Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jan 5.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2017 Apr 17;29(4):1469–1482. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000396

Parental depressive history, parenting styles, and child psychopathology over six years: The contribution of each parent’s depressive history to the other’s parenting styles

Daniel C Kopala-Sibley 1, Caitlin Jelinek 1, Ellen Kessel 1, Allison Frost 1, Anna ES Allmann 1, Daniel N Klein 1,2
PMCID: PMC5755697  NIHMSID: NIHMS931107  PMID: 28414019

Abstract

The link between parental depressive history and parenting styles is well established, as is the association of parenting with child psychopathology. However, little research has examined whether a depressive history in one parent predicts the parenting style of the other parent. As well, relatively little research has tested transactional models of the parenting-child psychopathology relationship in the context of parents’ depressive histories. In this study, mothers and fathers of 392 children were assessed for a lifetime history of major depression when their children were 3 years old. They then completed measures of permissiveness and authoritarianism and their child’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms when children were 3, 6, and 9 years old. Results showed that a depressive history in one parent predicted the other parent’s permissiveness. Analyses then showed that child externalizing symptoms at age 3 predicted maternal permissiveness and authoritarianism and paternal permissiveness at age 6. Maternal permissiveness at age 6 predicted child externalizing symptoms at age 9. No relationships in either direction were found between parenting styles and child internalizing symptoms. Results highlight the importance of considering both parents’ depressive histories when understanding parenting styles, and support transactional models of parenting styles and child externalizing symptoms.

Keywords: Parental depression, parenting, child internalizing, child externalizing


Parenting styles and behavior towards children have long been recognized as an important influence on children’s psychosocial development in general (Bradley & Vandell, 2007), and on their risk for psychopathology, in particular (e.g., Collins et al., 2000; McLeod et al., 2007). As such, it is unsurprising that researchers have also sought to understand the origins of specific parenting styles and behaviors (e.g., Belsky, 1984; Clark et al., 2000; Pardini, 2008; Kopala-Sibley, Zuroff, & Koestner, 2011; Seng & Prinz, 2008). Much of this literature has focused on the effects of parental history of depression on parents’ own parenting behaviors (see Goodman et al., 2011; Kane & Garber, 2004; Lovejoy et al., 2000; Wilson & Durbin, 2010), which in turn has been linked to child risk for psychopathology (e.g., McLeod et al., 2007). Indeed, although there are undoubtedly numerous biological, social, and psychological processes by which risk for psychopathology is passed across generations, much evidence supports negative parenting as one such mediator (e.g., Kane & Garber, 2004; Lovejoy et al., 2000; Wilson & Durbin, 2010).

However, despite evidence supporting the effect of parental depressive history on parents’ own parenting behaviors (for reviews, see Downey & Coyne, 1990; Lovejoy et al., 2000), which in turn predict child risk for psychopathology (e.g., McLeod et al., 2007; Collins et al., 2000), there remain a number of gaps in this literature. For instance, no research of which we are aware has considered the possibility that one parent’s depressive history may influence the other parent’s behaviors towards their child, over and above each parent’s own depressive history. This is despite a long history of theory and research that co-parent characteristics and behaviors influence one’s own behaviors towards one’s child (Belsky, 1984; Kendler, Sham, & MacLean, 1997). In addition, while much literature has focused on the effects of parenting on child psychopathology, less has examined the effects of child psychopathology on parenting. Finally, much research on this topic has been cross-sectional or, when longitudinal, limited to periods of a year or two, and has generally relied on only one informant in each study. As such, using data collected from both mothers and fathers when their child was age 3, 6, and 9 years old, the first goal of this paper is to examine whether one parent’s lifetime depressive history influences the parenting behaviors of the other parent, over and above the effects of their own depressive history. Our second goal is to examine transactional relationships between parental authoritarianism and permissiveness, two well-established maladaptive parenting styles, and children’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms, as rated by both parents, over a six year period ranging from early to late childhood.

Parental depression and parenting behaviors

Research has shown a well-replicated link between a parental history of depression and parenting towards offspring. For instance, early studies and reviews supported the link between maternal depression and negative parenting behaviors (Downey & Coyne, 1990; Weissman & Paykel, 1974). A meta-analysis showed that maternal depression is related to more hostility and less engagement, even for mothers who are not currently depressed but have experienced a past depressive episode (Lovejoy et al., 2000). Furthermore, associations between experimentally manipulated mood and parenting have also been demonstrated (Jouriles, Murphy, & O’Leary, 1989; Zekoski, O’Hara, & Wills, 1987). Subsequent research has confirmed that maternal depressive history is robustly related to more negative parenting behaviors (e.g., Foster, Garber, & Durlak, 2008; Frye & Garber, 2005; see Goodman, 2007, for a review).

These findings also extend to the influence of a paternal history of depression on paternal parenting behaviors. An initial meta-analysis (Kane & Garber, 2004) found that fathers with a history of depression experience more conflict with their children. Subsequently, Wilson & Durbin (2010) meta-analyzed results from 28 studies and concluded there is a significant relationship between paternal depressive history and more negative parenting behaviors, such as hostility, control, restriction, and rejection.

However, despite these bodies of literature, almost no research to date has examined whether one parent’s depressive history influences the parenting styles and behaviors of the other parent, over and above the effects of that parent’s own depressive history. This is despite a long history of theory suggesting that co-parent characteristics, including psychopathology, should deleteriously influence the other parent’s parenting behaviors (e.g., Belsky et al., 1984; Abidin, 1992). Only one study of which we are aware tested this question. In a cross-sectional study of mother-father pairs with a nine-month-old infant, Paulson et al. (2006) found that when mothers experienced post-partum depression, fathers were less likely to sing songs to their children on a daily basis. Conversely, when fathers were depressed, mothers were less likely to tell stories to their child every day. Moreover, non-depressed mothers engaged in more enrichment activities with their infants when the father was also not depressed, compared to when fathers were depressed. Results from this study provide some tentative evidence of effects of mothers’ depression on fathers’ parenting, and vice versa.

There is evidence, albeit contradictory, that concurrent co-parent depressive symptoms may influence the other parent’s behaviors. For instance, some studies have found that non-depressed paternal parenting behaviors were worsened (e.g., Cummings et al., 2013; Goodman, 2008; Jacob & Johnson, 1997), improved (e.g., Edhborg, Lundh, Seimyr, & Widstrom, 2003; Hops et al., 1987), or unrelated to mothers’ depressive symptoms (e.g., Chabrol, Bron, & Le Camus, 1996).

Moreover, evidence suggests that a co-parent’s traits, behaviors, or subclinical symptoms which may be influenced by his or her history of depression likely influence the other parent’s parenting (e.g., Belsky et al., 1979; Coyne et al. 2002; Proulx et al., 2007). It is also important to note that individuals with a history of depression are more likely to marry someone else with a history of depression relative to someone without such a history (Nordsletten et al., 2016). Given the multiple associations between both parents’ mental health and their parenting behaviors, it is important to demonstrate the unique effect of one parent’s depressive history on the other parent’s parenting, above and beyond the other parent’s mental health history. Early in the history of this research, Belsky (1979) found that mothers’ co-parent support strongly influenced their husbands’ parenting behaviors, and concluded that spousal or co-parent support, which would presumably be undermined by a history of depression in that co-parent, is an important determinant of parenting behaviors. Similarly, intraparental conflict, which is increased in relationships where one partner is depressed (e.g., Coyne et al., 2002), is related to more negative parenting (Simons et al., 1992; see Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000 for a meta-analyses). Depression in one parent may also diminish the quality of the overall inter-parental relationship, thereby affecting each parent’s parenting behaviors (Belsky, 1984; Coyne et al., 2002; Proulx et al., 2007).

This evidence suggests that spousal depressive history may have an additive effect on the other parent’s parenting styles. That is, while a history of depression, via a variety of potential mechanisms, may undermine a parent’s parenting styles, it is likely that also having a co-parent with a history of depression will additively impact these parenting styles. For instance, subclinical depressive symptoms, which would presumably be more common in an individual with a history of depression, have been found to influence their partner’s parenting behaviors (Cummings et al., 2013; Goodman, 2008; Jacob & Johnson, 1997). As such, a co-parent’s depressive history may prove to be an additional burden on top of that parent’s own depressive history in terms of its effects on their parenting styles. The current study will further explore these issues by examining whether maternal and paternal depressive history respectively predict paternal and maternal parenting styles as well as change in parenting styles over a three year period, over and above each parent’s own depressive history.

Parenting and child psychopathology

Parenting styles and child symptoms of psychopathology are often conceptualized as bidirectional and mutually influential (Belsky, 1984; Collins et al., 2000; Patterson, 1982), although the majority of research to date has focused on the influence of parents on children. Regarding the effects of parenting, research has examined both externalizing and internalizing outcomes in children. Externalizing behaviors are associated with, and sometimes longitudinally predicted by, harsh or otherwise negative parenting (e.g., Conger et al., 1992; Conger, Ge, Elder, Lorenz, & Simons, 1994; O’Leary, Slep, & Reid, 1999; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992). A meta-analysis of 161 studies found that parental monitoring, psychological control, rejection, and hostility accounted for 11% of the variance in delinquency (Hoeve et al. 2009). Additionally, the success of parent management training (PMT; as documented in Graziano & Diament, 1992; Kazdin, 2005; Ogden & Hagen, 2008) in treating the symptoms of externalizing disorders in children further highlights that a variety of parenting practices may contribute to the maintenance or amelioration of symptoms.

There are also numerous studies evaluating the impact of parenting on child internalizing symptoms. Rapee’s (1997) review suggests that rejection and control by parents predicts future anxiety and depression in children, while McLeod et al. (2007a,b) found that parenting accounted for 8% of the variance in childhood depression and 4% of the variance in childhood anxiety in meta-analyses of 45 and 47 studies, respectively, although specific types of parenting behaviors, such as control, predicted a greater amount of variance in these outcomes. Further corroborating those findings, Yap et al. (2014) meta-analyzed 181 articles and found less warmth, more inter-parental conflict, overinvolvement, and parental aversiveness to the child predicted higher levels of both childhood anxiety and depression. In sum, a substantial body of literature has confirmed the effects of parenting on both child internalizing and externalizing outcomes. In this paper, we aim to provide further evidence of the effects of parenting on change over time in internalizing and externalizing symptoms from age 3 to 9 years.

Child psychopathology and parenting

Just as parenting influences offspring psychopathology, it is also well established that children are active participants in shaping their environment, including the parenting they receive (Belsky, 1984; Lytton, 1990; Plomin et al., 1977; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). For example, Patterson’s (1982; Granic & Patterson, 2006; Patterson et al., 1989) model of coercive interactions describes a pattern in which parents either provide a directive or refuse a child’s request, thereby increasing the child’s distress and aversive behaviors such as screaming or crying. Ultimately the parent accedes to the child’s wishes in order to avoid the further escalation of undesirable child behaviors and possible public embarrassment. The child then learns that increased negative behaviors will eventually be rewarded, while the parent learns that acquiescing to the child’s desires will immediately decrease the aversive behaviors. This is one example of a process that likely occurs not only in moment-to-moment interactions, but also on a larger scale of child symptoms and parenting practices.

Several longitudinal studies have supported this transactional model and found that while negative parenting behaviors, such as permissiveness, inconsistent discipline, or punishment (corporal or otherwise), or a lack of parental involvement predict increases in externalizing symptoms, externalizing symptoms also predict increases in these parental behaviors (Burke et al., 2008; Combs-Ronto et al., 2009; Hawes et al., 2011; Pardini et al., 2008).

Much less research, however, has studied transactional models of parenting and child internalizing symptoms, especially depression. What evidence there is, moreover, suggests that any effects seen of child internalizing symptoms on parenting behaviors may be due to the overlap of internalizing and externalizing symptoms. For instance, conduct problems, but not depression, predict increases in harsh parenting (Hipwell et al., 2008), while externalizing symptoms in youth were a better predictor of parenting behaviors then were depressive symptoms (Reitz, Deković, Meijer, & Engels, 2006; Reitz, Deković, Meijer, 2006). Similarly, one study of 190 clinic-referred children age 7–13 found that the significant correlation between overprotective parenting and child anxiety symptoms became non-significant when child behavior problems were also in the model, suggesting that child anxiety may not be a unique predictor of parental overprotection over and above child behavior problems (Gere et al. 2012). As such, the current study aims to provide further evidence regarding the effects of internalizing versus externalizing symptoms in children on their mothers’ and fathers’ permissiveness and authoritarianism, two parenting styles that are robustly linked to both internalizing and externalizing symptoms in offspring (e.g., McLeod et al., 2007b; Milevsky et al., 2007; Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994).

Overview and hypotheses

The primary goal of this study is to examine the respective effect of maternal and paternal lifetime depressive history on paternal and maternal permissiveness and authoritarianism towards their three year old children, over and above each parent’s own depressive history, and to examine these effects on change in these behaviors as their children age from three to six years old. In this study, we follow Baumrind’s (1971) conceptualizations, such that permissive parenting is characterized by high warmth, but low control, whereas authoritarian parenting is characterized by high control but low warmth. A number of studies have examined the factor structure of parenting behaviors. Permissiveness and authoritarianism, or similar constructs, consistently emerge, along with authoritativeness (e.g., Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 1995; Schludermann, & Schludermann, 1970; Schaefer, 1965), which is not examined here as we were primarily interested in maladaptive forms of parenting. Although these broad parenting styles can be broken down into more specific parenting behaviors, such as hostility, rejection, warmth, control, structure, and coercion (see Sessa et al., 2000; Skinner et al., 2005; Wilson & Durbin, 2012), and a number of studies have examined the influence of parental depressive history on these behaviors (e.g., Wilson & Durbin, 2010; Kane & Garber, 2004), we chose to focus on parental authoritarianism and permissiveness as we were primarily interested in the influence on, and of, broader, more general parenting factors, rather than specific behaviors. Notably, the specific behaviors examined in previous studies are included in the broader parenting styles examined in the current study (Robinson et al., 1995, 1996). In addition, we aim to further examine the transactional relationships between parental permissiveness and authoritarianism and children’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms at ages 3, 6, and 9 years old.

We expect that a maternal lifetime history of depression will predict more paternal permissiveness and authoritarianism, over and above the father’s lifetime history of depression. Conversely, we expect that a paternal lifetime history of depression will predict more maternal permissiveness and authoritarianism, over and above the mother’s lifetime history of depression. We then expect higher levels of permissiveness and authoritarianism, both from fathers and from mothers, to predict increases in internalizing and externalizing symptoms over childhood. Finally, we expect externalizing, but not internalizing symptoms, to predict increases in both maternal and paternal authoritarianism and permissiveness. We also examined whether effects varied by child gender, although we consider these analyses exploratory and had no a priori hypotheses.

Method

Participants

Our sample consisted of 392 three-year olds (M age = 43.5 months, SD = 2.8) and their mothers and fathers from a larger longitudinal study of 559 children (see Olino et al., 2010 for details). Briefly, participants were recruited through commercial mailing lists and screened by phone to select children with no significant medical problems or developmental disabilities and who had at least one English-speaking biological parent who could participate. As part of this larger, ongoing study, mothers and children were assessed at three-year intervals, i.e., at ages 3 (2004–2006), 6 (2007–2009), and 9 (2010–2012). At the age 3 assessment, 392 pairs of parents had complete data on parental depressive history, parenting variables, and child symptomatology, which is therefore our effective sample size.

Our effective sample (N = 392; 154 females) was primarily middle-class as measured by Hollingshead’s four-factor index of social status, which is based on a combination of parental education and occupational prestige (M = 45.33, SD = 10.99; Hollingshead, 1975). Most children lived with both biological parents (88.8%). Of the mothers, 55.2% had completed a four-year college degree, while 48.5% of fathers had completed a four-year college degree. Of the children, 92.5% were Caucasian and non-Hispanic.

Number of parents in a household was not an inclusionary or exclusionary criteria. At age 3, parental history of depression was assessed only among biological parents, so step-parent depressive history is not included in the current study. We required complete baseline data for our effective sample. Hence, in our effective sample of 392, 383 (97.7%) of children lived with both biological parents, who were married or cohabiting. By the age 9 assessment, 304 (77.6%) of the 392 mothers’ again reported being married or cohabiting.

Our effective sample of 392 did not differ from the remaining 167 families in the original sample who lacked complete data on socioeconomic status (SES), child sex, the likelihood of coming from a two-parent home, race, ethnicity, and internalizing or externalizing symptoms at age 3 (all ps > .20). Of the 392 children for whom there was complete data at age 3, 107 participants had missing data on one or more variables at age 6 or 9. Sample sizes for each variable are in Table 1. These 107 with at least some missing data did not differ significantly from the 285 with complete data at all time points in terms of SES, child sex, the likelihood of coming from a two-parent home, race, ethnicity, parental lifetime depressive history, parenting behaviors, and internalizing or externalizing symptoms at any time point (all ps > .15). Little’s MCAR test also confirmed that missingness was unrelated to any variable in our study: χ2 (139) = 146.98, p = .31. Data can thus be viewed as missing at random for analyses. Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) procedures in MPlus 7.31 were used to estimate the means and intercepts in the presence of missing data. This approach is generally acknowledged to be preferable to other methods for dealing with missing data, such as listwise deletion or mean imputation, as these latter approaches are more likely to yield biased estimates (Little & Rubin, 1989; Muthén, Kaplan, & Hollis, 1987; Schafer & Graham, 2002).

Table 1.

Descriptives and bivariate correlations for all study variables.

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13 14 15 16
1. Maternal lifetime depression --
2. Paternal lifetime depression .17** --
3. Age 3 Maternal Authoritarianism .07 .07 --
4. Age 3 Maternal Permissiveness .09+ .14** .39** --
5. Age 3 Paternal Authoritarianism .07 .10* .33** .15** --
6. Age 3 Paternal Permissiveness .18** .10* .20** .34** .27** --
7. Age 3 Internalizing Symptoms .12* .15** .18** .28** .11* .23** --
8. Age 3 Externalizing Symptoms .16** .12* .38** .34** .28** .36** .53** --
9. Age 6 Maternal Authoritarianism .14* .10+ .67** .33** .26** .22** .17** .40** --
10. Age 6 Maternal Permissiveness .22** .19** .31** .63** .18** .36** .21** .33** .44** --
11. Age 6 Paternal Authoritarianism .10+ .17** .34** .16** .54** .23** .14* .32** .35** .23** --
12. Age 6 Paternal Permissiveness .14* .10+ .21** .24** .15* .57** .16** .25** .21** .33** .36** --
13. Age 6 Internalizing .23** .20** .14* .17** .08 .11+ .55** .35** .21** .14* .15* .21** --
14. Age 6 Externalizing .19** .13* .24** .29** .26** .31** .36** .66** .38** .33** 36** .29** .56** --
15. Age 9 Internalizing .21** .09 .12* .08 .14* .20** .45** .34** .11+ .10+ .15** .19** .72** .40** --
16. Age 9 Externalizing .15** .06 .32** .27** .26** .29** .38** .58** .38** .36** .33** .26** .44** .73** .60** --

Frequency 134 70
% 34.2 17.9
N 392 392 392 392 392 392 392 392 325 325 291 291 285 285 311 311
Mean .34 .18 19.98 10.85 20.42 11.21 8.11 12.01 19.68 10.30 20.52 10.45 3.38 4.94 3.77 4.29
SD .47 .38 4.34 3.24 4.72 3.19 4.87 6.45 4.17 3.10 4.70 2.99 3.25 4.77 4.16 4.66
**

p < .01,

*

p < .05,

+

p < .10.

Depressive history assessed via the SCID, maternal and paternal permissiveness and authoritarianism via the PSDQ, and child symptoms via the CBCL.

Procedure

At the age 3 wave of assessments, both parents completed the Structured Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID; Williams et al. 1992; First et al. 1996), from which a lifetime history of depressive disorders was ascertained. At the age 3 and 6 assessment waves, both parents completed the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ; Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 2001) as a measure of authoritarianism and permissiveness. At the age 3, 6, and 9 waves, both parents also completed the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000, 2001), from which their child’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms were scored.

Materials

Lifetime history of depressive disorders

History of depressive disorders (MDD and dysthymic disorder) was determined from the non-patient version of the SCID (First et al. 1996) at the age 3 assessment. The SCID is among the most widely used diagnostic interviews, and its inter-rater reliability and procedural validity have been well documented (Williams et al. 1992). The SCID interviews were conducted by master’s-level clinicians and clinical psychology graduate students. Inter-rater reliability for a lifetime diagnosis of a depressive disorder was excellent (κ = 0.93). In the current sample, 134 (34.2%) mothers and 70 (17.9%) fathers met criteria for a lifetime depressive disorder. Both parents, where available, completed the SCID again when children were age 9. 44 mothers (11.2%) experienced an episode during the age 3 to 9 interval, 16 (4.1%) of which were first onsets, while 15 fathers (3.8%) experienced an episode, 5 (1.2%) of which were first episodes. Thus, of the parents in our sample who ever had a depressive episode, the majority of both mothers (66.7%) and fathers (78.6%) only had an episode prior to the beginning of the study. The effects of episodes after the onset of the study were not examined in the current study.

Parenting Styles

Parenting styles at the age 3 and age 6 waves were assessed via the authoritarianism and permissiveness subscales of the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ; Robinson et al., 2001), a widely used parent-report measure of parenting styles. Authoritarianism assesses parental behaviors of physical coercion, verbal hostility, and non-reasoning and punitive behaviors, and comprises 12 items (e.g., “I use physical punishment as a way of disciplining my child). Permissiveness assesses the extent to which parents are overly indulgent towards their children, and comprises five items (e.g., “I give in to my child when the child causes a commotion about something”). Parents are asked “for each item, rate how often you exhibit this behavior with your child.” Response options range from 1 (“Never”) to 3 (“About half of the time”) to 5 (“Always”). The PSDQ has shown good internal consistency, agreement with informants, and convergent validity with child psychosocial functioning (Robinson et al., 2001; see Olivari, Tagliabue, & Confalonieri, 2013, for a review). In the current sample, at the age 3 and age 6 assessments, Cronbach alphas for mother/father permissiveness were .74/.69 and .74/.67, respectively. For mother/father authoritarianism, at the age 3 and age 6 waves, they were .75/.77 and .74/.76.

Child internalizing and externalizing symptoms

At the age 3 wave, internalizing and externalizing symptoms were assessed with the Child Behavior Checklist for ages 1.5–5 (CBCL 1.5–5; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). At the age 6 and 9 waves symptoms were assessed via mothers’ and fathers’ reports on the Child Behavior Checklist for ages 6–18 (CBCL 6–18; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The CBCL 1.5–5 is a 100 item, and the CBCL 6–18 is a 119-item, parent-report checklist assessing emotional and behavioral problems in children. The internalizing and externalizing subscales show good internal consistency and convergence with interview-based symptom measures (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The CBCL 1.5–5 internalizing problems subscale consists of 36 items, while the externalizing subscale consists of 24 items. The CBCL 6–18 internalizing problems subscale consists of 32 items, while the externalizing subscale consists of 35 items. Each item is rated for the past 6 months on a scale from 0 (not true) to 2 (very or often true). For mothers, the age 3, 6, and 9 wave coefficient alphas were .84, .87, and .86 for internalizing problems, and .90, .88, and .88, for externalizing symptoms, respectively. For fathers, the age 3, 6, and 9 wave coefficient alphas were .83, .78, and .89 for internalizing problems, and .91, .89, and .94, for externalizing symptoms, respectively. Mothers’ and father’s reports showed moderate correlations across the age 3, 6, and 9 assessments for internalizing (r = .39, .51, .43, ps < .001), and externalizing (r = .48, .60, .51, ps < .001) problems. Mothers’ and father’s scores on each symptom variable were therefore averaged at each time point. At the age 3 wave, 392 mothers and 392 fathers completed the CBCL. At the age 6 assessment, 321 mothers and 295 fathers completed the CBCL. At the age 9 wave, 329 mothers and 313 fathers completed the CBCL. If scores for only one parent were available, that parent’s scores were used, although this was only an issue for 26 cases at age 6 and 16 cases at age 9.

Regarding internalizing symptoms, based on the average of mothers’ and fathers’ reports of internalizing symptoms, 14 (3.6%), 20 (5.1%), and 26 (6.6%) participants respectively fell in the clinical or borderline range at ages 3, 6, and 9. Regarding externalizing symptoms, based on the average of mothers’ and fathers’ reports, 13 (3.3%), 29 (7.4%), and 23 (5.9%) participants respectively fell in the clinical or borderline range at ages 3, 6, and 9.

Data Analyses

Analyses were conducted in MPlus 7.31. Analyses consisted of lagged path models. Analyses adjusted for the effects of other symptoms (e.g., Age 3 externalizing predicting age 6 internalizing symptoms), thereby rendering our analyses quite conservative. Our model is depicted in Figure 1. All variables were covaried within each time point, with parental depressive histories being covaried with each other. All analyses initially adjusted for child sex and mother’s and father’s age; however, these were unrelated to any variables at the age 6 or 9 assessments, and were therefore dropped from our model. Differences in effects across child gender were examined via multigroup models in which the fit of a model where paths of theoretical interest are constrained to be equal across groups is compared to a model in which these paths are free to vary across groups.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Results relating paternal and maternal history of depression to parenting styles and child internalizing and externalizing symptoms at the age 3, 6, and 9 assessment waves.

Note: + p < .10, * p < .05, * p < .01. Covariances between endogenous terms refer to covariances on the error terms of those variables, although endogenous variable errors for age 3 and 6 variables are not depicted for visual clarity. Bolded lines are those included in mediation analyses.

One concern is that parental lifetime depressive history was assessed concurrently with their parenting behaviors and their children’s symptoms at the age 3 wave, and so the directionality of these relationships may not be clear, as both parenting and child symptoms may be confounded with parents’ current depressive status. However, of our effective sample, at the age 3 wave, only 7 mothers (1.8%) and 3 (0.76%) fathers were depressed during the month prior to the assessment. Results were recomputed after excluding these currently depressed parents, but were identical to those reported here, and so they were retained in our sample. Results were also recomputed after excluding parents who had a first onset of depression during the study interval, but were identical to those reported here, and so these parents were retained in our sample. Thus, in almost all cases, parents’ depression occurred before the assessments of parenting and child symptoms in the initial assessment wave.

As measures of goodness of fit, we present chi-square, ratio of chi-square to degrees of freedom, comparative fit index (CFI), and root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA). Generally, a χ2/df less than 2 (Carmines & McIver, 1981), CFI values greater than .90 (Hoyle & Panter, 1995), and a RMSEA of less than .08 (Kline, 1998) indicate acceptable fit. MPlus uses a bias-corrected bootstrapping procedure to estimate confidence intervals of indirect effects. Boostrapping is a non-parametric method based on resampling (5,000 in the current study) with replacement (Bollen & Stine, 1990; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). We present 95% confidence intervals as well as parameter estimates associated with the indirect effect. Estimates reported in Figures 1 are based on missing data being handled via FIML. For clarity, in Figure 1, the paths which were included in our mediation analyses are bolded. We retained non-significant paths so that results may be interpreted in the context of the effects of each variable.

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations are presented for all variables in Table 1. Mothers’ and father’s depressive histories were positively correlated. Both maternal and paternal depressive history were associated with children’s increased internalizing and externalizing symptoms at ages 3 and 6, although only maternal depressive history was associated with greater child internalizing and externalizing symptoms at age 9. Maternal depressive history was also associated with greater levels of paternal permissiveness, but only trended towards being associated with her own permissiveness. It was also associated with more maternal authoritarianism at age 6, and trended towards being associated with more paternal authoritarianism. Maternal depressive history was also positively associated with greater maternal and paternal permissiveness at age 6. Fathers’ depressive history was positively associated with paternal authoritarianism at age 3, and with both their own and mothers’ permissiveness. Finally, fathers’ depressive history was associated with greater levels of their own authoritarianism at age 6, and trended towards a positive association with mothers’ authoritarianism, while also correlating significantly and positively with mothers’ permissiveness at age 6, but only trending towards an association with their own permissiveness. Overall, correlations support the possibility of effects of each parent’s depressive history on the other’s parenting behaviors.

We also examined the relative stabilities of symptoms from one time point to the next. Contrast comparisons showed that from age 3 to 6 compared to age 6 to 9, there were no significant differences in the stability of externalizing symptoms, t (780) = 1.39, p = .17, although internalizing symptoms were more stable from age 6 to 9 compared to from age 3 to 6, t (780) = 4.37, p < .001.

Child gender analyses

Constraining all regression paths to be equal across child genders yielded the following fit statistic: χ2 (62, N = 392) = 91.191, p = .01, χ2/df = 1.45, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .034. Allowing them to vary across child genders yielded the following fit statistics: χ2 (38, N = 392) = 58.536, p = .02, χ2/df = 1.654, CFI = .99, RMSEA = .037. A chi-square difference test revealed that a model in which paths are free to vary across child gender does not fit the data significantly better than a model in which paths are constrained to be equal, Δχ2 (24) = 32.66, p = .11, indicating that our paths of theoretical interest do not vary significantly as a function of the gender of the child.

Parental depression, parenting styles, and child psychopathology

Given the lack of differences for child gender, analyses were collapsed across male and female children. Our model (Figure 1) showed the following acceptable fit indices: χ2 (25, N = 392) = 40.82, p = .02, χ2/df = 1.63, CFI = .99, RMSEA = .04. Figure 1 presents the standardized parameters for the final models. Maternal and paternal depressive history were modestly, but positively correlated. Maternal depressive history was unrelated to her parenting styles at the age 3 wave, but predicted higher levels of paternal permissiveness, as well as greater child internalizing and externalizing symptoms in the age 3 wave. Maternal depressive history also predicted increased maternal permissiveness in the age 6 wave. Paternal depressive history was unrelated to paternal permissiveness and authoritarianism at the age 3 wave. However, it was independently associated with elevated maternal permissiveness at the age 3 wave, as well as increased child internalizing symptoms at age 3, and trended towards predicting increased child externalizing symptoms at age 3. Neither parents’ depressive history predicted changes in fathers’ parenting from age 3 to 6. However, child externalizing symptoms at age 3 predicted increases in fathers’ authoritarianism in the age 6 wave, as well as increases in maternal permissiveness and authoritarianism in the age 6 wave. Finally, the effects of maternal permissiveness and authoritarianism at age 6 on child externalizing symptoms at age 9 appeared to cancel each other out. That is, when both paths were included in the model, neither was significant. However, trimming either resulted in the other becoming significant. As such, indirect effects predicting age 9 child externalizing symptoms are reported both when only an effect of only maternal permissiveness was included, and when only an effect of maternal authoritarianism was included.

We also computed a series of contrast comparisons to test whether the effects of maternal versus paternal history of depression on their permissiveness at age 3 significantly differed from each other. No significant differences emerged (all ps > .20), suggesting that while there was a significant effect of each parent’s depressive history on the other parents’ permissive parenting, it was not significantly greater in magnitude than the non-significant effects of the other parent’s own depressive history.

Results from boostrapped indirect effects are reported in Table 2. Results showed a significant indirect effects from maternal depressive history to age 6 maternal permissiveness and authoritarianism as well as to age 6 paternal authoritarianism via child externalizing symptoms at age 3. Similarly, there were significant indirect effects of paternal depressive history to age 6 maternal authoritarianism and permissiveness as well as to paternal authoritarianism via child externalizing symptoms at age 3. There was also a significant indirect effect of maternal depressive history to age 9 child externalizing symptoms via maternal permissiveness at age 6.

Table 2.

Results of mediation analyses.

Path b Bootstrapped 95% CI K2
Maternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Paternal Authoritarianism 6 .237 .054 – .560 .030
Maternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Authoritarianism 6 .253 .071 – .567 .044
Paternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Paternal Authoritarianism 6 .191 .001 – .567 .020
Paternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Authoritarianism 6 .204 .008 – .546 .028
Maternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Permissiveness 6 .129 .020 – .348 .027
Paternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Permissiveness 6 .104 .005 – .327 .018
Maternal Dx → Maternal Permissiveness 6 → Age 9 Externalizing .120 .017 – .310 .022
Maternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Authoritativeness 6 → Age 9 Externalizing .026 .003 – .092
Maternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Permissiveness 6 → Age 9 Externalizing .020 .002 – .081
Paternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Authoritativeness 6 → Age 9 Externalizing .021 .001 – .087
Paternal Dx → Age 3 Externalizing → Maternal Permissiveness 6 → Age 9 Externalizing .016 .001 – .067

Note: Estimates of indirect effects are unstandardized. Dx = Depressive history.

Results also showed several serial mediation effects (Table 1). Specifically, there were indirect effects of both maternal and paternal depressive history on age 9 child externalizing symptoms via age 3 child externalizing symptoms to age 6 maternal permissiveness as well as maternal authoritarianism. Taken together, mediational analyses in both mothers and fathers’ models suggest multiple transactional relationships over time between parental depressive history, parenting styles, and child externalizing symptoms.

Discussion

This study examined the effect of parental depressive history on parents’ own and their co-parents’ authoritarianism and permissiveness towards their children at ages 3 and 6 years, and the transactional relationship between these parenting behaviors and child internalizing and externalizing symptoms at the ages of 3, 6, and 9. We were able to examine these relationships over a fairly long developmental period, thereby allowing us to examine predictors of change in our variables over time. We also included both mothers’ and fathers’ reports of child symptoms at each time point, which diminishes the likelihood that effects were inflated due to method variance. This is the first study of which we are aware to simultaneously examine the role of each parent’s depressive history on the other parent’s parenting, while also integrating these factors with the development of child psychopathology from early to late childhood.

In fathers, and consistent with prior literature (Wilson & Durbin, 2010), a history of a depressive disorder was associated with greater authoritarianism, but was not significantly associated with their permissiveness, which was predicted by their co-parent having a history of depression. In parallel, while mother’s depressive history was unrelated to her parenting behaviors in the age 3 wave, paternal depressive history was associated with greater maternal permissiveness. However, we should acknowledge that the magnitude of the effect of paternal versus maternal depressive history on the other’s permissiveness did not significantly differ from each other. Moreover, maternal depressive history predicted increases in maternal permissiveness from the age 3 wave to the age 6 wave, while paternal depressive history showed a similar, but trend-level, effect.

In mothers, only her own history of depression was related to elevations in child externalizing symptoms at age 3, although there was a trend level effect of fathers’ depressive history, as well. Fathers’ permissiveness at the age 3 wave predicted increases in child externalizing symptoms from age 3 to 6, while child externalizing symptoms at age 3 predicted increases in paternal authoritarianism in the age 6 wave. However, we did not find any effects of paternal parenting in the age 6 wave on child psychopathology at age 9. Child externalizing symptoms at age 3 also predicted increases in mothers’ permissiveness and authoritarianism in the age 6 wave. While mothers’ parenting behaviors in the age 3 wave did not predict child symptoms at age 6, both parenting styles independently predicted increases in child externalizing symptoms at age 9. This may suggest that in older children, permissive and authoritarian parenting may additively influence their children’s externalizing symptoms.

Parental depressive history and parenting styles

Results are partially consistent with a large body of literature demonstrating the effects of parental depressive history on parenting behaviors. In both parents, a history of depression was not significantly associated with their own permissive parenting at the age 3 wave, although mothers’ history did predict increases in her own permissiveness from the age 3 to the age 6 wave. However, a history of depression in the mother predicted fathers’ permissiveness in the age 3 assessment, whereas a history of depression in the father predicted mothers’ permissiveness in the age 3 wave. Findings in both parents support the role of the co-parent’s depressive history in understanding each parent’s parenting styles, in particular permissiveness, over and above their own depressive history.

The reasons for this, and especially the lack of effect of parents’ depressive history on their own permissiveness in the age 3 assessment, are speculative and cannot be gleaned from the data. Bivariate results showed positive correlations between each parent’s parenting, and that parental depression was associated with elevated negative parenting styles in the age 3 wave. It is therefore possible that a parent with a history of depression is showing more adverse parenting behaviors, which are in turn either being modeled by, or explicitly taught to, the other parent. An alternative explanation, which is compatible with this first possibility, is that parents with a depressive history experienced more negative parenting themselves, which likely contributed to their depression risk. Furthermore, it is well-established that parenting is transmitted cross-generationally (e.g., Beaver & Belsky, 2012; Belsky, Conger, & Capaldi, 2009). Thus, a depressed parent who experienced negative parenting themselves, may engage in those parenting behaviors with their own children, which are then modeled for, or taught to, the other parent.

Yet another explanation may be that co-parenting a child with a partner who has a history of depression may be stressful and burdensome for that parent. That is, even though very few parents in our sample were currently depressed at the age 3 wave, those with a history of depression may show stable traits, such as high levels of neuroticism, self-criticism, or other elevated depressogenic personality or cognitive traits (see Klein et al., 2011; Kopala-Sibley et al., 2016; Kopala-Sibley & Zuroff, 2014), or other factors which rendered them vulnerable to depression, and which also impose a burden on the other parent. This may also explain the specificity of our effects to permissiveness; it is possible that when coping with the traits or behaviors associated with a co-parent’s depressive history, parents may have less energy to exert control over their children. Another possibility is that a depressive history in one parent may undermine the support they are able to provide the other, or that it leads to increased marital conflict (Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000), thereby influencing that parent’s parenting styles. This possibility would be consistent with spillover models of marital conflict that propose that adverse marital behaviors influence how each parent acts towards the child (see Gerard et al., 2006; Katz & Gottman, 1996). Assortative mating (Nordsletten et al., 2016) may also offer an explanation. That is, men and women with depressive histories are more likely than those without to pick mates with certain characteristics. It is possible that this extends to parenting behaviors, in that men and women with a depressive history are more likely to marry more permissive or authoritarian spouses. Should our findings replicate in future research, there are multiple potential avenues through which co-parent depression may affect the other parent’s parenting that should be investigated in future research.

Parental depressive history, parenting, and child psychopathology

Results are also consistent with prior literature linking parental history of psychopathology to child psychopathology (See Downey & Coyne, 1990; Goodman & Gotlib, 1999), as well as evidence that parenting behaviors may mediate this effect (Downey & Coyne, 1990). Our findings also provide further support for models of coercive parenting (Patterson, 1982; Granic & Patterson, 2006; Patterson et al., 1984) and transactional models of parenting and child psychopathology (Belsky, 1984; Lytton 1990; Plomin et al. 1977; Scarr & McCartney 1983). In fathers, results suggest a pathway from both parents’ depressive history to paternal permissiveness, which in turn contributes to increases in child externalizing symptoms three years later. At the same time both parent’s depressive histories were associated with greater child externalizing symptoms at age 3 which in turn predicted increases in paternal authoritarianism, and in both maternal permissiveness and authoritarianism, in the age 6 assessment, with these parenting styles subsequently predicting increases in child externalizing symptoms at age 9.

We should also note the lack of effects of parenting on child internalizing or vice versa, which is largely consistent with prior literature that has often failed to show an effect of child internalizing symptoms on parenting when adjusting for child externalizing symptoms (Hipwell et al., 2008; Reitz et al., 2006). The reasons for the lack of effects of parenting on child internalizing symptoms (McLeod et al., 2007a, 2007) are unclear from the current study. Although zero-order correlations supported longitudinal bivariate associations of maternal and paternal permissiveness as well as maternal authoritarianism with child age 6 internalizing symptoms, this association was non-significant when adjusting for baseline child internalizing and externalizing symptoms. It is also possible that the lack of effects is due to the fact that risk for depression only begins to increase in adolescence. As such, we may have found more effects of parenting on internalizing symptoms in an older sample. We also did not find any differences in effects across gender. These, too, may only emerge in early adolescence, as it is during this period that the gender difference in rates of depression emerges (Hankin et al., 1998). It is therefore possible that parenting may have a more pronounced effect in females relative to males at this age. Another finding to note is that parental depressive history was associated with elevated levels of internalizing symptoms at the age 3 wave, which in turn predicted age 6 internalizing symptoms. This may indicate a direct effect of the intergenerational transmission of depression.

Results pertaining to child externalizing symptoms suggest that, first, a history of depression in either parent confers an increased likelihood of externalizing symptoms in children, although we cannot know from our data if this transmission is attributable to environmental, genetic, or other biological mechanisms, as there is evidence for the contribution of each (e.g., Harold et al., 2011; Kim-Cohen et al., 2005; Silberg et al., 2010; Singh et al., 2011). The difficult behaviors characterizing externalizing symptoms in children may then result in some mothers attempting to be more controlling (authoritarian), while other mothers may become more permissive, while still others may attempt to use both parenting styles, or perhaps vacillate between the two in an attempt to alter their child’s externalizing behaviors. These attempts appear to be unsuccessful given that we found both of these parenting styles subsequently predicted increases in child externalizing symptoms three years later. Similarly, a history of depression in both parents is associated with risk for elevated child externalizing symptoms at age 3, albeit at a trend level for the effect of paternal depressive history, which in turn are related to increases in paternal authoritarianism. This suggests, consistent with Patterson’s (Granic & Patterson, 2006) model, that fathers may respond to children’s difficult behaviors by trying to control their child.

There was also evidence of differential relationships between parental depressive history, child psychopathology, and parenting at different ages, suggesting the importance of developmental timing in understanding our results. Indeed, child externalizing symptoms at age 3 predicted parenting styles at age 6, but not vice versa, whereas there were significant effects of parenting at age 6 on child externalizing symptoms at age 9. Some prior literature has examined the importance of the developmental timing of adverse parenting experiences for child psychopathology. While much theory suggests that early adverse experiences should have more potent effects on psychological development than later experiences (e.g., Carlson et al., 1997; Manly et al., 2001), Keiley et al. (2001) also note that in later childhood, children are better able to reflect consciously on the meaning of their experiences and therefore draw inferences about themselves based on these experiences, suggesting that experiences in later childhood may be at least as important as those in earlier childhood. Indeed, Keiley et al. (2001) found that later-harmed children showed greater externalizing symptoms compared to those experiencing harm earlier. In addition, Manly et al. (2001) reported that maltreatment experienced in later childhood predicts externalizing psychopathology over and above the effect of early childhood maltreatment, again highlighting the importance of developmental experiences beyond early childhood. While further research is needed to clarify this issue, our results suggest that parenting appears to have a greater impact on child externalizing symptoms in middle to late childhood, whereas child externalizing symptoms impact parenting earlier on.

We would also note that while the current study focus on transactional simple main effects between parenting and psychopathology, there may be interactive effects between each parent’s behaviors towards their child. For instance, some studies have found protective effects of positive father-child interactions on child outcomes when mothers are depressed (e.g., Goodman, Brogan, Lynch, & Fielding, 1993; Kahn, Brandt, & Whitaker, 2004; Tannenbaum & Forehand, 1994; but see also, e.g., Carro, Grant, Gotlib, & Compas, 1993; Mezulis, Hyde, & R. Clark, 2004). This issue should be further investigated in future research.

Clinically, these results further support the use of parenting interventions to ameliorate risk for psychopathology in children. However, they also suggest that parenting interventions may benefit from taking into consideration both parents’ depressive history when understanding each parent’s parenting. They also indicate that parent-child relationships appear to be more influenced by child externalizing than internalizing symptoms. As such, clinicians may wish to particularly attend to the parenting behaviors of parents of children who show behavioral problems, as both the parenting behaviors and the externalizing symptoms themselves may be ameliorated via directly treating the symptoms in the child, or the behaviors of the parent.

Limitations and future directions

While this study had several strengths, including repeated measurements at three time points over the course of six years, using a fairly large sample, studying fathers in addition to mothers, and including both parents’ reports of their child’s symptoms, several limitations should be noted. First, parenting behaviors and child symptoms were both assessed via parent-report, which may have inflated associations due to shared method variance. However, this concern is mitigated, first, by the inclusion of both parents’ reports of children’s symptoms and, second, by the use of clinical interviews to assess parental lifetime depressive history. Despite this, future studies may benefit from using observational measures of parenting and interview-based measures of children’s symptoms. We also cannot know if our results would extend later into childhood, or into adolescence, or generalize to interview-based assessments of child symptoms, or to diagnoses. Additionally, we did not examine parent reports of their parenting behaviors at age 9, and so could not further examine the transactional relationship between parenting and child psychopathology from age 6 to 9.

We should note that item content on the CBCL is somewhat different at age 3 compared to age 6 and 9. As such, means are not directly comparable, and this may have influenced rank order stability of participants, which may have in turn influenced the effects of our parenting measures on child symptoms. Finally, we cannot completely rule out the possibility that new parental depressive episodes between the age 3 and 9 assessments may have further impacted parenting styles, over and above lifetime depressive history.

Conclusion

We found that parental depressive history is associated with greater permissiveness in the other parent, over and above that parent’s own depressive history. The results also highlight the reciprocal relationships between parenting and child psychopathology, in particular in relation to externalizing symptoms. Our findings also provide further evidence regarding the effects of parental as well as co-parent depressive history on their own and their co-parent’s parenting. The present study helps to elucidate, and support the further investigation of, the complex interrelationships between parental depressive history, parenting, and child psychopathology. Indeed, a better understanding the etiology of parenting behaviors as well as their transactional relationships with child psychopathology is necessary for the further development of strategies to influence these important outcomes.

Acknowledgments

Supported by NIMH grant RO1 MH45757 (Klein). Supported by post-doctoral fellowship to D.C. Kopala-Sibley from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

References

  1. Abidin RR. The determinants of parenting behavior. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1992;21:407–412. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles: An Integrated System of Multi-informant Assessment; Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1 1/2–5; Language Development Survey; Caregiver-Teacher Report Form. University of Vermont; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  3. Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington: Aseba; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  4. Baumrind D. Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology. 1971;4(1):1–103. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beaver KM, Belsky J. Gene-environment interaction and the intergenerational transmission of parenting: testing the differential-susceptibility hypothesis. Psychiatric Quarterly. 2012;83:29–40. doi: 10.1007/s11126-011-9180-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Belsky J. The interrelation of parental and spousal behavior during infancy in traditional nuclear families: An exploratory analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1979;41(4):749–755. [Google Scholar]
  7. Belsky J. The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development. 1984;55(1):83–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00275.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belsky J, Conger R, Capaldi DM. The intergenerational transmission of parenting: introduction to the special section. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(5):1201. doi: 10.1037/a0016245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bradley RH, Vandell DL. Child care and the well-being of children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2007;161(7):669–676. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.7.669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burke JD, Pardini DA, Loeber R. Reciprocal relationships between parenting behavior and disruptive psychopathology from childhood through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(5):679–692. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9219-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carmines EG, McIver JP. Analyzing models with unobserved variables: Analysis of covariance structures. Social Measurement: Current Issues. 1981:65–115. [Google Scholar]
  12. Clark LA, Kochanska G, Ready R. Mothers’ personality and its interaction with child temperament as predictors of parenting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79(2):274. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 3. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  14. Collins WA, Maccoby EE, Steinberger L, Hetherington EM, Bornstein MH. Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist. 2000;55(2):218–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Combs-Ronto LA, Olson SL, Lunkenheimer ES, Sameroff AJ. Interactions between maternal parenting and children’s early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37(8):1151–1163. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Conger RD, Conger KJ, Elder GH, Lorenz FO, Simons RL, Whitbeck LB. A family process model of economic hardship and adjustment of early adolescent boys. Child Development. 1992;63(3):526–541. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01644.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Conger RD, Ge X, Elder GH, Lorenz FO, Simons RL. Economic stress, coercive family process, and developmental problems of adolescents. Child Development. 1994;65(2):541–561. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Coyne JC, Thompson R, Palmer SC. Marital quality, coping with conflict, marital complaints, and affection in couples with a depressed wife. Journal of Family Psychology. 2002;16(1):26–37. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.16.1.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Downey G, Coyne JC. Children of depressed parents: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;108(1):50. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders – Non-Patient Edition. Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  21. Foster CJE, Garber J, Durlak JA. Current and past maternal depression, maternal interaction behaviors, and children’s externalizing and internalizing symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(4):527–537. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9197-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fox J. Applied regression analysis, linear models, and related methods. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  23. Franck KL, Buehler C. A family process model of marital hostility, parental depressive affect, and early adolescent problem behavior: The roles of triangulation and parental warmth. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:614–625. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Frye AA, Garber J. The relations among maternal depression, maternal criticism, and adolescents’ externalizing and internalizing symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33(1):1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-0929-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gere MK, Villabø MA, Torgersen S, Kendall PC. Overprotective parenting and child anxiety: The role of co-occurring child behavior problems. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2012;26(6):642–649. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goodman SH. Depression in mothers. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2007;3:107–135. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2011;14(1):1–27. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Granic I, Patterson GR. Toward a comprehensive model of antisocial development: a dynamic systems approach. Psychological Review. 2006;113(1):101. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Graziano AM, Diament DM. Parent behavioral training an examination of the paradigm. Behavior Modification. 1992;16(1):3–38. doi: 10.1177/01454455920161001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Harold GT, Rice F, Hay DF, Boivin J, Van Den Bree M, Thapar A. Familial transmission of depression and antisocial behavior symptoms: disentangling the contribution of inherited and environmental factors and testing the mediating role of parenting. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:1175–1185. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hawes DJ, Dadds MR, Frost AD, Hasking PA. Do childhood callous-unemotional traits drive change in parenting practices? Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2011;40(4):507–518. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2011.581624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hipwell A, Keenan K, Kasza K, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Bean T. Reciprocal influences between girls’ conduct problems and depression, and parental punishment and warmth: A six year prospective analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(5):663–677. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9206-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hoeve M, Dubas JS, Eichelsheim VI, Van der Laan PH, Smeenk W, Gerris JR. The relationship between parenting and delinquency: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37(6):749–775. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9310-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hollingshead AB. Unpublished manuscript. Yale University; 1975. Four factor index of social status. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hoyle RH, Panter AT. Writing about structural equation models. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Structural equation modeling: concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1995. pp. 158–176. [Google Scholar]
  36. Jouriles EN, Murphy CM, O’Leary KD. Effects of maternal mood on mother-son interaction patterns. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1989;17(5):513–525. doi: 10.1007/BF00916510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kane P, Garber J. The relations among depression in fathers, children’s psychopathology, and father–child conflict: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24(3):339–360. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kazdin AE. Child, parent, and Family-Based Treatment of Aggressive and Antisocial Child Behavior. In: Hibbs ED, Jensen PS, editors. Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders: Empirically based strategies for clinical practice. 2. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2005. pp. 445–476. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kendler KS, Sham PC, MacLean CJ. The determinants of parenting: An epidemiological, multi-informant, retrospective study. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27(3):549–563. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797004704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kim-Cohen J, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Pawlby SJ, Caspi A. Maternal depression and children’s antisocial behavior: nature and nurture effects. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:173–181. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Klein DN, Kotov R, Bufferd SJ. Personality and depression: explanatory models and review of the evidence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2011;7:269–295. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kline RB. Software review: Software programs for structural equation modeling: Amos, EQS, and LISREL. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment. 1998;16(4):343–364. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kopala-Sibley DC, Kotov R, Bromet EJ, Carlson GA, Danzig AP, Black SR, Klein DN. Personality diatheses and Hurricane Sandy: Effects on post-disaster depression. Psychological Medicine. 2016;4:865–875. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kopala-Sibley DC, Zuroff DC. The developmental origins of personality factors from the self-definitional and relatedness domains: A review of theory and research. Review of General Psychology. 2014;18:137. [Google Scholar]
  45. Kopala-Sibley DC, Zuroff DC, Koestner R. The determinants of negative maternal parenting behaviours: maternal, child, and paternal characteristics and their interaction. Early Child Development and Care. 2012;182:683–700. [Google Scholar]
  46. Krishnakumar A, Buehler C. Interparental conflict and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Family Relations. 2000;49(1):25–44. [Google Scholar]
  47. Leinonen JA, Solantaus TS, Punämaki R-L. The specific mediating paths between economic hardship and the quality of parenting. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2002;26:423–435. [Google Scholar]
  48. Little RJ, Rubin DB. The analysis of social science data with missing values. Sociological Methods & Research. 1989;18(2–3):292–326. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lovejoy MC, Graczyk PA, O’Hare E, Neuman G. Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000;20(5):561–592. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00100-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lytton H. Child and parent effects in boys’ conduct disorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology. 1990;26(5):683. [Google Scholar]
  51. McLeod BD, Weisz JR, Wood JJ. Examining the association between parenting and childhood depression: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27(8):986–1003. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. McLeod BD, Wood JJ, Weisz JR. Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:155–172. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Milevsky A, Schlechter M, Netter S, Keehn D. Maternal and paternal parenting styles in adolescents: Associations with self-esteem, depression and life-satisfaction. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2007;16:39–47. [Google Scholar]
  54. Muthén B, Kaplan D, Hollis M. On structural equation modeling with data that are not missing completely at random. Psychometrika. 1987;52:431–462. [Google Scholar]
  55. Nordsletten AE, Larsson H, Crowley JJ, Almqvist C, Lichtenstein P, Mataix-Cols D. Patterns of nonrandom mating within and across 11 major psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(4):354–361. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.3192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ogden T, Hagen KA. Treatment effectiveness of Parent Management Training in Norway: a randomized controlled trial of children with conduct problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76:607. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.4.607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. O’Leary SG, Slep AMS, Reid MJ. A longitudinal study of mothers’ overreactive discipline and toddlers’ externalizing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27(5):331–341. doi: 10.1023/a:1021919716586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Olino TM, Klein DN, Dyson MW, Rose SA, Durbin CE. Temperamental emotionality in preschool-aged children and depressive disorders in parents: associations in a large community sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119(3):468–478. doi: 10.1037/a0020112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Papp LM, Cummings EM, Goeke-Morey MC. Parental psychological distress, parent–child relationship qualities, and child adjustment: Direct, mediating, and reciprocal pathways. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2005;5:259–283. [Google Scholar]
  60. Pardini DA, Fite PJ, Burke JD. Bidirectional associations between parenting practices and conduct problems in boys from childhood to adolescence: The moderating effect of age and African-American ethnicity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(5):647–662. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9162-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pardini DA. Novel insights into longstanding theories of bidirectional parent–child influences: Introduction to the special section. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(5):627–631. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9231-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Patterson GR. Coercive Family Process. Vol. 3. Castalia Publishing Company; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  63. Patterson GR, Reid JB, Dishion TJ. Antisocial boys. Vol. 4. Castalia Pub Co; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  64. Patterson GR, DeBaryshe BD, Ramsey E. A Developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist. 1989;44(2):329–335. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.44.2.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Paulson JF, Dauber S, Leiferman JA. Individual and combined effects of postpartum depression in mothers and fathers on parenting behavior. Pediatrics. 2006;118(2):659–668. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Plomin R, DeFries JC, Loehlin JC. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 1977;84(2):309. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Proulx CM, Helms HM, Buehler C. Marital quality and personal well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2007;69(3):576–593. [Google Scholar]
  68. Rapee RM. Potential role of childrearing practices in the development of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review. 1997;17(1):47–67. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(96)00040-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Reitz E, Deković M, Meijer AM. Relations between parenting and externalizing and internalizing problem behaviour in early adolescence: Child behaviour as moderator and predictor. Journal of Adolescence. 2006;29(3):419–436. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Reitz E, Deković M, Meijer AM, Engels RC. Longitudinal relations among parenting, best friends, and early adolescent problem behavior testing bidirectional effects. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2006;26(3):272–295. [Google Scholar]
  71. Robinson CC, Mandleco B, Olsen SF, Hart CH. Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: Development of a new measure. Psychological Reports. 1995;77:819–830. [Google Scholar]
  72. Robinson CC, Mandleco B, Olsen SF, Hart CH. The parenting styles and dimensions questionnaire (PSDQ) Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques. 2001;3:319–321. [Google Scholar]
  73. Rothbaum F, Weisz JR. Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:55. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Scarr S, McCartney K. How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype→ environment effects. Child Development. 1983;54:424–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1983.tb03884.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Schaefer ES. A configurational analysis of children’s reports of parent behavior. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 1965;29:552. doi: 10.1037/h0022702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:147–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Schludermann E, Schludermann S. Replicability of factors in children’s report of parent behavior (CRPBI) The Journal of Psychology. 1970;76:239–249. [Google Scholar]
  78. Seng AC, Prinz RJ. Parents who abuse: What are they thinking? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2008;11:163–175. doi: 10.1007/s10567-008-0035-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Silberg JL, Maes H, Eaves LJ. Genetic and environmental influences on the transmission of parental depression to children’s depression and conduct disturbance: an extended Children of Twins study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:734–744. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02205.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Simons RL, Lorenz FO, Conger RD, Wu CI. Support from spouse as mediator and moderator of the disruptive influence of economic strain on parenting. Child Development. 1992;63:1282–1301. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01695.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Singh AL, D’Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, … Martin NG. Parental depression and offspring psychopathology: a Children of Twins study. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:1385–1395. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710002059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Soper DS. A-priori Sample Size Calculator for Multiple Regression [Software] 2015 Available from http://www.danielsoper.com/statcalc.
  83. Weissman MM, Paykel ES. The depressed woman: A study of social relationships. U Chicago Press; 1974. [Google Scholar]
  84. Williams JB, Gibbon M, First MB, Spitzer RL, Davies M, Borus J, … Wittchen HU. The structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R (SCID): II. Multisite test-retest reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1992;49(8):630–636. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080038006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wilson S, Durbin CE. Effects of paternal depression on fathers’ parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(2):167–180. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yap MBH, Pilkington PD, Ryan SM, Jorm AF. Parental factors associated with depression and anxiety in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;156:8–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Zekoski EM, O’Hara MW, Wills KE. The effects of maternal mood on mother-infant interaction. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1987;15(3):361–378. doi: 10.1007/BF00916455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES