Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2018 Jan 10;13(1):e0189752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189752

Thinking of me: Self-focus reduces sharing and helping in seven- to eight-year-olds

Sandra Weltzien 1,*, Lauren E Marsh 1, Bruce Hood 1
Editor: Jordy Kaufman2
PMCID: PMC5761840  PMID: 29320506

Abstract

By 7-to 8-years of age, most children readily adhere to prosocial norms aimed at benefiting others through giving up time and effort (helping) or resources (sharing). Two studies explored whether sharing and helping by 7-to 8-year olds (N = 180) could be influenced by priming children’s attention on themselves or their friends through a semi-structured interview. Results revealed that self-priming led to reductions in both sharing and helping compared to friendship-priming or a control condition. These findings are considered as indicative of the fragile state of prosocial behaviours at this age that can be easily shifted towards more selfish biases by simple priming.

Introduction

Most human beings are willing to give up resources, time, and effort to benefit others across a range of interactions. In traditional models of moral development (e.g., [1], [2], [3], [4]) there is general agreement that with age, children's reasoning develops from being more hedonistic and self-concerned, to becoming more other-oriented and concerned with adherence to social norms and acceptance from others. There is also abundant empirical evidence that prosocial behaviours increase with age [5], [6], [7], [8], [9]. However, not all prosocial behaviours show the same developmental pattern. For example, sharing and helping are both prosocial behaviours that exhibit different developmental trajectories. While there may be procedural issues when comparing helping and sharing, naturalistic observational studies (e.g. [10], [11], [12]) indicate that the differences reflect true dispositions in most children.

Helping is an early emerging prosocial behaviour [13], [14]. Soon after their first birthday, infants will pick up and return an object accidentally dropped on the floor, and open cabinets for people if their hands are full [13], [15]. They do this irrespective of praise or encouragement, and even if the helping is costly, for example requiring the child to leave a fun activity or surmount obstacles in order to help [15]. To deliver the appropriate help in these situations, infants must understand the behaviour of others as goal-oriented, and be motivated to help them achieve their aim. Such findings have fuelled the argument that humans begin life as indiscriminate helpers [16], and thus that children may have a “natural tendency to help other persons” [13].

In contrast, spontaneous sharing behaviour seems to emerge later in development. While there are some examples of simple resource distribution during infancy [17], [18], [19], [20], [21], these are typically limited to direct requests (but see [22]). There is also evidence that infants may hold certain expectations regarding how resources are typically distributed. For example, 15-month-old infants demonstrate sensitivity to fairness violations [23], and by two years of age, children expect a fair split of resources between individuals when the distributer is a third party, but expect an uneven split if unequal effort has been contributed [24]. Yet, despite these early expectations of fairness, young children typically behave selfishly in situations that require relinquishing their own possessions, resulting in a disparity between their own sharing behaviour and the principles of sharing they endorse for others (e.g., [25], [26], [27].

Smith and colleagues [27] investigated the gap between sharing standards and sharing behaviour in 3- to 8-year-olds. At all ages, children endorsed norms for sharing, but 3- to 6-year-olds still favoured themselves in real sharing situations. When explaining their sharing behaviour, older children focused spontaneously and explicitly on sharing norms, whereas the younger children focused on themselves and their own desires [27]. This could suggest that the development of sharing norms must overcome an initial self-interested bias to optimise own gain. In other words, young children are aware of sharing norms, but these principles appear overridden by self-focus before age 7. The attenuation of selfish behaviour with age is typically explained as a consequence of socialization. As children move from an informal preschool setting to formal schools, they may gradually learn and internalize the normative rules endorsed by authority figures such as teachers and other adults (e.g., [25], [7], see [28] for cross-cultural evidence). This is also a period where children become increasingly sensitive to others opinions about themselves, which may increase their desire and motivation to appear generous and behave in a normatively appropriate way [29], [30].

In summary, children appear to show an early appreciation of both sharing and helping norms (e.g. [23], [24]) but whereas helping is often enacted at an early age, sharing is typically not [31], [32]. Why is this? The disparity between sharing and helping may be due to a number of differences between the two behaviours. Sharing is arguably more costly, as it usually requires the reallocation and loss of resources that are already in one’s possession. However, even in situations where there is no personal cost, young children are still reluctant to share unless specifically requested to do so [19]. Helping is not so much a loss of physical resources, but rather of time and effort not yet expended. While both sharing and helping benefit others, it could be that the cost of relinquishing personal resources weighs more heavily than the cost of assistance. It could also be that helping is more other-directed in that it requires interpreting another’s immediate need for assistance. Moreover, the likelihood of helping is influenced by prior positive social engagement with the individual who subsequently requires assistance. Infants are more likely to help another person if that person has previously engaged in mutual play [33] or danced in synchrony [34]. These findings are consistent with the interpretation that early helping is a social-interactional behaviour that “develops through reciprocal interactions” which by definition requires coordinating with another [35].

One way to explore the extent to which sharing and helping are driven by self- and other-focus motivations is to examine whether these behaviours can be enhanced or diminished through external manipulations, such as priming. Priming refers to the activation of a schema or category that can have an impact on subsequent reasoning and behaviour. Hebb [36] proposed that such internal mental representations remain latent, but can be elicited by both internal and external triggers. For example, adult studies have shown that priming the concept of helping increases willingness to assist in a subsequent task [37]. Mental representations also include complex aspects of the self-concept, and recent studies with adults have demonstrated that the salience of specific self-construals can be manipulated through priming [38], [39], [40], [41]. Importantly, it has been found that activation of independent and interdependent self-construals can lead to transformation of motives, which in turn can affect levels of prosocial behaviours. For example, adults primed with independence words such as "individual" were more selfish in subsequent sharing tasks compared to adults primed with interdependence words such as "friendship" [41].

Social development research, however, has rarely used priming despite evidence that this technique is promising for helping researchers untangle causal connections between children’s mental representations and children’s social development [42]. Moreover, the existing research on priming prosociality in children has typically focused on the relationship between empathic concern and prosocial behaviours (e.g., [43], [44], [45]). Here, we adopted a self-construal priming paradigm developed by Hood, Weltzien, Marsh and Kanngiesser [46] which revealed that children changed their evaluation of personal possessions when primed to think about themselves. Specifically, following self-focus, it was found that participants increased their evaluation of an owned object, and were less willing to trade that object upon request from an experimenter compared to participants who completed a neutral control task. Conversely, when participants had completed a friend-focus task, they were more willing to subsequently trade with the experimenter. As these were identical objects, there was no advantage in trading other than complying with the request. If willingness to trade in this context can be considered a pro-social behaviour, we predicted that similar self-construal priming would also influence other pro-social behaviours.

Despite different developmental trajectories, children will both share and help reliably by 7 years of age. But how substantiated are these behaviours? In the current line of work, we investigated whether self- and friendship-priming would differentially influence these two prosocial behaviours. More specifically, in Study 1 we examined the effects of self- and friendship-priming on sharing behaviour using a Dictator Game [47]. We adapted the self-construal priming paradigm previously used with 3- to 4-year-olds [46] into one more suitable for primary school children. This was done using a semi-structured interview and drawing task that either focused children’s attention on themselves, their relationship with a close friend, or on a neutral farm (control). Study 2 explored the effects of the same priming manipulations on a helping task.

It was predicted that if sharing requires children to overcome a self-interest bias, then children primed towards self-focus should be more reluctant to share their resources compared to the control-condition. We also sought to evaluate whether sharing could be enhanced through friendship-priming, although due to the self-focused nature of sharing we did not hold strong predictions regarding the effectiveness of this priming manipulation. Helping, on the other hand, is enacted early in development across a range of situations, suggesting that there may not be such a strong bias for self-interest related to this behaviour. If this holds true, then helping should be relatively resilient to self-priming. Moreover, it has been found that young children primed with stimuli that evoke affiliation are more inclined to help a person in need than children primed towards individuality [48]. Thus, in the present study we predicted that focusing children on their relationship with others should increase their helping behaviour compared to the control by making them more other-oriented.

Study 1: Sharing

The study protocol was approved by the Faculty of Science Research Ethics Committee at the University of Bristol. Informed consent was obtained in written form from the parents of all children who participated in this study. All children provided verbal agreement that they wished to partake in the research.

Method

Participants

Ninety 7-to 8-year olds (Mage = 94.17 months, SDage = 7.47, range = 84–107 months; 40 males) participated in Study 1. This sample size was selected based on a power calculation conducted on the effect size taken from Guinote, Cotzia, Sandhu and Siwa [49] which reports an effect of social status on generosity in a Dictator Game in 5-year olds. That study used a social manipulation of pairings of children playing the Dictator Game, which is the most similar design to the present sharing study that we could find. Based on the calculated effect size of f = .4, a sample of 27 children per group should yield 95% power. Thus, with 30 children in three groups, we have 97.5% power to detect an effect of the same size in the current study. Four additional children were tested but excluded from analysis because they failed to pass one or more of the control questions. All children were tested individually in quiet rooms at the Bristol Cognitive Development Centre or at the At-Bristol science centre. A majority of the children were of Western descent (92%) and came from middle-class, educated families.

Procedure

Sticker allocation

Each testing session began by the child choosing six of ten colourful animal stickers to keep. Once the selection was made, the remaining stickers were removed from sight. The child’s selected stickers remained in view, but out of reach, on the table.

Priming-procedure

Each child took part in one of three priming conditions: self-priming, friendship-priming, or a neutral control condition. Using a semi-structured script, the experimenter asked a series of questions about the child (self-priming), a best friend and their relationship (friendship-priming), or a farm (neutral-priming), whilst drawing a picture based on the child’s description. The self- and friendship-priming interviews had 20 similar questions related to appearances, preferences, clothing and activities. During self-priming, the experimenter used second person singular pronouns (you, your, yours, child’s name) whenever apt in order to prime notions of independence and steer the child’s attention towards his or her self. In the friendship-priming condition, the experimenter used third person pronouns (he, she, him, her, friend’s name) and second person plural pronouns (you, your) whenever apt to prime notions of interdependence and steer the child’s attention towards the friend and their relationship. In the neutral-priming condition, the experimenter was careful to avoid use of personal pronouns (for full priming scripts and experimental instructions, see supporting information (S1 File).

Dictator game

In the present experiment, two envelopes were introduced to the child; one brown and one white. The child was informed that the brown envelope belonged to him or her, and that everything put into this envelope could be taken home. The child’s name was written on the envelope to further emphasise ownership. Following this, the child was told that another child would come tomorrow, and that if he or she wanted to, he or she could give away some of the stickers to this other child. The experimenter emphasised that the child did not have to give away any stickers, and explained that the child’s decision would be completely anonymous. The child was told that if he or she wished to give away any stickers, then these could be placed into the white envelope before posting it into a box. The stickers that the child wished to keep should be placed in the brown envelope. It was further explained that the experimenter and any parents present would turn away and cover their eyes during the division. Three control questions were asked to ensure comprehension: “Where will you put the stickers that you want to keep?” “Which envelope belongs to the other child?” and “Do you have to give away any stickers?” Children who failed either of the control questions were excluded.

Data coding and preliminary analyses

Children’s sharing behaviour was coded live and from videotape. The number of stickers shared was converted to a percentage for each child. Child age in months at the date of testing was calculated and included as a variable in all analyses. This was done to control for any potential variance associated with age within the sample. Two dependent measures were calculated from the sharing data. 1) The number of children deciding to share in each priming-condition (i.e. sharing nothing vs. sharing one or more stickers) was analysed using a logistic regression. 2) The percentage of stickers shared was analysed with an ANCOVA. Prime type, was entered as a between subjects factor and age in months was entered as a covariate. Preliminary analyses indicated no effects of gender or sibling status in any of our analyses so these will not be considered further. All summary statistics are presented in Table 1, and the raw data file is available as supporting information (S2 File). Time spent on the priming-task did not differ significantly across conditions (F(2, 87) = .27, p = .77, ηp2 = .01, M self = 239.83s, 95% CI self = [236.28, 243.39]; M friendship = 242.10, 95% CI friendship = [237.14, 247.06]; M neutral = 240.57, 95% CI neutral = [235.51, 245.63]). Therefore, this factor will not be considered further.

Table 1. Summary statistics for sharing and helping data.

Study 1: Sharing
Prime Type Self Neutral Friendship
N 30 30 30
Decision to share 22 28 28
% shared 19.44 35.56 34.44
[95% CI] [14.00, 24.88] [29.73, 41.39] [26.97, 41.92]
Study 2: Helping
Prime Type Self Neutral Friendship
N 30 30 30
Optimal helpers 6 18 17
Prompt score 2.07 1.63 1.57
[95% CI] [1.61, 2.53] [1.09, 2.17] [1.03, 2.10]
Time helping (sec) 96.70 135.43 137.37
[95% CI] [73.89, 119.51] [112.43, 158.44] [113.60, 161.14]
Stickers sorted 14.87 22.03 22.17
[95% CI] [11.09, 18.64] [18.17, 25.90] [17.97, 26.36]

Results

Decision to share

Priming condition and age were not significant predictors of decision to share (X2 (3) = 6.655, p = .084), although there was a non-significant trend towards a reduction in willingness to share following self-priming compared to the neutral condition (β = 1.588, p = .060).

Percentage of stickers

There was a significant effect of priming condition on percentage of stickers shared (F(2, 84) = 8.95, p < .001, ηp2 = .18). Specifically, children shared a significantly smaller percentage of their stickers following self-priming (M self = 19.44%), compared to neutral-priming (M neutral = 35.56%, t(58) = 4.13, p< .001, d = 1.07, Bonferroni corrected), and friendship-priming (M friendship = 34.44%, t(58) = -3.32, p = .006, d = 0.86, Bonferroni corrected). There was no significant difference in the percentage of stickers shared following neutral-priming and friendship-priming (t(58) = .24, p = .811, d = 0.06). A significant effect of age (F(1, 84) = 4.48, p = .037, ηp2 = .051), indicated that older children shared a higher percentage of their stickers than younger children regardless of priming condition. There was no significant interaction between age and condition on sharing behaviour (F(2, 84) = 1.88, p = .160, ηp2 = .043).

Study 2: Helping

Overall, in study 1, completing a self-focused activity led to a reduction in sharing relative to the neutral condition; children shared a significantly smaller percentage of their stickers following self-priming. This may be because the natural development of sharing is to move from a position of self-interest to one that is more other-oriented. Thus, self-priming may have reverted participants back to a more egotistical position typically observed in younger children. There was no evidence that friendship-priming made children more generous in comparison to the neutral condition, although we did not have strong predictions in this regard. Helping, on the other hand, is evident much earlier in development, and as the nature of helping is arguably more other-focused, it may show greater sensitivity to manipulations that are designed to focus on affiliation with others. Moreover, some argue that helping is, in fact, a natural inclination [13] and so helping behaviour should be resilient to self-priming as there should be no self-focused bias related to helping. Thus, in Study 2, we sought to explore whether friendship-priming would boost children’s helping behaviour relative to self-priming and the neutral-priming task.

Method

Participants

Ninety 7-to 8-year olds (Mage = 95.92 months, SDage = 6.97, range = 84–107 months; 43 males) participated in Study 2. This sample size was selected to match the sample size in Study 1. Six additional children were tested but excluded from analysis (a) because they were not in the right age group (n = 2), (b) due to interference from parents (n = 2), or (c) due to experimenter error (n = 2). All children were tested individually in quiet rooms at the Bristol Cognitive Development Centre or at the At-Bristol science centre. A majority of the children were of Western descent (96%) and came from middle-class, educated families. All ethical guidelines noted in Study 1 were also followed in Study 2.

Procedure

Each session began by the child taking part in one of three priming conditions: self-priming, friendship-priming, or neutral-priming (control). The priming-tasks were identical to Study 1. Following the priming task, experimenter 1 (E1) stated: “Before you leave, I need your mum and/or dad to go to the room next door to fill in another form. You can just wait here with me. It will only take a few minutes”. A second experimenter (E2) led the parents out of the testing room. Next, E1 placed a bag with colourful animal stickers on the table whist explaining: “I use stickers for a different study with younger children, and while we wait I have to pick out all the frogs and elephants and put them into this box”. E1 then began to sort the stickers. If the child did not offer to help spontaneously, the experimenter waited for ten seconds before giving prompt 1: “There’s a lot of stickers for me to sort”. If the child did not join, the experimenter waited a further ten seconds before giving prompt 2: “This is going to take me a long time”. If the child still did not join, the experimenter waited a final ten seconds before giving prompt 3: “Could you help me a little”? A few moments after the direct request, E2 returned to the testing room, explaining that help was needed with the forms next door. Before leaving the testing room, E1 explained to the child: “We will be back soon. You don’t have to sort all of the stickers. Just do as many as you want and then you can leave the rest and pick out your prize from that bag over there” (pointing to a bag the opposite side of the room). After three minutes, E1 returned to the room, thanked the child for helping, and let him or her choose a prize if this had not already been done (see S3 File in supporting information for full experimental instructions).

Data coding and preliminary analysis

The required number of prompts needed to elicit helping, time spent helping, and the number of stickers sorted was coded from videotape. Four dependent measures were calculated from the helping data. 1) Number of prompts needed before helping commenced ranged from 0–3 (0 = spontaneous helping; 1 = helped after the first prompt; 2 = helped after the second prompt; 3 = helped after direct request) and was analysed with an ordinal regression. 2) The number of optimal helpers (children who spend the full three minutes helping vs children who stop helping before instructed) was compared across conditions with a binary logistic regression. 3) The average time spent on the helping task was analysed with an ANCOVA. Prime type, was entered as a between subjects factor and age in months was entered as a covariate. 4) The number of stickers sorted was analysed with an ANCOVA, with prime type entered as the between subjects factor, and age in months entered as a covariate. All summary statistics are presented in Table 1, and the raw data file is available as supporting information (S4 File). Preliminary analyses revealed that time spent on the priming-task did not differ significantly across conditions (F(2, 87) = 1.29, p = .28, ηp2 = .03, M self = 236.23s, 95% CI self = [233.67, 238.80]; M friendship = 238.27, 95% CI friendship = [236.41, 240.12]; M neutral = 238.73, 95% CI neutral = [236.05, 241.42]).

Results

Number of prompts & optimal helping

Priming condition and age were not significant predictors of the number of prompts needed before helping commenced (X2 (3) = 3.415, p = .332). However, the number of children who engaged in optimal helping was predicted by the priming-condition to which they were assigned (X2 (3) = 16.165, p = .001, R2 = .164). Specifically, fewer children helped for the full three minutes following self-priming (n = 6) compared to neutral-priming (n = 18, β = 1.992, p = .001). No significant difference was found between neutral-priming and friendship-priming (n = 17, β = .166, p = .756).

Time on helping

Time spent on the helping task differed as a function of prime type (F(2, 84) = 4.52, p = .014, ηp2 = .097). Children who completed self-priming spent significantly less time on the subsequent helping task (M self = 96.70 seconds) compared to friendship-priming (M friendship = 137.37 seconds), t(58) = 2.53, p = .042, d = 0.65, Bonferroni corrected). There was also a non-significant trend for children to spend less time on the helping task following self-priming compared to neutral-priming (M neutral = 135.43 seconds, t(58) = 2.45, p = .054, d = 0.63, Bonferroni corrected). No significant difference in helping time was found between neutral-priming and friendship-priming (t(58) = .12, p = .91, d = 0.03, Bonferroni corrected).

Stickers sorted

Finally, a significant effect was found of priming condition on number of stickers sorted (F(2, 84) = 4.49, p = .014, ηp2 = .097). Children sorted significantly fewer stickers following self-priming (M self = 14.87) compared to neutral-priming (M neutral = 22.03, t(58) = -2.71, p = .027, d = -0.70) and friendship-priming (M friendship = 22.17, t(58) = -2.65, p = .03, d = -0.68). No significant difference in stickers sorted was found between neutral-priming and friendship-priming (t(58) = .05, p = .96, d = 0.01).

Overall, the results from Study 2 replicated the negative effect of inducing self-focus on subsequent prosocial behaviour found in Study 1. Children were less likely to spend the full three minutes on the helping task and sorted fewer stickers following self-priming, compared to friendship-priming and the neutral-priming tasks.

General discussion

The present study examined whether sharing and helping by 7-to 8-year olds could be influenced by a temporary activation of self vs. friendship concepts primed during semi-structured interviews. It was revealed that children in Study 1 shared a smaller percentage of their stickers following self-priming. In Study 2, children also spent less time on the subsequent helping task, and sorted fewer stickers following self-priming. Thus, self-priming reduced both sharing and helping behaviours compared to a neutral control condition. In the case of sharing, the results confirmed our prediction that self-focus would reduce the willingness of young children to donate stickers to another child. However, self-priming also reduced helping. Intriguingly, this finding could suggest that helping may be more self-focused than previously anticipated. Indeed, an uncharitable interpretation could be that helping behaviour has less to do with selflessly assisting another, and instead, is more strategic. Exploring this possibility goes beyond the scope of the current paper, but the interpretation is consistent with recent arguments by Dunfield and Kuhlmeier [50] and Wynn [51].

In contrast to self-priming, there was no evidence for an effect of friendship-priming on either sharing or helping. This is consistent with a growing literature on the dominance of self-oriented cognition such as the self-reference effect [52], but it still requires explanation. It is well established that pre-schoolers are more reluctant to share in comparison to other pro-social behaviours such as helping and cooperating (e.g., [18]). Thus, for sharing behaviour, the lack of a friendship-priming effect may not be too surprising because the current sharing task required children to give up resources without receiving anything in return, and their decision was kept strictly anonymous. As a result, the desire to keep more for oneself may have been too strong to be attenuated by the friendship-priming manipulation.

There are studies where friendship-priming has been shown to work. For example, it has been found that 3- to 4-year-old children primed to focus on their friends were more willing to trade toys with an experimenter [46]. One possible explanation for why friendship-priming worked for trading but not sharing is that these situations present the child with two very different predicaments. Only sharing presents a true cost. In the trading study, there was no real loss as the child was swapping for an identical object. Moreover, trading is a reciprocal activity whereas sharing is not necessarily so. In fact, the current Dictator Game involved sharing with an unknown child where there was no expectation to receive anything in return.

On the other hand, we predicted that helping would respond to friendship-priming because we considered it a more other-focused behaviour [53]. Moreover, it has previously been found that young children primed with stimuli that evoked feelings of affiliation were more likely to help a person in need compared to children primed towards individuality [48]. Consequently, we expected that focusing children on their relationship with a friend should evoke feelings of affiliation and lead to increased helping in a similar way. The absence of a friendship-priming effect for helping was thus surprising and worthy of further consideration.

Talking about friends could have evoked self-referential priming, as good friends can be considered an extension of one's self and may trigger a self-bias. However, if this was the case then there should have been a negative effect on prosocial behaviour relative to the control condition, which was not observed. A related point is that talking about a specific friend does not necessarily translate to prosociality directed towards others. We asked participants to talk about a specific friend because we reasoned that this would be a familiar concept for children of the current age group. Additionally, this enabled the experimenter to construct a picture based on the interview. Possibly, a more effective interdependence prime could have focused on others in general, or on mutual dependence and relatedness between individuals. Indeed, research indicates that people have universal and fundamental needs for relatedness and belonging that are essential to human development and growth [54], [55], [56]. In adults, it has been found that highlighting feelings of relatedness can lead to greater intentions to volunteer, and higher donations to charity [57], and that threatening the sense of belonging can decrease helping [58]. As children move in to a formal school setting, the experience of belonging, and the need to feel a sense of identification with others, become increasingly important (e.g., [59], [29], [60]). Thus, highlighting general feelings of connectedness, belonging and reliance on others may be a more powerful way of evoking prosocial actions in children. These issues may be worthy of future investigation, but we reiterate that our simple interdependence priming did work in our previous studies with younger children [46] and has also been shown to work in adults [37].

The striking effect from the current studies is that inducing self-focus through a simple priming task has a negative impact on prosocial behavior in young children. We consider this to be a significant contribution to the field of social development; especially given the paucity of developmental work using priming. However, the mechanism operating during self-priming is in need of further investigation. As previously discussed, it is possible that self-priming reverted participants back to a more egotistical position typically observed in younger children by boosting a self-interested bias. A potentially valuable avenue for future research would therefore be to explore whether children’s prosocial tendencies stabilize with age and become less vulnerable to self-priming. A related query is whether prosociality in younger children would be even more influenced by such priming effects. These are interesting questions, but it should be noted that the current study was not developmental, and it remains to be seen whether the same priming method would be as effective at different ages. We predict that priming will continue to influence children with increasing age as we note that adults are not immune to priming effects on prosocial behavior [41].

A further possibility is that the observed reduction in willingness to share following self-priming may be driven by an “endowment effect” [61]. The endowment effect can be described as a bias to over-value personal possessions. According to the “extended self” hypothesis, this overvaluation occurs because personal possessions are perceived as tangible manifestations of the self [62]. As people’s implicit self-evaluations are typically highly positive [63], this self-object association may lead to an enhanced perceived value of owned items [64], [65]. In support of this theory, it has been shown that the endowment effect can be strengthened in adults [66] and induced in young children [46] by focusing their attention on themselves. Children in Study 1 were endowed with stickers before entering the priming conditions so it is possible that the stickers in Study 1 were considered personal possessions. Therefore, the higher reluctance to give away stickers following self-priming could have been the result of an endowment effect. Future research could test this hypothesis by manipulating the time at which the children receive the stickers (i.e. before or after priming) and examine whether this attenuates the reduction in sharing.

While an endowment effect gives a neat mechanistic explanation for the reduction in sharing, it cannot explain why self-priming also led to a reduction in helping. Specifically, as helping involves relinquishing time and effort, rather than personal possessions, self-focus triggered biases in feelings of ownership do not predict changes in helping behaviour. Conversely, the interpretation that self-priming triggered a bias towards self-interest in both sharing and helping tasks provides a more parsimonious account.

It is also possible that self-priming task led to expectations of a positive outcome that were contradicted by the subsequent appeal to share or help. Indeed, if the self-focus interview was perceived as a positive experience that was “all about the child”, then the children may have been surprised by the request to help or share, which are activities that benefit others. Consequently, the resulting disappointment may have been reflected in their effort to help and willingness to share. Whilst this interpretation does not refute the main finding that self-focus significantly reduces sharing and helping, it could provide an alternative account of how self-priming operates.

Finally, we need to consider how the induction of self-focus can lead to different results on prosociality. For example, the presence of mirrors when performing prosocial acts has been shown to increase prosociality in both adults and children [67], [68]. Mirror reflections and talking about one’s self both trigger activation of the self-concept. Yet one context produces more selfless behaviour, whereas the other produces more selfish behaviour. We believe that reflections by mirrors can increase both private and public self-awareness and cause individuals to become highly self-conscious [69], [70], [71], [72]. This, in turn, will activate pro-social norms, which is also why both adults and children are more willing to make donations when they are being observed [73], [74], [75]. By contrast, in our studies, both sharing and helping was relatively unobserved as the experimenters turned their backs or left the room during the critical period so that children would not feel under pressure to conform to pro-social standards.

In conclusion, we have provided evidence that inducing self-focus in 7- to 8-year-olds can result in a reduced willingness to share and help. Children at this age are fully aware of, and adhere to prosocial norms [29], [30]. It is therefore noteworthy that they can be so easily influenced to become less prosocial by simply focusing on themselves. It may well be the case that the self-concept is easily triggered in childhood, whereas self-other construals may require stronger priming or may develop later as the child becomes more socially integrated with unfamiliar others.

Supporting information

S1 File. Priming scripts and experimental instructions for study 1: Sharing.

(DOCX)

S2 File. Raw data study 1: Sharing.

(XLSX)

S3 File. Experimental instructions for study 2: Helping.

(PDF)

S4 File. Raw data study 2: Helping.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the support of the At-Bristol Science Museum and willingness of children and parents to participate in testing at the Bristol Cognitive Development Centre. We also thank Beth Twine and Ellie Martin for their help with recruitment. This research was supported by an ESRC grant ES/K010131/1 awarded to the third author.

Data Availability

All relevant data are available within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

BH was supported by a grant from the Economic and Social Research Council, grant number: ES/K010131/1. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Eisenberg-Berg N (1979) Development of Children’s Prosocial Moral Judgment. Developmental Psychology 51: 128–137. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Eisenberg N, Lennon R, Roth K (1983) Prosocial development: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology 19: 846. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hoffman ML (1982) Development of prosocial motivation: Empathy and guilt In Eisenberg N, editor. The development of prosocial behavior. Academic Press, New York; pp. 281–313. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hoffman ML (2001) Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice Cambridge University Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Benenson JF, Pascoe JE, Radmore N (2007) Children’ s altruistic behavior in the dictator game. Evol Hum Behav 28: 168–175. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Spinrad TL (2006) Prosocial Development In: Damon W, Lerner RM, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology, 6th ed. John Wiley & Sons, New Jersey: pp. 646–718. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fehr E, Bernhard H, Rockenbach B (2008) Egalitarianism in young children. Nature 454: 1079–83. doi: 10.1038/nature07155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Harbaugh WT, Krause K, Liday SJ (2003) Bargaining by children. University of Oregon Economics Working Paper, (2002–4). doi: 10.2139/ssrn.436504
  • 9.Yu J, Zhu L, Leslie AM (2016). Children's Sharing Behavior in Mini‐Dictator Games: The Role of In‐Group Favoritism and Theory of Mind. Child Development 87: 1747–1757. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12635 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Birch LL, Billman J (1986). Preschool children's food sharing with friends and acquaintances. Child Development, 387–395. doi: 10.2307/1130594 [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Eisenberg-Berg N, Hand M,(1979). The relationship of preschoolers' reasoning about prosocial moral conflicts to prosocial behavior. Child development, 356–363. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yarrow MR, Waxler CZ, Barrett D, Darby J, King R, Pickett M, Smith J (1976). Dimensions and correlates of prosocial behavior in young children. Child Development, 118–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Warneken F, Tomasello M (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science 311: 1301–1303. doi: 10.1126/science.1121448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Zahn-Waxler C, Radke-Yarrow M, Wagner E, Chapman M (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental psychology 28: 126–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Warneken F, Tomasello M (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy 11: 271–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Warneken F, Tomasello M (2009). The roots of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology 100: 455–471. doi: 10.1348/000712608X379061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Brownell CA (2013). Early development of prosocial behavior: Current perspectives. Infancy 18: 1–9. doi: 10.1111/infa.12004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brownell CA, Iesue SS, Nichols SR, Svetlova M (2013). Mine or yours? Development of sharing in toddlers in relation to ownership understanding. Child development 84: 906–920. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brownell C, Svetlova M, Nichols S (2009). To share or not to share: When do toddlers respond to another’s needs? Infancy 14: 117–130. doi: 10.1080/15250000802569868 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dunfield K, Kuhlmeier VA, O’Connell L, Kelley E (2011). Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior: Helping, sharing, and comforting in infancy. Infancy 16: 227–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Schmidt MF, Sommerville JA (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PloS one 6: e23223 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hay DF, Caplan M, Castle J, Stimson CA (1991). Does sharing become increasingly" rational" in the second year of life?. Developmental Psychology 27: 987. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sommerville JA, Schmidt MF, Yun JE, Burns M (2013). The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy 18: 40–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sloane S, Baillargeon R, Premack D (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological science 23: 196–204. doi: 10.1177/0956797611422072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Blake PR, McAuliffe K, Warneken F (2014). The developmental origins of fairness: The knowledge-behavior gap. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18: 559–561. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rochat P, Dias MD, Liping G, Broesch T, Passos-Ferreira C, Winning A, Berg B (2009). Fairness in distributive justice by 3-and 5-year-olds across seven cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 40: 416–442. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Smith CE, Blake PR, Harris PL (2013). I should but I won’t: why young children endorse norms of fair sharing but do not follow them. PLoS One 8; e59510 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.House BR, Silk JB; Henrich J, Barrett HC, Scelza BA, Boyette AH,…Laurence S (2013). Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110: 14586–14591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hill V, Pillow BH (2006). Children's understanding of reputations. The Journal of genetic psychology 167: 137–157. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.167.2.137-157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Shaw A, Montinari N, Piovesan M, Olson KR, Gino F, Norton MI (2014). Children develop a veil of fairness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143: 363–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Svetlova M, Nichols SR, Brownell CA (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child development 81: 1814–1827. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Levitt MJ, Weber RA, Clark MC, McDonnell P, (1985). Reciprocity of exchange in toddler sharing behavior. Developmental Psychology 21: 122. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Barragan RC, Dweck CS (2014). Rethinking natural altruism: simple reciprocal interactions trigger children’s benevolence. Proceedings of the National Academy 111: 17071–17074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cirelli LK, Einarson KM, Trainor LJ (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science 17: 1003–1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dahl A (2015). The developing social context of infant helping in two U.S. samples. Child Development 86: 1080–1093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hebb DO (1949). Organization of behavior New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Macrae CN, Johnston L (1998). Help, I need somebody: automatic action and inaction. Social Cognition 4: 400–417. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gardner WL, Gabriel S, Lee AY (1999). “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science 10: 321–326. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Haberstroh S, Oyserman D, Schwarz N, Kühnen U, Ji LJ (2002). Is the interdependent self more sensitive to question context than the independent self? Self-construal and the observation of conversational norms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38: 323–329. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kühnen U, Hannover B (2000). Assimilation and contrast in social comparisons as a consequence of self-construal activation. European Journal of Social Psychology 30: 799–811. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Utz S (2004). Self-construal and cooperation: is the interdependent self more cooperative than the independent self? Self and Identity 3: 177–190. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stupica B, Cassidy J (2014). Priming as a way of understanding children’s mental representations of the social world. Developmental Review 34: 77–91. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Vaish A, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2009). Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental psychology 45: 534 doi: 10.1037/a0014322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Williams A, O’Driscoll K, Moore C (2014). The influence of empathic concern on prosocial behavior in children. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 425 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hepach R, Vaish A, Tomasello M, (2013). A new look at children’s prosocial motivation. Infancy 18: 67–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hood B, Weltzien S, Marsh L, Kanngiesser P (2016). Picture yourself: Self-focus and the endowment effect in preschool children. Cognition 152: 70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kahneman D, Knetsch J, Thaler RH (1986). Fairness and the Assumptions of Economics. Journal of Business 59: S285–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Over H, Carpenter M (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show increased helping following priming with affiliation. Psychological Science 20: 1189–1193. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02419.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Guinote A, Cotzia I, Sandhu S, Siwa P (2015). Social status modulates prosocial behavior and egalitarianism in preschool children and adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112: 731–736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Dunfield KA, Kuhlmeier VA (2010). Intention-mediated selective helping in infancy. Psychological science 21: 523–527. doi: 10.1177/0956797610364119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wynn K (2009). Constraints on natural altruism. British Journal of Psychology 100: 481–485. doi: 10.1348/000712609X441312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Rogers TB, Kuiper NA, Kirker WS (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of personality and social psychology 35: 677–688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hepach R, Haberl K, Lambert S, Tomasello M (2017). Toddlers help anonymously. Infancy 22: 130–145. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Baumeister RF, Leary MR (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological bulletin 117: 497 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Deci EL, Vallerand RJ, Pelletier LG, Ryan RM (1991). Motivation and education: The self-determination perspective. Educational psychologist 26: 325–346. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ryan RM (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal of personality 63: 397–427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Pavey L, Greitemeyer T, Sparks P (2011). Highlighting relatedness promotes prosocial motives and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37: 905–917. doi: 10.1177/0146167211405994 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Twenge JM, Baumeister RF, DeWall CN, Ciarocco NJ, Bartels JM (2007). Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology 92: 56 doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.56 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Betts LR, Rotenberg J, Trueman M, Stiller J (2012). Examining the components of children's peer liking as antecedents of school adjustment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 30: 303–325. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02041.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Osterman KF (2000). Students' need for belonging in the school community. Review of educational research 70: 323–367. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Thaler RH (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 1: 39–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Belk RW (1988). Possession and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research 15: 139–168. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Koole S.L, Dijksterhuis A, van Knippenberg A (2001). What's in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of personality and social psychology 80: 669–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gawronski B, Bodenhausen GV, Becker AP (2007). I like it, because I like myself: Associative self-anchoring and post-decisional change of implicit evaluations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43: 221–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Morewedge CK, Shu LL, Gilbert DT, Wilson TD (2009). Bad riddance or good rubbish? Ownership and not loss aversion causes the endowment effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45: 947–951. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Maddux WW, Yang H, Falk C, Adam H, Adair W, Endo Y, Carmon Z, Heine SJ (2010). For whom is parting with possessions more painful? Cultural differences in the endowment effect. Psychological Science 21: 1910–1917. doi: 10.1177/0956797610388818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Beaman AL, Klentz B, Diener E, Svanum S (1979). Self-awareness and transgression in children: Two field studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37: 1835–1846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Ross J, Anderson JR, Campbell RN (2011). Situational changes in self-awareness influence 3-and 4-year-olds’ self-regulation. Journal of experimental child psychology 108: 126–138. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Diener E, Wallbom M (1976). Effects of self-awareness on antinormative behavior. Journal of Research in Personality 10: 107–111. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Gibbons FX, Wright RA (1983). Self-focused attention and reactions to conflicting standards. Journal of Research in Personality 17: 263–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Niwa Y, Hiraishi K, Oda R.(2011). A mirror has no effect on giving in the dictator game. Letters on Evolutionary Behavioral Science 2: 16–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Wiekens CJ, Stapel DA (2008). The mirror and I: When private opinions are in conflict with public norms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44: 1160–1166. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Alpizar F, Carlsson F, Johansson-Stenman O (2008). Anonymity, reciprocity, and conformity: Evidence from voluntary contributions to a national park in Costa Rica. Journal of Public Economics 92: 1047–1060. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Leimgruber KL, Shaw A, Santos LR, Olson KR (2012). Young children are more generous when others are aware of their actions. PloS one 7: 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Van Vugt M, Hardy CL (2010). "Cooperation for reputation: Wasteful contributions as costly signals in public goods." Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 13: 101–111. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 File. Priming scripts and experimental instructions for study 1: Sharing.

(DOCX)

S2 File. Raw data study 1: Sharing.

(XLSX)

S3 File. Experimental instructions for study 2: Helping.

(PDF)

S4 File. Raw data study 2: Helping.

(XLSX)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are available within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES