Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jan 3.
Published in final edited form as: Obesity (Silver Spring). 2017 Oct 17;25(12):2018–2044. doi: 10.1002/oby.21940

A systematic review of calorie labeling and modified calorie labeling interventions: Impact on consumer and restaurant behavior

Sara N Bleich 1, Christina D Economos 2, Marie L Spiker 3, Kelsey Vercammen 1, Eric M VanEpps 4, Jason P Block 5, Brian Elbel 6, Mary Story 7, Christina A Roberto 8
PMCID: PMC5752125  NIHMSID: NIHMS929097  PMID: 29045080

Abstract

Background

Evidence on the effects of restaurant calorie labeling on consumer and restaurant behavior is mixed. This paper examined: 1) consumer responses to calorie information alone or compared to modified calorie information, and 2) changes in restaurant offerings following or in advance of menu labeling implementation.

Methods

We searched PubMed, Web of Science, Policy File and PAIS International to identify restaurant calorie labeling studies through October 1, 2016, that measured calories ordered, consumed, or available for purchase on restaurant menus. We also searched reference lists of calorie labeling articles.

Results

Fifty-three studies were included: 18 in real-world restaurants, 9 in cafeterias, and 21 in laboratory or simulation settings. Five examined restaurant offerings.

Conclusion

Due to a lack of well-powered studies with strong designs, the degree to which menu labeling encourages lower calorie purchases and whether that translates to a healthier population is unclear. Although there is limited evidence that menu labeling affects calories purchased at fast-food restaurants, some evidence demonstrates that it lowers calories purchased at certain types of restaurants and in cafeteria settings. The limited data on modified calorie labels find that such labels can encourage lower-calorie purchases, but may not differ in effects relative to calorie labels alone.

Keywords: menu labeling, calorie labeling, nutrition labeling, food policy

INTRODUCTION

Obesity is associated with adverse health consequences(14) and substantial healthcare costs(5). Overconsumption of calories has been a key driver of rising obesity(6,7), and dining out is thought to play a significant role. Because people substantially underestimate the calories in prepared food(8), restaurant menu labeling was implemented in several cities and states(9,10) and is included in the 2010 Affordable Care Act(11,12). Chain restaurants, grocery stores, and other food retail establishments with 20 or more locations must post calorie information on their menus by May 2018 along with the statement: “2,000 calories a day is used for general nutrition advice, but calorie needs vary.” The hope is such information will encourage consumers to choose, and restaurants to offer, lower calorie items.

This paper synthesizes the evidence on the effectiveness of menu labeling. Although we identified nine prior menu labeling reviews(1321), we extend this research by reviewing: 1) newer studies; 2) research across restaurant, cafeteria, and laboratory settings; 3) studies comparing responses to calorie information (e.g., 400 calories) relative to modified calorie information or nutrition symbols (e.g., traffic light labels); and 4) studies of menu offerings following local menu labeling regulations and in advance of national regulations.

METHODS

We searched PubMed, Web of Science, Policy File and PAIS International for all articles published through October 1, 2016, using a combination of the terms: ”restaurant,” “cafeteria,” “food service,” “fast food,” “labeling,” “calories” and “energy.” (See Appendix for search details). We also examined reference lists of calorie labeling articles. After removing duplicate studies, one author (K.V.) screened titles and abstracts and reviewed the full text for inclusion. Another author (S.B.) confirmed inclusion of these studies, and a third author (C.R.) adjudicated differences. Included studies had to examine the effects of calorie information displayed on menus using calories offered, ordered, purchased, or consumed as an outcome. Studies of menu offerings included research conducted before and after local menu labeling regulations were implemented and in advance of national calorie labeling implementation. We did not examine the effect of labeling on intake of other nutrients, although some study menus displayed other nutrition information (e.g., sodium). We also included studies that compared calorie information to modified presentations of calorie information such as traffic light labels, total recommended daily calorie statements, and physical activity labels (presenting the amount of time one would have to exercise to burn off the calories eaten). We included studies conducted among adults, adolescents, and children. Studies were excluded for the following reasons: 1) did not report calories offered, ordered, purchased, or consumed as an outcome; 2) did not use restaurant-like menus or used menus with a small number of items (<6 items) that may not generalize to typical restaurant settings; 3) only compared self-reported calorie label users to non-users; 4) evaluated nutrition labels on packaged foods; 5) studied another intervention (e.g., price changes, educational sessions) in combination with calorie information such that the calorie label effect could not be isolated; or 6) tested whether participants changed menu orders after being asked to immediately re-order from the same menu containing calorie information.

Tables 14 present details of each study’s design, methods, and outcomes based on setting. We summarize each study below based on setting (restaurant, cafeteria, or laboratory/simulation) and grouped by study design (e.g. randomized controlled trial (RCT). Finally, we describe studies of changes in restaurant offerings after enacted or anticipated menu labeling regulations. Results reported as kilojoules have been converted to calories.

Table 1.

Real-world restaurant studies of menu labeling

Author, Year Setting and Sample size Study design Data Collection Methods Outcome reported in this table Mean Calories purchased without labeling Mean Calories purchased during labeling intervention Difference: (labeled - unlabeled) Significance of Effect & Direction

Randomized controlled trial

Ellison et al., 2013 138 customers at a full service restaurant on Oklahoma State University campus. Randomized controlled field experiment.
3 conditions:
1) control; 2) calorie labels; 3) calorie labels plus traffic lights
Collected sales data Unadjusted mean total calories ordered Control: Calorie Labels: Null
765 817 +522
Measures of variation not reported for this study Calories + traffic light labels:
696 −692

Pre/Post design with comparison site(s)
Bollinger et al., 2011 All 222 Starbucks locations in NYC, and all 94 Starbucks locations in Boston and Philadelphia (control sites). Over 100 million sales transactions. Natural experiment. Difference-in-differences analysis.
Cross-sectional data collection 3 months before and eleven months after calorie labeling law implemented in an intervention and control cities.
Analyzed electronic sales transaction data from Starbucks locations
Collected in-store customer surveys in intervention (Seattle) and control cities.
Mean total calories per transaction 247 232 −15*2 Significant reduction in calories
Measures of variation not reported for this study

Cantor et al., 2015 7,699 adult customers in low-income, racial/ethnic minority communities in NYC, Newark, and Jersey City.
19 unique restaurant locations of 4 large fast food chains (McDonalds, Burger King, Wendy’s, KFC)
were surveyed in 2008 and 60 unique restaurant locations were surveyed in 2013–2014.
Natural experiment. Difference-in-differences analysis.
Cross-sectional data collection 4 weeks before calorie labeling law implemented and 4 weeks after as well as the following post-labeling time points: 4.5 years, 5 years, and 5.5 years.
Collected customer receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant.
Participants in 2013–2014 were also invited to participate in a separate follow-up telephone survey.
Adjusted mean total calories purchased
Covariates: age, sex, race/ethnicity, restaurant chain, and whether meal was to go or eat in
Newark (control): Newark Post 4 wks: Null
773 756 −172
Newark and Jersey City Post 4.5 years:
845 +722
Newark and Jersey City Post 5 years:
802 +292
Newark and Jersey City Post 5.5 years:
857 +842
NYC (intervention): NYC Post 4 wks:
796 783 −132
Measures of variation not reported for this study NYC + McDonald’s Post 4.5 years:
839 +432
NYC + McDonald’s Post 5 years:
835 +392
NYC + McDonald’s Post 5.5 years:
804 +82

Elbel et al., 2009 1,156 adult customers in low-income, racial/ethnic minority communities.
19 neighborhood-matched fast-food restaurants representing 4 large chains (McDonalds, BK, Wendy’s, KFC). 14 restaurants in NYC (labeled), 5 in Newark (unlabeled).
Natural experiment. Difference-in-differences analysis.
Cross-sectional data collection 4 weeks before and 4 weeks after calorie labeling law implemented in an intervention and control city.
Collected customer receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant. Adjusted mean total calories purchased.
Covariates: age, sex, race/ethnicity, and whether food was to go or eat in.
Newark (control): Newark (control): Null
823 [95%CI: 802–890] 826 [95%CI: 746–906] +32
NYC (control): NYC (intervention):
825 [95%CI: 779–870] 846 [95%CI: 758–889] +212

Elbel et al., 2011 349 children and adolescents in low-income, racial/ethnic minority communities in NYC and Newark.
19 neighborhood-matched fast-food restaurants representing 4 large chains (McDonalds, BK, Wendy’s, KFC). 14 restaurants in NYC (labeled), 5 in Newark (unlabeled).
Natural experiment. Difference-in-differences analysis.
Cross-sectional data collection 4 weeks before and 4 weeks after calorie labeling law implemented in an intervention and control city.
Collected customer receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant. Unadjusted mean total calories purchased by children in full sample Newark (control): Newark (control): Null
611 (SD: 366) 673 (SD: 265) +622
NYC (intervention): NYC (intervention):
643 (SD: 334) 652 (SD: 330) +92

Elbel et al., 2013 2,083 adult customers
from 23 McDonald’s and Burger King locations in Philadelphia (labeled) and Baltimore (unlabeled) before and after calorie labeling law implemented. Low-income participants were oversampled.
Natural experiment. Difference-in-differences analysis.
Cross-sectional data collection two months before and four months after calorie labeling law implemented in an intervention and control city.
Collected customer receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant.
Random-digit-dialing telephone interviews to assess self-reported exposure to menu labeling and restaurant visits.
Adjusted mean total calories purchased.
Covariates: gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, restaurant chain, being overweight or obese
Baltimore (control): Baltimore (control): Null
992 940 −522

Philadelphia (intervention): Philadelphia (intervention): Null
959 904 −552
Measures of variation not reported for this study

Finkelstein et al., 2011 14 Taco Time locations: 7 in King County, WA (labeled) and 7 in adjacent counties (unlabeled) Natural experiment. Difference-in-differences analysis.
Compared intervention and control cities before and after calorie labeling law implemented. Two post-labeling time points included.
Post-labeling time point 1: law in effect but drive-thru menus not changed.
Post-labeling time point 2: drive-thru menus changed.
Total monthly restaurant sales transaction data between January 2008 and January 2010 (13 months after law implemented). Unadjusted mean total calories per transaction Adjacent counties (control): Adjacent counties (control): Null
1391 Post 1: 1392 +12
Post 2: 1376 −152
King County (intervention): King County (intervention):
1211 Post 1: 1217 +62
Measures of variation not reported for this study Post 2: 1214 +32

Tandon et al., 2011 133 families with 6–11 year old children in King County, WA (n=75, labeled county) and San Diego County, CA (n=58, control county). Prospective cohort, collected data before and after implementation of calorie labeling in King County comparing intervention and control city. Participants given $10 gift card to a fast-food restaurant.
Collected mailed customer receipts after visits to restaurants; phone interviews
Unadjusted mean total calories purchased for parents and children San Diego County (control): San Diego County (control): Null
Children: Children:
984 949 −352
Parents: Parents:
895 789 −1062
King County (intervention): King County (intervention):
Children: Children:
823 822 −12
Parents: Parents:
823 720 −1032

Pre/Post design without comparison site(s)

Downs et al., 2013 1094 adult customers at 2 McDonalds locations in NYC. Cross-sectional data collection 2 months before and 2 months after calorie labeling law implemented.
In addition, participants randomized to: given slip of paper with daily recommended calories OR meal recommended calories OR no slip given before entering McDonald’s.
Collected customer receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant. Adjusted mean total calories among group exposed to calorie labeling
Covariates: day of week, gender, race/ethnicity, age, and location of restaurant
Control: Control: Null
812 (SE: 36) 833 (SE: 41) +212
Per-Meal Calorie Recommendation: Per-Meal Calorie Recommendation
880 (SE: 34) 830 (SE: 38) −502
Daily Calorie Recommendation: Daily Calorie Recommendation
865 (SE: 34) 897 (SE: 35) +322

Dumanovsky et al., 2011 7309 adult customers in 2007 and 8489 in 2009.
168 restaurant outlets of 11 fast food chains in NYC.
Cross-sectional data collection one year before and nine months after calorie labeling law. Collected customer receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant. Unadjusted mean total calories purchased 2007 Overall: 2009 Overall: Overall: Mixed
828 [95%CI: 807, 849] 846 [95%CI: 826, 866] +182
McDonald’s McDonald’s McDonald’s
829 [95%CI: 803, 836] 785 [95%CI: 759, 812] −44*2
Burger King Burger King Burger King
924 [95%CI: 889, 959] 967 [95%CI: 928, 1007] 432
Wendy’s Wendy’s Wendy’s
858 [95%CI: 807, 909] 821 [95%CI: 771, 871] −372
Subway Subway Subway
749 [95%CI: 722, 776] 882 [95%CI: 860, 905] 133*2
Au Bon Pain Au Bon Pain Au Bon Pain
555 [95%CI 513, 596] 475 [95%CI 455, 494] −80*2
KFC KFC KFC
927 [95%CI: 896, 957] 868 [95%CI: 842, 894] −59*2
Popeye’s Popeye’s Popeye’s
949 [95%CI: 884, 1013] 975 [95%CI: 943, 1006] 262
Domino’s Domino’s Domino’s
1309 [95%CI: 979, 1640] 1029 [95%CI: 862, 1196] −2802
Pizza Hut Pizza Hut Pizza Hut
1039 [95%CI: 963, 1115] 943 [95%CI: 837, 1049] −962
Papa John’s Papa John’s Papa John’s
623 [95%CI: 469, 777] 571 [95%CI: 487, 656] −512
Taco Bell Taco Bell Taco Bell
773 [95%CI: 670, 876] 808 [95%CI: 765, 851] 352

Holmes et al., 2013 Sit-down restaurant at a private club in a US town (population of 53,311 people).
1275 kids’ meal orders.
Longitudinal design with control and 3 types of menu labels, which were each introduced for 2 months.
Labeling appeared only on combo meals (453 combos were ordered out of 1275 transactions).
Restaurant sales data Unadjusted mean total calories purchased in kids’ meals Baseline (no label): Calorie and fat labeling: Null
611 601 −102
Measures of variation not reported for this study Healthy symbol:
607 −42
Nutrition bargain price:
605 −62

Ge et al., 2014 Table service restaurant on campus of Purdue University Longitudinal design with control and 3 types of menus, each introduced for 1 week
Restaurant specials were not included
Analyzed lunch sales transaction data Unadjusted mean total calories purchased Control: Calories-only: Calories-only: Significant reduction in calories
856 730 −126*2
Measures of variation not reported for this study Healthy Symbol: Healthy Symbol:
825 −312
Nutrient List: Nutrient List:
771 −852

Krieger et al., 2013 7325 customers ages ≥14
40 fast-food and 10 coffee restaurants representing 10 restaurant chains in King County, WA.
Cross-sectional data collection before and after calorie labeling law.
Collected data at two post-labeling time points: 4–6 months post and 16–18 months post.
Collected customer receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant. Unadjusted mean total calories purchased Overall food chains: Food chains Mixed
Post 1:
909 [95%CI: 876, 941] 921 [95% CI:888, 954] +132
Post 2:
870 [95% CI:842, 899] −38*2
Burger chains Burger chains
Post 1
905 [95%CI: 830, 979] 895 [95%CI: 834, 957] −92
Post 2
892 [95%CI: 831, 953] −132
Sandwich chains Sandwich chains
Post 1
872 [95%CI: 842, 919] 907 [95%CI: 866, 947] 352
Post 2
862 [95%CI: 820, 904] −102
Taco chains Taco chains
Post 1
980 [95%CI: 936, 1023] 971 [95%CI: 885, 1057] −92
Post 2
867 [95%CI: 816, 918] −113*2
Coffee chains: Coffee chains
Post 1:
154 [95%CI: 43,166] 144 [95% CI:120,168] −112
Post 2:
132 [95%CI:117,147] −22.1*2

Pulos et al., 2010 6 sit-down restaurants in Pierce County, WA. ~16,000 entrees purchased Cross-sectional data collection 30 days before and 30 days after voluntary calorie labeling implemented. Itemized historical restaurant sales data of entrée items only. Adjusted mean total calories purchased
Covariates:
entrée cost
Not reported Not reported Overall: Mixed
Measures of variation not reported for this study (except for overall difference of means) −15* (95% CI: −23, −7)1
Restaurant 1:
−16.8*2
Restaurant 2:
−55.6*2
Restaurant 3:
−53.6*2
Restaurant 4:
−20.6*2
Restaurant 5:
12.12
Restaurant 6:
−1.02

Schwartz et al., 2012 (Experiment 2) 399 customers at a Chinese fast-food restaurant on the campus of Duke University and its adjacent Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina. Data collection before and after calorie labeling implemented on menu. Collected restaurant sales transaction data. Unadjusted mean total calories purchased Baseline (control): Calorie Labeling: +132 Null
1020 1033
Measures of variation not reported for this study

Cross-sectional comparing labeled vs. unlabeled site

Auchincloss et al., 2013 648 adult customers (327 at intervention site; 321 at control site).
7 outlets of one full-service chain restaurant; 2 outlets in Philadelphia with menu labeling, 5 outlets outside of Philadelphia with no labeling (control sites).
Cross-sectional, comparing an intervention and control city. Collected customer transaction receipts and conducted surveys upon exiting restaurant. Unadjusted mean total calories ordered.
Covariates
age, gender, race/ethnicity, income, education, day of week, frequency of dining out at sit-down restaurant, and body size
Control: Philadelphia: Significant reduction in calories
1891 (SD: 785) 1778 (SD: 824) −155* [95%CI: −284,−27]1

Rendell et al., 2014 245 adult customers from two Cosi chain restaurants located in New Rochelle, New York (labeled) and Stamford, Connecticut (unlabeled) Cross-sectional, comparing an intervention and control city Conducted surveys upon exiting Unadjusted mean total calories ordered Stamford (Control): New Rochelle (Menu Labeling): Null
743 (SD: 169) 705 (SD: 173) −392

Quasi-real world interventions

Wisdom et al., 2010 Real-world. 638 diners at 1 fast-food sandwich chain in Pittsburgh, PA Randomized-controlled field experiment.
Two separate studies conducted, but calorie labeling analysis combined because manipulation did not differ across studies.
Factorial design:
2 calorie labeling conditions (label versus no labeling) × 2 daily calorie recommendation information (yes/no) × 3 convenience manipulations (first page of menu had either most caloric, least caloric, or mixed sandwich options and consumers had a choice to order from a second menu if they wanted to see all options (study 1) or see all options on the next page (study 2)
Approached customers upon entering restaurant.
Participants given a menu based on factorial design.
Asked to choose sandwich, side and drink, then given a coupon for that meal to redeem inside.
Participants received free meal in exchange for completing the study.
Adjusted mean total calories purchased
Covariates gender, age, race, study number, and convenience manipulation
Control: Calorie labeling: Significant reduction in calories
851 (SE: 36) 790 (SE: 19) −61*
Daily calorie recommendation:
813 (SE: 19) −38*
Calorie label and daily calorie recommendation:
752 (SE: NR) −99*
N not reported for subgroups so cannot calculate CI for difference of means
*

p <.05

1

Author reported 95% CI for difference in means

2

The study sample was non-independent and we lacked the information to calculate the confidence intervals.

Unadjusted means are reported unless paper only reported adjusted numbers.

An absence of standard deviation, standard errors, or confident intervals indicates it was not reported in the paper.

Table 4.

Restaurant reformulation studies

Author, Year Setting and Sample size Study design Methods Outcome reported in this table Mean Calories (baseline or unlabeled) Mean Calories (follow up or labeled) Difference: (labeled - unlabeled) Significance of Effect & Direction

Pre/Post design with comparison site

Namba et al., 2013 9 restaurants in 2005 and in 2011 after labeling law went into effects.
5 restaurants were in areas subject to labeling laws (case) and 4 were not (control)
Menus from 5 fast-food chains (labeled) were compared with menus from 4 food chains (not labeled) in 2005 and in 2011 Trend analysis to assess whether restaurants subject to labeling laws improved the healthfulness of their menus relative to the restaurants not subject to the labeling law. Mean calories of menu items at labeled restaurants
Mean calories of menu items at non-labeled restaurants
Labeled 2005 Labeled 2011 Null
Entrees: Entrees:
419 (SD: 192) 422 (SD: 186) +32
Sides: Sides:
288 (SD: 140) 264 (SD: 143) −242
Non-labeled 2005 Non-labeled 2011
Entrees: Entrees:
474 (SD: 190) 481 (SD: 199) +72
Sides: Sides:
302 (SD: 139) 287 (SD: 146) −152

Restaurant Trends in presence of menu labeling, without comparison site

Bruemmer et al., 2012 37 chain restaurants (including sit-down and quick-service) of 92 labeled chains in King County, WA.
Analyzed 3941 menu items, including 2300 entrees.
Examined menu items 6 months and 18 months after implementation. Audited menus at restaurants 6 months and 18 months post-implementation Mean entrée item calories at all restaurant types (sit-down + quick-service)
Mean entrée item calories at sit-down restaurants
Mean entrée item calories at quick-service restaurants
6 months post: 18 months post: Significant reduction in calories
818 (SD: 407) 777 (SD: 388) −41(SD:156)*2
Sit down: Sit down:
1044 (SD: 438) 970 (SD: 425) −73(SD:217)*2
Quick service: Quick service:
668 (SD: 304) 650 (SD: 300) −19(SD:91)*2

Restaurant Trends in advance of national menu labeling

Bleich et al., 2015 66 chain restaurants from a nationwide database.
Analyzed 23,066 menu items.
Compared menu items from 2012, 2013, and 2014 Used MenuStat to obtain calorie information from menu items in 2012–2014 Mean calories for all menu items
Mean calories in newly introduced items
Year 2012: Year 2013: Null
345 345 02
Year 2014:
349 +42
Year 2013:
470 399 −71*2
Year 2014:
401 −69*2

Bleich et al., 2016 66 chain restaurants from a nationwide database, 5 of which voluntarily posted calorie labels.
Analyzed 23,066 menu items, 3,675 of which were sold by the five voluntary-label restaurant chains.
Compared menu items from restaurants with voluntary menu labeling vs. without voluntary menu labeling Used MenuStat to obtain calorie information from menu items in 2012–2014 Mean calories for all menu items
Mean calories in new items for that year
No Voluntary Menu Labeling With Voluntary Menu Labeling Significant reduction in calories
Year 2012: Year 2012: −139*2
399 260
Year 2013: Year 2013:
398 262 −136*2
Year 2014: Year 2014:
403 263 −140*2
Year 2012: Year 2012:
519 232 −286*2
Year 2013: Year 2013:
445 263 −182*2
Year 2014: Year 2014:
419 309 −110*2

Wu and Sturm, 2014 213 chain restaurant brands, including 109 with children’s menu data.
Analyzed 26,256 (12,843 at baseline, 13,413 at follow-up) adult entrees and 1,794 (859 at baseline, 935 at follow-up) children’s menu entrees.
Examined menu items before and after passage of Affordable Care Act (calorie posting only implemented in a few locations) Analyzed nutrient content of menu items based on information available on restaurant websites Unadjusted mean entrée item calories in regular menu items at all restaurant types
Unadjusted mean entrée item calories in children’s menu items at all restaurant types
Adjusted mean entrée item calories in children’s menu items at fast food restaurants
Adjusted mean entrée item calories in children’s menu items at upscale restaurants
Year 2010: Year 2011: Null
670 (SD: 398) 670 (SD: 397) 02
462 (SD: 214) 468 (SD: 219) +62
Not reported Not reported −40*2
Not reported Not reported +46*2
*

p <.05

2

The study sample was non-independent and we lacked the information to calculate the confidence intervals.

Unadjusted means are reported unless paper only reported adjusted numbers.

An absence of standard deviation, standard errors, or confident intervals indicates it was not reported in the paper.

RESULTS

Our search yielded 3384 citations across 4 databases (see Appendix for PRIMSA flow diagram). After removal of duplicates (n=568), 2816 titles and abstracts were screened and 2737 were excluded. Following full-text review, 53 articles were included.

Real-World Restaurant Settings

Eighteen of 48 studies evaluated calorie information in real-world restaurant settings (Table 1). There was one RCT(22), one quasi-real world RCT(23), seven natural experiments evaluating menu labeling before and after implementation and compared to control locations (2430), seven studies evaluating labeling before and after implementation without a control comparison (3137), and two using cross-sectional designs to compare labeled vs. unlabeled locations(38,39) Three of these studies included children and/or adolescents(27,30,33).

Randomized controlled trials

Ellison et al. reported no difference in calories ordered after randomizing a sample of 138 customers of a full-service university campus restaurant to menus with either no calorie information, calorie labels, or calorie labels plus traffic lights(22), but the small cell size greatly limits the statistical power of the study.

In two quasi-real world RCTs, Wisdom and colleagues approached 638 customers entering a fast-food sandwich restaurant to complete a survey in exchange for a free meal(23). Using a 2×2×3 design, participants were randomized to either a daily calorie recommendation statement or not, item calorie information or not, and conditions that made healthy sandwiches more or less convenient to order (healthy sandwiches were featured on an initial page and patrons had to open a sealed or unsealed menu to view the remaining sandwiches). The two studies only varied in the strength of the healthy sandwich convenience manipulation, so were combined for analysis. Statistically significantly fewer calories were ordered in both the calorie label and daily calorie recommendation conditions compared to the no information group. The combination of both calorie labels and daily calorie recommendations led to a 100 calorie reduction.

Natural experiment with comparison site(s)

The natural experiment with the strongest design and largest sample size was conducted by Bollinger et al. They analyzed over 100 million transactions before and after the implementation of the New York City (NYC) menu labeling law at multiple Starbucks locations, including control sites in Boston and Philadelphia(24). There was a statistically significant 6% decrease in mean calories per transaction (15 calories on average) in NYC locations driven by changes in food, not beverage, calories.

Another natural experiment with a large sample size and strong design was conducted by Finkelstein and colleagues. They saw no effect of menu labeling over 1 year when evaluating pre/post transaction data from seven locations of a Mexican fast food chain inside King County, Washington (labeled), compared to seven locations adjacent to King County (unlabeled)(25).

Elbel et al. reported no change in calories ordered based on 1,156 surveys of customers exiting fast food restaurants in low-income neighborhoods of NYC (labeled) versus Newark, New Jersey (unlabeled), before and four weeks after labeling(26). Although they reported no decrease in calories ordered among children and adolescents (n=349)(27), the small sample size (e.g., Newark n=49 pre- and n=34 post-labeling) makes it difficult to draw conclusions. A 5-year follow-up study in the same cities found no effect of labeling among adults at four fast-food restaurant chains(28). Elbel and colleagues also observed no decrease in calories ordered in a similar study where they collected 2,083 surveys outside of McDonald’s and Burger King locations in Philadelphia (labeled) compared to Baltimore (unlabeled) two months before and four months after labeling(29). Although these studies have strong designs, they were powered to detect only large effects of calorie labeling (i.e. the first New York City evaluation had 80% power to detect a 125 kcal reduction).

Using a strong longitudinal design, but limited by a small sample size, Tandon and colleagues observed no differences in calories ordered among a sample of 75 parent-child pairs in King County (labeled) compared to 58 parent-child pairs in San Diego County (unlabeled)(30).

Pre/Post design without comparison site(s)

Downs et al. collected data before and after menu labeling implementation in NYC (no control city)(31). In addition, they randomly assigned 1,094 adults entering two McDonald’s locations to receive a paper slip with either recommended daily calories or per meal calories (for women and men) or no recommendation. Neither calorie labeling nor the addition of the recommendation messages had a statistically significant impact on calories purchased.

Dumanovsky et al. gathered receipts from 7,309 fast-food patrons prior to calorie labeling and 8,489 patrons after calorie labeling across 11 fast food chains at 168 locations in NYC(32). There was no overall effect of labeling, but statistically significant declines in calories purchased occurred at McDonald’s, Au Bon Pain, and KFC, while calories ordered at Subway increased.

Holmes et al. circulated menus for 2 months each at a full-service restaurant in a private club that either had: 1) no calorie information), 2) calorie and fat information, 3) an apple symbol added to three combos to denote “healthier” choices, or 4) nutrition bargain prices (the monetary price divided by a nutrition scaling factor)(33). Based on sales data, there was no association between label condition and calories ordered for 1,257 children’s meals.

Schwartz et al. analyzed transaction data for 399 customers eating at a Chinese quick-service chain over several weeks(34). The primary goal of the study was to evaluate a portion downsizing intervention, but calorie labels were introduced during a data collection period. Labels were not associated with decreases in calories ordered or eaten.

Ge and colleagues analyzed lunchtime sales data at a table-service restaurant in Indiana(35). Each of the following interventions were delivered for one week: 1) control, 2) calorie information, 3) healthy symbol, and 4) calorie, fat, cholesterol, sodium and fiber information, alongside daily recommended values. Compared to the control week, calorie labels resulted in a statistically significant decrease in calories purchased. Krieger et al. collected receipts from 7,325 customers ≥14 years old before and after menu labeling implementation at 50 locations of 10 chain restaurants in King County, Washington(36). At 18 months post-labeling, there was a non-statistically significant mean reduction of 38 calories in all food chains (not including coffee chains) and a statistically significant decrease in calories purchased at taco restaurants and coffee chains.

Pulos and Leng conducted a pre/post analysis at 6 full-service restaurants in Pierce County, Washington, 30 days before and after displaying information on calories, fat, sodium, and carbohydrates(37). Beverages, certain side items, and daily specials were unlabeled. An analysis of 16,000 entrée orders revealed statistically significant decreases in calories purchased at four of six restaurants (the average entrée ordered post-labeling had about 15 fewer calories); total calories ordered were not evaluated.

Cross-sectional comparing labeled vs. unlabeled sites

Auchincloss et al.(38) collected dinnertime receipts from 648 customers at two locations of a full-service restaurant chain in Philadelphia (labeled) and five outside of Philadelphia (unlabeled). Customers at labeled restaurants ordered statistically significantly fewer calories (151 calorie reduction) than those at unlabeled restaurants; results held when different propensity scoring methods were used to improve causal inference(40).

Rendell and colleagues asked 127 participants at fast-casual chain restaurant in New York (labeled) and 118 participants at the same restaurant in Connecticut (unlabeled) to complete a survey as they exited the restaurant identifying which lunch items they ordered. The researchers did not observe a statistically significant difference when comparing calories ordered between restaurants over a 6-month period(39).

Cafeteria Settings

There were nine cafeteria studies(4149) (Table 2). Eight evaluated menu labeling before and after implementation (4248) and one was a quasi-real world RTC (49). Eight studies reported fewer calories purchased after labeling(4249).

Table 2.

Real-world cafeteria studies of menu labeling

Author, Year Setting and Sample size Study design Methods Outcome reported in this table Calories purchased: Control Group (unlabeled) Calories purchased: Intervention Group (labeled) Difference: (labeled - unlabeled) Significance of Effect & Direction

Pre/Post design without comparison site

School/University Cafeteria

Chu et al., 2009 1 college dining hall at Ohio State University.
Labeled 12 hot entrees with the following information: calories and serving size, fat, protein, and carbohydrates in grams.
Quasi-experimental, single group interrupted time series.
3 periods:
  1. baseline (14 days)

  2. nutrition labels (14 days)

  3. post-labels (13 days)

Dining hall electronically tracked sales data for the 12 labeled hot entrees Unadjusted mean entrée calories Pre-labeling Difference: Last day of baseline – first day of labeling Significant reduction in calories
647 Not Reported −12*2
Measures of variation not reported for this study

Hammond et al., 2015 159 university students at least 16 years of age.
University cafeteria in southwestern Ontario, Canada.
Naturalistic cohort study.
Data collected on same individuals before and after calorie labeling intervention.
Cafeteria patrons were approached to complete an exit survey at baseline and 6 week follow-up during lunch and dinner hours.
Calories ordered and consumed were based on self-reported items purchased and guided estimates of amount eaten.
Mean total calories ordered and consumed in a meal
Note: Mean calories are unadjusted;
difference in calories are adjusted
Covariates
sex, BMI, race, perceived general health, weight perception, and weight aspiration
Pre-labeling Calorie labeling Significant reduction in calories
Calories ordered: Calories ordered:
825 (SD: 336) 734 (SD: 331) −91*2
Calories consumed: Calories consumed:
769 (SD: 342) 671 (SD: 327) −98*2

Hunsberger et al., 2015 Average of 531 students per day aged 11 to 15 at a rural, low-income middle school cafeteria. In Madras, Oregon. Gathered data 17 days pre-calorie labeling and 17 days post- calorie labeling.
Conducted qualitative interviews with 32 students.
Gross calories served per student were measured each day.
The identical menu was served on days with and without labels.
Unadjusted mean calories ordered per student Pre-labeling Calorie labels Significant reduction in calories
668 [95%CI: 530, 806] 621 [95%CI: 499, 743] −47*1 [95%CI: −77, −18]1

Lillico et al., 2015 299 female university students at least 16 years of age.
University cafeteria in southwestern Ontario, Canada.
Naturalistic cohort study.
Data collected on same individuals before and after calorie labeling intervention.
Cafeteria patrons were approached to complete an exit survey at baseline and follow-up during lunch and dinner hours.
Calories ordered and consumed were based on self-reported items purchased and guided estimates of amount eaten.
Mean total calories consumed
Note: Mean calories are unadjusted;
difference in calories are adjusted
Covariates
eating disturbance, BMI, race, perceived stress level, weight perceptions, and weight aspirations
Pre-labeling Calorie labels Null
661 (SD: 309) 601 (SD: 282) −602

Milich et al., 1976 450 female employees.
Workplace cafeteria at North Carolina Memorial Hospital, Chapel Hill, North Carolina.
Cross-sectional data collection before and after calorie labeling intervention.
2 weeks of baseline, one week of a price increase (5–10 cents on approximately half of the food items – cafeteria management did this, was not part of original study design and not intended to impact healthfulness of purchases) and one week of calorie labeling.
Recorded body size, every food item purchased (excluding sodas and condiments), and total price indicated on cash register every Monday, Wednesday, and Friday. Unadjusted mean total calories purchased, excluding sodas and condiments Pre-labeling Calorie labels Significant reduction in calories
507 459 −482
Unexpected Price Increase (no labels):
525 459 −66*2
Measures of variation not reported for this study

Nikolaou et al., 2014 120 university students.
University cafeteria at the University of Glasgow, Scotland.
Interrupted time-series design.
Year 1: no calorie labels.
Year 2: calorie calorie labels.
Observed and recorded all items on the trays of first 100 meals selected during 14 days identified as having choices with wide calorie-range during each labeling period.
Assessed self-reported mean weight changes (validated with sub-sample of objective weights).
Unadjusted mean calories ordered Pre-labeling Calorie labels: Significant reduction in calories
females: females:
709 (SD: 101) 628 (SD: 105) −81*2
males: males:
734 (SD: 101) 692 (SD: 105) −42*2
Calorie label + daily caloric recommendation
females:
534 (SD: 116) −175*2
males:
622 (SD: 116) −112*2

Workplace Cafeteria

Schmitz and Fielding, 1986 2000 employees, dining on-site.
Company located in southern California.
Cross-sectional data collection before and after calorie labeling intervention. Research assistant recorded every 10th customer’s purchase for 2 weeks pre- and post-calorie labeling Mean total calories purchased per tray Control: Calorie Labeling: Significant reduction in calories
638 (SD: 400) 567 (SD: 353) −71*2

Ussher et al., 2015 2004 staff and visitors dining at a hospital cafeteria in Ireland Cross-sectional data collection before and after calorie labeling intervention. Observed calories purchased during breakfast and lunch meals for 5 days pre-labeling and 6 weeks post labeling Mean total calories purchased per meal Males: Males: Males: Significant reduction in calories
Breakfast: Breakfast: Breakfast:
598 585 −132
Lunch: Lunch: Lunch:
813 622 −191*2
Both: Both: Both:
668 612 −56*2
Females: Females: Females:
Breakfast: Breakfast: Breakfast:
419 406 −132
Lunch: Lunch: Lunch:
635 551 −84*2
Both: Both: Both:
530 496 −342

Quasi-real world interventions

VanEpps et al., 2015 249 corporate employees dining at on-site cafeteria placed lunch orders on a newly launched website.
Company location in Louisville, Kentucky.
Randomized, controlled field experiment.
4 conditions:
  1. no labels

  2. calorie labels,

  3. traffic light labels

  4. calorie labels plus traffic lights

Participants ordered lunches to be picked up from on-site corporate cafeteria via website over 4 week study period. Mean total calories ordered per meal
Covariates
multiple orders by individual participants
Control: Calorie labeling: Significant reduction in calories
601 (SE: 18) 538 (SE: 31) −63* [95%CI: −124, −2]
Traffic light labeling:
532 (SE: 33) −69* [95%CI: −134, −5]
Calorie + traffic light labeling:
528 (SE: 33) −73* [95%CI: −139,−6]
*

p <.05

1

Author reported 95% CI for difference in means

2

The study sample was non-independent and we lacked the information to calculate the confidence intervals.

Unadjusted means are reported unless paper only reported adjusted numbers.

An absence of standard deviation, standard errors, or confident intervals indicates it was not reported in the paper.

School/University Cafeteria, pre/post design without comparison site

Chu et al. examined calories ordered in response to labeling 12 entrées with calories, serving size, fat, protein, and carbohydrates in a university cafeteria(45). Sales data were collected over six weeks (2 weeks each for baseline, labels, and no labels). After labeling, there was a statistically significant 12 calorie decrease in average entrée calories ordered; information on overall calories purchased was not available.

Hammond et al. conducted a longitudinal study in a university cafeteria where 159 students completed surveys during a one-week baseline period followed six weeks later by a one-week period when calorie labels were displayed during lunch and dinner(44). Calorie labels were associated with a statistically significant decrease of 91 calories ordered and 98 calories consumed (based on self-reported intake).

Hunsberger and colleagues examined calorie labeling in a rural, low-income middle school cafeteria, with a baseline period of one month, followed by one month of calorie labeling(42). Consumption was measured at the group level by weighing food before and after meal service. There was a statistically significant drop in estimated mean calories ordered per student (based on an average of 531 students daily) from 668 to 621.

In one study, 299 female undergraduates completed a survey one month before and one week after calorie labels were displayed in a cafeteria. Results revealed a non-statistically significant mean reduction of 58 calories post labeling(44).

In one of the earliest studies of calorie labeling, Milich et al. studied 450 female employees at a hospital cafeteria(46). Two weeks of baseline data collection were followed by one week of an unexpected price increase (5–10 cents on about half of the items) followed by one week of calorie labels. The mean calories purchased during the baseline, price increase, and calorie information periods were 507, 525, and 459 respectively. Calorie labels were associated with statistically significantly fewer calories purchased relative to the price increase week (p=0.008) (a more valid comparison because prices were held constant in both weeks) and marginally statistically significantly fewer calories purchased relative to no intervention (p=0.057) (when prices also differed).

Nikolaou and colleagues followed 120 students dining at a college cafeteria over two years(43). No calories were displayed in year one, calorie labels were introduced for 20 weeks in year two, then removed for 6 weeks, and then displayed for 10 weeks along with posters showing daily calorie requirements for young adults. Females ordered 709, 628, and 534 mean calories and males ordered 734, 692, and 622 calories for the no label, calorie label, and calorie label plus daily caloric recommendation phases, respectively. Both calorie label phases statistically significantly differed from the control phase and from each other.

Workplace Cafeteria, quasi-real world RCT

VanEpps et al. conducted an RCT of 249 corporate employees who placed lunch orders for the on-site cafeteria through a website over four weeks(49). Employees were randomized to view menus with either: 1) no labels; 2) calorie labels; 3) traffic lights; or 4) calorie labels plus traffic lights. Each of the label types statistically significantly reduced calories ordered by about 60–78 calories relative to no labels.

Workplace Cafeteria, pre/post design without comparison site

Schmitz and Fielding compared a two week baseline period without labels to a two week period of labeling calories, sodium, and fat at a large worksite cafeteria in southern California(47). Based on 823 meals, statistically significantly fewer (71 kcal) calories were ordered per tray following labeling.

Ussher et al. compared the calorie content of breakfast and lunch ordered at a hospital cafeteria in Ireland before and after calorie labeling(48). Based on 999 customers pre-labeling and 1005 customers post-labeling, males purchased statistically significantly fewer calories at both meals, and females purchased statistically significantly fewer calories for lunch.

Laboratory and Simulation Settings

A total of 21 studies were conducted in simulation or laboratory settings (Table 3). Of the ten laboratory studies, five measured hypothetical food selections(5054) and five measured actual food selections and consumption(5559). Eleven simulation studies measured hypothetical food selections(6070). The laboratory and simulation results are heterogeneous, even among studies with the strongest designs and many are limited by small sample sizes.

Table 3.

Laboratory or simulation studies of menu labeling

Author, Year Setting and Sample size Study design Methods Outcome reported in this table Calories purchased: Control Group (unlabeled) Calories purchased: Intervention Group (labeled) Difference: (labeled - unlabeled) Significance of Effect & Direction

Laboratory, actual food selection and consumption

Hammond et al., 2013 Laboratory.
635 Canadian adults 18 years of age or older.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Four conditions:
  1. no information;

  2. calorie information;

  3. calories plus traffic lights with high/med/low text;

  4. calorie, sodium, fat and sugar content plus multiple traffic lights with high/med/low text

Participant ordered a free meal from Subway menu.
Food intake measured by weighing food.
Mean calories ordered
Mean calories consumed
Note: Mean calories and differences for calories ordered are unadjusted; difference in calories
Calories consumed are adjusted.
Covariates
age, sex, education, ethnicity, and BMI
Control: Calorie label: Significant reduction in calories
Calories ordered: Calories ordered:
903 (SD: 319) 851 (SD: 366) −52 [95%CI: −127, 23]
Calories consumed: Calories consumed:
840 (SD: 319) 744 (SD: 368) −96* [95%CI: −171, −21]
Calories + traffic light labeling:
Calories ordered:
857 (SD: 366) −46[95%CI: −122, 30]
Calories consumed:
777 (SD:351) −63 [95%CI: −137, 11]
Multiple traffic light labeling:
Calories ordered:
856 (SD: 345) −47 [95%CI: −121, 27]
Calories consumed:
765 (SD: 326) −75 [95%CI: −147, −3]

Harnack et al., 2008 Laboratory.
594 adults and adolescents 16 or older in Minneapolis and St. Paul, MN.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Four conditions:
  1. standard menu;

  2. calorie labels;

  3. value size pricing;

  4. calorie labels plus value size pricing

Menus with calorie labels also included information about daily recommended caloric intake.
Participants ordered meals from McDonald’s menus. They were initially told they would pay for their meals, but ultimately did not.
Food intake measured by weighing food.
Unadjusted mean total calories ordered. Control: Calorie label + daily recommendation: Null
828 (SD: 401) 874 (SD: 439) +46 [95%CI: −49, 141220]
Value pricing:
882 (SD: 354) +54 [95%CI: −33, 141]
Calorie Label + Daily recommendation + Value Pricing:
842 (SD: 425) +14 [95%CI: −80, 108]

James et al. 2014 Laboratory.
300 participants from Texas Christian University, ages 18–30
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Three conditions:
  1. control;

  2. calorie label;

  3. exercise equivalent (minutes of brisk walking) label

Participants ordered from menus.
Food intake was measured by weighing food.
Adjusted mean total calories ordered
Adjusted mean total calories consumed
Covariates
pre-meal hunger level and sex
Control: Calorie label: Mixed
Calories ordered: Calories ordered:
902 [95% CI: 840, 963] 827 [95% CI: 766,888] −75 [95%CI; −84, −66]
Calories consumed: Calories consumed:
770 [95% CI: 717,823] 722 [95% CI: 669, 776] −48 [95%CI: −56, −40]
Exercise label:
Calories ordered:
763 [95% CI: 703, 824] −139* [95%CI: −148, −130]
Calories consumed:
673 [95% CI: 620, 725] −97 [95%CI: −104, −90]

Platkin et al., 2014 Laboratory.
62 overweight or obese females, ages 18–34, recruited at a public university in southern Florida.
Randomized-controlled experiment with repeated measures.
In the follow-up, participants were randomized to one of three conditions:
  1. no calorie information;

  2. calorie labels;

  3. calorie labels and exercise equivalents

In week 1, participants ordered lunch from a Burger King menu.
At week 2 lunch, participants were randomized to one of the label conditions and ordered a second lunch.
The amount of food eaten was weighed.
Adjusted mean calories ordered and consumed
Covariates
age, BMI, race, dietary restraint
Control Control: Null
Lunch 1 calories ordered: Lunch 2 calories ordered:
1201 (SE: 100) 1176 (SE: 100) −25 [95%CI: −228, 178]
Lunch 1 calories consumed: Lunch 2 calories consumed:
987 (SE: 84) 995 (SE: 92) +9 [95%CI: −170, 188]
Calorie labels (but no label on menu for this order) Calorie Labels:
Lunch 1 calories ordered: Lunch 2 calories ordered:
1283 (SE: 90) 1077 (SE: 114) −206 [95%CI: −427, 30]
Lunch 1 calories consumed: Lunch 2 calories consumed:
1060 (SE: 73) 899 (SE: 88) −161 [95%CI: −388, 66]
Calorie labels + exercise equivalent labels (but no label on menu for this order): Calorie labels + exercise equivalent labels
Lunch 1 calories ordered: Lunch 2 calories ordered:
1163 (SE: 141) 1001 (SE: 98) −162 [95%CI: −444, 120]
Lunch 1 calories consumed: Lunch 2 calories consumed:
841 (SE: 89) 841 (SE: 82) 0 [95%CI: −164, 164]

Roberto et al., 2010 Laboratory.
287 adults in New Haven, CT.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Three conditions:
  1. control,

  2. calorie label, or

  3. calorie label + daily intake recommendation

Participants ordered from full-service restaurant menu and food intake was measured by weighing food.
Participants returned the next day to complete a 24-hour dietary recall interview.
Unadjusted mean calories ordered and consumed Control: Calorie labeling: Significant reduction in calories
Calories ordered: Calories ordered:
2189 (SD: 1081) 1862 (SD: 937) −327* [95%CI: −617, −37]
Calories consumed: Calories consumed:
1459 (SD: 725) 1335 (SD: 621) −124 [95%CI: −318, 70] (only significant when two calorie label groups are combined and compared to control)
Self-reported calories consumed after dinner: Self-reported calories consumed after dinner:
179 (SD: 310) 293 (SD: 387) +114* [95%CI: 13,215]
Calories consumed at dinner + after dinner Calories consumed at dinner + after dinner
1630 (SD: 811) 1625 (SD:741) −5 [95%CI: −228, 218]
Calorie labeling + daily intake recommendation:
Calories ordered:
1860 (SD: 1063) −329* [95%CI: −630, −28]
Calories consumed:
1256 (SD: 688) −203 [95%CI: −402, −4] (only significant when two calorie label groups are combined and compared to control)
Self-reported calories consumed after dinner:
177 (SD: 309) −2 [95%CI: −89, 85]
Calories consumed at dinner + after dinner
1380 (639) −250*[95%CI: −456, −44]

Laboratory, hypothetical food selection

Gerend et al., 2009 Laboratory.
288 college students from introductory psychology course, Florida State University.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
2 × 3 design.
Two label conditions:
  1. no labels;

  2. calorie labels

Three dinner scenarios: “quick dinner,” “starving,” and “not too hungry”
Participants made hypothetical choices from one of 2 McDonalds menus, under one of 3 dinner scenarios Unadjusted mean calories per meal (all scenarios averaged together) Control: Calorie label: Mixed
women: women:
934 (SD: 371) 788 (SD: 274) −146*
men: men:
1052 (SD: 313) 1144 (SD: 362) +92
Subgroup sample size not reported so could not calculate CI of difference

Lee et al., 2016 Laboratory.
643 undergraduate students from large southeastern state university’s online participant pool
Randomized controlled experiment
  1. no labels;

  2. calorie labels

  3. calorie labels, plus miles

  4. control

Participants made hypothetical choices from fast food menu using online survey platform Unadjusted mean calories ordered Control: Calorie labels: Null
1041 (SD: 521) 1022 (SD: 548) −19 [95%CI: −122, 84]
Calorie labels, plus miles:
1046 (SD: 626) +5 [95%CI: −103, 113]

Reale and Flint, 2016 Laboratory.
61 people with obesity from weight management service.
Randomized crossover design.
  1. calories-only

  2. nutrient content

  3. energy expenditure

  4. control (always received first)

Participants ordered from physical menu for a hypothetical evening meal. Unadjusted mean total calories ordered Control: Calorie labels: Significant reduction in calories
919 (SD: 416) 601 (SD: 254)* − 318*

Stutts et al., 2011 Laboratory.
236 children ages 6–11, recruited through Girl Scouts and Boy Scouts in U.S.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Factorial design:
3 label conditions (calorie and fat content, heart symbol or no information) × 2 gender × 2 age (6–8 or 9–11)
Children made hypothetical food choices from two menu boards with items from McDonald’s and Wendy’s Adjusted mean total calories ordered
Covariates
gender, age, ethnicity, weekly fast food frequency, weight category, daily television viewing, parent’s perception of child’s weight category, socioeconomicstatus, frequency parent talks to child about eating, strictness, attentiveness, and child’s nutrition knowledge
Control Calories & Fat Label Mixed
Wendy's menu: Wendy’s menu: Wendy’s:
447 (SD: 416) 522 (SD: 393) +75 [95%CI: −52, 202]
McDonald’s menu: McDonald’s menu: McDonald’s:
527 (SD: 377) 540 (SD: 356) +13 [95%CI: −102, 128]
Heart Symbol: Heart Symbol:
Wendy’s menu: Wendy’s:
416 (SD: 420) −31 [95%CI: −163, 101]
McDonald’s menu: McDonald’s:
369 (SD: 380) −158* [95%CI: −278, −38]

Wei et al, 2013 Laboratory.
178 participants from a mid-western town in Indiana, recruited from a community e-newsletter
Randomized-controlled experiment.
2×2 factorial design with calorie information (yes/no) and restaurant type (healthful versus unhealthful)
Participants made hypothetical menu choices from menu boards. Mean total calories ordered Control: Calorie labeling: No significant overall effect of menu labeling Mixed
Healthful restaurant: Healthful restaurant: −96*
715 (SD: 313) 619 (SD: 254)
Unhealthful restaurant: Unhealthful restaurant:
612 (SD: 234) 668 (SD: 276) +56
Subgroup sample size not reported so could not calculate CI of difference

Simulation, hypothetical food selection

Antonelli et al., 2015 Online simulation.
823 parents from a U.S. survey.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Four conditions:
  1. calorie label,

  2. calorie + minutes to walk label;

  3. calories + miles to walk label.

Online participants ordered food from hypothetical fast food menu and completed a survey Unadjusted median total calories ordered by parents for themselves Control: Calorie label: Calorie label: Significant reduction in calories
1580 1200 −380*
Measures of variation not reported for this study Calorie + minutes to walk label: Calorie + minutes to walk label:
1140 − 440*
Calorie + miles to walk label: Calorie + miles to walk label:
1210 −370*

Dodds et al., 2014 Telephone simulation.
329 Australian parents with a child between 3–12 years of age were randomly sampled from a
larger household cohort study.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Three conditions:
  1. standard menu;

  2. menu with kj labels + a statement indicating the daily energy intake for adults;

  3. menu with traffic light labels

Participants were mailed one of the three menus and then completed a telephone interview where they selected a hypothetical meal for themselves and a hypothetical meal for their child. Unadjusted mean total energy of meals selected for parents and children (kilojoules converted to calories) Control: Kilojoule labeling: Null
Parents: Parents:
509 (SD: 257) 521 (SD: 288) +12[95%CI: −61, 86]
Parents for child: Parents for child:
630 (SD: 205) 616 (SD: 168) −14 [95%CI: −64, 36]
Traffic Light Label:
Parents:
458 (SD: 277) −51 [95%CI: −121, 20]
Parents for child:
625 (SD: 211) −5 [95%CI: −60, 49]

Downs et al. 2015 Street simulation in shopping and recreation street in Pittsburgh.
921 participants.
Randomized controlled experiment.
10 conditions, categorized into:
  1. control;

  2. basic numeric information

  3. contextualized information

  4. heuristic cues

The latter 3 groups each had 3 sub-techniques.
Participants were recruited from a high-traffic pedestrian downtown street corner and made choices from a menu. Adjusted mean total calories of snack chosen
Covariates:
Demographics
Control: Calorie Labels: Calorie Labels: Mixed
222 (SD: 117) 200 (SD: 112) −22 (95%CI: −47, 3]
Basic Information: Basic Information:
206 (SD: 112) −16 [95%CI: −37,5]
Contextualized Information: Contextualized Information:
198 (SD: 118) −24* [95%CI: −47, −1]
Heuristic Cues: Heuristic Cues:
192 (SD: 118) −30* [95%CI: −53, −7]

Dowray et al., 2013 Online simulation.
802 employees from a university system in North Carolina
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Three conditions:
  1. Calorie label;

  2. Calorie + minutes walking label;

  3. Calorie + miles walking label

Online participants ordered from hypothetical fast food menu and completed a survey Unadjusted mean total calories ordered Control: Calorie label: Mixed
1020 (SD: 579) 927 (SD: 682) −93 [95%CI: −217, 31]
Calorie + minutes walking label:
916 (SD: 664) −104 [95%CI: −226, 18]
Calorie + miles walking label:
826 (SD:539) −194* [95%CI: −305, −83]

Haws and Liu, 2016 Online simulation.
245 adults in the U.S.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
2×2 design. Randomized to: calorie labels (present vs. absent) or pricing (linear vs. quantity discounted such that price per unit of a product was lower for larger portion sizes).
Online participants placed hypothetical dinner orders from a menu with 10 entrée choices.
Each item had a full or half size option.
Adjusted mean total calories ordered.
Covariates
Gender
There was a significant main effect of calorie information on calories ordered* Significant reduction in calories
No calories + linear pricing Calorie label + linear pricing:
976 801 −175*
No calories + non-linear pricing Calorie label + non-linear pricing:
891 866 −25
Measures of variation not reported for this study

Liu et al. 2012 Online simulation.
419 U.S. participants recruited from online database
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Four conditions:
  1. Control;

  2. Calorie label + daily recommended intake statement;

  3. Calorie label ranked from low to high calories + daily recommended intake statement;

  4. Calorie label ranked from low to high calories + daily recommended intake statement + green or red circles

Participants ordered from hypothetical menu adapted from Chili’s and Applebee’s. Unadjusted mean total calories ordered
Differences reported here are unadjusted, but statistical analyses were conducted controlling for frequency of nutrition label use, pre-survey hunger, and gender
Control: Calories + daily intake statement: Mixed
1760 (SE: 195) 1676 (SE: 133) −84 [95%CI: −549, 381]
Rank-ordered calories+daily intake statement:
1606 (SE: 146) −154* [95%CI: −635, 327] (significant with covariates only)
Rank-ordered calories with green or red circles+daily intake statement:
1455 (SE: 86) −305 [95%CI: −726, 116]

Morley et al., 2013 Online simulation. 1294 adults ages 18–49 in Victoria, Australia, who had purchased fast food in the last month Randomized-controlled experiment.
Five conditions:
  1. control;

  2. kJ label;

  3. kJ + % Daily intake label;

  4. kJ + traffic light label;

  5. kJ + traffic light + %DI label

Online participants ordered dinner from hypothetical fast food menu.
Menu restricted to 3 items from mains and sides, 2 items from drinks and desserts
Unadjusted mean total energy content of meal selection (reported here in calories, reported as kilojoules in original paper) Control: kJ (Calorie) label: Significant reduction in calories
1105 988 − 117*
Measures of variation not reported for this study kJ (Calorie) + % DI label:
1014 −91
kJ (Calorie) + traffic light label:
986 −119*
kJ (Calorie) + traffic light + %DI label:
1082 −23

Pang and Hammond, 2013 Laboratory simulation. 213 undergraduate university students from the University of Waterloo, Canada, that were recruited from 4 health-related classes. Students had to be over 18 years of age. Randomized-controlled experiment.
Four conditions:
  1. no calorie information;

  2. calorie labels;

  3. calorie + health statement;

  4. calorie +exercise equivalent statement.

Participants selected a hypothetical snack from a Tim Horton’s menu. Adjusted mean calories ordered
Covariates
age, gender, and perceived importance of a healthy diet
Control: Calorie labeling: Significant reduction in calories
333 (CI: 283, 383) 302 (CI: 243, 361) −31* [95%CI: −107, 45]
Calorie labeling +recommended daily caloric intake statement:
298 (CI: 234, 361) −35* [95%CI: −115, 45]
Calorie labeling+ exercise equivalent statement:
310 (CI: 251, 369) −23 [95%CI: −100, 53]

Roseman et al., 2013 Street simulation in a medium-sized US city.
302 adult participants
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Two conditions:
  1. no labels,

  2. calorie labels

Participants were recruited from a high-traffic pedestrian downtown street corner and made hypothetical choices from a menu. Unadjusted mean calories ordered Control Calorie labeling No significant overall effect of menu labeling Null
Not reported Not reported
Report looking at nutrition labels when grocery shopping: Report looking at nutrition labels when grocery shopping:
714 (SD: 188) 687 (SD: 170) −27*
Report not looking at nutrition labels when grocery shopping: Report not looking at nutrition labels when grocery shopping:
737 (SD: 220) 831 (SD: 205) +94*
Subgroup sample size not reported so could not calculate CI of difference

Tandon et al., 2010 Clinic in Seattle, WA.
99 families recruited from a pediatric clinic.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Two conditions:
  1. no labels;

  2. calorie labels

Parents made hypothetical choices for themselves and their child from a McDonalds’ menu. Unadjusted mean total calories ordered for child and for self. Control: Calorie label: Significant reduction in calories
Calories ordered for child: Calories ordered for child:
672 (SD: 264) 569 (SD: 208) −103*[95%CI: −198, −8]
Calories ordered for parent: Calories ordered for parent:
759 (SD: 524) 766 (SD: 386) +7 [95%CI: −178, 192]

Viera et al., 2015 Online simulation.
823 parents from a U.S. survey.
Randomized-controlled experiment.
Four conditions:
  1. calorie label;

  2. calorie + minutes to walk label;

  3. calories + miles to walk label.

Online participants ordered food for their children from hypothetical fast food menu and completed a survey Unadjusted mean total calories ordered by parents for children Control: Calorie label: Significant reduction in calories
1294 1066 −228*
Measures of variation not reported for this study Calorie + minutes to walk label:
1060 − 234*
Calorie + miles to walk label:
1099 −195*
*

p <.05

1

author reported 95% CI for difference in means

2

The study sample was non-independent and we lacked the information to calculate the confidence intervals.

Unadjusted means are reported unless paper only reported adjusted numbers.

An absence of standard deviation, standard errors, or confident intervals indicates it was not reported in the paper.

Laboratory, actual food selection and consumption

Hammond et al. randomized 635 Canadian adults to select a free dinner from one of four Subway menus: 1) control, 2) calorie information, 3) calories in traffic light format, or 4) traffic lights for calorie, sodium, fat and sugar content(55). Food intake was measured. There were no statistically significant differences in calories ordered, but calorie labels led to fewer calories consumed compared to control.

Harnack et al. randomized 594 participants ≥16 years old to either a modified McDonald’s menu with calorie labels plus a daily recommended calories statement or no labels; typical or modified menu pricing structures was also manipulated(59). Consumption of actual McDonald’s meals was measured. Calories ordered and consumed did not statistically significantly differ across conditions.

James et al. recruited 300 young adults to eat lunch at a lab and randomized them to menus with no labels, calorie labels plus a statement of daily recommended caloric intake, or labels identifying how many minutes of brisk walking would be needed to burn off the calories in each item(56). Consumption was measured and post-meal intake was assessed with a dietary recall interview. Only the exercise labels resulted in statistically significantly fewer mean calories ordered and consumed relative to the control group, but it did not statistically significantly differ from the calorie label group.

Platkin et al. recruited a very small sample of 62 female participants with overweight or obesity from a college campus to order and eat a Burger King lunch in the lab(57). They returned one week later for a second lunch where they were randomized to menus with either no labels, calorie labels, or calories plus physical activity labels (minutes to burn off calories by walking). At the second lunch, the no label, calorie, and physical activity groups ordered 25, 206, and 162 fewer calories, respectively. Mean calories consumed at the second lunch were 995 (control), 899 (calorie labels), and 841 (physical activity labels). These differences were not statistically significant.

Roberto et al. randomized 295 participants to one of the following menus: 1) control, 2) calorie labels, or 3) calorie labels and a statement of recommended daily caloric intake(58). Food intake was measured, and post-dinner intake was assessed via a dietary recall interview. Although participants in both calorie label groups ate statistically significantly fewer calories at dinner compared to the control group, those who received a menu with only calorie labels ate more calories after dinner. Only the group viewing the calorie recommendation ate statistically significantly fewer overall calories at dinner plus after dinner (approximately 250 kcal reduction) compared to the control group.

Laboratory, hypothetical food selection

A study by Gerend et al. randomly assigned 288 college students to a McDonald’s-like menu that either did or did not have calorie information(50). Participants made hypothetical choices based on three different dinner descriptions (quick dinner, participant is very hungry, or not too hungry). The effects of calorie information were similar across the three scenarios, so data were combined. There was a statistically significant interaction such that men were unaffected by calorie labels, but women ordered 146 fewer calories when shown labels.

Lee and Thompson recruited 643 undergraduate students for an online survey(51). Participants were randomized to one of three groups: 1) no nutrition information; 2) calorie information; or 3) calorie information plus miles needed to burn off item’s calories and ordered from a hypothetical fast food menu. Total calories ordered did not statistically significantly differ across groups.

Reale and Flint recruited 61 people with obesity from a weight management service and conducted a randomized crossover trial where participants were asked to place a hypothetical order from one of the following four menu conditions: 1) no label, 2) calorie label, 3) information on fat, protein, carbohydrates, salt and fiber, or 4) energy expenditure label (52). Participants were exposed to each menu condition on a separate day, starting with the no label condition on the first day. All 3 experimental conditions resulted in a statistically significant reduction in calories ordered compared to the control group. The calories group had a 26% reduction.

Stutts et al. randomized 236 children (6–11 years old) to a McDonald’s menu with either no labels, calories and fat information, or a healthy heart symbol placed next to healthier menu items(53). Calorie labels had no effect, but the healthy heart symbol led to statistically significantly fewer calories ordered.

Wei and Miao randomly assigned 189 participants to place hypothetical orders from menu boards with or without calorie information from either a restaurant perceived as “healthful” (i.e., Subway or Panera) or “unhealthful” (i.e., McDonald’s or Wendy’s)(54). Calorie labels led to a 96 calorie reduction when ordering from healthful restaurants, but there was no statistically significant effect when ordering from unhealthful restaurants.

Simulation, hypothetical food selection

Antonelli and Viera randomized 823 parents of a child aged 2 to 17 to one of four online fast food menus: 1) no labels, 2) calorie labels, 3) calories plus minutes of walking required to burn off calories, or 4) calories plus miles to walk(60). All labeling conditions resulted in statistically significantly fewer hypothetical calories ordered compared to no labels, with the calorie label leading to 380 fewer calories ordered. Minutes of walking led to 440 fewer calories ordered, while miles of walking led to 370 fewer calories ordered compared to no labels. The labels did not statistically significantly differ from one another(60,70).

Dodds et al. randomly sampled 329 Australian parents with a child (3–12 years) from a larger random study sample(61). After a telephone interview, participants were randomized to receive a fast food menu in the mail with either: 1) no labels; 2) kilojoule labels plus a statement indicating the daily energy intake for adults; or 3) traffic light labels. The labels had no effect on total energy selected for parents or children.

Downs et al. recruited 921 pedestrians from busy public locations using a mobile research lab and randomized them to one of the following 10 groups: 1) control group, 2) basic information (3 sub-groups, including calorie labels), 3) contextualized numeric information (3 sub-groups) and 4) heuristic cues (3 sub-groups; e.g., traffic light ratings)(62). Participants made hypothetical snack selections. There was no effect of calorie labels, but contextualized numeric information and heuristic cue labeling led to a statistically significant decrease in calories ordered compared to the control group.

Dowray et al. randomized 804 online participants to one of four menus with either: 1) no labels, 2) calorie labels, 3) calorie labels plus miles of walking, or 4) calorie labels plus minutes of walking needed to burn calories in each item(63). Only the miles of walking label led to statistically significantly fewer hypothetical calories ordered relative to no label, but it did not differ from the other labels.

Haws and Liu randomized 245 online participants to calorie labels (present vs. absent) or pricing (linear vs. quantity discounted, such that price per unit of a product was lower for larger portion sizes)(64). Participants made a hypothetical dinner choice from a menu with 10 entrées each with a full or half size option. People ordered statistically significantly fewer calories when viewing labels, and calorie labeling plus linear pricing led to the fewest calories ordered.

Liu et al. randomly assigned 418 online participants to one of four menus: 1) no labels 2) calorie labels, 3) calorie labels arranged from low to high, or 4) calorie labels arranged from low to high plus red or green traffic lights indicating higher/lower calorie choices(65). All label conditions included a statement about recommended daily caloric intake. None of the label groups statistically significantly differed from control, but when analyses were repeated controlling for gender, hunger, and reported frequency of nutrition label use, rank-ordered calorie labels led to statistically significantly fewer calories ordered (1606 calories) compared to no labels (1760 calories).

Morley et al. randomized 1,294 online Australian participants to one of five hypothetical menus: 1) no information, 2) kilojoule information, 3) kilojoule information plus each item’s percentage of recommended daily intake, 4) kilojoule information plus traffic lights, or 5) kilojoule information plus each item’s percentage of recommended daily intake plus traffic lights(66). Only calories and calories plus traffic lights led to statistically significantly fewer calories ordered (117 and 119 fewer calories, respectively) relative to no labels.

Pang and Hammond randomized 213 undergraduate students to one of four Tim Horton’s snack menus: 1) control, 2) calorie information, 3) calorie information plus recommended daily caloric intake statement, or 4) calorie information plus a physical activity label(67). Only calorie labels and calories with a daily intake statement led to statistically significantly fewer calories ordered compared to control. None of the label groups statistically significantly differed from one another.

Roseman et al. recruited 302 urban pedestrians from a downtown street of a medium-sized US city(68). Participants completed a survey and were randomized to either a calorie-labeled or unlabeled menu that displayed seven items from several fast food restaurants. There were no statistically significant differences in hypothetical calories ordered.

Tandon et al. randomized 99 parents in a primary care pediatric clinic in Seattle, Washington, to a McDonald’s menu with or without calorie labels(69). Calorie labels led parents to hypothetically order 102 fewer calories for their children compared to no labels. The parents’ choices for themselves did not differ.

Changes in Restaurant Offerings

Five studies examined changes in the calories of restaurant menu offerings after local menu labeling regulations were implemented or in advance of national implementation (Table 4)(7175). Two studies evaluated menu offerings where local menu labeling laws were implemented(73,75); one of these studies included a control comparison(75). Three studies examined restaurant nutrition data at the national level(71,72,74). No studies looked at cafeteria offerings in response to calorie labeling.

Pre/Post design with comparison site(s)

Namba et al. examined 9 fast food restaurant chains in 2005 and again in 2011 post-local labeling(75). Five of these chains had some labeled locations, while 4 had restaurants not subject to labeling. Although the mean calories of menu items offered in labeled cities did not change, the proportion of healthy items statistically significantly increased in restaurant chains with some labeled locations (from 3% to 20%).

Restaurant trends where labeling is present, without comparison site(s)

Bruemmer et al. examined calorie content of menu items at 37 chain restaurants in King County, Washington, and found a statistically significant decline in overall average entrée calories (41 fewer calories post-labeling; 73 fewer calories at full-service restaurants and 19 fewer at quick-service restaurants) when comparing 6 and 18 months post labeling(73).

National restaurant trends in advance of national menu labeling

Bleich et al. reported that the mean calorie content of national chain restaurant menu items available between 2012–2014 did not change, but new items in 2013 and 2014 had fewer calories than items only available in 2012(71). In addition, cross-sectionally, chains with voluntary labeling (n=5) had lower calories than those without labeling (n=61)(72).

Wu and Sturm tracked changes in the energy and sodium content of U.S. chain restaurant main entrees between spring 2010 (right after the passage of national menu labeling legislation) and spring 2011(74). They found no overall changes, but fast-food restaurants statistically significantly decreased mean energy in children’s menu entrees by 40 calories whereas upscale restaurants statistically significantly increased mean energy in children’s menu entrees by 46 calories.

DISCUSSION

The strongest research design to evaluate menu labeling is a randomized-controlled field experiment. Unfortunately, only one such study was identified and the sample size was too small to be able to detect even medium-to-large effects(22). Two other quasi-real world randomized-controlled field experiments of menu labeling demonstrate that it led to a statistically significant and fairly large reduction in calories ordered(23,49). The best designed natural experiment with a large sample size reported a 15 calorie reduction in response to calorie labeling among Starbucks customers(24). In contrast, a well-powered, well-designed natural experiment at taco restaurants reported a null effect(25). Although two other well-designed natural experiments reported null effects of calorie labeling at fast food restaurants, both studies were only powered to detect large effects of calorie labeling(26,29). Further, there was only one real-world, full-service chain restaurant analysis with an adequate sample size, which found that calorie labeling was associated with a 150 calorie reduction, but this study was limited by a cross-sectional design(38). Although other reviews have concluded that menu labeling has little impact on fast food purchases (1316), there is an extraordinary dearth of well-designed and adequately powered studies to truly test this hypothesis in both fast food and full-service chain settings.

In general, laboratory/simulations studies produce similar mixed results. Although these studies use RCT designs, many are also limited by small sample sizes. Many laboratory studies of fast food orders generally reported no change in calories ordered(51,57,59,61,68), while laboratory studies mimicking full-service chain restaurants or restaurants typically perceived as healthier found labels led to fewer calories ordered or consumed(55,58,67).

These divergent findings across restaurant settings and studies may emerge because restaurant type affects calorie label use or attracts different types of patrons. It is possible that patrons of coffee chains, full-service sit-down chains, or fast food outlets that market themselves as “healthy” attract patrons with higher incomes, education levels, and/or health consciousness who are more likely to use labeling(7679). Secondary analyses and laboratory studies also report that awareness and/or use of menu labels is higher among certain consumers, such as women and those with higher incomes and health consciousness. It is also possible that calorie labeling is more influential in certain settings. Fast food customers, for example, may enter the restaurant knowing what they want to order, while full-service sit-down patrons may spend more time reviewing the menu before making their decision. In addition, full-service chain restaurants, compared to fast food, are more likely to have very high calorie items, even for items like salads that most people think are low-calorie. There was also more consistent evidence that calorie labeling can promote lower calorie choices in cafeteria settings, perhaps because people eat there more regularly and are less likely to view the meal as a “treat” compared to dining out. The sole randomized-controlled cafeteria field experiment reported a statistically significant and substantial reduction in response to calorie labels, but this may have been because people were ordering food ahead of time, which might have enabled them to exert more control over their decisions compared to ordering on impulse when in the cafeteria (49). Many of the other cafeteria studies did not have comparison sites to control for secular trends, suggesting a need for more research in these settings.

Fewer studies have examined modified calorie information (e.g., daily recommended intake statement) relative to calorie labels and these results are highly variable across settings (22,23,31,33,35,43,49,5153,5567,70). Presumably, making calorie information easier to understand and more accessible to a greater range of individuals, particularly those with lower numeracy levels, would increase the reach and impact of such information, but real-world randomized-controlled trials are needed to know for sure.

Finally, although preliminary evidence suggests that recent calorie labeling regulations (enacted or anticipated) may be correlated with healthier restaurant offerings, the small number of studies, considerable differences in design, lack of comparison sites, and heterogeneity across included restaurants make it difficult to draw conclusions at this point.

This review has several limitations. First, we may not be capturing a number of null studies because of publication bias. In addition, article screening was conducted by a single author which could have led to a biased sample; however all potential articles for inclusion were reviewed by a second author and adjudicated by a third, as necessary. Strengths of this review include examining a large number of studies of both consumer and restaurant responses to calorie labeling across multiple settings and comparing modified to absolute calorie information.

Taken together, evaluations of menu labeling in different settings are mixed and much of the research is plagued by inadequate study designs and/or underpowered studies, highlighting a considerable need for more research. First, data from well-powered RCT field experiments or natural experiments testing menu labeling are needed, especially at full-service chain restaurants. These studies are very difficult to conduct because they require the cooperation of a food retail establishment that has not already implemented labeling. Second, we do not have a good understanding of how people might compensate later if they reduce calories at one meal in response to menu labeling and nearly all real-world studies only assessed calories purchased, not consumed. Third, because much of this legislation is fairly recent, and the federal law’s implementation date is May 2018, there are also limited data on the long-term impacts of calorie labeling. Fourth, more research is needed to understand whether different presentations of this information can increase its impact or whether accompanying educational campaigns might increase the effect of menu labeling. Fifth, we lack good data on whether calorie labeling in other settings impacted by the regulation, such as supermarkets, influences purchases. Finally, research examining impacts on BMI in jurisdictions that have implemented menu labeling would be useful. Currently, there is one such study that we are aware of. Restrepo used data between 2004 and 2012 from 103,220 respondents from the behavioral risk factor surveillance system who self-reported height and weight. Using a natural experiment design, the authors compare BMI trends over time in NY and nearby counties with and without calorie labeling. The results revealed that, on average, calorie labeling laws were associated with a statistically significant reduction of 0.38 BMI units(81). This further suggests that underpowered studies may not be picking up meaningful reductions in calories purchased at restaurants.

CONCLUSION

Overall, because of a lack of well-powered studies with strong designs, the jury is still out on the degree to which menu labeling encourages lower calorie purchases and whether that translates to a healthier population. Although the limited existing research finds little evidence of menu labeling shifting fast-food purchases, there are more promising findings that it may influence consumers at certain types of restaurants and in other types of establishments such as cafeterias. It is difficult to know what a meaningful reduction in calorie intake amounts to, particularly when it is hard to measure how people compensate over the course of a week. Researchers have estimated that if we want to return to 1970 levels of excess weight in the population, adults would need to consume 220 fewer calories daily(82), while children would need to consume 165 fewer calories daily(83). Reducing consumer purchases in chain restaurants by even a small amount may help reduce this excess calorie intake. Finally, menu labeling may encourage restaurants to offer lower calorie items, but it is currently unknown whether studies focused on calorie changes in chain restaurants are capturing responses to menu labeling legislation (enacted or anticipated) rather than responses to other forces encouraging restaurants to change their menus.

Supplementary Material

Supp info

What is known?

  • Menu labeling is one of the few major, federal laws passed in the last decade to address obesity and poor diet

  • The primary goal of menu labeling is to inform consumers about the number of calories in restaurant food

  • Prior reviews of menu labeling have shown little impact on customers’ purchases at fast-food restaurants

What this study adds?

  • Unlike prior reviews of menu labeling, this review examines multiple settings and includes research evaluating restaurant menu offerings following local menu labeling regulations and in advance of national menu labeling regulations

  • There is some evidence that menu labeling lowers calories purchased at certain types of restaurants and in cafeteria settings

  • Menu labeling may encourage restaurants to offer lower calorie items, but longer-term research is needed before firm conclusions can be drawn

Acknowledgments

Funding: This work was partially supported by ChildObesity180. Dr. Roberto is supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number P30AG034546.

We thank Tracy Fox and Margo Wootan for their invaluable feedback on this manuscript. The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Disclosure: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Freedman DS, Khan LK, Dietz WH, Srinivasan SR, Berenson GS. Relationship of childhood obesity to coronary heart disease risk factors in adulthood: the Bogalusa Heart Study. Pediatrics. 2001;108:712–718. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.3.712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Steinberger J, Moran A, Hong C-P, Jacobs DR, Sinaiko AR. Adiposity in childhood predicts obesity and insulin resistance in young adulthood. The Journal of pediatrics. 2001;138:469–473. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2001.112658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dietz WH. Childhood weight affects adult morbidity and mortality. The Journal of nutrition. 1998;128:411S–414S. doi: 10.1093/jn/128.2.411S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kopelman PG. Obesity as a medical problem. Nature. 2000;404:635–643. doi: 10.1038/35007508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Finkelstein EA, Trogdon JG, Cohen JW, Dietz W. Annual medical spending attributable to obesity: payer-and service-specific estimates. Health affairs. 2009;28:w822–w831. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.28.5.w822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bleich SN, Ku R, Wang Y. Relative contribution of energy intake and energy expenditure to childhood obesity: a review of the literature and directions for future research. International journal of obesity. 2010 doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bleich S, Murray CJL, Adams A. NBER working paper series working paper 12954. National Bureau of Economic Research; Cambridge, MA: 2007. National Bureau of Economic Research. Why is the developed world obese? [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Block JP, Condon SK, Kleinman K, Mullen J, Linakis S, Rifas-Shiman S, et al. Consumers’ estimation of calorie content at fast food restaurants: cross sectional observational study. 2013 doi: 10.1136/bmj.f2907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Legislature NCoS. Trans fat and menu labeling legislation, 2013–2013 [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Interest CfSitP. State and local menu labeling policies. 2011 [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Food, Drug Administration H. Food labeling; nutrition labeling of standard menu items in restaurants and similar retail food establishments. Final rule. Federal register. 2014;79:71155. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.FDA finalizes menu and vending machine calorie labeling rules 2014.
  • 13.VanEpps EM, Roberto CA, Park S, Economos CD, Bleich SN. Restaurant menu labeling policy: review of evidence and controversies. Current obesity reports. 2016;5:72–80. doi: 10.1007/s13679-016-0193-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Long MW, Tobias DK, Cradock AL, Batchelder H, Gortmaker SL. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the impact of restaurant menu calorie labeling. American journal of public health. 2015;105:e11–e24. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kiszko KM, Martinez OD, Abrams C, Elbel B. The influence of calorie labeling on food orders and consumption: a review of the literature. Journal of community health. 2014;39:1248–1269. doi: 10.1007/s10900-014-9876-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sinclair SE, Cooper M, Mansfield ED. The influence of menu labeling on calories selected or consumed: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2014;114:1375–1388. e1315. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2014.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Swartz JJ, Braxton D, Viera AJ. Calorie menu labeling on quick-service restaurant menus: an updated systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2011;8:1. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Roberto CA, Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. Rationale and evidence for menu-labeling legislation. American journal of preventive medicine. 2009;37:546–551. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Harnack LJ, French SA. Effect of point-of-purchase calorie labeling on restaurant and cafeteria food choices: a review of the literature. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2008;5:1. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-5-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sarink D, Peeters A, Freak-Poli R, Beauchamp A, Woods J, Ball K, et al. The impact of menu energy labelling across socioeconomic groups: A systematic review. Appetite. 2016;99:59–75. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fernandes AC, Oliveira RC, Proenca RP, Curioni CC, Rodrigues VM, Fiates GM. Influence of menu labeling on food choices in real-life settings: a systematic review. Nutr Rev. 2016;74:534–548. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuw013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ellison B, Lusk JL, Davis D. Looking at the label and beyond: the effects of calorie labels, health consciousness, and demographics on caloric intake in restaurants. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2013;10:1. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wisdom J, Downs JS, Loewenstein G. Promoting healthy choices: Information versus convenience. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2010;2:164–178. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bollinger B, Leslie P, Sorensen A. Calorie posting in chain restaurants. American Economic Journal: Economic Policy. 2011;3:91–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Finkelstein EA, Strombotne KL, Chan NL, Krieger J. Mandatory menu labeling in one fast-food chain in King County, Washington. American journal of preventive medicine. 2011;40:122–127. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2010.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Elbel B, Kersh R, Brescoll VL, Dixon LB. Calorie labeling and food choices: a first look at the effects on low-income people in New York City. Health affairs. 2009;28:w1110–w1121. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.28.6.w1110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Elbel B, Gyamfi J, Kersh R. Child and adolescent fast-food choice and the influence of calorie labeling: a natural experiment. International journal of obesity. 2011;35:493–500. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cantor J, Torres A, Abrams C, Elbel B. Five years later: Awareness Of New York City’s calorie labels declined, with no changes in calories purchased. Health Affairs. 2015;34:1893–1900. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2015.0623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Elbel B, Mijanovich T, Dixon LB, Abrams C, Weitzman B, Kersh R, et al. Calorie labeling, fast food purchasing and restaurant visits. Obesity. 2013;21:2172–2179. doi: 10.1002/oby.20550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tandon PS, Zhou C, Chan NL, Lozano P, Couch SC, Glanz K, et al. The impact of menu labeling on fast-food purchases for children and parents. American journal of preventive medicine. 2011;41:434–438. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.06.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Downs JS, Wisdom J, Wansink B, Loewenstein G. Supplementing menu labeling with calorie recommendations to test for facilitation effects. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103:1604–1609. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dumanovsky T, Huang CY, Nonas CA, Matte TD, Bassett MT, Silver LD. Changes in energy content of lunchtime purchases from fast food restaurants after introduction of calorie labelling: cross sectional customer surveys. Bmj. 2011;343:d4464. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d4464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Holmes AS, Serrano EL, Machin JE, Duetsch T, Davis GC. Effect of different children’s menu labeling designs on family purchases. Appetite. 2013;62:198–202. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Schwartz J, Riis J, Elbel B, Ariely D. Inviting consumers to downsize fast-food portions significantly reduces calorie consumption. Health Affairs. 2012;31:399–407. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ge L, Behnke C, Almanza B. An Evaluation of Three Nutrition Labeling Formats for Restaurant Menus. FIU Hospitality Review. 2014:31. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Krieger JW, Chan NL, Saelens BE, Ta ML, Solet D, Fleming DW. Menu labeling regulations and calories purchased at chain restaurants. American journal of preventive medicine. 2013;44:595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2013.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pulos E, Leng K. Evaluation of a voluntary menu-labeling program in full-service restaurants. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100:1035–1039. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.174839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Auchincloss AH, Mallya GG, Leonberg BL, Ricchezza A, Glanz K, Schwarz DF. Customer responses to mandatory menu labeling at full-service restaurants. American journal of preventive medicine. 2013;45:710–719. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2013.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rendell SL, Swencionis C. Point-of-purchase calorie labeling has little influence on calories ordered regardless of body mass index. Current obesity reports. 2014;3:368–375. doi: 10.1007/s13679-014-0116-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Mayne SL, Lee BK, Auchincloss AH. Evaluating Propensity Score Methods in a Quasi-Experimental Study of the Impact of Menu-Labeling. PloS one. 2015;10:e0144962. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0144962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lillico H, Hanning R, Findlay S, Hammond D. The effects of calorie labels on those at high-risk of eating pathologies: a pre-post intervention study in a University cafeteria. Public health. 2015;129:732–739. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2015.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hunsberger M, McGinnis P, Smith J, Beamer BA, O’Malley J. Calorie labeling in a rural middle school influences food selection: Findings from community-based participatory research. Journal of obesity. 2015;2015 doi: 10.1155/2015/531690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Nikolaou CK, Hankey CR, Lean MEJ. Preventing weight gain with calorie-labeling. Obesity. 2014;22:2277–2283. doi: 10.1002/oby.20885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hammond D, Lillico HG, Vanderlee L, White CM, Reid JL. The impact of nutrition labeling on menus: A naturalistic cohort study. American journal of health behavior. 2015;39:540–548. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.39.4.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Chu YH, Frongillo EA, Jones SJ, Kaye GL. Improving patrons’ meal selections through the use of point-of-selection nutrition labels. American Journal of Public Health. 2009;99:2001–2005. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.153205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Milich R, Anderson J, Mills M. Effects of visual presentation of caloric values on food buying by normal and obese persons. Perceptual and motor skills. 1976 doi: 10.2466/pms.1976.42.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Schmitz MF, Fielding JE. Point-of-choice nutritional labeling: evaluation in a worksite cafeteria. Journal of Nutrition Education. 1986;18:S65–S68. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ussher G, Kielthy S, Emerson K, Douglas F, Lyons O, Flynn M. Calories on menus in Ireland-who’s counting? The Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 2015:74. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.VanEpps EM, Downs JS, Loewenstein G. Calorie Label Formats: Using Numeric and Traffic Light Calorie Labels to Reduce Lunch Calories. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing. 2016;35:26–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gerend MA. Does calorie information promote lower calorie fast food choices among college students? Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;44:84–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lee MS, Thompson JK. Exploring enhanced menu labels’ influence on fast food selections and exercise-related attitudes, perceptions, and intentions. Appetite. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Reale S, Flint SW. Menu labelling and food choice in obese adults: a feasibility study. BMC obesity. 2016;3:1. doi: 10.1186/s40608-016-0095-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Stutts MA, Zank GM, Smith KH, Williams SA. Nutrition information and children’s fast food menu choices. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2011;45:52–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wei W, Miao L. Effects of calorie information disclosure on consumers’ food choices at restaurants. International Journal of Hospitality Management. 2013;33:106–117. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hammond D, Goodman S, Hanning R, Daniel S. A randomized trial of calorie labeling on menus. Preventive medicine. 2013;57:860–866. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.James A, Adams-Huet B, Shah M. Menu labels displaying the kilocalorie content or the exercise equivalent: effects on energy ordered and consumed in young adults. American Journal of Health Promotion. 2015;29:294–302. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.130522-QUAN-267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Platkin C, Yeh M-C, Hirsch K, Wiewel EW, Lin C-Y, Tung H-J, et al. The effect of menu labeling with calories and exercise equivalents on food selection and consumption. BMC obesity. 2014;1:1. doi: 10.1186/s40608-014-0021-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Roberto CA, Larsen PD, Agnew H, Baik J, Brownell KD. Evaluating the impact of menu labeling on food choices and intake. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100:312–318. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.160226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Harnack LJ, French SA, Oakes JM, Story MT, Jeffery RW, Rydell SA. Effects of calorie labeling and value size pricing on fast food meal choices: results from an experimental trial. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2008;5:1. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-5-63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Antonelli R, Viera AJ. Potential effect of physical activity calorie equivalent (PACE) labeling on adult fast food ordering and exercise. PloS one. 2015;10:e0134289. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Dodds P, Wolfenden L, Chapman K, Wellard L, Hughes C, Wiggers J. The effect of energy and traffic light labelling on parent and child fast food selection: a randomised controlled trial. Appetite. 2014;73:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Downs JS, Wisdom J, Loewenstein G. Helping consumers use nutrition information: Effects of format and presentation. American Journal of Health Economics. 2015 [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dowray S, Swartz JJ, Braxton D, Viera AJ. Potential effect of physical activity based menu labels on the calorie content of selected fast food meals. Appetite. 2013;62:173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Haws KL, Liu PJ. Half-size me? How calorie and price information influence ordering on restaurant menus with both half and full entrée portion sizes. Appetite. 2016;97:127–137. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Liu PJ, Roberto CA, Liu LJ, Brownell KD. A test of different menu labeling presentations. Appetite. 2012;59:770–777. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Morley B, Scully M, Martin J, Niven P, Dixon H, Wakefield M. What types of nutrition menu labelling lead consumers to select less energy-dense fast food? An experimental study. Appetite. 2013;67:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Pang J, Hammond D. Efficacy and consumer preferences for different approaches to calorie labeling on menus. Journal of nutrition education and behavior. 2013;45:669–675. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Roseman MG, Mathe-Soulek K, Higgins JA. Relationships among grocery nutrition label users and consumers’ attitudes and behavior toward restaurant menu labeling. Appetite. 2013;71:274–278. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Tandon PS, Wright J, Zhou C, Rogers CB, Christakis DA. Nutrition menu labeling may lead to lower-calorie restaurant meal choices for children. Pediatrics. 2010;125:244–248. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-1117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Viera AJ, Antonelli R. Potential effect of physical activity calorie equivalent labeling on parent fast food decisions. Pediatrics. 2015;135:e376–e382. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-2902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Bleich SN, Wolfson JA, Jarlenski MP. Calorie changes in large chain restaurants: declines in new menu items but room for improvement. American journal of preventive medicine. 2016;50:e1–e8. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Bleich SN, Wolfson JA, Jarlenski MP, Block JP. Restaurants with calories displayed on menus had lower calorie counts compared to restaurants without such labels. Health Affairs. 2015;34:1877–1884. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2015.0512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bruemmer B, Krieger J, Saelens BE, Chan N. Energy, saturated fat, and sodium were lower in entrées at chain restaurants at 18 months compared with 6 months following the implementation of mandatory menu labeling regulation in King County, Washington. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2012;112:1169–1176. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wu HW, Sturm R. Changes in the energy and sodium content of main entrees in US chain restaurants from 2010 to 2011. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2014;114:209–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2013.07.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Namba A. Exploratory analysis of fast-food chain restaurant menus before and after implementation of local calorie-labeling policies, 2005–2011. Preventing chronic disease. 2013:10. doi: 10.5888/pcd10.120224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Breck A, Cantor J, Martinez O, Elbel B. Who reports noticing and using calorie information posted on fast food restaurant menus? Appetite. 2014;81:30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Chen R, Smyser M, Chan N, Ta M, Saelens BE, Krieger J. Changes in awareness and use of calorie information after mandatory menu labeling in restaurants in King County, Washington. American journal of public health. 2015;105:546–553. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Green JE, Brown AG, Ohri-Vachaspati P. Sociodemographic disparities among fast-food restaurant customers who notice and use calorie menu labels. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2015;115:1093–1101. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2014.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Taksler GB, Elbel B. Calorie labeling and consumer estimation of calories purchased. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2014;11:1. doi: 10.1186/s12966-014-0091-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Gi L, Behnke C, Almanza B. An Evaluation of Three Nutrition Labeling Formats for Restaurant Menus. FIU Hospitality Review. 2014:31. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Restrepo BJ. Calorie labeling in chain restaurants and body weight: evidence from New York. Health Economics. 2016 doi: 10.1002/hec.3389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Hall KD, Sacks G, Chandramohan D, Chow CC, Wang YC, Gortmaker SL, et al. Quantification of the effect of energy imbalance on bodyweight. The Lancet. 2011;378:826–837. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60812-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Wang YC, Gortmaker SL, Sobol AM, Kuntz KM. Estimating the energy gap among US children: a counterfactual approach. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e1721–e1733. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp info

RESOURCES