The central question of this debate is whether dynamic cerebral autoregulation (dCA) should be quantified using spontaneous or induced blood pressure fluctuations. Simpson & Claassen (2018) have argued in favour of the latter based on the analogy that induced blood pressure fluctuations would provide ‘cobblestones, potholes and speed bumps’ that engages a car's suspension system to give insight into its properties. At first blush their argument appears to be a clincher, but the analogy is misleading because a car's suspension system scarcely resembles the cardiovascular system. Several points warrant specific mention.
First, we agree that testing the response characteristics of a system does require an input, but it is remiss to assume that spontaneous blood pressure (BP) fluctuations are not inputs. Spontaneous BP fluctuations are associated with poor neurological outcomes in patients with acute stroke (Manning et al. 2014; Lattanzi et al. 2015) suggesting they are physiologically significant inputs. Also, when dealing with the concept of BP variability, time scale matters as the physiological factors that dominate cerebral haemodynamics are frequency dependent (Kontos, 1989; Zhang et al. 2009; Tzeng & Ainslie, 2014). This means that the haemodynamic response to rapid blood pressure transients may not reflect mechanisms buffering against low or very low (i.e. slower) BP changes.
Consistent with this notion, cerebral blood flow variance associated with external perturbation manoeuvres, such as those during oscillatory lower‐body negative pressure (LBNP) (Tzeng et al. 2011), thigh cuff deflation (Tzeng et al. 2014) and other orthostatic manoeuvres (Tzeng et al. 2014) might also be affected by compliance properties of cerebrovasculature (Olufsen et al. 2002). So testing the system with large and abrupt stimuli (i.e. cobblestones, potholes and speed bumps) that are associated with a greater rate of change in BP (i.e. ∆BP/dt) might tell you more about the visco‐elastic properties of the cerebrovascular system than the capacity of cerebral arteries to actively dilate and constrict in response to changes in BP (Tzeng et al. 2011).
As mentioned in our initial statement, a major problem with inducing BP changes is that other physiological control mechanisms will also be stimulated, thus producing alterations in respiration (and therefore ), stroke volume, heart rate and sympathetic nervous system activity, all of which influence dCA (Willie et al. 2014). The corollary to this, and the observation above that spontaneous fluctuations also generate a BP ‘input’, is that perhaps the discussion should not focus only on ‘spontaneous versus induced’. Perhaps spontaneous fluctuations are simply one of many BP input modalities that have their own specific physiological context (Tzeng et al. 2012).
An alternative view on this key methodological aspect of dCA assessment is the need to clarify what is meant by better ‘signal‐to‐noise ratio’ associated with induced changes in BP. Here it is important to clarify what we designate as ‘noise’. One general type of noise affecting all physiological measurements could be described as ‘physical noise’, involving electrical interference, distortions resulting from non‐invasive measurements (transcranial doppler and finger BP), or artefacts caused by subject movement, etc. This kind of noise can often be spotted at the data editing stages and in some cases minimised by filtering or spike removal.
A much more serious (but more interesting) kind of noise is what could be termed ‘physiological noise’ introduced by complex physiological interactions. These would involve mental activation, temperature changes, baroreceptor reflex activation, or autonomic nervous system effects (Willie et al. 2014). Our contention is that in dCA assessment, ‘physiological noise’ is more problematic than ‘physical’ noise, tilting the balance towards the use of spontaneous fluctuations as a more physiologically stable condition for dCA assessment.
Notwithstanding the above, we agree that consensus is required around what is meant by ‘best’ method for assessing dCA. The key priority should be on establishing construct validity, defined as ‘the degree to which a test measures what it claims, or purports, to be measuring’. The evaluation of construct validity can be achieved through, for example, iterative assessment of convergent validity (the extent to which two measures of constructs that are theoretically related are in fact related) and discriminant validity (whether two theoretically unrelated measures are indeed unrelated). Currently none of the dCA measures that are in popular use (spontaneous or induced) exhibit convergent validity (Tzeng et al. 2012) so we clearly have a construct problem. For all the reasons raised in our original and current submission, we maintain that the resolution is unlikely to come from simply forcing the cerebrovascular system with induced BP fluctuations.
Call for comments
Readers are invited to give their views on this and the accompanying CrossTalk articles in this issue by submitting a brief (250 word) comment. Comments may be submitted up to 6 weeks after publication of the article, at which point the discussion will close and the CrossTalk authors will be invited to submit a ‘LastWord’. Please email your comment, including a title and a declaration of interest, to jphysiol@physoc.org. Comments will be moderated and accepted comments will be published online only as ‘supporting information’ to the original debate articles once discussion has closed.
Additional information
Competing interests
None declared.
Author contributions
Both authors contributed equally to the conception, writing and editing of this article. Both authors have approved the final version of the manuscript and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work. All persons designated as authors qualify for authorship, and all those who qualify for authorship are listed.
Funding
Y.C.T. is funded by the New Zealand Ministry of Business, Innovation and Employment (UOOX1506).
Linked articles This article is part of a CrossTalk debate. Click the links to read the other articles in this debate: https://doi.org/10.1113/JP273899, https://doi.org/10.1113/JP273900 and https://doi.org/10.1113/JP275041.
Edited by: Francisco Sepúlveda & Kim Dora
References
- Kontos HA (1989). Validity of cerebral arterial blood flow calculations from velocity measurements. Stroke 20, 1–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lattanzi S, Cagnetti C, Provinciali L & Silvestrini M (2015). Blood pressure variability and clinical outcome in patients with acute intracerebral hemorrhage. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 24, 1493–1499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning L, Hirakawa Y, Arima H, Wang X, Chalmers J, Wang J, Lindley R, Heeley E, Delcourt C, Neal B, Lavados P, Davis SM, Tzourio C, Huang Y, Stapf C, Woodward M, Rothwell PM, Robinson TG & Anderson CS; INTERACT2 investigators (2014). Blood pressure variability and outcome after acute intracerebral haemorrhage: a post‐hoc analysis of INTERACT2, a randomised controlled trial. Lancet Neurol 13, 364–373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olufsen MS, Nadim A & Lipsitz LA (2002). Dynamics of cerebral blood flow regulation explained using a lumped parameter model. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 282, R611–R622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson D & Claassen J (2018). CrossTalk opposing view: dynamic cerebral autoregulation should be quantified using induced (rather than spontaneous) blood pressure fluctuations. J Physiol 596, 7–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzeng YC & Ainslie PN (2014). Blood pressure regulation IX: cerebral autoregulation under blood pressure challenges. Eur J Appl Physiol 114, 545–559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzeng YC, Ainslie PN, Cooke WH, Peebles KC, Willie CK, MacRae BA, Smirl JD, Horsman HM & Rickards CA (2012). Assessment of cerebral autoregulation: the quandary of quantification. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 303, H658–H671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzeng YC, Chan GS, Willie CK & Ainslie PN (2011). Determinants of human cerebral pressure–flow velocity relationships: new insights from vascular modelling and Ca2+ channel blockade. J Physiol 589, 3263–3274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzeng YC, MacRae BA, Ainslie PN & Chan GS (2014). Fundamental relationships between blood pressure and cerebral blood flow in humans. J Appl Physiol (1985) 117, 1037–1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willie CK, Tzeng YC, Fisher JA & Ainslie PN (2014). Integrative regulation of human brain blood flow. J Physiol 592, 841–859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang R, Behbehani K & Levine BD (2009). Dynamic pressure–flow relationship of the cerebral circulation during acute increase in arterial pressure. J Physiol 587, 2567–2577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]