, & Miller, 2000; Sanchez et al., 2013), whereas stronger switching abilities are associated with more effective emotion regulation (i.e., downregulation of negative affect) in response to a negative mood induction (Malooly, Genet, & Siemer, 2013). In two recent studies of youth, difficulty inhibiting negative emotional information on a switching task (Hilt, Leitzke, & Pollak, 2014) and difficulty disengaging attention from emotional stimuli (Hilt, Leitzke, & Pollak, in press) were associated with trait depressive rumination. Providing more direct support for the idea that strong attentional control may diminish stress reactivity, cognitive bias training studies reveal that teaching individuals to disengage their attention from negative stimuli and to increase cognitive control reduces rumination (Siegle et al., 2014; Siegle et al., 2007) and reactivity to stressful events (for a review, see Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014).
More broadly, theoretical accounts of temperament, executive function, and coping suggest that deficits in attentional control processes can impede adaptive emotion regulation, responses to stress, and more general social competence (Compas et al., 2004). For instance, self-reports and behavioral indices of stronger attentional and effortful control (including attentional focus) are positively associated with adaptive emotion regulation (Simonds, Kieras, Rueda, & Rothbart, 2007) and inversely associated with rumination in youth (Hilt, Armstrong, & Essex, 2012; Verstraeten et al., 2011) and young adults (White & Turner, 2014). Moreover, poor attentional control (e.g., selective attention, shifting) is linked to heightened stress reactivity and maladaptive coping responses (Hocking et al., 2011; for a review, see Compas et al., 2004). Finally, teacher and parent reports of stronger attentional control (e.g., focus; Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, & Reiser, 2000; Rudasill & Konold, 2008) and effortful control (including attentional focus, shifting; Spinrad et al., 2006) are associated with higher levels of social competence, which may facilitate responding to stressful peer experiences.
In sum, several theoretical perspectives and some empirical evidence suggest that attentional control deficits cause individuals to dwell on negative thoughts or emotions, particularly under conditions of stress, and may interfere with youths’ ability to effectively navigate their social contexts. However, much of the prior evidence involves adults and uses concurrent research designs, making the direction of effect between attentional control difficulties and stress reactivity unclear. We therefore examined whether shifting deficits in the context of everyday life (e.g., impaired ability to disengage from certain topics or situations; difficulty transitioning between ideas or activities) would predict heightened stress reactivity over time in adolescents, as reflected in higher levels of emotional and physiological arousal, cognitive perseveration, and sensitivity to challenges and stressors.
Stress Reactivity and Depression
In turn, heightened stress reactivity may serve as a risk factor for subsequent depression. Involuntary, dysregulated stress responses intensify youths’ negative emotions and undermine their ability to engage in adaptive coping responses that successfully address stressors (Agoston & Rudolph, 2011; Compas, Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Thomsen, & Wadsworth, 2001; Flynn & Rudolph, 2007; 2010; Troop-Gordon, Rudolph, Sugimura, & Little, 2015). Heightened negative emotions and failure to resolve stressors may lead to a sense of helplessness, low self-efficacy, and consequent depression. Maladaptive stress responses also may cause youth to generate new stressors, which then foster depression (Flynn & Rudolph, 2011). Adolescence may be a period during which maladaptive stress responses play an especially strong role in depression given that youth start to face more stress, with girls exposed to particularly high levels of interpersonal stressors (Rudolph, 2009). Indeed, several studies support the idea that heightened stress reactivity (in adults; Krueger, Caspi, Moffitt, Silva, & McGee, 1996; Lara, Leader, & Klein 1997; Miller, Greif, & Smith, 2003) as well as involuntary cognitive (e.g., rumination; Abela & Hankin, 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema, Stice, Wade, & Bohon, 2007; Rood, Roelofs, Bogels, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schouten, 2009), emotional (Charbonneau et al., 2009), and physiological (e.g., cortisol reactivity; for a review, see Guerry & Hastings, 2011) engagement with stressors (Troop-Gordon et al., 2015) predict concurrent and subsequent depression in adolescence.
Gender Differences
A secondary goal of this study was to examine gender differences in the hypothesized pathway from attentional control deficits to depressive symptoms. During adolescence, girls are faced with particularly high levels of social stress (Rudolph, 2009) and are more likely to engage in perseverative thinking (Jose & Brown, 2008). This heightened stress exposure may place demands on girls’ cognitive resources and amplify the link between attentional control and stress reactivity in girls relative to boys. In turn, perseverative thinking (Hilt, McLaughlin, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Stange, Hamilton, Abramson, & Alloy, 2014) and maladaptive responses to stress (Agoston & Rudolph, 2011) more strongly predict depression in adolescent girls than boys. Consistent with this idea, a recent study (Agoston & Rudolph, 2015) revealed that exposure to peer stress predicted subsequent depression in adolescent girls but not boys with executive function deficits (including poor shifting), suggesting the need to consider potential gender differences in the risk posed by cognitive control deficits. Thus, we hypothesized that the path from attentional control deficits to depression via stress reactivity would be stronger in girls than in boys.
Alternative Directions of Effect
Although our primary focus was on the extent to which heightened stress reactivity accounts for the contribution of attentional control deficits to subsequent depressive symptoms, it also is reasonable to expect transactional processes. For example, engaging in more cognitive perseveration and involuntary reactions to stress can overload cognitive resources and thus interfere with attentional control processes (Watkins, 2008); in one study, inducing state rumination interfered with depressed adults’ ability to switch between two tasks (Whitmer & Gotlib, 2013). Likewise, being in a negative mood can increase rumination (Moberly & Watkins, 2008), suggesting a reciprocal relation between stress reactivity and depression. Thus, reciprocal influences among poor attentional control, heightened stress reactivity, and depressive symptoms may create a self-perpetuating cycle of impairment. Using a prospective longitudinal design allowed us to examine the direction of influence among these processes.
Study Overview
The overall goal of this study was to examine whether heightened stress reactivity serves as one pathway through which attentional control deficits contribute to subsequent depression across early adolescence. We focused on attentional control as reflected in behavioral manifestations of shifting in everyday life for several reasons. First, the dimension of shifting best matches with theoretical models focused on attentional control processes, stress reactivity, and depression (e.g., Whitmer & Gotlib’s attentional scope model of depressive rumination; Joormann & Vanderlind’s model of attentional control, rumination, and depressive symptoms). Second, because our measures of stress reactivity assessed cognitive, emotional, and physiological over-engagement with stressors (e.g., rumination, emotional/physiological arousal), cognitive inflexibility (i.e., poor shifting) seems most relevant as a risk factor for heightened reactivity. Third, we focused on manifestation of shifting in everyday life because it reflects the accumulation of cognitive deficits across time and situations. Studying attentional processes within laboratory settings is a critical part of identifying their role as a risk factor for psychopathology. However, performance-based tasks typically tap the ability to exert attentional control under optimal conditions (e.g., low emotions and stress; dampened investment in task outcomes), which can be viewed as attentional control capacity. These abilities may differ from typical or daily manifestations of attentional control, when cognitive resources may be taxed, motivation is high, and emotions often are intensified (Denckla, 2002; Muris et al., 2008), which can be viewed as attentional control allocation. To understand the role of daily behavioral manifestations of attentional control deficits, teachers provided information about general difficulties in the ability to shift attention as needed, engage in flexible problem solving, and cope effectively with new task demands and situations. Assessing these deficits in daily contexts likely taps into youths’ allocation of attentional control.
A three-wave prospective longitudinal design was used to test the proposed pathway following youth from 6th through 8th grade, a critical developmental transition period during which rates of depression begin to rise, particularly in girls (Hankin & Abramson, 2001). This design allowed us to examine the contributions of attentional control and stress reactivity to depressive symptoms over time, to identify bidirectional effects, and to adjust for prior and concurrent levels of depressive symptoms when examining the link between attentional control and stress reactivity. Moreover, we examined whether there were gender differences in the pathways of interest.
Method
Participants and Procedures
Participants were 636 youth (298 boys, 338 girls; M age at recruitment = 7.97 years, SD = .37; 66.7% White, 21.7 % African American, 11.6 % other; 34.7 % received a subsidized school lunch) from several Midwestern towns. In 2nd grade, parents provided written consent, and youth provided oral assent. Of the 725 eligible children, 576 (80%) received parental consent to participate. In 2nd grade, participants and nonparticipants did not significantly differ in gender, χ2(1) = .15, ns; age, t(723) = .63, ns; ethnicity (white vs. minority), χ2(1) = .59, ns; or school lunch status (full pay vs. subsidized), χ2(1) = .35, ns. In the 3rd grade, an additional 60 classmates of the participating children were recruited, yielding a total of 636 participants. Given our focus on the development of depressive symptoms during adolescence, the present analyses used data from the 6th, 7th, and 8th grades.
Of the original 636 participants, 548 (86%) had data on shifting deficits in 6th grade, 474 (75%) had data on stress reactivity in 7th grade, 470 (74%) had data on involuntary engagement in 7th grade, and 474 (75%) had data on depressive symptoms in 8th grade. Youth with 6th grade self-report or teacher-report data did not differ from those without data in gender, χ2(1) = .02, ns; age, t(634) = 1.68, ns; ethnicity (white vs. minority), χ2(1) = 1.37, ns; school lunch status (full pay vs. subsidized), χ2(1) = .35, ns; or 2nd grade depressive symptoms, t(574) = .52, ns. All of the participating 636 children were included in the central analyses (see Results).
All study procedures were approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board. In the winter of each year, questionnaires were administered during two classroom sessions to groups of 15–20 students. Teachers returned their surveys to a locked box at their school, in person, or by mail. Youth received a small gift and teachers received a monetary reimbursement for participation; each participating middle school received a school-wide honorarium.
Measures
Table 1 presents descriptive data and reliability of the measures for girls and boys. All of the measures showed strong internal consistency across waves.
Table 1.
Measure | α | Girls | Boys | d | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | Range | M (SD) | Range | ||||||||||||
1. 6th Grade Shifting Deficits | .90 | 1.20 (.36) | 1.00 – 3.00 | 1.26 (.38)a | 1.00 – 2.88 | 0.16 | --- | .38** | .16* | .12 | .19** | .07 | .14* | .09 | .02 |
2. 7th Grade Shifting Deficits | .89 | 1.16 (.29) | 1.00 – 2.75 | 1.25 (.38)b | 1.00 – 3.00 | 0.27 | .36** | --- | .22** | .14* | .12 | .04 | .12 | .13* | .08 |
3. 6th Grade MPQ Stress Reactivity | .84 | 0.36 (.29) | 0.00 – 1.00 | 0.30 (.27)b | 0.00 – 1.00 | 0.21 | .17** | .25** | --- | .60** | .61** | .38** | .56** | .45** | .39** |
4. 7th Grade MPQ Stress Reactivity | .83 | 0.35 (.27) | 0.00 – 1.00 | 0.27 (.26)b | 0.00 – 1.00 | 0.30 | .25** | .28** | .63** | --- | .47** | .62** | .46** | .62** | .51** |
5. 6th Grade Involuntary Engagement | .86 | 1.92 (.67) | 1.00 – 3.80 | 1.78 (.57)b | 1.00 – 4.00 | 0.23 | .13* | .19** | .69** | .54** | --- | .44** | .52** | .39** | .39** |
6. 7th Grade Involuntary Engagement | .86 | 1.91 (.62) | 1.00 – 3.80 | 1.66 (.54)b | 1.00 – 3.40 | 0.43 | .23** | .16** | .53** | .67** | .65** | --- | .35** | .44** | .38** |
7. 6th Grade Depressive Symptoms | .90 | 1.49 (.55) | 1.00 – 4.69 | 1.40 (.44)a | 1.00 – 3.69 | 0.18 | .16** | .28** | .70** | .56** | .61** | .49** | --- | .42** | .38** |
8. 7th Grade Depressive Symptoms | .89 | 1.45 (.53) | 1.00 – 3.92 | 1.32 (.33)b | 1.00 – 2.77 | 0.29 | .17** | .22** | .54** | .65** | .48** | .54** | .57** | --- | .53** |
9. 8th Grade Depressive Symptoms | .92 | 1.55 (.61) | 1.00 – 3.77 | 1.30 (.31)b | 1.00 – 2.62 | 0.50 | .23** | .25** | .44** | .54** | .43** | .49** | .50** | .58** | --- |
Note: Intercorrelations below the diagonal are in girls; intercorrelations above the diagonal are in boys.
MPQ = Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire Simplified-Wording Form.
p < .05.
p < .01.
Mean difference at p < .05.
Mean difference at p < .01.
Attentional control
To assess attentional control in the 6th and 7th grades, teachers completed the shifting subscale of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF; Gioia et al., 2000a). This measure assesses everyday behavioral manifestations of shifting deficits (8 items; e.g., “Thinks too much about the same topic.” “Tries the same approach to a problem over and over, even when it does not work.” “Has trouble getting used to new situations [classes, groups, friends].” “Resists or has trouble accepting a different way to solve a problem with schoolwork, friends, chores, etc.”). Each item was rated on a 3-point scale (1 = Never a Problem to 3 = Often a Problem). Scores were calculated as the mean of the items, with higher scores reflecting more shifting deficits. The BRIEF has strong internal consistency (Gioia & Isquith, 2004) and inter-rater and test-retest reliability (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000b). It also shows strong ecological validity (Denckla, 2002) and construct validity, including high convergent and divergent validity for the full measure (Gioia et al., 2000a; Gioia et al., 2000b) and the subscale scores (e.g., Joyner, Silver, & Stavinoha, 2009; Semrud-Clikeman, Walkowiak, Wilkinson, & Butcher, 2010). Factor analyses (e.g., Gioia & Isquith, 2004) support the subscales for both community and clinical samples (Gioia et al., 2000a; Gioia & Isquith, 2004; Reddy, Hale, & Brodzinsky, 2011; Slick, Lautzenhiser, Sherman, & Eryl, 2006), as well as across both parent and teacher reports (Gioia, Isquith, Retzlaff, & Espy, 2002). The measure has been validated in preschool-age children to adults (e.g., Christ, Kanne, & Reiersen, 2010).
Stress reactivity
To assess stress reactivity in the 6th and 7th grades, youth completed two measures. First, youth completed the stress reactivity subscale of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire—Simplified-Wording Form (MPQ-SF; Patrick, Curtin, & Tellegen, 2002; Patrick, Kramer, Tellegen, Verona, & Kaemmer, 2013). This 12-item scale assesses youths’ general tendency to be reactive to everyday stressors (e.g., “I am too sensitive for my own good.” “Little hassles sometimes irritate me too much.”). Youth checked a box indicating whether each item was True (1) or False (0) for them. Scores were computed as the mean of the items. The MPQ-SF items converge strongly with the original MPQ items and replicate the original factor structure; further, the MPQ-SF stress reactivity subscale demonstrates adequate internal consistency and both convergent and discriminant validity (Patrick et al., 2013).
Second, youth completed the involuntary engagement subscale of a revised version (Rudolph, Abaied, Flynn, Sugimura, & Agoston, 2011) of the Responses to Stress Questionnaire (Connor-Smith, Compas, Wadsworth. Thomsen, & Saltzman, 2000), modified to assess responses to peer stress (i.e., when other kids are mean). This subscale assesses youths’ tendency to show automatic responses involving over-engagement with stressors, such as rumination (e.g., “I keep remembering what happened or can’t stop thinking about what might happen.”), emotional arousal (“I get upset by things that don’t usually bother me.”), and physiological arousal (e.g., “I feel sick to my stomach or get headaches.”). Youth checked a box indicating on a 4-point scale (Not at All to Very Much) how often they engaged in each response. Scores were computed as the mean of the items, with higher scores reflecting more involuntary engagement. Validity of the original (Connor-Smith et al., 2000) and revised (Rudolph et al., 2011; Troop-Gordon et al., 2015) versions of the measure has been well-established.
Depressive symptoms
Youth completed the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (Angold, Costello, Messer, & Pickles, 1995). This measure includes 13 items describing depressive symptoms (e.g., “I felt unhappy or miserable.”). Youth checked a box indicating on a 4-point scale (1 = Not at All to 4 = Very Much) how often they had experienced each symptom in the past two weeks (Lau & Eley, 2008). Scores were computed as the mean of the items, with higher scores reflecting more depressive symptoms. Reliability and validity have been documented (Angold et al., 1995), and this measure differentiates depression from other psychiatric diagnoses (Thapar & McGuffin, 1998). This measure has good convergent validity with a widely used diagnostic depression measure, and discriminates clinically depressed children from non-depressed children, and clinically referred psychiatric children from pediatric patients (Angold et al., 1995).
Overview of Analytic Approach
First, we conducted descriptive analyses to examine gender differences in the variables and intercorrelations among variables. Second, we conducted structural equation modeling using Amos Version 17.0 (Arbuckle, 2008) to examine whether 6th grade shifting deficits indirectly predicted 8th grade depressive symptoms through 7th grade stress reactivity as well as to test whether this path was stronger in girls than in boys. All models were tested using full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML; Enders & Bandalos, 2001), allowing for parameter estimation based on all available data from the 636 children. Shifting deficits were represented by manifest variables in 6th and 7th grade; stress reactivity was represented by latent variables in 6th and 7th grade with MPQ stress reactivity and involuntary engagement as indicators; depressive symptoms were represented by manifest variables in 6th, 7th, and 8th grades (measures of shifting and MPQ stress reactivity were not administered in 8th grade). To test the hypothesized indirect effect, Model 1 included lagged paths between 6th grade shifting deficits and 7th grade stress reactivity, and between 6th and 7th grade stress reactivity and 7th and 8th grade depressive symptoms, respectively. To provide a conservative estimate of the lagged effects, the model also included stability paths for each variable and covariances between constructs within each wave. To test the alternative directions of effect, we compared the original model with a model (Model 2) that included the reciprocal paths (6th grade stress reactivity predicting 7th grade shifting deficits, and 6th grade depressive symptoms predicting 7th grade stress reactivity). Multi-group comparison analyses were conducted to test for gender differences in the paths. Several model fit indices were examined, including Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Incremental Fit Index (IFI), and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Good model fit is indicated by CFI and IFI values ≥ .97, and RMSEA values ≤ .05 (Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 1998; Schermelleh-Engel, Moosbrugger, & Muller, 2003).
Results
Descriptive Analyses
A series of t-tests revealed several significant gender differences (see Table 1). Compared to girls, boys showed higher levels of shifting deficits in 6th and 7th grade. Compared to boys, girls showed higher levels of MPQ-SF stress reactivity and involuntary engagement in 6th and 7th grades as well as higher levels of depressive symptoms across all grades. In girls, shifting deficits were significantly associated with MPQ-SF stress reactivity and involuntary engagement as well as with depressive symptoms within and across waves; MPQ-SF stress reactivity and involuntary engagement also were significantly associated with depressive symptoms within and across waves. In boys, the pattern of intercorrelations was less consistent, with shifting primarily associated with MPQ stress reactivity, involuntary engagement, and depressive symptoms concurrently but not over time.
Examination of Mediation
A multi-group comparison analysis comparing the fully constrained Model 1 (all seven directional paths were constrained to be equal across gender) with a fully unconstrained model (all seven directional paths were allowed to vary across gender) revealed that the fully unconstrained model provided a significantly better fit than the fully constrained model, Δχ2(7) = 22.17, p = .002. Moreover, the unconstrained model showed a good fit to the data, χ2(38) = 91.06, CFI = .97, IFI = .97, RMSEA = .05. To verify that the focal mediation pathway differed by gender, the fully constrained model also was compared to (a) a model in which the path from 6th grade shifting deficits to 7th grade stress reactivity was allowed to vary by gender; and (b) a model in which the path from 7th grade stress reactivity to 8th grade depressive symptoms was allowed to vary by gender. Both models provided a significantly better fit, Δχ2(1) = 5.47, p = .02 and Δχ2(1) = 10.59, p = .001, than the fully constrained model, supporting gender differences in each of these paths.
Adding the reciprocal paths to the fully unconstrained model (Model 2) significantly improved the model fit, Δχ2(4) = 18.21, p = .001. Further, a multi-group comparison analysis comparing the fully constrained Model 2 (all nine directional paths were constrained to be equal across gender) with a fully unconstrained model (all nine directional paths were allowed to vary across gender) revealed that the fully unconstrained model provided a significantly better fit than the fully constrained model, Δχ2(9) = 23.17, p = .006. To verify that the focal pathways differed by gender, the fully constrained model was compared to (a) a model in which the path from 6th grade shifting deficits to 7th grade stress reactivity was allowed to vary by gender; and (b) a model in which the path from 7th grade stress reactivity to 8th grade depressive symptoms was allowed to vary by gender. Both models provided a significantly better fit, Δχ2(1) = 5.48, p = .02 and Δχ2(1) = 10.62, p = .001, than the fully constrained model, supporting gender differences in each of these paths. The fully unconstrained model was adopted as the final model, χ2(34) = 72.85, CFI = .98, IFI = .98, RMSEA = .04.
Figure 1 displays the standardized path coefficients for the final model. All stability paths were significant in girls and boys, with the exception of the stability of depressive symptoms from 6th grade to 7th grade in boys; moreover, the paths from 6th grade stress reactivity to 7th grade depressive symptoms and from 7th grade stress reactivity to 8th grade depressive symptoms were significant in girls and boys. However, the path from 6th grade shifting deficits to 7th grade stress reactivity was significant in girls but not boys. The indirect effect of 6th grade shifting deficits on 8th grade depressive symptoms via 7th grade stress reactivity was significant in girls (IE = .049, 95% CI [.012−.167]; MacKinnon, Fritz, Williams, & Lockwood, 2007; Tofighi & MacKinnon, 2011) but not boys (IE = −.01, 95% CI [−.042 −.026]; MacKinnon et al., 2007; Tofighi & MacKinnon, 2011). The path from 6th grade stress reactivity to 7th grade shifting deficits was significant in girls and boys (βs > .15, ps < .05) whereas the path from 6th grade depressive symptoms to 7th grade stress reactivity was nonsignificant in girls and boys (|β|s < .06, ns).
Discussion
Adolescence is characterized by changes across multiple domains of functioning and increasing exposure to stress (Rudolph, 2009), which may tax youths’ coping resources. Building on prior research, this study examined the idea that poor attentional control would interfere with adolescents’ ability to effectively regulate their cognitive, emotional, and physiological responses to stress, which would heighten depressive symptoms over time, particularly in girls. In support of this proposition, prospective longitudinal modeling analyses revealed that 6th grade shifting deficits predicted higher levels of involuntary engagement responses to social stress and general sensitivity to minor stressors in 7th grade in girls but not in boys. In turn, 7th grade stress reactivity predicted depressive symptoms in 8th grade, yielding an indirect pathway from shifting deficits to depressive symptoms in early adolescent girls.
Attentional Control Deficits Predict Stress Reactivity
Compromised attentional control during the early adolescent years, as reflected in cognitive inflexibility and an inability to deal with new demands or situations in everyday life, predicted subsequent stress reactivity in adolescent girls but not boys. When girls had difficulty shifting attention according to daily task demands or feedback and showed inflexible approaches to solving daily problems, they were more likely to engage in perseverative thinking, to be overly sensitive, and to show heightened emotional and physiological arousal in response to stress. It is possible that this link is strengthened in adolescent girls relative to boys because girls are exposed to higher levels of social stress (Rudolph, 2009) and are more likely to engage in perseverative thinking (Jose & Brown, 2008); thus, heightened stress reactivity stemming from poor attentional control may be activated more frequently or more intensely in girls than in boys.
These findings are consistent with theory and prior research suggesting that being unable to shift or refocus attention in a purposeful manner (Compas et al., 2004; Compton et al., 2000; Hocking et al., 2011) or a narrowing in attentional focus (Whitmer & Gotlib, 2012) interferes with constructive coping efforts and results in heightened reactivity to stress and negative emotions (Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014; Koster et al., 2011). When youth are unable to readily shift attention from stressful stimuli, they may maintain a negative mindset and continue to dwell on unpleasant thoughts, emotions, and events. However, this study builds on prior research establishing concurrent associations by demonstrating that shifting deficits in everyday life contribute to stress reactivity over time, potentially helping to account for increasing emotional and biological sensitivity to social and emotional stimuli in adolescent girls (Guyer et al., 2009). Moreover, by examining bidirectional effects, this research established that stress reactivity predicts poorer attentional control over time, suggesting that dysregulated responses to stress may deplete cognitive resources and impede purposeful and adaptive shifting of attention (Watkins, 2008). Thus, these findings shed light on the dynamic interchange between cognitive processing and stress reactivity across a critical stage of development, suggesting that a self-perpetuating cycle may develop in which adolescent girls with poor attentional control experience heightened stress reactivity, which then further compromises their cognitive flexibility and problem-solving ability.
Stress Reactivity Predicts Depressive Symptoms
In turn, stress reactivity predicted subsequent depressive symptoms in both girls and boys although the link was significantly stronger in girls than in boys. We proposed that youths’ tendency to show involuntary and dysregulated responses to stress may cause them to get “caught up” in stress rather than to cope in an effortful and adaptive manner. These uncontrolled cognitive, emotional, and physiological reactions may threaten their sense of self, intensify negative emotions, and generate further stress in their lives, creating risk for depressive symptoms (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2007; Rood et al., 2009; Troop-Gordon et al., 2015). Consistent with our proposed model, heightened stress reactivity helped to explain the prospective association between shifting deficits and depressive symptoms in girls, providing novel data about one explanatory pathway through which attentional control processes heighten risk during adolescence (for a similar study in adults, see Demeyer et al., 2012). Moreover, the relevance of this pathway to girls but not boys helps to shed light on gender-specific risk for depressive symptoms across early adolescence, a stage during which girls begin to surpass boys in rates of depression.
Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions
The prospective longitudinal design provided an opportunity to examine dynamic linkages among attentional control, stress reactivity, and depressive symptoms across a key developmental period, allowing us to better understand how girls’ vulnerability to depression emerges over time as well as to elucidate the bidirectional effects between attentional control and stress reactivity. The design also was enhanced by the use of teacher reports of attentional control, thereby reducing the effects of shared method variance. However, stress reactivity and depressive symptoms were both assessed via self-reports. Although the influence of method variance was likely reduced by adjusting for prior levels of each construct, it would be informative in future research to assess stress reactivity through multiple informants (e.g., parents, teachers) as well as methods (e.g., in vivo assessment of physiology and behavior).
This research clarifies the role that everyday attentional control processes play in stress reactivity and depression. There are several advantages of measuring attentional control in everyday contexts. First, the BRIEF likely taps the molar expression of attentional control across real-world situations and time, making it a more reliable and generalizable assessment than performance-based measures, which tend to have low test-retest reliability (Miyake et al., 2000) and thus may provide more situation-specific and variable assessments. Second, attentional control deficits may be most salient or detrimental under “hot” conditions when cognitive systems are challenged, youth are more invested, or emotions are intensified (Muris et al., 2008), which is more likely to be the case in everyday contexts. Indeed, research suggests that executive function deficits, including attentional control, predict rumination (De Lissnyder et al., 2012) and depressive symptoms (Agoston & Rudolph, 2015) specifically in the presence of high stress exposure. Because performance-based measures often assess attentional control in “cold” conditions, they may be less relevant to understanding the interrelation between attentional control and stress reactivity. The allocation of attentional control in everyday situations may therefore be more meaningful than attentional control capacity when predicting how youth respond to everyday stressors.
However, using the BRIEF also introduces some limitations. First, it is difficult to distinguish exactly which aspects of shifting (e.g., ignoring distractors in the environment vs. inhibiting previously, but no longer, relevant information; Friedman & Miyake, 2004) are assessed by the BRIEF. Second, the BRIEF assesses attentional control only in the form of shifting, thus omitting other important aspects of attentional control such as attentional focus. Third, associations between questionnaire and performance-based measures of attentional control and related executive functions tend to be modest (Toplak, West, & Stanovich, 2013). Given these limitations, it would be beneficial for future research to directly examine the relative contributions of performance-based versus everyday manifestations of attentional control as well as to carefully assess distinct aspects of attentional control and their contribution to stress reactivity and depressive symptoms. It also will be important to examine the specificity of the observed pathway to attentional control deficits versus other aspects of executive function.1
Finally, it is important to note that the indirect effect from shifting deficits to depressive symptoms in girls was relatively small. Thus, it is likely that processes other than stress reactivity help to account for the contribution of shifting deficits to depression. In fact, a recent study found that self-generated stressors and rumination mediated the link between poor cognitive control and subsequent depressive symptoms (Snyder & Hankin, 2016), suggesting that individuals with cognitive control deficits both create more stress in their lives and react to this stress in ways that heighten risk for depressive symptoms.
Implications for Practice
This research provides a novel perspective on adolescent vulnerability to depression as well as gender-specific vulnerability that may help explain the greater rise in depression over the course of adolescence in girls relative to boys. Effective prevention may involve intervening at different stages of the observed process. On the one hand, teaching effective coping strategies (e.g., how to problem solve or reappraise stressful situations) may interrupt the progression from compromised attentional control to depressive symptoms. On the other hand, girls with poor attentional control may have difficulty learning or implementing such strategies, such that direct training and practice in attentional control strategies (e.g., Siegle et al., 2007; Siegle et al., 2014) may be needed to help them take advantage of coping interventions aimed at fostering more effortful, planful strategies and diminishing involuntary and dysregulated responses. Most likely, intervening at both levels would be most beneficial for inoculating girls against the downward spiral of negative cognitions and emotion leading to depression across the early adolescent years and beyond.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the families and schools who participated in this study. We are grateful to Jamie Abaied, Hannah Banagale, Elie Hessel, Jo Pauly, Nicole Llewellyn, Niwako Sugimura, and Michelle Miernicki for their assistance in data collection and management. This research was supported by a University of Illinois Arnold O. Beckman Award and National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH68444 awarded to Karen D. Rudolph
Footnotes
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
In our study, we examined specificity to three other dimensions on the BRIEF: inhibition, working memory, and planning/organization. These analyses indicated that there was a significant indirect effect of inhibition on depression via stress reactivity for girls (IE = .05, 95% CI [.01 − .13], but not for boys (IE = −.02, 95% CI [−.03 − .01]). The indirect effects of working memory and planning on depression via stress reactivity were not significant for girls (for working memory, IE = .03, 95% CI = −.01 − .08; for planning, IE = .03, 95% CI = −.01 − .08) or for boys (for working memory, IE = −.04, 95% CI = −.05 − .01; for planning, IE = −.03, 95% CI = −.04 − .01).
Contributor Information
Karen D. Rudolph, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
Jennifer D. Monti, Nationwide Children’s Hospital
Megan Flynn, Medica Research Institute.
References
- Abela JRZ, Hankin BL. Rumination as a vulnerability factor to depression during the transition from early to middle adolescence: A multiwave longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120:259–271. doi: 10.1037/a0022796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agoston AM, Rudolph KD. Transactional associations between youths’ responses to peer stress and depression: The moderating roles of sex and stress exposure. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:159–171. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9458-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agoston AM, Rudolph KD. Interactive contributions of cumulative peer stress and executive function deficits to depression in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2015 doi: 10.1177/0272431615593176. EPub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altamirano LJ, Miyake A, Whitmer AJ. When mental inflexibility facilitates executive control: Beneficial side effects of ruminative tendencies on goal maintenance. Psychological Science. 2010;21:1377–1382. doi: 10.1177/0956797610381505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angold A, Costello E, Messer SC, Pickles A. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1995;5:237–249. [Google Scholar]
- Arbuckle JL. AMOS 17.0 [Computer Software] Chicago: Small Waters Corp; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Banich MT. Executive function: The search for an integrated account. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18:89–94. [Google Scholar]
- Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–162. [Google Scholar]
- Chambers R, Lo BCY, Allen NB. The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2008;32:303–322. [Google Scholar]
- Charbonneau AM, Mezulis AH, Hyde JS. Stress and emotional reactivity as explanations for gender differences in adolescents’ depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2009;38:1050–1058. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9398-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christ SE, Kanne SM, Reiersen AM. Executive function in individuals with subthreshold autism traits. Neuropsychology. 2010;24:590–598. doi: 10.1037/a0019176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compas BE, Connor-Smith J, Jaser SS. Temperament, stress reactivity, and coping: Implications for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33:21–31. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compas BE, Connor-Smith JK, Saltzman H, Thomsen AH, Wadsworth ME. Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:87–127. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton RJ, Heller W, Banich MT, Palmieri PA, Miller GA. Responding to threat: Hemispheric asymmetries and interhemispheric division of input. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:254–264. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connor-Smith JK, Compas BE, Wadsworth ME, Thomsen AH, Saltzman H. Responses to stress in adolescence: Measurement of coping and involuntary stress responses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:976–992. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis RN, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2000;24:699–711. [Google Scholar]
- De Lissnyder E, Koster EH, Goubert L, Onraedt T, Vanderhasselt MA, De Raedt R. Cognitive control moderates the association between stress and rumination. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2012;43:519–525. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demeyer I, De Lissnyder E, Koster EH, Raedt R. Rumination mediates the relationship between impaired cognitive control for emotional information and depressive symptoms: A prospective study in remitted depressed adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2012;50:292–297. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denckla MB. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function: Commentary. Child Neuropsychology. 2002;8:304–306. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.304.13512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Guthrie IK, Reiser M. Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78:136–157. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling. 2001;8:430–457. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flynn M, Rudolph KD. Perceptual asymmetry and youths’ responses to stress: Understanding vulnerability to depression. Cognition and Emotion. 2007;21:773–788. doi: 10.1080/02699930600824635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flynn M, Rudolph KD. Neuropsychological and interpersonal antecedents of youth depression. Cognition and Emotion. 2010;24:94–110. doi: 10.1080/02699930802584417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flynn M, Rudolph KD. Stress generation and adolescent depression: Contribution of interpersonal stress responses. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:1187–1198. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9527-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133:101–135. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK. Ecological assessment of executive function in traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2004;25:135–158. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2004.9651925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000a. [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology. 2000b;6:235–238. doi: 10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Retzlaff PD, Espy KA. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychology. 2002;8:249–257. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guerry JD, Hastings PD. In search of HPA axis dysregulation in child and adolescent depression. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2011;14:135–160. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0084-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Wewerka S, Frenn K, Long JD, Griggs C. Developmental changes in hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: Normative changes and associations with puberty. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:69–85. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunther T, Konrad K, De Brito SA, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Vloet TD. Attentional functions in children and adolescents with ADHD, depressive disorders, and the comorbid condition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:324–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02320.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guyer AE, McClure-Tone EB, Shiffrin ND, Pine DS, Nelson EE. Probing the neural correlates of anticipated peer evaluation in adolescence. Child Development. 2009;80:1000–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01313.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:773–796. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Mermelstein R, Roesch L. Sex differences in adolescent depression: Stress exposure and reactivity models in interpersonal and achievement contextual domains. Child Development. 2007;78:279–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilt LM, Armstrong JM, Essex MJ. Early family context and development of adolescent ruminative style: Moderation by temperament. Cognition and Emotion. 2012;26:916–926. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.621932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilt LM, Leitzke BT, Pollak SD. Cognitive control and rumination in youth: The importance of emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 2014;5:302–313. doi: 10.5127/jep.038113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilt LM, Leitzke BT, Pollak SD. Can’t take my eyes off of you: Eye tracking reveals how ruminating young adolescents get stuck. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1121824. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilt LM, McLaughlin KA, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Examination of the response styles theory in a community sample of young adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2010;38:545–556. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9384-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hocking MC, Barnes M, Shaw C, Lochman JE, Madan-Swain A, Saeed S. Executive function and attention regulation as predictors of coping success in youth with functional abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2011;36:64–73. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsq056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Bentler P. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
- Hyde JS, Mezulis AH, Abramson LY. The ABCs of depression: Integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression. Psychological Review. 2008;115:291–313. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joormann J, Vanderlind WM. Emotion regulation in depression: The role of biased cognition and reduced cognitive control. Clinical Psychological Science. 2014;2:402–421. [Google Scholar]
- Jose PE, Brown I. When does the gender difference in rumination begin? Gender and age differences in the use of rumination by adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37:180–192. [Google Scholar]
- Joyner KB, Silver CH, Stavinoha PL. Relationship between parenting stress and ratings of executive functioning in children with ADHD. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment. 2009;27:452–464. [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Kyte ZA, Goodyer IM, Sahakian BJ. Selected executive skills in adolescents with recent first episode major depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:995–1005. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00400.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koster EHW, De Lissnyder ED, Derakshan N, De Raedt R. Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31:138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R. Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: A multitrait-multidiagnosis study of an adolescent birth cohort. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1996;105:299–312. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.105.3.299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lara ME, Leader J, Klein DN. The association between social support and course of depression: Is it confounded with personality? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:478–482. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.3.478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau JY, Eley TC. Attributional style as a risk marker of genetic effects for adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:849–859. doi: 10.1037/a0013943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lengua LJ. The contribution of emotionality and self-regulation to the understanding of children’s response to multiple risk. Child Development. 2002;73:144–161. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lengua LJ. Growth in temperament and parenting as predictors of adjustment during children’s transition to adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:819–832. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Letkiewicz AM, Miller GA, Crocker LD, Warren SL, Infantolino ZP, Mimnaugh KJ, Heller W. Executive function deficits in daily life prospectively predict increases in depressive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2014;38:612–620. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9629-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Fritz MS, Williams J, Lockwood CM. Distribution of the product confidence limits for the indirect effect: Program PRODCLIN. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:384–389. doi: 10.3758/bf03193007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malooly AM, Genet JJ, Siemer M. Individual differences in reappraisal effectiveness: The role of affective flexibility. Emotion. 2013;13:302–313. doi: 10.1037/a0029980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melendez R, Bechor M, Rey J, Pettit JW, Silverman WK. Attentional Control Scale for children: Factor structure and concurrent validity among children and adolescents referred for anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2016 doi: 10.1002/jclp.22346. Online ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller MW, Greif JL, Smith AA. Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire profiles of veterans with traumatic combat exposure: Externalizing and internalizing subtypes. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15:205–215. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.2.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake M. Relation of generalized self-efficacy to changes in task-specific self-efficacy. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology. 2000;48:42–51. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moberly NJ, Watkins ER. Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:314–323. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muris P, Mayer B, van Lint C, Hofman S. Attentional control and psychopathological symptoms in children. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;44:1495–1505. [Google Scholar]
- Muris P, van der Pennen E, Sigmond R, Mayer B. Symptoms of anxiety, depression, and aggression in non-clinical children: Relationships with self-report and performance-based measures of attention and effortful control. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2008;39:455–467. doi: 10.1007/s10578-008-0101-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Stice E, Wade E, Bohon C. Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:198–207. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick CJ, Curtin JJ, Tellegen A. Development and validation of a brief form of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment. 2002;14:150–163. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.14.2.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick CJ, Kramer MD, Tellegen A, Verona E, Kaemmer BA. Development and preliminary validation of a simplified-wording form of the multidimensional personality questionnaire. Assessment. 2013;20:405–418. doi: 10.1177/1073191112467051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reddy LA, Hale JB, Brodzinsky LK. Discriminant validity of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Parent Form for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly. 2011;26:45–55. [Google Scholar]
- Rood L, Roelofs J, Bogels SM, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schouten E. The influence of emotion-focused rumination and distraction on depressive symptoms in non-clinical youth: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29:607–616. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE. Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78:122–135. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Lerner R, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development. 6. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 99–166. [Google Scholar]
- Rudasill KM, Konold TR. Contributions of children’s temperament to teachers’ judgments of social competence from kindergarten through second grade. Early Education and Development. 2008;19:643–666. [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD. Gender differences in emotional responses to interpersonal stress during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2002;30:3–13. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(01)00383-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD. The interpersonal context of adolescent depression. In: Nolen-Hoeksema S, Hilt L, editors. Handbook of depression in adolescence. New York, NY: Routledge; 2009. pp. 377–418. [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD. Puberty as a developmental context of risk for psychopathology. In: Lewis M, Rudolph KD, editors. Handbook of developmental psychopathology. New York, NY: Springer; 2014. pp. 331–354. [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD, Abaied JL, Flynn M, Sugimura N, Agoston AM. Developing relationships, being cool, and not looking like a loser: Social goal orientation predicts children’s responses to peer aggression. Child Development. 2011;82:1518–1530. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01631.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rueda MR, Purificacion C, Rothbart MK. Contributions of attentional control to socioemotional and academic development. Early Education and Development. 2010;21:744–764. [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez A, Vazquez C, Marker C, LeMoult J, Joormann J. Attentional disengagement predicts stress recovery in depression: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122:303–313. doi: 10.1037/a0031529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H, Muller H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research. 2003;8:23–74. [Google Scholar]
- Semrud-Clikeman M, Walkowiak J, Wilkinson A, Butcher B. Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominately inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40:1017–1027. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0951-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shih JH, Eberhart NK, Hammen CL, Brennan PA. Differential exposure and reactivity to interpersonal stress predict sex differences in adolescent depression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:103–115. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3501_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegle GJ, Price RB, Jones NP, Ghinassi F, Painter T, Thase ME. You gotta work at it: Pupillary indices of task focus are prognostic for response to a neurocognitive intervention for rumination in depression. Clinical Psychological Science. 2014;2:455–471. [Google Scholar]
- Siegle GJ, Thompson W, Carter CS, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME. Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: Related and independent features. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:198–209. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simonds J, Kieras JE, Rueda MR, Rothbart MK. Effortful control, executive attention, and emotional regulation in 7–10-year-old children. Cognitive Development. 2007;22:474–488. [Google Scholar]
- Slick DJ, Lautzenhiser A, Sherman EMS, Eryl K. Frequency of scale elevations and factor structure of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in children and adolescents with intractable epilepsy. Child Neuropsychology. 2006;12:181–189. doi: 10.1080/09297040600611320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder HR, Hankin BL. Spiraling out of control: Stress generation and subsequent rumination mediate the link between poorer cognitive control and internalizing psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science. 2016 doi: 10.1177/2167702616633157. Online ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Fabes RA, Valiente C, Shepard SA, … Guthrie IK. Relation of emotion-related regulation to children’s social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion. 2006;6:498–510. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stange JP, Hamilton JL, Abramson LY, Alloy LB. A vulnerability-stress examination of response styles theory in adolescence: Stressors, sex differences, and symptom specificity. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2014;43:813–827. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.812037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, McGuffin P. Validity of the shortened Mood and Feelings Questionnaire in a community sample of children and adolescents: A preliminary research note. Psychiatry Research. 1998;81:259–268. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(98)00073-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tofighi D, MacKinnon DP. Rmediation: An R package for mediation analysis confidence intervals. Behavior Research Methods. 2011;43:692–700. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0076-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toplak ME, West RF, Stanovich KE. Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2013;54:131–143. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troop-Gordon W, Rudolph KD, Sugimura N, Little TD. Peer victimization in middle childhood impedes adaptive responses to stress: A pathway to depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2015;44:432–445. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.891225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verstraeten K, Bijttebier P, Vasey MW, Raes F. Specificity of worry and rumination in the development of anxiety and depressive symptoms in children. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2011;50:364–378. doi: 10.1348/014466510X532715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verstraeten K, Vasey MW, Raes F, Bijttebier P. Temperament and risk for depressive symptoms in adolescence: Mediation by rumination and moderation by effortful control. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37:349–361. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9293-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner S, Muller C, Helmreich I, Huss M, Tadic A. A meta-analysis of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with major depressive disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;24:5–19. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0559-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins ER. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134:163–206. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White BA, Turner KA. Anger rumination and effortful control: Mediation effects on reactive but not proactive aggression. Personality and Individual Differences. 2014;56:186–189. [Google Scholar]
- Whitmer AJ, Gotlib IH. Switching and backward inhibition in major depressive disorder: The role of rumination. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:570–578. doi: 10.1037/a0027474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitmer AJ, Gotlib IH. An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139:1036–1061. doi: 10.1037/a0030923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson PO, Goodyer IM. Attention difficulties and mood-related ruminative response style in adolescents with unipolar depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:1284–1291. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01660.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]