Summary
Corticobasal degeneration is a rare disorder, which usually consists of a combination of complex movement disorders, apraxia and cortical changes. Its definition is still evolving and in 2013 an international consortium tried to develop new criteria, based on a systematic literature review. Over a long period of time, we carefully selected 23 patients who fulfilled the criteria for a diagnosis of corticobasal degeneration; all had the so-called corticobasal syndrome phenotype, in accordance with Armstrong et al. (2013). Through a dedicated study, we set out to study behavioral alterations, specifically apathy, and to compare the results obtained with those deriving from a well-defined Parkinson’s disease population.
On the basis of our limited but specific results, we argue for a possible role of the parietal neural networks as a determinant of apathy, and provide an overview of emerging data in the imaging and pathology literature.
Keywords: apathy, clinician-rated corticobasal degeneration (cr-CBD), corticobasal syndrome (CBS), parietal network
Introduction
Corticobasal degeneration (CBD) is recognized as an asymmetric, neurodegenerative disorder associated with pathological accumulation of tau protein. It causes disruption of body motor schemata and significant impairment of higher cortical functions, and is characterized by a combination of cortical and basal ganglia signs (Togasaki et al., 2000). Its most distinctive clinical feature is its unilateral, or markedly asymmetric, presentation; indeed, among the entire constellation of parkinsonian syndromes, only idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (PD) has been found to present, at the earliest stage, such an asymmetry (Reich et al., 2009; Armstrong et al., 2013).
Different presentation phenotypes have been associated with this pathology: probable/possible corticobasal syndrome (CBS), frontal behavioral spatial syndrome, the non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia, and progressive supranuclear palsy syndrome (Armstrong et al., 2013). To facilitate diagnosis in neurological settings, the current diagnostic criteria have been categorized, on the basis of the different presentation phenotypes, into two different sets to allow two different diagnoses.
The first set is more reliable for clinical and research purposes and, accordingly, results in a diagnosis termed cr-CBD; this diagnosis requires a presentation pattern that mainly includes the motor feature as described in the probable CBS phenotype and only a few higher cortical features such as the alien limb phenomenon, limb apraxia, some behavioral changes or general aspecific cognitive impairment (Armstrong et al., 2013).
The second set is less specific, and allows a diagnosis of possible corticobasal degeneration (p-CBD) (Togasaki et al., 2000); this diagnosis requires the presence of the possible CBS phenotype, but otherwise the criteria are less restrictive, including a positive family history and possibly a genetically defined mutation of tau protein; there is no minimum age at onset (Armstrong et al., 2013).
Clinical diagnosis of CBD is challenging for three reasons: the full complement of findings is rarely found at presentation; moreover, if CBD is not clinically suspected, subtle but relevant findings (e.g. extinction, language impairment, myoclonus or apraxia) may not be searched for, or may not be properly appreciated; the clinical picture of CBD has some substantial overlaps with a huge variety of other parkinsonian and dementing illnesses (Reich and Grill, 2009).
Behavioral aspects of CBD have been described, but not fully studied, in specific series.
In the present study we set out to focus on a specific behavioral symptom, apathy, with the aim of clarifying its importance among other neuropsychological symptoms in two groups of patients: one group affected by cr-CBD and the other by idiopathic PD. PD is a simpler and more common clinical condition than cr-CBD. Apathy is an intriguing symptom in PD, too, and one of the most studied in recent years (Moretti and Signori, 2016); it has been considered one of its most disturbing psychiatric symptoms (Starkstein and Leentjens, 2008). Some studies have related PD apathy to dysfunction of the nigro-striatal pathway (Starkstein et al., 1992; Cutberth and Insel, 2013; Stuss et al., 2000), a suggestion supported by functional connectivity studies (Rejinders et al., 2010; Robert et al., 2012) and evidence of a marked impairment of connectivity in striatal and ventrolateral prefrontal regions (Baggio et al., 2015), although data remain controversial (Huang et al., 2013; Skidmore et al., 2013; Kos et al., 2016; Moretti and Signori, 2016).
In the present paper, we examine our findings and discuss the emerging role of parietal-frontal loops as one of the most recently discovered neural correlates underlying apathy in motor-dominant pathologies.
Materials and methods
Over a period of time (1st January 2005 to 1st July 2014) that was necessarily long due to the low prevalence rate of CBD, we collected data relating to 23 patients (12 males and 11 females) who came to our observation and were given a diagnosis of cr-CBD. These patients were included in study Group A. A comparison group, Group B, comprised 96 patients with PD, all of whom met the criteria for a diagnosis of idiopathic PD (Hughes et al., 1992).
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed in all the patients in both the groups at baseline; the baseline characteristics are reported in Table I.
Table I.
Characteristics | Group A | Group B | F chi2 value | DF | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 63.5 (2.4) | 58.1 (4.1) | 2.76 | 2.24 | 0.09 |
Gender (male/female) | 12/11 | 49/47 | 0.81 | 2 | 0.75 |
Handedness | 22.6 (1.2) | 22.1 (1.4) | 0.79 | 2 | 0.83 |
Educational level (years) | 7.7 (3.4) | 8.1 (2.4) | 0.67 | 3.1 | 0.45 |
Response to DOPA | 0 | + | NA | NA | NA |
Abbreviations: DF= degree freedom; NA= not applicable. Values are expressed as mean (SD).
The patients in Group A had a mean age of 63.5 ± 2.4 years, all of them were right-handed (with a mean score of +22.6 on the Briggs and Nebes test) (Briggs and Nebes, 1975). When recruited, all these patients showed an asymmetric akinetic syndrome, associated with bradykinesia, alien limb syndrome, gait difficulties and slurred speech, which had developed over an average period of 9.6 + 6.3 months prior to the diagnosis. No patient reported specific cognitive impairment. Objectively, none of the 23 patients responded to levodopa therapy. Parkinsonism is associated with characteristic dystonic postures of the arm and hand (flexed and adducted arm) and the baseline neurological evaluation of the Group A patients showed:
- focal and stimulus-sensitive myoclonus in 4 patients;
- asymmetric ideomotor apraxia in 19 patients;
- alien hand syndrome in 3 patients;
- astereognosia in 7 patients, 2 of whom showed visual neglect;
- a defect of up-gaze movements in 16 patients;
- speech disruption, characterized by low vocal intensity, mono-loudness, and slurred articulation, in 7 patients.
All these patients, both at the time of recruitment (baseline) and at the end of the one-year follow-up, underwent a brain MRI scan, performed with a 1.5T magnet. The examination was performed using axial and coronal slices, employing the following sequences: T1, SE proton density and T2-weighted sequences (TR/TE: 2780/2080), turbo-FLAIR (TR/TE/TI: 9832/150/2000) and TSE T2-weighted sequences (TR/TE: 2876/120). The results at baseline were the following:
- 18 patients presented asymmetrical parietal cortical atrophy contralateral to the more affected limbs;
- 8 patients showed both asymmetrical frontal and parietal atrophy, mostly involving the posterior frontal areas on the side contralateral to the clinically more affected one;
- 5 patients displayed bilateral parietal atrophy;
- 7 patients showed evident enlargement of the lateral ventricle on the same side as the more affected parietal area, with subcortical atrophy of the white matter bulk of the corresponding parietal area. The frontoparietal distribution of the atrophy was best appreciated on sagittal T1-weighted images;
- 13 patients showed a loss of the putamen signal on high-field T2-weighted MRI.
The results at one year of follow-up may be summarized as follows:
- 23 patients showed asymmetrical parietal cortical atrophy contralateral to the more affected limbs;
-12 showed asymmetrical frontal and parietal atrophy in the hemisphere contralateral to the clinically more affected side;
- 10 patients displayed bilateral parietal atrophy;
- 15 patients showed subcortical atrophy of the white matter bulk of the corresponding parietal area.
- 17 patients showed a loss of the putamen signal on high-field T2-weighted MRI.
The comparison group, Group B, was recruited by enrolling 96 consecutive outpatients with PD who met the criteria for a diagnosis of idiopathic PD (Hughes et al., 1992). They had a mean age of 58.1 ± 4.1 years, and a mean age at disease onset of 54.7 ± 3.1 years. Their mean disease duration was 3.3 ± 1.5 years. At their baseline evaluation, all these patients underwent brain MRI with a 1.5 T magnet; this examination was repeated at the one-year follow-up. In accordance with the criteria for a diagnosis of idiopathic PD (Hughes et al., 1992), we did not find any specific alteration of brain images at baseline or one year later. Patients were evaluated in ON-pharmacological states. The mean L-dopa equivalent dosage was 660 ± 130.5 mg/day. Forty-two patients received dopamine agonists during their treatment.
All the patients were followed for a period of 12 months starting from their recruitment date through periodic neurological and neuropsychological examinations. Follow-up visits were scheduled to take place at 1, 4, 8, and 12 months from baseline. A complete neuropsychological examination was conducted at baseline and at the last visit, and the results were compared.
The trial was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and with our institute’s ethical guidelines. Written informed consent was obtained from all the participants prior to the study.
Outcomes measures
The main outcomes of the study are described below.
Global performance was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005); MoCA results are reported as raw scores, and as scores adjusted for age and education, expressed as years of schooling (Conti et al., 2015; Santangelo et al., 2015).
Executive functions, attention, judgment and analogical reasoning were assessed using the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) (Dubois et al., 2000).
Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices were employed to assess logical judgment and analogical resolution of problems (Raven et al., 1981).
Apathy was assessed using the clinician-/researcher-rated version of the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES-C), and the parallel self-report version of the same instrument (AES-S) (Marin et al., 1991).
Global behavioral symptoms were assessed using the Neuro-Psychiatric Inventory, NPI (Cummings et al., 1994); symptom frequency was rated on a scale of 1 to 4 (1 = less than once a week; 2 = once a week; 3 = several times a week; 4 = every day), and severity was rated on a scale of 1 to 3 (1 = mild; 2 = moderate; 3 = severe). A composite score ranging from 1 to 12, defined as the product of frequency and severity, was calculated. Caregiver distress was also recorded and scored for each neuropsychiatric symptom complex. The caregiver was asked to rate their own emotional or psychological distress caused by each symptom on a scale of 0 to 5 (0 = no distress; 1 = minimal; 2 = mild; 3 = moderate; 4 = moderately severe; 5 = very severe distress). A total caregiver distress score was obtained by summing the individual scores on the 12 items.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 16.0). Within-group changes from baseline to 12 months were tested using the Wilcoxon Signed Ranks test. Between-group comparisons of changes from baseline were tested using the Wilcoxon two-sample test.
This was done for the overall scores for each efficacy variable. In addition, sub-analyses of behavioral data obtained at baseline and at 12 months using the AES and NPI were performed to determine which items of these scales showed particular deterioration. Results are presented as mean changes from baseline with standard deviations, and p-values are presented where appropriate.
Results
Baseline demographic variables are reported in Table I. The demographic variables age, gender, handedness and years of education were not significantly associated with either of the two clinical conditions.
Table II, showing numerical data, provides a synopsis of the cognitive performances in the two groups. At baseline, all the patients performed quite well on the MoCA and Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices, used to evaluate general cognition, and the FAB, analyzing frontal executive and abstract reasoning (considering both the total score and the specific sub-items). We did not find any significant within-group differences in either of the two groups at baseline, according to a Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test. At the 12-month follow-up, within-group comparisons (Wilcoxon Signed Rank test) showed that the group A patients showed worse than baseline performances on the MoCA (−1.8 (0.7), p<0.05) and on FAB, both for the global score [−2.1 (0.6), p<0.05], and all the sub-scores: analogies [−0.6 (0.2), p<0.05], phonemic fluency [−1.1 (0.7), p<0.05], motor series [−0.5 (0.2), p<0.05], contrast instructions [−0.5 (0.7), p<0.05], go/no-go instructions [−0.9 (0.2), p<0.05], prehension behavior [−1.0 (0.1), p<0.05]; the results remained within the range of normal values. On the other hand, no differences emerged at 12 months compared to baseline in Group B.
Table II.
Baseline Group A | Baseline Group B | 12 months Group A within-group (12 months vs baseline) p-value | 12 months Group B within-group (12 months vs baseline) p-value | 12 months Between-groups (B vs A) p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
MOCA | 27.1 (0.9) | 28.1 (0.3) | 25.3 (0.2) p<0.05 | 27.3 (0.4) | +2.0 (0.2) |
RAVEN’S STANDARD MATRICES | 123.8 (5.6) | 134.1 (2.3) | 119.1 (4.5) | 123.1 (6.7) | +4 (2.2) |
FAB total score (0–18) | 12.2 (0.5) | 14.2 (0.5) | 10.1 (1.1)* | 13.6 (0.3) | +3.5 (0.8)** |
Analogies | 2.5 (0.2) | 2.6 (0.3) | 1.9 (0.6)* | 2.2 (0.3) | +0.3 (04)* |
Phonemic fluency | 2.4 (0.2) | 2.7 (0.3) | 1.3 (0.5)* | 2.1 (0.2) | +0.8 (0.2)* |
Motor series | 2.1 (0.7) | 2.3 (0.1) | 1.6 (0.2)* | 1.9 (0.2) | +0.3 (0.2) |
Contrast instructions | 2.1 (0.8) | 2.5 (0.3) | 1.6 (0.5)* | 2.6 (0.7) | +1.0 (0.2)* |
Go/no-go | 1.6 (0.5) | 2.1 (0.5) | 0.7 (0.4)* | 2.1 (0.6) | +1.4 (0.2)* |
Prehension behavior | 1.5 (0.9) | 1.9 (0.9) | 0.5 (0.8)* | 1.5 (0.3) | +1.0 (0.5)* |
Abbreviations: MOCA= Montreal Cognitive Assessment; FAB= Frontal Assessment Battery.
MOCA scores are reported corrected for Italian normative values (see text). Values are expressed as mean (SD).
p<0.05;
p<0.01
The between-groups differences recorded at 12 months, evaluated using the Wilcoxon Two Sample Test, are also reported in Table II. With the exception of the motor series sub-item of FAB and the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices scores, all the item scores differed significantly between the groups.
At the end of the 12-month study, no subject in Group A was able to mime to verbal commands given by the examiner, while they were slightly better able to imitate the examiner performing mimes: they showed signs of ideomotor apraxia, not ideational apraxia, of the affected hand. On the contrary, when given a specific tool, their performance with the affected hand was virtually normal, although the movements were awkward and slightly clumsy. With the opposite hand they were able to carry out orders properly and could mime quite well. These patients recognized only the less affected personal and extrapersonal hemispace, and they showed modest signs of bucco-facial apraxia.
Table III reports the baseline NPI results, giving the sub-item scores; the incidence of symptoms in both groups was described through a comparison between groups (t-test).
Table III.
NPI sub-items | Number of patients Group A (and %) | Number of patients Group B (and %) | Frequency × severity Group A | Frequency × severity Group B | Caregiver distress Group A | Caregiver distress Group B |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hallucinations | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Delusions | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Agitation/aggression | 3 (13%) | 4 (4%) ** | 2 | 2 | 1 | 1 |
Dysphoria/depression | 13 (56%) | 34 (35%) ** | 4 * | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Anxiety | 15 (65%) | 41 (43%) ** | 6 | 6 | 2 | 2 |
Irritability | 1 (4%) | 12 (12%) ** | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Disinhibition | 1 (4%) | 4 (4%) | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Euphoria | 0 | 1 (1%) | 0 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Apathy | 20 (87%) | 23 (24%) ** | 8 ** | 8 | 4 | 4 |
Aberrant motor behavior | 1 (4%) | 2 (2%) | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Sleep behavior change | 2 (8%) | 7 (7%) | 4 | 4 | 3 | 3 |
Appetite change | 2 (8%) | 2 (2%) ** | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
p<0.05;
p<0.01
From a behavioral perspective, CBD is more complex than PD. From the start of the study, the Group A (CBD) patients displayed more agitation and aggression, depression, anxiety and appetite changes than the Group B (PD) patients (p<0.01); on the other hand, Group B showed more irritability than Group A (p<0.01). Apathy was found in 87% of the Group A patients, but in only 24% of the PD patients at recruitment (p<0.01).
Composite scores (ranging from 1 to 12), defined as the product of frequency and severity, were calculated for each NPI item in each group. The most salient findings were the higher scores for depression and apathy recorded in Group A compared with Group B (p<0.01). We estimated and recorded caregiver distress, too: in both groups, caregivers reported increased distress due to apathy and sleep changes.
Table IV presents the NPI results at the 12-month evaluation, giving the incidence of symptoms in both groups; it also reports comparisons both within groups (12 months vs baseline) and between groups (at 12 months). The former showed a higher incidence of agitation, depression, irritability, apathy, appetite and sleep behavior changes (p<0.01) at 12 months versus baseline in Group A, and increased agitation, apathy and appetite changes (p<0.01) in Group B. With regard to the between-groups comparison, a Wilcoxon two-sample test showed a significant difference in Group A vs Group B in agitation, depression, anxiety, irritability, apathy, sleep behavior changes, disinhibition and appetite disorders (Table IV). Apathy was reported in all the Group A patients (100%), versus only 28% of the Group B patients. The composite scores (frequency × severity) were significantly modified at 12 months vs baseline (within groups) being found to be increased for agitation, depression and apathy in Group A (p<0.01), and for depression and disinhibition (p<0.05) in Group B. The groups were found to differ significantly in caregiver distress (according to a between-groups comparison, mediated by Wilcoxon two-sample test) due to agitation, depression and apathy (p<0.05); this distress was more evident in caregivers of Group A than of Group B patients.
Table IV.
NPI sub-items | Number of patients Group A within-group vs baseline t-test | Number of patients Group B within-group vs baseline t-test | Between groups (B vs A) t-test | Frequency × severity Group A within-group vs baseline t-test | Frequency × severity Group B within-group vs baseline t-test | Caregiver distress Group A within-group vs baseline t-test | Caregiver distress Group B within-group vs baseline t-test | Caregiver distress Between groups t-test |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hallucinations | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | ||
Delusions | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | ||
Agitation/aggression | 7 (30%) ** | 8 (8%) ** | ** | 4 * | 2 | 2 * | 1 | * |
Dysphoria/depression | 19 (83%) ** | 36 (36%) | ** | 6 * | 4 * | 4 * | 2 | * |
Anxiety | 16 (66%) | 42 (43%) | * | 6 | 6 | 2 | 2 | |
Irritability | 5 (22%) ** | 10 (10%) | ** | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | |
Disinhibition | 0 ** | 6 (6%) | ** | 1 NS | 4 * | 2 | 2 | |
Euphoria | 0 | 2 (2%) | 0 | 1 | 2 | 2 | ||
Apathy | 23 (100%) ** | 27 (28%) | ** | 12 ** | 6 | 5 * | 4 | * |
Aberrant motor behavior | 0 ** | 2 (2%) | 0 | 2 | 2 | 2 | ||
Sleep behavior change | 3 (13%) ** | 7 (7%) | * | 2 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
Appetite change | 5 (22%) ** | 2 (2%) ** | ** | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
p<0.05;
p<0.01
While aware of the fact that neurodegenerative disorders are frequently characterized by coexistence of various behavioral problems, we recorded separately the data for the patients reporting single behavioral disturbances and for those with coexistent behavior disorders. The numbers and percentages of patients in the two groups (isolated or coexistent behavioral symptoms) are reported in Table V. The qualitative assessment of apathy (AES-S and AES-C) reflected the NPI scores, as shown in Table VI.
Table V.
CBD baseline (n. pts and %) | CBD 12-month follow-up (n. pts and %) | PD baseline (n. pts and %) | PD 12-month follow-up (n. pts and %) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Apathy | 16 (69.5%) | 19 (82.6%) | 20 (20.8%) | 17 (17.7%) |
Apathy+agitation +depression | 4 (17.3%) | 12 (52.1%) | 6 (6.25%) | 24 (25%) |
Depression+anxiety | 8 (34.7%) | 11 (47.8%) | 12 (12.5%) | 20 (20.8%) |
Anxiety | 10 (43.4%) | 10 (43.4%) | 35 (36.4%) | 28 (29.1%) |
Depression | 10 (43.54%) | 6 (26%) | 27 (28%) | 27 (28.7%) |
Abbreviations: CBD=corticobasal degeneration; PD=Parkinson’s disease
Table VI.
Group A Baseline | Group A 12-months (within groups Wilcoxon test) | Group B Baseline | Group B 12-months (within groups Wilcoxon test) | Between groups B vs A baseline/12months | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
AES-S | 53.3 (3.2) | 58.9 (1.8) | 21.3 (1.5) | 28.9 (1.2) | |
(+5. 6 (1.8), *) | (+7. 6 (0.4), *) | (+30. 0 (0.7), **) | |||
AES-C | 52.5 (2.1) | 58.5 (1.3) | 22.5 (2.7) | 27.3(1.1) | |
(+6. 0(1.8), *) | (+5. 2(1.3), *) | (+31. 2 (0.7), **) |
Abbreviations: AES-S=Apathy Evaluation Scale, self-report version; AES-C= Apathy Evaluation Scale, clinician-/researcher-rated version.
p<0.05;
p<0.01
With regard to a possible/hypothesized correlation between regional differences on MRI and behavior reports, our study produced some interesting findings. As we did not use voxel-based morphometry or any other procedure to measure the brain volumes, we can only report a numerical correlation of behavioral symptoms with prevalent regional atrophy in CBD patients. Therefore, considering apathy as a unique symptom and also considering the coexistence of different behavioral problems, we advance some observations regarding their relationship with MRI findings.
To weigh up the qualitative apathy scores (AES-C scores), we arbitrarily applied a cut-off between scores of 30–45 and 45–60. At baseline in CBD, we found 14 patients with a score between 30 and 45: 6 of these patients showed asymmetrical parietal cortical atrophy contralateral to the more affected limbs; 3 patients showed asymmetrical posterior frontal atrophy, associated with the parietal atrophy; 2 patients showed bilateral parietal atrophy; 3 patients showed, on the more atrophic side, an evident enlargement of the lateral ventricle, associated with subcortical atrophy of the white matter bulk of the corresponding parietal area. At baseline, 6 patients had an AES-C score of between 45 and 60; 3 of these patients showed asymmetrical parietal cortical atrophy contralateral to the more affected limbs; 1 patient showed frontal and parietal atrophy, in the hemisphere contralateral to the clinically more affected side; 2 patients showed cortical parietal plus subcortical atrophy of the white matter bulk of the corresponding parietal area.
One year later, the results can be summarized as follows: 18 patients had an AES-C score of between 30 and 45 and 6 patients an AES-C score of between 45 and 60.
In the first group, 8 patients showed asymmetrical parietal cortical atrophy contralateral to the more affected limbs; 6 patients showed asymmetrical frontal and parietal atrophy; 2 patients showed, on the more atrophic side, evident subcortical atrophy of the white matter bulk of the corresponding parietal area; 2 patients showed marked bi-parietal atrophy.
In the second group, 2 patients showed asymmetrical parietal cortical atrophy contralateral to the more affected limbs; 1 patient showed both posterior frontal and parietal atrophy; 2 patients showed, on the more atrophic side, subcortical atrophy of the white matter bulk of the corresponding parietal area; 1 patient showed marked bi-parietal atrophy.
When we considered the main behavioral aspects in CBD, as shown in Table V, we found, at baseline, that 4 patients showed apathy, agitation and depression and 16 pure apathy.
The 4 with coexistent behavioral symptoms showed frontal and posterior parietal atrophy; the 16 who manifested pure apathy showed more evident parietal atrophy (isolated cortical or cortical plus subcortical atrophy). One year later, 10 patients showed a combination of apathy, agitation and depression and 19 patients isolated apathy. The former group was found to show more marked coexistence of frontal and parietal atrophy, while the latter revealed posterior parietal or bilateral parietal atrophy.
A Spearman’s rank correlation analysis (at 12 months) highlighted the presence of significant correlations, in both the groups, between:
- higher NPI scores and caregiver distress (r=0.78, p<0.01);
- NPI apathy score and AES-S and AES-C (r=0.71, p<0.01 and r=0.78, p<0.01, respectively).
Discussion
Compared with specific cognitive signs of CBD, such as “limb kinesthetic apraxia” (Pillon et al., 1995), ideomotor apraxia (Massman et al., 1996), constructive apraxia (Massman et al., 1996), linguistic and speech alterations (Armstrong et al., 2013) and a dysexecutive syndrome (Pillon et al., 1995; Armstrong et al., 2013), all of which have been described and well accepted, behavioral aspects remain quite neglected in descriptions of cr-CBD (Litvan et al., 2000; Kertesz et al., 2004; Massman et al., 1996; Armstrong et al., 2013). Apathy has not been specifically studied in CBD since a key work, by Litvan et al. (1998), defined it as a salient aspect. However, since apathy is becoming increasingly recognized as very important in PD, Parkinson’s disease dementia complex (PDD) and parkinsonism, new data have been drawn from neuroimaging studies in PD and PDD. In patients affected by these conditions, higher apathy and lower gray matter density values were localized in the bilateral inferior parietal gyrus and right precuneus (Reijnders et al., 2010). Skidmore et al. (2013) confirmed these data, showing an association between apathy and altered imaging sequences in the right middle or-bitofrontal cortex and bilateral subgenual cingulate cortex, the left supplementary motor cortex, the left inferior parietal lobule, and the left fusiformis gyrus (Skidmore et al., 2013); this association has also been reported in the left posterior cingulate cortex (Robert et al., 2014). Therefore, even though the frontal-subcortical regions are widely regarded as the neural correlates of apathy (Duffy et al., 2000; Kos et al., 2016), new data seem to relate apathy to parietal cortical alterations. Pathological and histochemical data confirm well-established alterations of the parietal regions in CBD (Litvan et al., 2000; Togasaki et al., 2000) and this also emerges in the new cr-CBD syndrome classification (Armstrong et al., 2013). Against this background, our study was conducted to determine, in the context of a behavioral evaluation, the role of apathy in cr-CBD, from the time of clinical diagnosis through to the end of 12 months of follow-up, comparing the findings to those obtained in PD patients in the ON-phase. We are fully aware of the small number of patients recruited for this study, and therefore that the ideas deriving from our observations are speculative and cannot be considered axiomatic or somehow demonstrative.
Our aim, therefore, was to tentatively attribute a role to primary parietal cortical-subcortical atrophy (present in CBD patients) in defining apathy.
What we observed in our study can be summarized in the following points:
albeit in a limited number of patients, cr-CBD criteria were employed to allow the most specific diagnosis of this complex syndrome;
the cr-CBD patients showed cognitive impairments (i.e. apraxia and dysexecutive alterations) but they were not demented. On the other hand, from the very beginning, they manifested some behavior complex disorders, which progressed during the follow-up. They manifested affective disorders (depression was very frequent) and anxiety (with a remarkable incidence of somatic pain);
apathy is a dominant sign of behavioral impairment in cr-CBD, and it was reported as an isolated symptom in 69.5% of the cases at the beginning of the study, and in 82.6% at the end of the follow-up; among the PD patients it was reported as an isolated symptom in 20.8% at the beginning of the study and in 17.7% at the end of the follow-up;
cr-CBD patients with high apathy scores (or presenting apathy as an isolated symptom) showed more evident contralateral parietal cortical and subcortical atrophy; on the other hand, patients showing different behavioral symptoms associated with apathy presented posterior frontal and parietal atrophy.
Even though it is impossible to generalize, we can argue that apathy is a dominant behavior disorder in CBD, and was found to emerge quite significantly in cr-CBD. Apathy does not seem to be related to cognitive impairment but, taking into account the limits of our study (we did not use volumetric imaging measures), it was found to be related to parietal cortical/subcortical alterations. A direct measure, obtainable through a voxel-based morphometry approach for example, could provide a positive parametric demonstration of the hypothesized connections. Thus, a further limit of this study is the lack of objective measures to substantiate our speculations.
Nonetheless, we envisage that the parietal cortex will be shown to play a role in the genesis of apathy. However, data intended to establish a key role for this area must take into account its neural projections to many different brain regions, which are fundamental for motivational and attention processes. We therefore focused on certain functional neural networks that might provide an explanation of the parietal involvement in these “frontal” processes.
In fact, the inferior parietal lobule (LIP) mediates the integrative analysis of visual information, i.e. analysis of its type and spatial location (Ungerleider and Mishkin; 1982; Heilman et al., 1999). The LIP receives inputs from the cingulate gyrus, which is fundamental in orienteering attention; the LIP also collects inputs from the dorsolateral frontal lobes, related to goal-oriented behaviors (Heilman et al., 1999). Many projections from the frontal eye fields (FEFs) reach the LIP (Paus, 1996). The FEFs are involved in the preparation and triggering of intentional visually guided saccades, predictive saccades, and memory-guided saccades (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2003), and in target selection and attentive polarization. The projections from the supplementary eye fields (SEFs) are directed towards the LIP (Grosbras et al., 1999); SEFs are connected with all the areas involved in eye movement control — the FEFs, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex (Luppino et al., 2003) —, which are involved in motor visual programs, including saccadic control, peripersonal motor programs and the program for sequential saccadic movements (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2003). Moreover, the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) includes the intraparietal sulcus, extending from the post-central sulcus anteriorly to the parietal-occipital sulcus posteriorly, where parietal eye fields (PEFs) appear to be located (Müri et al., 1996). PEFs are involved in the control of saccades (Leigh and Zee, 1999), but also in the visual attention process (Bilsey and Goldberg, 2003), in eye-hand coordination (Simon et al., 2002), and in the visual fixation process (Rivaud et al., 1994).
The projections from the anterior cingulate cortex and from the posterior cingulate cortex reach the PEFs (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2003), and thus provide adequate control of intentional saccades (Gaymard et al., 1998).
A supposed ‘cingulate eye field’ is located at the limit between Broadmann areas 23 and 24 and it can be considered capable, via an intentional motivation process, of preparing all the frontal ocular motor areas involved in intentional saccade control to correct the forthcoming motor behavior (Goodale and Haffenden, 2003), and of controlling an eventual goal-directed action (Goodale and Haffenden, 2003).
More evidence suggests that the inferior parietal lobe (area 6) circuits have complex storage capacities, to allow memorization of elementary motor schemes and to retrieve these as required (Rizzolatti et al., 1988; Rizzolatti et al., 1988 B). Finally, it is widely accepted that many neurons of F6 become active well before movement onset (Matsuzaka et al., 1992). Most of them appear to command global reaching-grasping movements, without any indication that they are coding a specific type of grip. These data on the organization of F6 suggest that this area represents a nodal point in transmitting cognitive and motivational information from pre-frontal and cingulate areas to the premotor areas.
Motivational and cognitive signals prompting movements are continuously sent to F6, where they are supplemented with information derived from posterior parietal areas (Schlaug et al., 1994).
In conclusion, the PPC compares visual information on the location of a target to be reached with an estimate of the current limb position. The most medial parts of the superior parietal lobule (including V6a, 7m, medial intra-parietal) discharge and are modulated as a function of different combinations of inputs, including the direction of intended and executed arm movements, static postures, retinal input, motion of the target and visual feedback about movements of the arm, and the direction of gaze and eye movements (Kalaska et al., 2003).
All these aspects have led to the acknowledgment that the parietal lobe participates in motor and spatial control, particularly the coordinate-transformation system for sensory driven strategies of the eyes, arm and hand (Wiesendanger et al., 1997; Heilman et al., 1999). Superior parietal areas appear more involved in voluntary, anticipatory allocation of attention to peripheral locations (motor planning), whereas inferior parietal regions are activated for stimulus-driven reorienting of attention (Macaluso and Driver, 2003).
Parietal areas might contribute to the purposeful, goal-directed action, and therefore, to the complex network which guarantees visual control for planned and goal-directed actions. Parietal cortical disruption might therefore contribute to the apathy symptom in cr-CBD.
Acknowledgements
The Authors thank Joy Bates, PhD, for her valuable assistance in editing the text.
References
- Armstrong MJ, Litvan I, Lang AE, et al. Criteria for the diagnosis of corticobasal degeneration. Neurology. 2013;80:496–503. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0fd1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baggio HC, Segura B, Garrido-Millan JL, Marti MJ, Compta Y, et al. Resting-state frontostriatal functional connectivity in Parkinson’s disease-related apathy. Mov Disord. 2015;30:671–679. doi: 10.1002/mds.26137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bilsey JW, Goldberg ME. Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science. 2003;299:81–86. doi: 10.1126/science.1077395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briggs GC, Nebes RD. Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex. 1975;11:230–238. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(75)80005-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conti S, Bonazzi S, Laiacona M, et al. Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)-Italian version: regression based norms and equivalent scores. Neurological Sciences. 2015;36:209–214. doi: 10.1007/s10072-014-1921-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al. The Neu-ropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308–2314. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.12.2308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med. 2013;11:126. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, et al. The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology. 2000;55:1621–1626. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duffy J. Apathy in neurological disorders. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2000;2:434–439. doi: 10.1007/s11920-000-0029-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaymard B, Rivaud S, Cassarini JF, et al. Effects of anterior cingulate cortex lesions on ocular saccades in humans. Exp Brain Res. 1998;120:173–183. doi: 10.1007/s002210050391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodale MA, Haffenden AM. Interactions between the dorsal and ventral streams of visual processing. In: Siegel AM, Andersen RA, Freund HJ, Spencer DD, editors. The parietal lobes Advances in neurology. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; Philapdelphia: 2003. pp. 249–267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosbras MH, Lobel E, Van de Moortele PF, et al. An anatomical landmark for the supplementary eye fields in human revealed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex. 1999;9:705–711. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.7.705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilman KM, Valenstein E, Watson RT. Treatment of neglect and related disorders. American Academy of Neurology lecture; Philadelphia: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Huang C, Ravdin LD, Nirenberg MJ, et al. Neuroimaging markers of motor and nonmotor features of Parkinson’s disease: an 18f fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission computed tomography study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;35:183–196. doi: 10.1159/000345987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, et al. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s Disease: a clinicopathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psych. 1992;55:181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalaska JF, Cisek P, Gosselin-Kessiby N. Mechanism of selection and guidance of reaching movements in the parietal lobe. In: Siegel AM, Andersen RA, Freund HJ, Spencer DD, editors. The parietal lobes Advances in neurology. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; Philapdelphia: 2003. pp. 95–119. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kertesz A, Munoz D. Relationship between frontotemporal dementia and corticobasal degeneration/progressive supranuclear palsy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;17:282–286. doi: 10.1159/000077155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kos C, van Tol MJ, Marsman JBC, et al. Neural correlates of apathy in patients with neurodegenerative disorders, acquired brain injury, and psychiatric disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;69:381–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leigh RJ, Zee DS. The neurology of eye movements. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Litvan I, Cummings JL, Mega M. Neuropsychiatric features of corticobasal degeneration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1998;65:717–21. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.65.5.717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litvan I, Grimes DA, Lang AE. Phenotypes and Prognosis: Clinicopathologic Studies of Corticobasal Degeneration. In: Litvan I, Goetz CK, Lang AE, editors. Advances in Neurology Corticobasal degeneration and related disorders. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; Philadelphia: 2000. pp. 183–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luppino G, Rozzi S, Calzavara R, et al. Pre-frontal and agranular cingulate projections to the dorsal premotor areas F2 and F7 in the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:559–578. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02476.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macaluso E, Driver J. Multimodal spatial representations in the human parietal cortex: evidence from functional imaging. In: Siegel AM, Andersen RA, Freund HJ, Spencer DD, editors. The parietal lobes Advances in neurology. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; Philapdelphia: 2003. pp. 219–233. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S. Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psychiatr Res. 1991;38:143–162. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90040-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massman PJ, Kreiter KT, Jankovic J, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in corticalbasal ganglionic degeneration: differentiation from Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1996;46:720–726. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.3.720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuzaka Y, Aizawa H, Tanji I. A motor area rostral to the supplementary motor area (presupplementary motor area) in the monkey: neuronal activity during a learned motor task. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:653–662. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.3.653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moretti R, Signori R. Neural correlates for apathy: frontal-prefrontal and parietal corticalsubcortical circuits. Front Ag Neurosci. 2016;8:289. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müri RM, Vermersch AI, Rivaud S, et al. Effects of single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices during memory-guided saccades in humans. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:2102–2106. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.2102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paus T. Location and function of the human frontal eye field: a selective review. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:475–483. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00134-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Müri RM, Ploner CJ, et al. Cortical control of ocular saccades in humans: a model for motricity. Prog Brain Res. 2003;142:3–17. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)42003-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillon B, Blin J, Vidailhet M, et al. The neuropsychological pattern of corticobasal degeneration. Comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1995;45:1477–1483. doi: 10.1212/wnl.45.8.1477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 2003, updated 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Reich SG, Grill SE. Corticobasal Degeneration. Current Treatment Options in Neurology. 2009;11:179–185. doi: 10.1007/s11940-009-0021-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reijnders JS, Scholtissen B, Weber WE, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of apathy in Parkinson’s disease: a magnetic resonance imaging study using voxel-based morphometry. Mov Disord. 2010;25:2318–2325. doi: 10.1002/mds.23268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rivaud S, Müri RM, Gaymard B, et al. Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans. Exp Brain Res. 1994;102:110–120. doi: 10.1007/BF00232443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Camarda R, Fogassi M, et al. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Exp Brain Res. 1988;71:491–507. doi: 10.1007/BF00248742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Gentilucci M. Motor and visual-motor functions of the premotor cortex. In: Rakic P, Singer W, editors. Neurobiology of Neocortex. Chichester: Wiley; 1988. pp. 269–284. [Google Scholar]
- Robert G, Le Jeune F, Lozachmeur C, et al. Apathy in patients with Parkinson disease without dementia or depression: a PET study. Neurology. 2012;79:1155–1160. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182698c75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robert G, Le Jeune F, Dondaine T, et al. Apathy and impaired emotional facial recognition networks overlap in Parkinson’s disease: a PET study with conjunction analyses. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2014;85:1153–1158. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-307025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santangelo G, Siciliano M, Pedone R, et al. Normative data for the Montreal cognitive assessment in an Italian population sample. Neurological Sciences. 2015;36:585–591. doi: 10.1007/s10072-014-1995-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlaug G, Knorr U, Seitz RJ. Inter-subject variability of cerebral activation in acquiring a motor skill: a study with positron emission tomography. Exp Brain Res. 1994;98:523–534. doi: 10.1007/BF00233989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon O, Mangin JF, Cohen L, et al. Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron. 2002;33:475–487. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00575-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skidmore FM, Yang M, Baxter L, et al. Apathy, depression, and motor symptoms have distinct and separable resting activity patterns in idiopathic Parkinson disease. Neuroimage. 2013;81:484–495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkstein SE, Leentjens AF. The nosological position of apathy in clinical practice. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr. 2008;10:202–209. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.136895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkstein SE, Mayberg SE, Preziosi TJ, et al. Reliability, validity and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry. 1992;4:134–197. doi: 10.1176/jnp.4.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuss DT, Van Reekum R, Murphy KJ. Differentiation of states and causes of apathy. In: Borod JC, editor. The Neuropsychology of emotion. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2000. pp. 340–363. [Google Scholar]
- Togasaki DM, Tanner CM. Epidemiologic aspects. In: Litvan I, Goetz CG, Lang AE, editors. Corticobasal Degeneration Advances in Neurology. Vol. 82. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; Philapdelphia: 2000. pp. 53–59. [Google Scholar]
- Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M. Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJW, editors. Analysis of visual behavior. MA: MIT press; Cambridge: 1982. pp. 549–586. [Google Scholar]
- Wiesendanger M, Kazennikov O, Perrig S, et al. Reaching, grasping and bimanual coordination with special reference to the posterior parietal cortex. In: Thier P, Karnath HO, editors. Parietal lobe contributions to orientation in 3D space. Springer Verlag; Berlin: 1997. pp. 271–288. [Google Scholar]