Abstract
Recent studies support the idea that stimulus processing in latent inhibition can vary during the course of preexposure. Controlled attentional mechanisms are said to be important in the early stages of preexposure, while in later stages animals adopt automatic processing of the stimulus to be used for conditioning. Given this distinction, it is possible that both types of processing are governed by different neural systems, affecting differentially the retrieval of information about the stimulus. In the present study we tested if a lesion to the dorso-lateral striatum or to the medial prefrontal cortex has a selective effect on exposure to the future conditioned stimulus (CS). With this aim, animals received different amounts of exposure to the future CS. The results showed that a lesion to the medial prefrontal cortex enhanced latent inhibition in animals receiving limited preexposure to the CS, but had no effect in animals receiving extended preexposure to the CS. The lesion of the dorso-lateral striatum produced a decrease in latent inhibition, but only in animals with an extended exposure to the future conditioned stimulus. These results suggest that the dorsal striatum and medial prefrontal cortex play essential roles in controlled and automatic processes. Automatic attentional processes appear to be impaired by a lesion to the dorso-lateral striatum and facilitated by a lesion to the prefrontal cortex.
Introduction
Latent inhibition (LI) is a retardation in the acquisition of a conditioned response during Pavlovian conditioning, if the CS has previously been presented on its own [1]. A variety of theoretical explanations have been offered for this effect. Some of these theories attribute LI to retrieval failure [2–4], other theories attribute it to acquisition failure [5–8]. With regard to the latter, it has been proposed on more than one occasion that the acquisition failure results from a loss of attention to the CS [9, 10]. For example, according to Pearce and Hall [7] when the significance of a stimulus is uncertain, such as when it is novel, or during the early stages of conditioning, then it will receive controlled processing that will enable leaning about the stimulus. However, when the significance of a stimulus is certain, such as when it has been repeatedly followed by nothing or by the same US, then it will receive automatic processing [7], which will make it difficult for further learning about the stimulus to take place [11–13]. Even though both process are well defined, none of the current associative model predict when processing will became automatic during preexposure learning. Thus, once it is accepted that automatic and controlled attentional processes influence the course of learning, it is then important to identify the role played by different regions of the brain in these processes. The purpose of the present article is to explore the role played by the dorsal striatum and the medial prefrontal cortex.
In recent years, several studies have aided in the growth in our understanding of the role for striatal-frontal interactions supporting higher cognitive functions like attention or cognitive control of flexible actions [14–26]. Corticostriatal loops have been proposed to be involved in these functions, even though studies focused specifically on attention have been scarce. Recently, we argued that the dorsal striatum supports the cognitive control of memory retrieval. Díaz et al. [10] analyzed the role of dorsal striatum in retrieval processes of LI using different amounts of exposure to the CS. More specifically, one group of rats was exposed to many presentations of the future CS of saccharine without consequences, over a period of five days, while a second group received only two days of preexposure treatment. Both groups then received a single trial of taste aversion conditioning with saccharine, before being tested with saccharine by itself. Critically, the dorso-lateral striatum (dls) was blocked during the final test phase. The results showed that the blockade of dls during the test phase disrupted LI in the group with five days of preexposure, but not in the group with two days of preexposure. It was concluded that successful LI, after extensive preexposure, depends upon automatic processing of the relevant stimulus, and that this processing is effective after many, but not a few preexposure trials. If this conclusion is correct, then a similar outcome will be observed if the dls is rendered inactive before the preexposure training, rather than before the test trial. One purpose of the present experiments is to test this prediction. An implication of the study by Diaz et al. [10], is that a mechanism other than automatic processing is responsible for LI after a few trials. One possibility is that some aspect of controlled processing, which is presumably directed to a stimulus after a few exposure trials, is responsible for any loss in associability that might be observed. A further purpose of the experiments is to explore whether the medial prefrontal cortex (mpfc) plays a role in this type of processing.
The mpfc has previously been implicated in the attribution of incentive value to stimuli and goal directed behavior [27–29]. The mpfc sends a dense glutamatergic projection to nucleus accumbens, a structure that is itself necessary for LI expression. In addition, corticostriatal loops include projections from mpfc to dorso-medial striatum (dms), which work together in goal directed behavior [30, 31]. While these findings are compelling, the role of mpfc in controlled and automatic processes of a future CS has not yet been investigated.
Here, we analyzed the involvement of dls and mpfc in attentional processes. Specifically, we tested if dls and mpfc are involved in automatic and controlled processes by assessing dls and mpfc lesions in rats followed by extended and limited exposure to a future CS in the conditioned taste aversion paradigm (CTA). Specifically, we analyzed if lesion to these structures resulted in selective deficits that were dependent upon the duration of exposure to future CS. If dls is involved in the automatic stimulus processing, the subjects should display a decreased LI with extended exposure. In contrasts, lesion to this structure should not affect the performance after limited exposure, where the controlled attentional processes would facilitate a normal LI expression. On the other hand, if the mpfc is involved in controlled attentional processes, the lesion should facilitate automatic processes, and it should increase the LI expression in a limited exposure. Taken together, these studies will further the understanding of the role of the mpfc and dls in controlled and automatic processes of LI.
General methods
Subjects
174 adult male Wistar rats (300-400g) were used in these experiments, but 51 were excluded after histological analysis (see section below). All the animals were obtained from the Centro de Producción y Experimentación Animal (Universidad de Sevilla). Animals were individually housed in plastic cages (35×20×20 cm). The controlled temperature of the room was 21°C. The room was illuminated by four 100-W halogen lamps with a 14h light and 10h dark cycle. Rats were maintained and used for experimentation in accordance with the Guidelines of the European Union Council established by the Directive 2010/63/EU, and following the Spanish regulations (R.D 53/02013) for the use of laboratory animals. An ethical commission of University of Seville and Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Sanidad Animal, Junta de Andalucía) supervised and approved all the procedures and all protocols used in the this specific study. The code of the supervision report is: 31/08/2016/153. All animals were randomly distributed among the experimental conditions.
Surgery
Under deep isoflurane anesthesia (2–5% in air, flow rate 1 l/min, 5% induction; 2% maintenance; McKinley type 2, Everest), rats were placed in a David Kopf stereotaxic instrument. The skin was incised to expose the skull, and following coordinates from Paxinos & Watson [32]: 1.6; 1.1; 0.6; and 0.1mm anterior to bregma; ±3.2; 3.5; 4.0 and 4.0mm lateral to the midline, and 5.0mm ventral to brain surface for dls lesion (for animals of experiment 1A and 1B), and 4.7; 4.2; 3.7mm anterior, ±0.6 lateral, and 4.0mm ventral for mpfc lesion (for animals of experiment 2A and 2B). The dls and mpfc lesion was made with NMDA (1 mg in 0.1 ml phosphate buffered saline, PBS 0.1M). The drug was injected into the brain through a 10μl Hamilton syringe (Model 1701 RN). The amount of NMDA solution injected at each site was 0.25 μl, at a rate of 0.05μl/min. The needle was left in place for an additional minute after the infusion to allow the diffusion of the solution into the tissue. No unexpected adverse effects exceeding moderate severity were observed 48 h after the surgery. Sham animals received a similar manipulation but no injection of the drug was carried out.
Behavioral procedure
Behavioral procedures used were similar to that described in Díaz et al. [10]. After a one-week recovery period, the rats were randomly distributed among the experimental groups that received different amounts of exposure to the the future CS (see next paragraph). Three days before the preexposure phase, a deprivation program was implemented. Animals were allowed access to water for 10 min at a time at 10.00, 14.00 and 18.00 h for this period. All behavioral sessions were conducted in the vivarium. The water, and the saccharine (0.04%) that was used during the experiment was administered at room temperature in 150 ml glass bottles with stainless-steel mouthpieces. The bottles were attached to a frontal aperture in the grill of the home cage so that they remained immobile during the experimental session. The quantity of liquid consumed was calculated by weighing the bottle before and after each trial.
Extended exposure to the future CS
This phase lasted five days and was used in Experiments 1A and 2A. During this stage, all animals were allowed 10 min of access to water or saccharine 3 times per day (at 10.00 h, 14.00 h and 18.00 h). Rats were assigned to two different conditions. The extended exposure groups (E-exp) received saccharine three times a day for five days during this phase (dls lesion n = 7, dls sham n = 7; mpfc lesion n = 8, mpfc sham n = 8). The non-exposure groups to future CS (N-exp) were allowed 10 min access to water three times a day (dls lesion n = 7, dls sham n = 7; mpfc lesion n = 6, mpfc sham n = 6). After this period, the bottles were removed for all groups.
Limited exposure to the future CS
In experiment 1B and 2B, the groups given limited exposure group to the future CS (L-exp) received saccharine three times a day for two days during this phase (dls lesion n = 10, dls sham n = 10; mpfc lesion n = 13, mpfc sham n = 10). For the previous three days, they consumed only water. The groups given non-exposure to saccharine (N-exp) were just given water during the five days of this stage (dls lesion n = 6, sham n = 6; mpfc lesion n = 6, sham n = 6).
Conditioning
This phase consisted of a single session that took place on the day after the completion of the preexposure phase. All animals received only saccharine at 10.00 h for 10 min, followed by an intraperitoneal injection of LiCl dissolved in saline (0.4 M, 0.5% body weight). During the same day, at 14.00 and 18.00 hr, all groups were allowed 10 min access to water.
Test
This phase was run at 10.00 h on the day after conditioning. During this phase all rats had access to the saccharine solution for 10 min. Saccharine consumption for this trial reflected the level of taste aversion. As for conditioning, the treatment in this phase was the same in all the experiments.
Histological analysis. Assessment of mpfc and dls lesion placement
Upon completion of behavioral testing, rats were deeply anesthetized and perfused transcardially with a fixative solution (10% formalin in phosphate buffer 0.1 M, pH 7.4). The brains were then removed from the skull and placed in 10% formalin and buffered for 3–4 days. Next, the brains were cut with a microtome at the coronal plane at 50 μm thickness oriented according to the atlas of Paxinos & Watson [32] and Nissl stained for histological analysis. Specifically, we stained the tissue with cresyl violet method. This allows to determine the extent of dls and mpfc lesion in the experimental groups. Lesions were quantified in dls and mpfc groups. Animals included in this study showed lesions between 52–94% of damage in dls (Fig 1) and 56–89% in mpfc (Fig 2), and without significant damage to the adjacent structures.
Data analysis
Preexposure phase: repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) test was performed to assess the effects of the trials and their interaction with lesion and exposure (between subject factors). The factors for the analyses were lesion, exposure and trials of saccharin consumption. Conditioning and test phase included the factors lesion and exposure. Significant interactions were analyzed with simple main effects based on the pooled error term (Bonferroni corrections). In addition, simple effects in test phase were analyzed with Student’s t test. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05.
Results
Experiment 1A. Extended exposure to future CS in animals with dls lesion
Preexposure phase
The mean liquid consumption across preexposure trials as a function of the preexposure and the lesion conditions showed differences between groups. A mixed 5 (trials) x 2 (preexposure) x 2 (lesion) ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of trials, F(4,96) = 8.89, p<0.01, and a main effect of preexposure, F(1,24) = 11.73, p <0.01. These data indicate that animals increased the consumption across the sessions, probably because of the effect of neophobia was reduced across sessions. However, the main effect of lesion was not significant on comsumption, F(1, 24) = 4.05, p = 0.055 (Fig 3A). The Trials x Preexposure interaction was significant, F(4,96) = 5.84, p<0.01, ƞp2 = 0.196, probably reflecting a general habituation of neophobia effect in the E-exp group. No other interaction was significant (all ps>0.05; Fig 3A).
Conditioning phase
The analysis of the conditioning session with an ANOVA 2 x 2 (preexposure x lesion) revealed a significant effect of the preexposure on consumption, F(1,24) = 9.39, p<0.01, since animals of both E-exp groups consumed more solution that N-exp groups. However, neither the effect of the lesion F(1, 24) = 2.79, p = 0.10 nor the interaction Preexposure x Lesion, F(1, 24) = 0.47, p = 0.83, were significant (Fig 3A).
Test phase
Fig 3B shows mean liquid intake in test trials as a function of preexposure and lesion. To analyze the consumption during the test, a 2 x 2 ANOVA (preexposure x lesion) was conducted. The analysis revealed a significant main effect of preexposure, F(1, 24) = 68.09, p<0.01, and lesion, F(1, 24) = 19.91, p<0.01. The interaction Preexposure x Lesion was also significant, F(1,24) = 28.73, p<0.01, ƞp2 = 0.517. The analysis of the interaction revealed that there were differences between E-exp and N-exp groups in the Sham condition t(12) = 12.48, p<0.01. However, LI effect was not observed in the lesion condition, t(12) = 1.88, p = 0.085. The E-exp sham group drank more saccharin than the lesion group, t(12) = 5.10, p<0.01. However, the N-exp groups did not differ between themselves, t(12) = 0.85, p = 0.40.
Experiment 1B. Dls lesion and limited exposure to future CS
Preexposure phase
An ANOVA 2(trials) x 2(preexposure) x 2(lesion) was conducted on the mean of liquid consumed across the preexposure trials. Similar to Experiment 1, this analysis showed an interaction effect of Trials x Preexposure, F(1, 28) = 12.38, p<0.01, ƞp2 = 0.267, due to the preexposure groups drinking more liquid than the non-exposure groups in the second trial, F(1, 28) = 10.49, p<0.01 (Fig 4A). Rats of L-exp condition increased consumption of saccharin from trial 1 to trial 2, F(1, 28) = 4.53, p = 0.042. In contrast, the water intake decreased in the N-exp condition the second day of exposure, F(1, 28) = 7.84, p<0.01; however, consumption of both groups remained high. No other effect was significant (all ps>0.05; Fig 4A).
Conditioning phase
Consumption on the conditioning day was analyzed by an ANOVA 2 (preexposure) x 2 (lesion). We found the same effect as for Experiment 1. There was a main effect of preexposure, F(1, 28) = 8.62, p<0.01, because saccharin consumption by the N-exp groups was lower than the consumption of the L-exp groups (Fig 4A). No other effect approached significance (all ps>0.05).
Test phase
Fig 4B shows the results of the consumption test. LI effect was evident for sham and lesioned animals. An ANOVA 2 (preexposure) x 2 (lesion) conducted on the mean amount of saccharin consumed in the test trials revealed a significant main effect of preexposure, F(1, 28) = 82.31, p<0.01, reflecting a general LI effect. Neither the main effect of lesion (p = 0.16) nor the interaction Preexposure x Lesion (p = 0.56) were significant, showing that the lesion did not affect the preexposure effect.
Results experiment 2A. Extended exposure to future CS after mpfc lesion
Preexposure and conditioning phase
A repeated measured ANOVA 5x2x2 using preexposure trials as a within-subjects factor and preexposure and lesion as between-subjects factors showed a significant main effect of preexposure, (F(1, 25) = 6.715, p = 0.016, of trials, F(4,100) = 8.60, p<0.01, an interaction between these factors, F (4, 100) = 3.702, p<0.01, ƞp2 = 0.129, and a trial x lesion interaction, F(4, 100) = 3.58, p<0.01, ƞp2 = 0.125. Simple main effect analysis revealed that the extended exposure groups consumed more liquid than the non-exposure groups in trials 2, 3, and 5 (all ps<0.05) probably due to the preference of animals for sweet taste of saccharin. The lesioned groups drank less saccharin than the sham groups in trial 3 (p<0.01) but there were no differences in any other trial (Fig 5A).
On the conditioning day, an ANOVA 2x2 conducted using preexposure and lesion as factors found no significant effect of preexposure (F(1,25) = 1.006, p = 0.326), no effect of lesion F(1, 25) = 0.476, p = 0.497, nor a preexposure x lesion interaction, F(1, 25) = 0.744, p = 0.397 (Fig 5A).
Test phase
Fig 5B shows the mean of saccharin consumption in the test trial as a function of the preexposure and the lesion conditions. There was a LI effect in both lesioned and sham groups. An ANOVA 2x2 with preexposure and lesion as factors showed main effect of Preexposure, F(1, 25) = 138.01, p<0.01. None other effect was significant (lesion, p = 0.752 and preexposure x lesion, p = 0.658) indicating that the LI effect was not affected by the mpfc lesion.
Results experiment 2B: Limited exposure to future CS after mpfc lesion
Preexposure and conditioning phase
Fig 6A shows mean liquid consumption across preexposure trials as a function of the preexposure and the lesion conditions. A mixed 2 (trials) x 2 (preexposure) x 2 (lesion) ANOVA with trials as the within-subject factor revealed a significant preexposure x lesion interaction effect, F(1,31) = 5.99, p = 0.02, ƞp2 = 0.145. This effect was due to the limited exposure mpfc group drank more liquid than non-exposure group (p = 0.019). The interaction trials x preexposure was also significant, F(1,31) = 4.38, p = 0.04, ƞp2 = 0.150, reflecting a habituation of the neophobia to the new flavor. No other effect was significant (all ps>0.05). During conditioning phase, there were not any significant effect (all ps>0.05, see Fig 6A).
Test phase
Fig 6B displays the results of the test. Lesioned animals showed a higher LI effect than sham animals. To analyze the consumption in the test, an ANOVA 2 (preexposure) x 2 (lesion) was conducted. The analysis revealed a significant main effect of preexposure, F(1, 31) = 48.61, p<0.01, reflecting a general LI effect. The main effect of lesion was not significant (p>0.05). The interaction Preexposure x Lesion was significant, F(1,31) = 7.15, p = 0.012, ƞp2 = 0.188. The analysis of this two-way interaction revealed that mpfc lesion exclusively affected the preexposure learning. There were differences between L-exp and N-exp groups in sham F(1, 31) = 8.83, p<0.01 and lesioned conditions, F(1, 31) = 48.74, p<0.01. In regard to the L-exp, lesioned group drank more saccharin than the sham group, F(1, 31) = 13.84, p<0.01. However, the N-exp groups did not differ between them, F(1, 31) = 0.36, p = 0.55.
Discussion
The results from the present experiments show a selective role for the mpfc and dls in LI. Lesions to dls structure did not disrupt LI after only a few trials of preexposure to saccharin (Experiment 1B). However, an effect of the dls lesion was revealed by the absence of LI in the lesioned group that received many exposures to saccharin prior to the conditioning trial (Experiment 1A). In contrast, mpfc animals showed an increased LI with a limited exposure to future CS (Experiment 2B) and a normal LI with the extended training (Experiment 2A). Likewise, we observed normal conditioning in lesioned animals without exposure to the future CS, which indicates that neither the mpfc nor dls are involved in the formation of the CS-US association in the CTA paradigm. These results demonstrate that mpfc and dorsal striatum could have a functional connectivity necessary for LI expression; probably based on the control of attentional processes. Previous work has shown the dorsal striatum and mpfc are involved in LI in different paradigms [23, 33, 34], but this study identifies a specific region of dorsal striatum and mpfc in the control of LI expression with different exposure to the future CS.
Several studies have investingated he involvement of mpfc in LI. For instance, Lacroix et al. [35] trained animals in a lick suppression paradigm to test the effects of dopamine agonist and antagonist administration in mpfc; and they did not find any effect on LI. Our results are congruent with this study, since we also found that LI is not disrupted by damage to the mpfc. Our results are also congruent with a previous study by George et al. [23] testing different areas of mpfc with a conditioned emotional response paradigm. Even though it is difficult to draw clear conclusions when comparing results from conditioned emotional response and taste aversion paradigms, it is noteworthy that this study found a clear LI following mpfc lesions, and an increased LI after lesion of ventromedial prefrontal cortex. In contrast, Nelson et al. [36] found a LI release after dopaminergic depletion in prelimbic cortex with the same paradigm. These results are similar to present data, since the exposure to future CS could be similar to a limited exposure group in our study. Based on Weiner’s model [37], they maintained that prefrontal cortex was concerned with a switching mechanism between the responses associated with an ambiguous stimulus. If this is so, our results suggest that this mechanism will only act in those situations where the preexposure learning is controlled. However, when the preexposure learning is under control of automatic processes, the prefrontal switching mechanisms will not be activated. In this regard (according to this suggestion), Killcross & Coutureau [38] found that animals with lesions of the prelimbic area lost the sensitivity to changes in the value of reward after both limited or extended training. These results could point to prefrontal cortex involvement in coordinating controlled and automatic response, and the prelimbic and infralimbic region should play an important role in the active inhibition of mechanisms that produce habitual response. Thus, it is plausible to explain the effects on LI as possible fluctuations in attentional processes. This idea emerges from the present results, where attentional processes change with the training sessions, and they may be modulated by different neural structures.
The results from these experiments are in line with those reported by Díaz et al. [10], who blocked activity in the dls only during the test stage of the experiment, and found a disruption of LI with many, but not a few preexposure trials. In light of these findings, it appears that the effect described is reliable, and can also be found when the dls is made inactive by being lesioned before starting the experiment. Moreover, by disrupting activity in the dls before the preexposure rather than immediately before the test trial (as was the case for Díaz et al. [10]), it is not possible to attribute the disruption of LI in the present study to a possible change in the internal context or physiological state before the test trial.
Thus the present results point to the conclusion that two distinct neural mechanisms are responsible for LI in conditioned taste aversion. One mechanism might require an intact dls, and results in a loss of attention to cues with extended exposure. The second mechanism would be more complex and would involve the mpfc, resulting in a LI after a limited amount of exposure to a cue. Presumably with extended exposure the influence of the first mechanism (or the mechanism involved in automatic processes) in animals without lesions suppresses the influence of the second process (or controlled), and thus results in LI with both limited and extended exposure to saccharin prior to taste aversion conditioning.
Dorsal striatum and mpfc play an important role in the cognitive control of actions [18, 39–41], and numerous neural mechanisms are involved in the underlying attentional processes. In this regard, Díaz et al. [10] presented some findings that support this hypothesis in animals with dms lesion. They found that lesion of this structure had effects on a possible suppression of the influence of the automatic processes. Furthermore, they found the expression of LI after extended exposure was similar to that after a limited exposure to a cue, suggesting a release of the controlled processes after dms lesion. These data are also congruent with Molero-Chamizo results [42]. This study found no effect in LI after dorsal striatum lesion. We found the same result with a limited expose to future CS, but not with an extended exposure. Taken as a whole, dorsal striatum could be involved in habituation to future CS, with dms and dls playing different roles in LI. While dms is involved in inhibition of the second mechanism (controlled processes), dls would be essential to the expression of the automatic one. However, the region that is responsible for this second mechanism remained to be identified. Results from our experiments identify the mpfc as the region responsible for this mechanism. It is probable that each mechanism involves a distributed network of corticostriatal circuits [18, 19, 31, 43–47], emphasizing specific circuits. It is also possible that attentional processes are closely linked to other cortical and subcortical phenomenon [23, 48] where the dorsal striatum and mpfc play an important role in the activity of both mechanisms.
In summary, our results provide the first demonstration of a specific corticostriatal activity in LI. These data expand our understanding of how the mpfc and dls could work to inhibit each other in regard to control the attentional processes. Given the importance of LI to gate out sensory and cognitive information [49–51], these results have important implications in understanding possible risk factors and symptoms of severe mental illness.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We thank Dr. Michael Grannan and Dra. Catherine Thorn for their suggestions and the critical reading of an early version of the manuscript. This research was supported by PSI2015-65500-P grant (MINECO, FEDER, UE). The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This research was supported by PSI2015-65500-P grant (MINECO, FEDER, UE).
References
- 1.Lubow RE, Weiner I. Latent Inhibition: Cognition, Neuroscience, and Applications to Schizophrenia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bouton ME. Contex, Time, and Memory Retrieval in the Interference Paradigms of Pavlovian Learning. Psichol Bull 1993; 114: 80–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Miller RR, Matzel LD. The Comparator Hypothesis: A Response Rule for The Expression of Associations In: Bower GH, editor. The psychology of learning and motivation vol. 22 San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1988. pp. 51–92. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Escobar M, Arcediano F, Miller RR. Latent inhibition and contextual associations. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2002; 28: 123–136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lubow RE. Latent inhibition and conditional attention theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mackintosh NJ. A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 1975; 82: 276–298. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pearce JM, Hall G. A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol Rev. 1980; 87: 532–552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wagner AR. SOP A model of automatic processing in animal behavior In: Spear NE, Miller RR, editors. Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1981. pp. 5–24. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Díaz E, Medellín J, Sánchez N, Vargas JP, López JC. Involvement of D1 and D2 dopamine receptor in the retrieval processes in latent inhibition. Psychopharm. 2015; 232: 4337–4346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Díaz E, Vargas JP, Quintero E, de la Casa LG, O’Donnell P, and Lopez JC. Differential implication of dorsolateral and dorsomedial srtiatum in encoding and recovery processes of latent inhibition. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014; 111: 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.LaBerge D. Acquisition of automatic processing of perceptual and associative learning In: Rabbitt PMA, Dornic S, editors. Attention and performance V. New York: Accademic; 1975. pp. 50–64. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol Rev. 1977; 84: 1–66. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Shiffrin RM, Schneider W. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychol Rev. 1977; 84: 127–190. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hikosaka O, Isoda M. Switching from automatic to controlled behavior: cortico-basal ganglia mechanisms. Trends Cog Sci. 2010; 14: 154–161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lara AH, Wallis JD. Executive control processes underlying multi-item working memory. Nat Neurosci. 2014; 17: 876–83. doi: 10.1038/nn.3702 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lebedev MA, Messinger A, Kralik JD, Wise SP. Representation of attended versus remembered locations in prefrontal cortex. PLoS Biol. 2004; 2; p.e365 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020365 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jahanshahi M, Obeso I, Rothwell JC, Obeso JA. A fronto-striato-subthalamic-pallidal network for goal-directed and habitual inhibition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015; 16: 719–32. doi: 10.1038/nrn4038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Thorn CA, Atallah H, Howe M, Graybiel AM. Differential Dynamics of Activity Changes in Dorsolateral and Dorsomedial Striatal Loops during Learning. Neuron. 2010; 66: 781–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Daw ND, Niv Y, Dayan P. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8: 1704–11. doi: 10.1038/nn1560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Badre D, Frank MJ. Mechanisms of hierarchical reinforcement learning in cortico-striatal circuits 2: Evidence from fMRI. Cereb Cortex. 2012; 22: 527–536. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cools R. Dopaminergic control of the striatum for high-level cognition. Curr Op Neurobiol. 2011; 21: 402–407. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: Devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013; 37: 1946–1954. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.George DN, Duffaud AM, Pothuizen HHJ, Haddon JE, Killcross S. Lesions to the ventral, but not the dorsal, medial prefrontal cortex enhance latent inhibition. Eur J Neurosci. 2010; 31: 1474–1482. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07178.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Landau SM, Lal R, O’Neil JP, Baker S, Jagust WJ. Striatal dopamine and working memory. Cereb Cortex, 2009; 19: 445–454. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Willcocks AL, Mcnally GP. The role of medial prefrontal cortex in extinction and reinstatement of alcohol-seeking in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2013: 37: 259–268. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Balleine BW, Liljeholm M, Ostlund SB. The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning. Behav Brain Res. 2009; 199: 43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lopez JC, Karlsson RM, O’Donnell P. Dopamine D2 Modulation of Sign and Goal Tracking in Rats. Neuropsychopharm. 2015; 40: 2096–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Christakou A, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Prefrontal Cortical-Ventral Striatal Interactions Involved in Affective Modulation of Attentional Performance: Implications for Corticostriatal Circuit Function. J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 773–780. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0949-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Homayoun H, Moghaddam B. Differential representation of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer by prefrontal cortex subregions and striatum. Eur J Neurosci. 2009; 29: 1461–1476. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06679.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brown P, Marsden CD. What do the basal ganglia do? Lancet. 1998; 351: 1801–1804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Haber SN, Fudge JL, McFarland NR. Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. J Neurosci. 2000; 20: 2369–2382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Orlando: Academic Press; 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ellenbroek BA, Knobbout DA, Cools AR. The role of mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine in latent inhibition as measured with the conditioned taste aversion paradigm. Psychopharm. 1997; 129: 112–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Jeanblanc J, Hoeltzel A, Louilot A. Differential involvement of dopamine in the anterior and posterior parts of the dorsal striatum in latent inhibition. Neurosci. 2003; 118: 233–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lacroix L, Broersen LM, Feldon J, Weiner I. Effects of local infusions of dopaminergic drugs into the medial prefrontal cortex of rats on latent inhibition, prepulse inhibition and amphetamine induced activity. Behav Brain Res. 2000; 107: 111–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nelson AJD, Thur KE, Marsden CA, Cassaday HJ. Catecholaminergic depletion within the prelimbic medial prefrontal cortex enhances latent inhibition. Neurosci. 2010; 170: 99–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Weiner I. The ‘two-headed’ latent inhibition model of schizophrenia: Modeling positive and negative symptoms and their treatment. Psychopharm. 2003; 169: 257–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Killcross S, Coutureau E. Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex. 2003; 13: 400–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ito M, Doya K. Multiple representations and algorithms for reinforcement learning in the cortico-basal ganglia circuit. Curr Op Neurobiol. 2011; 21: 368–373. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006; 7: 464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Reward-guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: The integrative functions of cortico-basal ganglia networks. Eur J Neurosci. 2008; 28: 1437–1448. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06422.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Molero-Chamizo A. Excitotoxic lesion of the posterior part of the dorsal striatum does not affect the typically dopaminergic phenomenon of latent inhibition in conditioned taste aversion. Neurosci Res. 2015; 91: 8–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci. 1990; 13: 266–271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Houk JC, Wise SP. Feature article: Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: Their role in planning and controlling action. Cereb Cortex. 1995; 5: 95–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ikeda H, Saigusa T, Kamei J, Koshikawa N, Cools AR. Spiraling dopaminergic circuitry from the ventral striatum to dorsal striatum is an effective feed-forward loop. Neurosci. 2013; 241: 126–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Redgrave P, Vautrelle N, Reynolds JNJ. Functional properties of the basal ganglia’s re-entrant loop architecture: Selection and reinforcement. Neurosci. 2011; 198: 138–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Shepherd GMG. Corticostriatal connectivity and its role in disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013; 14: 278–91. doi: 10.1038/nrn3469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Agnoli L, Mainolfi P, Invernizzi RW, Carli M. Dopamine D1-like and D2-like receptors in the dorsal striatum control different aspects of attentional performance in the five-choice serial reaction time task under a condition of increased activity of corticostriatal inputs. Neuropsychopharm. 2013; 38: 701–714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Quintero E, Díaz E, Vargas JP, de la Casa LG, López JC. Ventral subiculum involvement in latent inhibition context specificity. Physiol Behav. 2011; 102: 414–420. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Quintero E, Díaz E, Vargas JP, Schmajuk N, López JC, de la Casa LG. Effects of context novelty vs. familiarity on latent inhibition with a conditioned taste aversion procedure. Behav Process. 2011; 86: 242–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Vargas JP, Díaz E, Portavella M, López JC. Animal Models of Maladaptive Traits: Disorders in Sensorimotor Gating and Attentional Quantifiable Responses as Possible Endophenotypes. Front Psychol. 2016; 7: 1–9, 2016. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.