Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jan 5.
Published in final edited form as: Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol. 2017 Jan;23(1):5–14. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000076

Examining the Association Between Perceived Discrimination and Heart Rate Variability in African Americans

LaBarron K Hill 1, Ashley S Richmond 2, Lori S Hoggard 3, DeLeon L Gray 4, Dewayne P Williams 5, Julian F Thayer 6
PMCID: PMC5755701  NIHMSID: NIHMS929295  PMID: 28045306

Abstract

Objective

Previous research attempting to delineate the role of discrimination in racial/ethnic disparities in hypertension has focused largely on blood pressure, which is chiefly governed by the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system. Consequently, few studies have considered the role of the parasympathetic branch and particularly its regulation of the heart via the vagus nerve.

Method

In the present cross-sectional study, we employed hierarchical linear regressions to examine associations between perceived ethnic discrimination and resting heart rate variability (HRV), an important biomarker of parasympathetic cardiac modulation and overall health, in a sample (N = 103) of young, healthy African American participants (58% female, Mage = 19.94 years, SD = 2.84).

Results

After accounting for demographic factors and health status characteristics, lifetime discrimination emerged as an inverse predictor of HRV. When subdomains of discrimination were considered, discrimination attributable to threats or actual acts of aggression was also predictive of lower HRV.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that a greater lifetime burden of discrimination and discriminatory harassment and/or assault is associated with lower resting HRV in African Americans. The implications of these findings are discussed in the context of past, present and emerging research emphasizing biological linkages between discrimination and health.

Keywords: African Americans, discrimination, heart rate variability, threat


African Americans continue to face the greatest burden of hypertension in the world (Go et al., 2013). Disparities in cardiovascular disease (CVD) prevalence, morbidity, and mortality account for a substantial portion of observed differences in life expectancy between African Americans and Whites (Kochanek, Arias, & Anderson, 2013; Miniño, 2013), and hypertension alone accounts for 15% of all CVD deaths among African Americans (Wong, Shapiro, Boscardin, & Ettner, 2002). Amid growing recognition of the important contribution of psychosocial stressors in CVD etiology and progression (Dimsdale, 2008; Hamer & Malan, 2010; Mensah, Mokdad, Ford, Greenlund, & Croft, 2005; Steptoe & Kivimäki, 2012), a stalwart hypothesis has been the notion that frequent experiences of racism and discrimination play a crucial role in the development of hypertension and broader health disparities faced by African Americans (Anderson & Armstead, 1995; Anderson, McNeilly, & Myers, 1991; Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999). Notably, in a seminal work, Clark and colleagues (1999) posited that the experience of racism and racial discrimination across multiple domains (i.e., structural, institutional and interpersonal) represents a unique form of chronic psychosocial stress. According to this Biopsychosocial Model of Racism, African Americans experience frequent exposure to discrimination, which may serve to initiate exaggerated psychological and physiological (i.e., fight-or-flight) responses. In addition, engaging in passive or other potentially maladaptive forms of coping (i.e., anger) may actually serve to prolong mental and physical reactions to the stressor. Over time, this chronic pattern of exposure, poor or dysfunctional coping, and exaggerated reactivity is argued to contribute to dysregulation of the cardiovascular system, ultimately giving rise to chronic elevations in blood pressure (BP) vis-á-vis hypertension (Anderson et al., 1991; Clark et al., 1999).

Consistent with the stress and coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and reactivity (e.g., Krantz & Manuck, 1984; Manuck, Kasprowicz, & Muldoon, 1990) frameworks on which it is based, the Biopsychosocial Model of Racism suggests a prominent role of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) as a major biological pathway linking discrimination-related stress to health and disease. In particular, the sympathetic branch of the ANS plays a substantial role in the regulation of BP and chronically heightened sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activity has been characterized as the primary driver of hypertension (Amerena & Julius, 1995; Brook & Julius, 2000; Julius, Schork, & Schork, 1988; Palatini & Julius, 2009), especially in African Americans (e.g., Hill, Sollers, Edwards, Thayer, & Whitfield, 2014; Taherzadeh, Brewster, van Montfrans, & VanBavel, 2010).

A number of studies have focused on the association between discrimination and BP, or hypertension (for reviews see Brondolo, Love, Pencille, Schoenthaler, & Ogedegbe, 2011; Brondolo, Rieppi, Kelly, & Gerin, 2003; Couto, Goto, & Bastos, 2012; Cuffee, Hargraves, & Allison, 2012; Dolezsar, McGrath, Herzig, & Miller, 2014; Harrell, Hall, & Taliaferro, 2003; Paradies, 2006; Williams & Neighbors, 2001); however, there is an ongoing debate regarding the consistency of this relationship (e.g., Brondolo et al., 2011; Couto et al., 2012; Dolezsar et al., 2014; Paradies, 2006). For instance, in a recent meta-analysis of perceived racial discrimination and hypertension/BP studies spanning a 30-year period, Dolezsar and colleagues (2014) found that perceived discrimination was consistently associated with hypertensive status; however, the size of this effect was relatively small (Fisher’s z = .05) and was moderated by such factors as Black race/ethnicity, male gender, older age and educational status. In addition, the extent to which discrimination was associated with BP also varied, as effects were much stronger for ambulatory blood pressure (ABP) among Blacks, and nighttime diastolic ABP, in particular, as compared with resting systolic (SBP) or diastolic (DBP) blood pressure (Dolezsar et al., 2014). As these findings suggest, a focus on sympathetic mechanisms alone may provide only a partial view of the complex, autonomic pathway between discrimination and cardiovascular outcomes. In the present study, we diverge from these previous approaches by examining the relationship between discrimination and parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) activity as a means of better understanding how discrimination experiences may erode health.

Although BP forms the essential basis for a diagnosis of hypertension, the heart is an essential determinant of BP. Regulation of the heart is complex, involving, among other factors, dynamic interactions of sympathetic and parasympathetic activity. In general terms, during periods of arousal, sympathetic neural activation drives an increase in heart rate (HR), which is important for mobilization of an adequate response (i.e., to fight or flee) to stressors. In contrast, parasympathetic modulation of the heart (via the vagus nerve) is primarily inhibitory, promoting a slower HR and the conservation of energy. Additionally, under resting conditions, parasympathetic (i.e., vagal) activity is predominant over sympathetic influence (Saul, 1990). Thus, parasympathetic activity is a more sensitive marker of the relative impact of acute and chronic stressors on cardiovascular functioning and health.

Heart rate variability (HRV) is a quantification of parasympathetic cardiac influence, determined from the continuous intervals in time from one heartbeat to the next (i.e., interbeat interval, IBI). Higher HRV is generally considered to be cardio-protective and is regarded as an indicator of better physical and mental health and well-being (Kemp & Quintana, 2013). Lower HRV has been related to established risk factors for CVD, including smoking, obesity, total and low density cholesterol, positive family history of CVD, and age (Thayer, Yamamoto, & Brosschot, 2010). Moreover, lower HRV has been shown to predict the onset of hypertension (Schroeder, Liao, Chambless, Prineas, Evans, & Heiss, 2003), as well as increased CVD risk and all-cause mortality (Thayer & Lane, 2007). Further, lower HRV has been linked to poorer mental health outcomes including depression (Larsen & Christenfeld, 2009) and anxiety (Friedman, 2007; Tully, Cosh, & Baune, 2013). Given the high rates of comorbidity between depression and CVD, some researchers have proposed that HRV may be a common mechanism linking mental and physical health (Larsen & Christenfeld, 2009).

Researchers have similarly hypothesized that lower HRV may, at least partially, explain how stressors arising from minority status and socioeconomic disadvantages “get under the skin” and contribute to the greater rates of CVD among African Americans (Lampert, Ickovics, Horwitz, & Lee, 2005). Indeed, some research has reported findings of lower HRV in African Americans relative to Whites (e.g., Choi et al., 2006; Lampert et al., 2005), whereas other research has shown higher HRV in African Americans (Liao et al., 1995; Wang et al., 2005). More recently, Hill and colleagues (2015) conducted a meta-analysis of 17 studies comprising more than 11,000 total participants, reporting ethnic differences in basal HRV between African Americans and Whites. These authors reported that African Americans actually exhibit higher resting HRV, or a relative ‘Vagal Advantage,’ compared with Whites. Importantly, this effect was robust even when accounting for moderating factors such as age or whether studies were conducted in healthy or nonhealthy samples. Interestingly, this effect was observed consistently in African American women, but was not significant for African American men (Hill et al., 2015). In previous research, resting HRV has been characterized as a stable, trait-like measure that reflects individual differences in the capacity to navigate challenges and demands in the environment (Appelhans & Luecken, 2006; Segerstrom & Nes, 2007; Thayer & Lane, 2000). In this regard, individuals with higher HRV are thought to possess better or more efficient emotion regulation capabilities (e.g., Williams, Cash, Rankin, Bernardi, Koenig, & Thayer, 2015) that arguably buffer against the psychological and physiological experience of myriad stressors, including racial discrimination.

Although there is a growing literature on the relationship between psychosocial stressors and HRV, very few studies have examined this association in African Americans. Even fewer studies have considered the relationship between discrimination and HRV. One notable exception is a study by Dorr et al. (2007) wherein the researchers examined hemodynamic and autonomic cardiovascular responses to a racist versus nonracist interaction in a sample of African American men. These researchers found that men instructed to express versus inhibit their anger following the racist interaction exhibited delayed HRV recovery during the 10-min postinteraction period (Dorr, Brosschot, Sollers, & Thayer, 2007). In another laboratory-based study, Neblett and Roberts (2013) found African American participants to exhibit decreases in HRV during a blatantly racist imaginal task. In particular, among individuals with moderate levels of private regard (i.e., moderately positive feelings about one’s racial group membership), there was a greater decrease in HRV during the blatantly racist imaginal scenarios when the perpetrator was White versus Black (Neblett & Roberts, 2013).

In another study, Wagner and colleagues (2015) found an inverse association between lifetime discrimination and HRV during a stressful speech task in a sample of 32 White and African American women with type II diabetes (Wagner, Lampert, Tennen, & Feinn, 2015). Finally, more recently, Hoggard and colleagues (2015) examined the effects of intergroup and intragroup discrimination on HRV across a 2-day period in a sample of African American women. They found that racial discrimination involving an African American perpetrator (i.e., confederate) was actually associated with an increase in HRV during the following 20-min recording period whereas racial discrimination involving a White perpetrator was associated with no change in HRV. On Day 2, those participants who had experienced discrimination from the White perpetrator exhibited lower HRV and higher HR than those who had experienced discrimination involving the African American perpetrator. The researchers concluded that merely returning to the environment in which one has previously experienced a discriminatory event may trigger a shift in cardiac autonomic functioning (Hoggard, Hill, Gray, & Sellers, 2015). Such a pattern could have significant implications for individuals living and working in settings where they frequently perceive or experience racial discrimination.

Fortunately, there have been some positive findings regarding the link between discrimination and HRV. Notably, Utsey and Hook (2007) assessed the impact of resting HRV on the relationship between race-related stress and psychological distress in 215 African American college students. Higher levels of institutional racism were associated with greater psychological distress in both men and women; however, the strength of this association was weaker in men with higher HRV (Utsey & Hook, 2007). In another report, Cooper and colleagues (2014) examined the association between cardiovascular function and the use of prayer coping in response to discrimination in a sample of 81 African American women. In this study, greater prayer coping was positively associated with HRV following a racism recall task (Cooper, Thayer, & Waldstein, 2014). These findings are consistent with the notion that higher HRV may serve to buffer the effects of discrimination-related stress in African Americans. To our knowledge, this handful of studies represents the current literature regarding the potential effects of perceived discrimination on HRV. Thus, the goal of the present study is to further elucidate an important biological mechanism by which accumulative racial discrimination experiences may compromise health.

Although it is evident that racial discrimination is a noxious stressor, researchers are unclear about whether the various forms or facets of interpersonal racial discrimination differentially impact health and well-being (Brondolo et al., 2005; Contrada et al., 2001; Sue et al., 2007). Researchers conducting correlational and survey studies have typically operationalized African Americans’ experiences with racial discrimination as the composite frequency and/or impact score for one of the various racial discrimination scales (i.e., Everyday Discrimination Scale, Schedule of Racist Events, Daily Life Experience Scale). Few, however, have examined racial discrimination as a multidimensional construct with multiple subdomains. Such an examination would clarify whether the various forms or subdomains of racial discrimination (e.g., being harassed or threatened vs. being excluded) differentially impact cardiac functioning. Similarly, evaluating the different dimensions of racism and the psychological and physiological correlates of these dimensions can facilitate an understanding of psychobiological mechanisms linking racism to health (Brondolo et al., 2005). The present study attempts to facilitate this understanding by examining whether the relationship between perceived racial discrimination and HRV is consistent across different subdomains or facets of perceived racial discrimination.

The Present Study

The present study examines the association between lifetime and subdomains of perceived racial discrimination and resting HRV in a sample of African American college students. Previous research on discrimination and HRV has either focused on this relationship in exclusively female (Cooper, Thayer, & Waldstein, 2014; Hoggard et al., 2015; Wagner et al., 2015) or male samples (Dorr et al., 2007); or has conceptualized HRV mainly as a moderator with little attention to its importance as an outcome (Utsey & Hook, 2007). Thus, it is unclear whether perceived racial discrimination has a direct negative impact on parasympathetic cardiac control, or whether this potential relationship is the same for both African American men and women. We hypothesized that perceived racial discrimination would be associated with lower HRV. We were particularly interested in examining whether this relationship was consistent across different aspects of perceived racial discrimination. Indeed, prior research suggests that discrimination related to threats and/or actual instances of harassment or physical harm may be more burdensome on health (Brondolo et al., 2008). Finally, we were interested in examining whether the relationship between perceived racial discrimination and HRV was moderated by gender, as previous research suggests that African American women may be more vulnerable to the psychological effects of racial discrimination (e.g., anxiety; Banks et al., 2006; Greer et al., 2009) whereas African American men may be more vulnerable to the physiological effects of racial discrimination (Morris-Prather et al., 1996).

Method

Participants

One hundred three self-identified African American college students were recruited at a large public university in the Midwest through the Research Experience Program (REP) pool, which allows students to participate in research for partial class credit in an introductory level psychology course. Participants were also recruited, outside of the REP pool, using flyers, campus newspaper ads, and email listservs; these participants received a small monetary compensation (i.e., $15.00) for their participation. All participants were instructed to not smoke, engage in vigorous physical activity, or consume caffeine for at least two hours prior to their scheduled appointment. The study was conducted in compliance with the university Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided their written informed consent. Physiological data (i.e., heart rate) were missing for four participants due to equipment failure. The final sample included 99 African American participants (58% female, Mage = 19.94 years, SD = 2.84) with complete data.

Procedure

The present data represent baseline physiological recordings from a larger psychophysiological protocol. All participants were greeted by a study researcher in a designated waiting area. The participants were escorted to a soundproof experimental room that was equipped with a camera and a microphone for observational and instructional purposes as well as a high definition TV for stimuli presentation. Participants were provided with a general description of the study purpose (i.e., to examine the relationship between stress and physiological activity) as well as a detailed explanation of the procedures for psychophysiological recording in both verbal and written form. After providing consent, participants completed a preliminary packet of study questionnaires including the perceived discrimination measure. Thereafter, participants were outfitted with equipment for physiological recording (i.e., ECG, continuous BP monitoring device). Participants were then instructed to sit quietly and breathe as they normally would, while their baseline physiological activity was recorded for a 5-min period.

Measures

Heart rate variability

Continuous heart rate (HR) data was assessed using a 3-lead ECG at a sampling rate of 1kHz. Electrodes were placed below the right clavicle and on the left and right lower abdomen. Data were visually inspected for artifacts and corrected per recommended guidelines (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996), using a custom software package (HRV 2.51, Mindware Technology, Gahanna, OH). The series of interbeat intervals (IBI), or time in milliseconds between successive R-spikes on the ECG waveform, were then written to a single text file. These files were then imported to the Kubios HRV analysis package 2.0 (Tarvainen, Niskanen, Lipponen, Rantaaho, & Karialainen, 2014) and subjected to spectral analysis yielding time and frequency domain measures of HRV. In the present study, we focus on the high frequency (HF) HRV power band (HF-HRV, 0.15–0.4 Hz), which has previously been shown to be a valid and reliable measure of vagal activity (Thayer, Hansen, & Johnsen, 2010). As the distribution of HF-HRV was positively skewed, the data were natural log-transformed resulting in a normal distribution.

Discrimination

Racial discrimination was assessed using the brief Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire-Community Version (PEDQ-CV; Brondolo et al., 2005). The brief PEDQ-CV is a 17-item questionnaire designed to assess lifetime experiences of racial discrimination and maltreatment, particularly in interpersonal and social contexts. The measure contains four subscales that assess differing domains of discrimination including: social exclusion (Exclusion), stigmatization (Stigma), discrimination at work or school (Work/School), and threats or actual acts of harassment and/or harm (Threats). Each subscale consists of four items and each item is preceded by the stem phrase: “Because of your ethnicity/race, how often …,” followed by statements reflecting each domain: “have others ignored or not paid attention to you” (Exclusion); “have others hinted that you must be lazy/not clean” (Stigma); “have you been treated unfairly by co-workers or classmates” (Work/School); “have others threatened/actually hurt you” (Threats). Participants rated the frequency (1 = never to 7 = very often) with which they have experienced unfair treatment in one of the four domains. A higher score for each subscale is indicative of more frequent experiences with racial discrimination. In addition, all 17 items are summed to create a Lifetime Discrimination scale. In the present sample, internal consistency for the total scale was (α = .90) and ranged from .68 to.78 for the subscales.

Covariates

Covariates for the present study were selected based on previously reported relationships with HRV, including age, gender, body mass index (BMI), and physical activity level (Thayer, Yamamoto, & Brosschot, 2010). Although there was no evidence of hypertension among study participants, we also included resting BP. Indeed, BP has been shown to be inversely related to HRV in African American adolescents and adults (e.g., Keen et al., 2015; Urbina et al., 1998); and is a relative marker of SNS activity. Resting BP was collected continuously during the baseline period using a noninvasive photoplethysmography device.

Demographics

Participants self-reported their age and gender.

Health characteristics

Participant height and weight were measured using a medical grade stadiometer and scale. BMI was calculated as weight in kilograms (kg) divided by height in meters squared (m2). Physical activity was assessed via self-report using the University of Houston Non-Exercise Questionnaire (Jackson et al., 1990), which assesses activity over the previous month on a 0 (i.e., avoid walking or exertion) to 7 (i.e., run 10 miles or 3 hours of comparable weekly physical activity) scale. Higher scores on this measure have been correlated with a greater level of cardio-respiratory fitness (Jackson et al., 1990).

Statistical analysis

We sought to test whether perceived racial discrimination predicts lower HRV, and whether the various aspects of perceived racial discrimination were associated with lower HRV to the same degree. We tested these questions in a hierarchical regression analysis using STATA 12. Specifically, we conducted two hierarchical regression analyses in which HRV served as the dependent variable. All variables were entered in blocks so that coefficients specified at subsequent steps predicted residual variance unexplained by the variables in previous steps. In terms of centering, we standardized all continuous variables. For the first hierarchical regression, gender and the standardized covariates of age, BMI, physical activity, SBP, and DBP were entered at Step 1. In Step 2, we added lifetime experiences of perceived racial discrimination (standardized). Finally, the cross-product of the gender and the standardized version of perceived racial discrimination was entered at Step 3 to assess whether the effect of lifetime experiences of perceived racial discrimination was contingent on whether the individual is male or female. The second hierarchical regression mirrored the first hierarchical regression with one exception: we replaced the lifetime experiences of discrimination predictor with the discrimination subscales (i.e., Exclusion, Stigma, Work & School, and Threat).

Results

Participant Characteristics

Descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations) are presented in Table 1. There were no gender differences in age, F(1, 98) = 1.129, p = .29, ηp2=.01; BMI, F(1, 98) = .538, p = .47, ηp2=.01; SBP, F(1, 98) = 1.022, p = .32, ηp2=.01; DBP, F(1, 98) = .104, p = .75, ηp2=.00; Lifetime Discrimination F(1, 98) = 2.405, p = .124, ηp2=.02; or in the Exclusion F(1, 98) = .086, p = .77, ηp2=.00; Work/School F(1, 98) = 2.329, p = .13, ηp2=.02; or Threat F(1, 98) = .948, p = .33, ηp2=.01 subscales (see Table 1). Males reported higher levels of physical activity F(1, 98) = 8.350, p = .005, ηp2=.08; and marginally higher levels of discrimination due to stigma F(1, 98) = 4.565, p = .06, ηp2=.01, compared with females. There was also a marginal trend, F(1, 98) = 3.106, p = .08, ηp2=.04; for higher resting HF-HRV in females, compared with males.

Table 1.

Means and Standard Deviations

Variable Male Female Total
N 43 56 99
Age (years) 19.63 (1.89) 20.25 (3.46) 19.98 (2.89)
BMI (kg/m2) 26.48 (6.17) 27.49 (7.16) 27.05 (6.73)
Phy. Activity 4.95 (2.16)** 3.79 (1.86) 4.29 (2.07)
SBP (mm Hg) 112.61 (16.69) 109.10 (17.43) 110.63 (17.12)
DBP (mm Hg) 57.64 (14.02) 56.59 (17.27) 57.05 (15.87)
PD-Lifetime 34.00 (10.34) 30.55 (11.41) 32.05 (11.04)
PD-Exclusion 9.58 (3.01) 9.79 (3.74) 9.70 (3.43)
PD-Stigma 8.05 (3.45)* 6.59 (3.3) 7.22 (3.42)
PD-Work/School 8.35 (2.95) 7.38 (3.29) 7.80 (3.17)
PD-Threat 5.65 (2.45) 5.20 (2.19) 5.39 (2.30)
HF-HRV 6.18 (1.15) 6.63 (1.09) 6.43 (1.13)

Note. BMI = body mass index; Phy. = physical; SBP = systolic blood pressure; DBP = diastolic blood pressure, PD = perceived discrimination, HF-HRV = high frequency heart rate variability.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

Hierarchical Regressions

Results of the first hierarchical regression are displayed in Table 2. In Step 1, gender was found to be the only significant predictor of HRV, b = .56, 95% CI [.08, 1.04]. Being female was associated with higher HRV—accounting for 5% of the variance in HRV, sr2 = .054. At Step 2, and consistent with predictions, lifetime experiences of discrimination significantly predicted lower HRV, b = −.26, 95% CI [−.51, −.02]. Approximately 4% of the variance in HRV was attributable to lifetime experiences of discrimination. Gender also remained a significant predictor of HRV after accounting for lifetime experiences with discrimination, b = .46, 95% CI [.02, .94]. The cross-product of gender and lifetime experiences of discrimination did not significantly predict HRV at Step 3, b = −.10, 95% CI [−.59, .39].

Table 2.

First Hierarchical Regression Predicting Heart Rate Variability

Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3



b SE 95% CI lower 95% CI upper b SE 95% CI lower 95% CI upper b SE 95% CI lower 95% CI upper
Intercept 6.12 .18 5.76 6.47 6.16 .18 5.81 6.51 6.15 .18 5.80 6.51
Age .00 .12 −.23 .24 .02 .12 −.22 .25 .01 .12 −.22 .25
Female .56 .24 .08 1.04 .46 .24 .02 .94 .46 .24 −.02 .95
BMI .05 .12 −.19 .28 .08 .12 −.15 .32 .08 .12 −.16 .32
Phy. Activity .21 .13 −.06 .47 .22 .13 −.04 .48 .22 .13 −.04 .48
SBP .10 .18 −.27 .46 .13 .18 −.23 .49 .13 .18 −.24 .49
DBP −.20 .17 −.54 .14 −.26 .17 −.59 .08 −.26 .17 −.59 .08
Discrimination (Lifetime) −.26 .13 −.51 −.02 −.21 .19 −.58 .16
Interaction: Female × Lifetime −.10 .25 −.59 .39

Note. Gender is coded as Male = 0, Female = 1. SBP = systolic blood pressure; DBP = diastolic blood pressure. All continuous variables are standardized. Standardized discrimination variable was used when creating the interaction term. Bold coefficients are significant at p < .05.

Results of the second hierarchical regression are displayed in Table 3. Female gender remained the only significant predictor of HRV in Step 1. When the perceived discrimination subscales were entered into the model at Step 2, discrimination attributable to threat predicted lower HRV, b = −.28, 95% CI [ −.55, −.01]. Approximately 4% of the variance in HRV was attributable to discrimination due to threat, sr2 = .041. Besides gender, no other predictors significantly predicted HRV, b = .53, 95% CI [.02, 1.05]. At Step 3, tests of interactions revealed that there were no gender-contingent associations between discrimination subscales and HRV.1

Table 3.

Second Hierarchical Regression Predicting Heart Rate Variability

Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3



b SE 95% CI lower 95% CI upper b SE 95% CI lower 95% CI upper b SE 95% CI lower 95% CI upper
Intercept 6.12 .18 5.76 6.47 6.13 .18 5.77 6.50 6.10 .19 5.73 6.47
Age .00 .12 −.23 .24 −.02 .13 −.27 .23 .00 .13 −.26 .25
Female .56 .24 .08 1.04 .55 .25 .04 1.05 .53 .26 .02 1.05
BMI .05 .12 −.19 .28 .07 .12 −.18 .31 .07 .12 −.18 .32
Phy. Activity .21 .13 −.06 .47 .24 .14 −.04 .51 .24 .14 −.04 .52
SBP .10 .18 −.27 .46 .12 .18 −.24 .48 .16 .19 −.21 .54
DBP −.20 .17 −.54 .14 −.27 .17 −.61 .07 −.29 .18 −.64 .06
Discrimination (Exclusion) −.13 .16 −.45 .19 −.34 .26 −.86 .18
Discrimination (Stigma) .07 .18 −.28 .42 .09 .25 −.40 .58
Discrimination (Work & School) .01 .18 −.34 .37 .17 .29 −.40 .75
Discrimination (Threat) −.28 .14 −.55 −.01 −.26 .20 −.66 .14
Interaction: Female × Exclusion .43 .35 −.26 1.13
Interaction: Female × Stigma −.12 .36 −.84 .60
Interaction: Female × Work & School −.26 .35 −.96 .43
Interaction: Female × Threat −.14 .28 −.71 .42

Note. Gender is coded as Female = 0, Male = 1. SBP = systolic blood pressure; DBP = diastolic blood pressure. All continuous variables are standardized. Standardized discrimination variable was used when creating the interaction term. Bold coefficients are significant at p < .05.

Discussion

Disparities in health and life expectancy persist for African Americans. Hypertension and related complications account for the largest proportion of CVD-related deaths in African Americans, and there are emerging data linking discrimination to mortality (e.g., Chae et al., 2015), as well. HRV is an important index of cardiovascular function and health, and has also been linked to psychological health and functioning. In the present study, we examined the relationship between perceived racial discrimination, across multiple domains, and resting HRV in a sample of African American emerging adults. We hypothesized that perceived discrimination would be associated with lower HRV. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found a direct, inverse effect of perceived discrimination on resting HRV. This association was not contingent on gender, or other covariates previously associated with lower HRV. In addition, the effect was most robust for more frequent experiences of discrimination involving being threatened or actually being physically harassed as a result of one’s ethnicity. These findings clearly extend the fledgling literature regarding racial discrimination and HRV by demonstrating that discriminatory experiences may gradually reduce an individual’s “first line of defense,” or the initial capacity for coping with chronic racial and nonracial stressors.

Being mindful of the relatively limited nature of our results, it is worth noting that our findings complement and give further context to previous research on discrimination and BP. In particular, early hypertension research revealed that elevations in BP were driven by increased, or hyperactive, SNS activity, whereas PNS activity was hypoactive (Julius, Pascual, & London, 1971). These observations formed the basis of the Autonomic Imbalance model of hypertension (Amerena & Julius, 1995). Additional findings of consistent elevations in SNS vascular activity, both under resting conditions and in response to stressors among African Americans, greatly informed the hypothesis that hypertension was predominately driven by greater SNS activity in this group. Thus, the focus of previous research on the relationship between discrimination and BP was well-grounded. Indeed, it has been relatively recent that scholars have broadened their view to reconsider the role of the PNS. Typically, during the initiation of the fight or flight response, PNS modulation decreases, allowing SNS tone to trigger an increase in HR and further facilitating the full cascade of physiological changes needed to respond to the stressor. Once the threat or challenge has passed, PNS dominance of HR is reasserted. It may be that African Americans have an intrinsically higher SNS vascular tone, and have adapted to this state by developing greater resting HRV (i.e., a Vagal Advantage). Hypothetically, once this rebalanced system is altered, perhaps as a result of chronic psychological distress (i.e., anxiety, Friedman, 2007), HRV is irreversibly diminished, paving the way for relatively unchecked SNS activity and chronic disease. In context, our findings are complementary to previous results showing a positive relationship between racial discrimination and increased BP, as it is possible, though speculative, that HRV may also have been lower among African Americans in these studies. Although HRV declines with age, longitudinal research will be invaluable to ultimately determining whether, how, and when the hypothesized shift from higher to lower resting HRV begins in African Americans.

Scholars have continually emphasized the importance of considering contextual factors that may influence the relationship between discrimination and health (Brondolo, 2015). The Biopsychosocial Model of Racism also acknowledges that factors such as age, gender, individual differences in coping styles (i.e., active vs. passive) and other personality characteristics (i.e., trait anger), socioeconomic status, differing types of discrimination, and the race/ethnicity of the perpetrator may all have important mediating and moderating effects on this relationship. Although we did not find that gender moderated the impact of discrimination on resting HRV, our finding of a relatively consistent gender difference in favor of females is consistent with previous work (Hill et al., 2015) as well as a more general pattern in the larger HRV literature. At first glance, this pattern, at least generally, comports with previous research indicating that African American women may be less susceptible to the physiological impacts of discrimination than African American men (Morris-Prather et al., 1996). Additional research is needed to further explicate the biopsychosocial underpinnings of gender differences in relation to the experience and impact of discrimination.

We found that both lifetime discrimination and discrimination attributable to threat or actually being physically harassed were associated with lower HRV. This supports that broadband measures of discrimination may be useful for future studies of HRV, but also illustrates the additional informative value of multidomain measures. Our findings for the Threat subscale are consistent with previous research indicating that discrimination related to threats and/or actual instances of harassment or physical harm may be more salient and detrimental to health (Brondolo et al., 2008). Although the items on the Threat subscale assess personal experiences, it is tantalizing to consider whether witnessing threats or overt acts of violence, and particularly events interpreted as discriminatory or race-related in origin, has a vicarious physiological impact. For instance, both news and social media have facilitated the broad and rapid sharing of information and profound imagery from recent events in Sanford, Jacksonville, Ferguson, Detroit, North Charleston, Tulsa, Staten Island, Baltimore, and unfortunately, numerous other locales, that may be experienced collectively by African Americans as racially motivated and/or discriminatory. Although far beyond the scope of the present study, it is nonetheless feasible that discriminatory threat and violence, experienced indirectly, influence subsequent behavior (e.g., Himmelstein, Young, Sanchez, & Jackson, 2015) and possibly physiological activity. For example, as Hicken and colleagues (2013) have suggested, heightened vigilance is one possible behavioral consequence of such events and attributions (Hicken, Lee, Ailshire, Burgard, & Williams, 2013). Chronic vigilance for threat is also a common feature of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) as well as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and autonomic dysregulation, including a shift in resting HRV, is a well-established consequence of chronic anxiety (Friedman, 2007; Tully et al., 2013). Current and future research exploring the parallels between racial discrimination and anxiety (e.g., Graham, Calloway, & Roemer, 2015; Hunter & Schmidt, 2010) may be particularly helpful in further determining the physiological costs of race-related vigilance.

Although we have focused primarily on HRV as an outcome, it is also a significant correlate and mediator of other biological mechanisms that have been additionally related to discrimination. In particular, there is emerging interest in the linkages between inflammatory processes and sleep with discrimination. For example, HRV has been inversely related to C-reactive protein (CRP), an important biomarker of systemic and vascular-related inflammation (Johnson, Abbasi, & Master, 2013). African Americans have been shown to exhibit higher CRP levels than Whites (Johnson et al., 2013), and there is growing evidence of a positive association between discrimination and CRP (Beatty Moody, Brown, Mathews, & Bromberger, 2014; Cunningham et al., 2012; Goosby, Malone, Richardson, Cheadle, & Williams, 2015); Lewis, Aiello, Leurgans, Kelly, & Barnes, 2010) as well as other inflammatory markers (Brody, Yu, Miller, & Chen, 2015). Similarly, there is significant correspondence between HRV and both objective and subjective measures of sleep (Stein & Pu, 2012). It is well established that African Americans experience poorer sleep than other groups in the United States (e.g., Durrence & Lichstein, 2006). Previous research has examined the influence of discrimination on nighttime BP (i.e., Hill, Kobayashi, & Hughes, 2007; Tomfohr, Cooper, Mills, Nelesen, & Dimsdale, 2010), and growing evidence suggests that sleep may be another important pathway through which the vigilance associated with discrimination negatively impacts health (e.g., Hicken et al., 2013; Slopen, Lewis, & Williams, 2015). It is not yet clear whether reduced HRV is the cause or consequence of dysfunction in these additional biological pathways. The disruption of multiple biological processes and systems is consistent with the notion of Weathering, or the accumulative impact of multiple co-occurring disruptions in health due to chronic stressors, which contribute to premature biological aging and increased disease risk in African Americans (e.g., Geronimus, Hicken, Keene, & Bound, 2006). Thus, it is crucial that future investigations more closely examine the impact of discrimination on the intersecting relationships among these important overlapping mechanisms.

We acknowledge that our findings reflect a cross-sectional ‘snapshot’ of the relationship between perceived racial discrimination and resting HRV that limits any inferences that can be drawn regarding causality. Moreover, recall bias may have influenced participants’ self-reported experiences of discrimination. However, we incorporated a multidimensional measure of discrimination and the items for some subscales (i.e., Threats) assessed discrete events that may be more salient in memory and thus retrieved with greater specificity. In addition, although our sample size was consistent with previous studies in this area (e.g., Neblett & Roberts, 2013; Utsey & Hook, 2007), our sample composition was relatively homogenous with respect to age and apparent health status. Although these characteristics pose a potential challenge to the generalizability of our findings, it is notable that similar effects were observed in studies of adults with greater variability in age and health status (i.e., Cooper et al., 2014; Dorr et al., 2007; Wagner et al., 2015). In addition, focusing on the impact of racial discrimination among emerging adults has become increasingly important as research suggests that discrimination may begin taking a serious biological toll on African Americans before or by emerging adulthood (Brody et al., 2014; Williams & Mohammed, 2009).

It has been suggested that higher HRV is an index, not only of cardiovascular health, but also of individual resources to adaptively cope with chronic stressors. Our findings suggest that above and beyond the influence of other plausible biological factors, greater lifetime burden of discrimination, and particularly having experienced discriminatory harassment and/or assault, is associated with lower resting HRV in African Americans. These results complement previous research on discrimination and BP, and extend the current literature regarding the relationship between discrimination and HRV. Collectively, these insights may prove informative for future research examining the unique and overlapping biological underpinnings of racial discrimination as a salient and pervasive form of chronic stress.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by funding from The Ohio State University Office of Diversity & Inclusion, The Todd Anthony Bell National Resource Center on the African American Male, The Ohio State University Graduate School, The Ohio State University College of Social, Behavioral, and Economic Sciences, and the National Institute of Aging (5T32AG000029) and the National Heart, Lung, And Blood Institute of the National Institutes of Health (R01HL121708) to the first author (L.K.H.). The second author (L.S.H.) was supported by funding from the National Science Foundation (537597).

Footnotes

1

Although, we focus on HF-HRV we also considered time domain measures, particularly, the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) and the percentage of successive R–R intervals differing by more than fifty milliseconds (pNN50). HF-HRV was strongly associated with both RMSSD, r = .90, p < .001, and pNN50, r = .85, p < .001. With the exception of lifetime discrimination on pNN50 (b = −.16, SE = 2.30, 95% C.I. [−8.18, .88], p = .112), regression results for discrimination were largely consistent with findings for HF-HRV, particularly for the influence of lifetime discrimination on RMSSD (b = −.21, SE =.06, 95% C.I. [−.24, −.001], p = .049), and discrimination attributable to threat on both RMSSD (b = −.33, SE = .07, 95% C.I. [−.31, −.04], p = .01) and pNN50 (b = −.32, SE = 2.60, 95% C.I. [−11.69, −1.38], p = .01).

Contributor Information

LaBarron K. Hill, Duke University Medical Center and Duke University

Ashley S. Richmond, North Carolina Central University

Lori S. Hoggard, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

DeLeon L. Gray, North Carolina State University

Dewayne P. Williams, The Ohio State University

Julian F. Thayer, The Ohio State University

References

  1. Amerena J, Julius S. The role of the autonomic nervous system in hypertension. Hypertension Research. 1995;18:99–110. doi: 10.1291/hypres.18.99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1291/hypres.18.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson NB, Armstead CA. Toward understanding the association of socioeconomic status and health: A new challenge for the biopsychosocial approach. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1995;57:213–225. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199505000-00003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006842-199505000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson NB, McNeilly M, Myers H. Autonomic reactivity and hypertension in blacks: A review and proposed model. Ethnicity & Disease. 1991;1:154–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology. 2006;10:229–240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229. [Google Scholar]
  5. Banks K, Kohn-Wood LP, Spencer M. An examination of the African American experience of everyday discrimination and symptoms of psychological distress. Community Mental Health Journal. 2006;42:555–570. doi: 10.1007/s10597-006-9052-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10597-006-9052-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beatty Moody DL, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT, Brown C. Everyday discrimination prospectively predicts inflammation across 7-years in racially diverse midlife women: Study of women’s health across the nation. Journal of Social Issues. 2014;70:298–314. doi: 10.1111/josi.12061. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josi.12061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brody GH, Lei MK, Chae DH, Yu T, Kogan SM, Beach SR. Perceived discrimination among African American adolescents and allostatic load: A longitudinal analysis with buffering effects. Child Development. 2014;85:989–1002. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brody GH, Yu T, Miller GE, Chen E. Discrimination, racial identity, and cytokine levels among African-American adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2015;56:496–501. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.01.017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brondolo E. Racial and ethnic disparities in health: Examining the contexts that shape resilience and risk. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2015;77:2–5. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brondolo E, Brady N, Thompson S, Tobin JN, Cassells A, Sweeney M, … Contrada RJ. Perceived racism and negative affect: Analyses of trait and state measures of affect in a community sample. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2008;27:150–173. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2008.27.2.150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2008.27.2.150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brondolo E, Kelly KP, Coakley V, Gordon T, Thompson S, Levy E, … Contrada RJ. The Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire: Development and preliminary validation of a community version. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2005;35:335–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02124.x. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brondolo E, Love EE, Pencille M, Schoenthaler A, Ogedegbe G. Racism and hypertension: A review of the empirical evidence and implications for clinical practice. American Journal of Hypertension. 2011;24:518–529. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2011.9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ajh.2011.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brondolo E, Rieppi R, Kelly KP, Gerin W. Perceived racism and blood pressure: A review of the literature and conceptual and methodological critique. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2003;25:55–65. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2501_08. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15324796ABM2501_08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brook RD, Julius S. Autonomic imbalance, hypertension, and cardiovascular risk. American Journal of Hypertension. 2000;13:S112–S122. doi: 10.1016/s0895-7061(00)00228-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0895-7061(00)00228-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chae DH, Clouston S, Hatzenbuehler ML, Kramer MR, Cooper HL, Wilson SM, … Link BG. Association between an internet-based measure of area racism and Black mortality. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0122963. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122963. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Choi JB, Hong S, Nelesen R, Bardwell WA, Natarajan L, Schubert C, Dimsdale JE. Age and ethnicity differences in short-term heart-rate variability. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2006;68:421–426. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221378.09239.6a. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000221378.09239.6a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, Williams DR. Racism as a stressor for African Americans. A biopsychosocial model. American Psychologist. 1999;54:805–816. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.10.805. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.10.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Contrada RJ, Ashmore RD, Gary ML, Coups E, Egeth JD, Sewell A, … Chasse V. Measures of ethnicity-related stress: Psychometric properties, ethnic group differences, and associations with well-being. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2001;31:1775–1820. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb00205.x. [Google Scholar]
  19. Cooper DC, Thayer JF, Waldstein SR. Coping with racism: The impact of prayer on cardiovascular reactivity and post-stress recovery in African American women. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2014;47:218–230. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9540-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12160-013-9540-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Couto PF, Goto JB, Bastos JL. Blood pressure and interpersonal discrimination: Systematic review of epidemiologic studies. Arquivos Brasileiros de Cardiologia. 2012;99:956–963. doi: 10.1590/s0066-782x2012005000090. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0066-782X2012005000090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cuffee YL, Hargraves JL, Allison J. Exploring the association between reported discrimination and hypertension among African Americans: A systematic review. Ethnicity & Disease. 2012;22:422–431. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cunningham TJ, Seeman TE, Kawachi I, Gortmaker SL, Jacobs DR, Kiefe CI, Berkman LF. Racial/ethnic and gender differences in the association between self-reported experiences of racial/ethnic discrimination and inflammation in the CARDIA cohort of 4 US communities. Social Science & Medicine. 2012;75:922–931. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.04.027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.04.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dimsdale JE. Psychological stress and cardiovascular disease. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2008;51:1237–1246. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2007.12.024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jacc.2007.12.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dolezsar CM, McGrath JJ, Herzig AJ, Miller SB. Perceived racial discrimination and hypertension: A comprehensive systematic review. Health Psychology. 2014;33:20–34. doi: 10.1037/a0033718. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dorr N, Brosschot JF, Sollers JJ, III, Thayer JF. Damned if you do, damned if you don’t: The differential effect of expression and inhibition of anger on cardiovascular recovery in black and white males. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007;66:125–134. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Durrence HH, Lichstein KL. The sleep of African Americans: A comparative review. Behavioral Sleep Medicine. 2006;4:29–44. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0401_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Friedman BH. An autonomic flexibility-neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biological Psychology. 2007;74:185–199. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Geronimus AT, Hicken M, Keene D, Bound J. “Weathering” and age patterns of allostatic load scores among blacks and whites in the United States. American Journal of Public Health. 2006;96:826–833. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.060749. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2004.060749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Go AS, Mozaffarian D, Roger VL, Benjamin EJ, Berry JD, Borden WB … on behalf of the American Heart Association statistics committee and stroke statistics subcommittee. Executive summary: Heart disease and stroke statistics—2013 update: A report from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2013;127:e1–e240. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e318282ab8f. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/CIR.0b013e318282ab8f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Goosby BJ, Malone S, Richardson EA, Cheadle JE, Williams DT. Perceived discrimination and markers of cardiovascular risk among low-income African American youth. American Journal of Human Biology. 2015;27:546–552. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22683. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.22683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Graham JR, Calloway A, Roemer L. The buffering effects of emotion regulation in the relationship between experiences of racism and anxiety in a Black American sample. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2015;39:553–563. [Google Scholar]
  32. Greer TM, Laseter A, Asiamah D. Gender as a moderator of the relation between race-related stress and mental health symptoms for African Americans. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2009;33:295–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/036168430903300305. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hamer M, Malan L. Psychophysiological risk markers of cardiovascular disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;35:76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Harrell JP, Hall S, Taliaferro J. Physiological responses to racism and discrimination: An assessment of the evidence. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93:243–248. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.2.243. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.93.2.243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hicken MT, Lee H, Ailshire J, Burgard SA, Williams DR. “Every shut eye, ain’t sleep”: The role of racism-related vigilance in racial/ethnic disparities in sleep difficulty. Race and Social Problems. 2013;5:100–112. doi: 10.1007/s12552-013-9095-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12552-013-9095-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hill LK, Hu DD, Koenig J, Sollers JJ, III, Kapuku G, Wang X, … Thayer JF. Ethnic differences in resting heart rate variability: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2015;77:16–25. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hill LK, Kobayashi I, Hughes JW. Perceived racism and ambulatory blood pressure in African American college students. Journal of Black Psychology. 2007;33:404–421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0095798407307042. [Google Scholar]
  38. Hill LK, Sollers JJ, III, Edwards CL, Thayer JF, Whitfield KE. A validation of estimated total peripheral resistance using twin data. Biomedical Sciences Instrumentation. 2014;50:210–218. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Himmelstein MS, Young DM, Sanchez DT, Jackson JS. Vigilance in the discrimination-stress model for Black Americans. Psychology & Health. 2015;30:253–267. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.966104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2014.966104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hoggard LS, Hill LK, Gray DL, Sellers RM. Capturing the cardiac effects of racial discrimination: Do the effects “keep going”? International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2015;97:163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hunter LR, Schmidt NB. Anxiety psychopathology in African American adults: Literature review and development of an empirically informed sociocultural model. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136:211–235. doi: 10.1037/a0018133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Jackson AS, Blair SN, Mahar MT, Wier LT, Ross RM, Stuteville JE. Prediction of functional aerobic capacity without exercise testing. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 1990;22:863–870. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199012000-00021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/00005768-199012000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Johnson TV, Abbasi A, Master VA. Systematic review of the evidence of a relationship between chronic psychosocial stress and C-reactive protein. Molecular Diagnosis & Therapy. 2013;17:147–164. doi: 10.1007/s40291-013-0026-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40291-013-0026-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Julius S, Pascual AV, London R. Role of parasympathetic inhibition in the hyperkinetic type of borderline hypertension. Circulation. 1971;44:413–418. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.44.3.413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.44.3.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Julius S, Schork N, Schork A. Sympathetic hyperactivity in early stages of hypertension: The Ann Arbor data set. Journal of Cardiovascular Pharmacology. 1988;12:S121–S129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005344-198800120-00017. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Keen L, II, Turner AD, Mwendwa D, Callender C, Campbell A., Jr Depressive symptomatology and respiratory sinus arrhythmia in a non-clinical sample of middle-aged African Americans. Biological Psychology. 2015;108:56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kemp AH, Quintana DS. The relationship between mental and physical health: Insights from the study of heart rate variability. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2013;89:288–296. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kochanek KD, Arias E, Anderson RN. How did cause of death contribute to racial differences in life expectancy in the United States in 2010. NCHS data brief. 2013;125:1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Krantz DS, Manuck SB. Acute psychophysiologic reactivity and risk of cardiovascular disease: A review and methodologic critique. Psychological Bulletin. 1984;96:435–464. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.435. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lampert R, Ickovics J, Horwitz R, Lee F. Depressed autonomic nervous system function in African Americans and individuals of lower social class: A potential mechanism of race- and class-related disparities in health outcomes. American Heart Journal. 2005;150:153–160. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2004.08.008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ahj.2004.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Larsen B, Christenfeld NJS. Cardiovascular disease and psychiatric comorbidity: The potential role of perseverative cognition. Cardiovascular Psychiatry and Neurology. 2009 doi: 10.1155/2009/791017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2009/791017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  52. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  53. Lewis TT, Aiello AE, Leurgans S, Kelly J, Barnes LL. Self-reported experiences of everyday discrimination are associated with elevated C-reactive protein levels in older African-American adults. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2010;24:438–443. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.11.011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2009.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Liao D, Barnes RW, Chambless LE, Simpson RJ, Jr, Sorlie P, Heiss G the ARIC investigators. Age, race, and sex differences in autonomic cardiac function measured by spectral analysis of heart rate variability—The ARIC study. Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities. The American Journal of Cardiology. 1995;76:906–912. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9149(99)80260-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-9149(99)80260-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Manuck SB, Kasprowicz AL, Muldoon MF. Behaviorally-evoked cardiovascular reactivity and hypertension: Conceptual issues and potential associations. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1990;12:17–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324796abm1201_2. [Google Scholar]
  56. Mensah GA, Mokdad AH, Ford ES, Greenlund KJ, Croft JB. State of disparities in cardiovascular health in the United States. Circulation. 2005;111:1233–1241. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Miniño AM. Death in the United States, 2011. Hyattsville, MD: U. S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  58. Morris-Prather CE, Harrell JP, Collins R, Leonard KL, Boss M, Lee JW. Gender differences in mood and cardiovascular responses to socially stressful stimuli. Ethnicity & Disease. 1996;6:123–131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Neblett EW, Jr, Roberts SO. Racial identity and autonomic responses to racial discrimination. Psychophysiology. 2013;50:943–953. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Palatini P, Julius S. The role of cardiac autonomic function in hypertension and cardiovascular disease. Current Hypertension Reports. 2009;11:199–205. doi: 10.1007/s11906-009-0035-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11906-009-0035-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Paradies Y. A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2006;35:888–901. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl056. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyl056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Saul JP. Beat-to-beat variations of heart rate reflect modulation of cardiac autonomic outflow. Physiology (Bethesda, MD) 1990;5:32–37. [Google Scholar]
  63. Schroeder EB, Liao D, Chambless LE, Prineas RJ, Evans GW, Heiss G. Hypertension, blood pressure, and heart rate variability: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Hypertension. 2003;42:1106–1111. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000100444.71069.73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.HYP.0000100444.71069.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Segerstrom SC, Nes LS. Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychological Science. 2007;18:275–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01888.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01888.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Slopen N, Lewis TT, Williams DR. Discrimination and sleep: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.012. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  66. Stein PK, Pu Y. Heart rate variability, sleep and sleep disorders. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2012;16:47–66. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.02.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2011.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Steptoe A, Kivimäki M. Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nature Reviews Cardiology. 2012;9:360–370. doi: 10.1038/nrcardio.2012.45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrcardio.2012.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sue DW, Capodilupo CM, Torino GC, Bucceri JM, Holder AM, Nadal KL, Esquilin M. Racial microaggressions in everyday life: Implications for clinical practice. American Psychologist. 2007;62:271–286. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.4.271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.4.271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Taherzadeh Z, Brewster LM, van Montfrans GA, VanBavel E. Function and structure of resistance vessels in black and white people. Journal of Clinical Hypertension. 2010;12:431–438. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7176.2010.00269.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-7176.2010.00269.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tarvainen MP, Niskanen JP, Lipponen JA, Ranta-Aho PO, Karjalainen PA. Kubios HRV—Heart rate variability analysis software. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine. 2014;113:210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2013.07.024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2013.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation. 1996;93:1043–1065. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Johnsen BH. The non-invasive assessment of autonomic influences on the heart using impedance cardiography and heart rate variability. In: Steptoe A, editor. Handbook of behavioral medicine. New York, NY: Springer; 2010. pp. 723–740. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-09488-5_47. [Google Scholar]
  73. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;61:201–216. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Thayer JF, Lane RD. The role of vagal function in the risk for cardiovascular disease and mortality. Biological Psychology. 2007;74:224–242. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Thayer JF, Yamamoto SS, Brosschot JF. The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. International Journal of Cardiology. 2010;141:122–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tomfohr L, Cooper DC, Mills PJ, Nelesen RA, Dimsdale JE. Everyday discrimination and nocturnal blood pressure dipping in black and white Americans. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2010;72:266–272. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d0d8b2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d0d8b2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tully PJ, Cosh SM, Baune BT. A review of the affects of worry and generalized anxiety disorder upon cardiovascular health and coronary heart disease. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2013;18:627–644. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2012.749355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2012.749355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Urbina EM, Bao W, Pickoff AS, Berenson GS. Ethnic (black-white) contrasts in heart rate variability during cardiovascular reactivity testing in male adolescents with high and low blood pressure: The Bogalusa Heart Study. American Journal of Hypertension. 1998;11:196–202. doi: 10.1016/s0895-7061(97)00314-2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0895-7061(97)00314-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Utsey SO, Hook JN. Heart rate variability as a physiological moderator of the relationship between race-related stress and psychological distress in African Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2007;13:250–253. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.13.3.250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.13.3.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wagner J, Lampert R, Tennen H, Feinn R. Exposure to discrimination and heart rate variability reactivity to acute stress among women with diabetes. Stress and Health. 2015;31:255–262. doi: 10.1002/smi.2542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smi.2542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wang X, Thayer JF, Treiber F, Snieder H. Ethnic differences and heritability of heart rate variability in African- and European American youth. The American Journal of Cardiology. 2005;96:1166–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.06.050. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.06.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Williams DP, Cash C, Rankin C, Bernardi A, Koenig J, Thayer JF. Resting heart rate variability predicts self-reported difficulties in emotion regulation: A focus on different facets of emotion regulation. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:261. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00261. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Williams DR, Mohammed SA. Discrimination and racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;32:20–47. doi: 10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Williams DR, Neighbors H. Racism, discrimination and hypertension: Evidence and needed research. Ethnicity & Disease. 2001;11:800–816. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wong MD, Shapiro MF, Boscardin WJ, Ettner SL. Contribution of major diseases to disparities in mortality. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2002;347:1585–1592. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa012979. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsa012979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES