Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2018 Jan 10;8:332. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18706-y

Exploring the consequences of social defeat stress and intermittent ethanol drinking on dopamine dynamics in the rat nucleus accumbens

Alex L Deal 1, Joanne K Konstantopoulos 1, Jeff L Weiner 2, Evgeny A Budygin 1,3,
PMCID: PMC5762836  PMID: 29321525

Abstract

The current study aimed to explore how presynaptic dopamine (DA) function is altered following brief stress episodes and chronic ethanol self-administration and whether these neuroadaptations modify the acute effects of ethanol on DA dynamics. We used fast-scan cyclic voltammetry to evaluate changes in DA release and uptake parameters in rat nucleus accumbens brain slices by analyzing DA transients evoked through single pulse electrical stimulation. Adult male rats were divided into four groups: ethanol-naïve or ethanol drinking (six week intermittent two-bottle choice) and stressed (mild social defeat) or nonstressed. Results revealed that the mild stress significantly increased DA release and uptake in ethanol-naïve subjects, compared to nonstressed controls. Chronic ethanol self-administration increased the DA uptake rate and occluded the effects of stress on DA release dynamics. Bath-applied ethanol decreased stimulated DA efflux in a concentration-dependent manner in all groups; however, the magnitude of this effect was blunted by either stress or chronic ethanol, or by a combination of both procedures. Together, these findings suggest that stress and ethanol drinking may promote similar adaptive changes in accumbal presynaptic DA release measures and that these changes may contribute to the escalation in ethanol intake that occurs during the development of alcohol use disorder.

Introduction

It is well known that a wide range of stressful events can effectively trigger alcohol drinking behaviors in humans13. This phenomenon can be reproduced in several animal models of alcohol use disorder (AUD). For example, studies in primates revealed that stress via social separation significantly increased ethanol consumption even in monkeys not particularly vulnerable to fear-related behaviors4. Similar stress-related effects on ethanol drinking behavior can also be observed in rodent models of AUD. Thus, mice and rats exposed to social defeat episodes or socially isolated during adolescence show a persistent escalation in two-bottle choice drinking paradigms510.

In fact, both ethanol consumption and stress exposure induce overlapping neuroadaptations in several brain circuitries, including the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway1115. For example, ex vivo neurochemical studies have reported that both contingent and non-contingent chronic ethanol exposure1619 as well as early-life social isolation stress9,20 trigger increases in dopamine (DA) reuptake rate in striatal subregions, including the nucleus accumbens. This outcome may result (at least in part) in a functional deficit in tonic DA release. Indeed, both stress and chronic ethanol exposure promote decreases in extracellular DA levels measured by microdialysis15,21. Based on these and other findings, it has been suggested that these conditions can induce a hypodopaminergic state that may alter the perception of rewarding and aversive effects of ethanol. Moreover, such neuroadaptations may contribute to the escalation in ethanol drinking behavior that drives AUD. However, relatively few studies have examined the effects of voluntary ethanol drinking on DA signaling dynamics or have examined possible interactions between the effects of stress and chronic ethanol intake on mesolimbic DA transmission, particularly in standard outbred animals. Therefore, the primary aim of the current study was to explore the effects of a relatively mild social defeat stress paradigm and an intermittent home-cage drinking procedure, which promotes moderate levels of ethanol intake, on DA dynamics in the nucleus accumbens. This study also sought to determine if a history of ethanol self-administration influenced any DA neuroadaptations induced by social defeat stress or if either procedure altered the acute effects of ethanol on accumbal DA release. These questions were addressed using fast-scan cyclic voltammetry (FSCV) coupled with single pulse electrical stimulation to evaluate changes in DA release and uptake parameters in the rat nucleus accumbens. These measures were performed on brain slices obtained from rats that were ethanol-naïve or chronically exposed to an intermittent home-cage ethanol drinking regimen and subsequently exposed or not exposed to stress (social defeat).

Results

Ethanol self-administration abolishes stress-induced increase in evoked DA release in the nucleus accumbens

Figure 1 schematically represents the experimental design of the current experiments for ethanol-drinking rats (ethanol-naïve subjects underwent the same design with access to water only). Electrically-evoked DA release data, obtained with ex vivo FSCV, were analyzed for naïve (EN) and ethanol drinking (EE) rats that were either exposed to social-defeat stress episodes or served as nonstressed control animals (Figs 2 and 3a). The data were proved to be normally distributed following a Shapiro-Wilk test for normality (p > 0.05). A two-way ANOVA analyzing the differences in concentration of DA released per stimulus pulse showed a main effect of stress condition (F(1,25) = 6.562, p = 0.0168) but not ethanol experience (F(1,25) = 0.430, p = 0.518) and a trend towards a significant interaction (F(1,25) = 3.174, p = 0.0870). Tukey’s multiple comparisons test revealed that this parameter was significantly greater in ethanol-naïve subjects exposed to social defeat stress compared to ethanol-naïve subjects not exposed to stress (843.4 ± 86.6 vs 504.4 ± 49.4 nM; p = 0.0056; Fig. 3a). In marked contrast, social defeat stress episodes did not change DA release in animals with a prior history of intermittent ethanol drinking (592.3 ± 19.9 vs 653.2 ± 107.2 nM; p > 0.05; Figs 2 and 3a). There was also no difference in the magnitude of DA release between the nonstressed ethanol-naïve and ethanol-drinking cohorts (504.4 ± 49.4 vs 592.3 ± 19.9 nM; p > 0.05; Figs 2 and 3a). Therefore, prior ethanol drinking did not alter electrically-evoked DA release in nonstressed control subjects but did prevent the increase in evoked DA release promoted by social defeat stress in rat nucleus accumbens.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic representation of performed experiments. Subjects were divided into 4 groups: Naïve, ethanol-exposed, stress-exposed, and exposed to both ethanol and stress rats. Briefly, ethanol-exposed subjects had access to water and ethanol on Monday, Wednesday, and Friday (dark boxes) and only water on other days (light boxes). After a minimum of 6 weeks, stress-exposed subjects underwent 3 days of social defeat stress exposure. One hour after the third social defeat stress session, subjects were taken for in vitro fast-scan cyclic voltammetry (FSCV).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Representative traces of electrically-evoked accumbal DA release measured in brain slices from naïve (a), stressed (b), ethanol drinking nonstressed (c) or stressed (d) rats (upper panel). These signals had an oxidation peak at +0.6 V and reduction peak at −0.2 V versus an Ag/AgCl reference, identifying the released species as DA. Representative color plots topographically depict the voltammetric data, with time on the x-axis, applied scan potential on the y-axis, and background-subtracted faradaic current shown on the z-axis in pseudo-color (lower panel). Inline graphic = electrical stimulus onset.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Prior ethanol drinking alters effects of social-defeat stress on DA dynamics in rat nucleus accumbens. Stressed subjects showed significantly higher DA release per stimulus pulse compared to nonstress control (A, left panel). Prior ethanol exposure completely blunted this effect (A, right panel). Both stress (B, left panel) and ethanol exposure (B, right panel) significantly increased accumbal DA uptake, measured as the V max. However, there was no significant difference between subjects exposed to defeat stress episodes and ethanol (B, left panel). Data presented as mean ± SEM and analyzed by a two-way ANOVA. A single slice was taken from each subject in every experimental group for FSCV recordings. The number of subjects for naïve nonstressed: n = 10; for naïve stressed: n = 9; for ethanol drinking and nonstressed: n = 5; for ethanol drinking and stressed: n = 5. *p < 0.5; **p < 0.01.

Social defeat stress and ethanol self-administration accelerate DA uptake in the nucleus accumbens

In parallel with the analysis of the magnitude of DA release, the DA uptake rate, expressed as V max, was analyzed and compared between groups to determine the effects of social stress and ethanol self-administration on this parameter (Fig. 3b). The data were shown to be normally distributed by passing a Shapiro-Wilk test for normality (p > 0.05). A two-way ANOVA found a significant interaction (F(1,27) = 9.084; p = 0.0056) but no main effect of stress condition (F(1,27) = 2.673, p = 0.114) or ethanol experience (F(1,27) = 0.703, p = 0.409). As observed with DA release, Tukey’s multiple comparisons test showed significantly higher uptake rates in stressed ethanol-naïve subjects relative to their nonstressed controls (2008 ± 164 vs 1251 ± 162 nM/s; p = 0.0058; Fig. 3b). Interestingly, a history of ethanol drinking also significantly increased the rate of DA uptake (2008 ± 164 vs 1878 ± 49 nM/s; p = 0.0323; Fig. 3b) and blocked any further stress-associated enhancement of this parameter in the nucleus accumbens (1878 ± 49 vs 1653 ± 253 nM/s; p > 0.05; Fig. 3b).

Stress and ethanol self-administration reduce effects of an acute ethanol challenge on electrically-evoked DA efflux

The effects of acute ethanol on evoked accumbal DA release were compared across all experimental conditions. These DA release changes were plotted as a percent of the baseline signal collected prior to application of the first ethanol concentration (Fig. 4). Due to unequal variances between groups, a nonparametric analysis was used to evaluate these data. Using the Friedman test revealed a significant effect of condition (χ2(3) = 45.04, p < 0.0001). Furthermore, linear regression analysis of DA release changes during acute ethanol application found that all slopes for all groups were significantly different from 0 (p < 0.0001) and negative. Correlation coefficients (r2) were 0.856 (EN-Nonstress), 0.846 (EN-Stress), 0.664 (EE-Nonstress), and 0.685 (EE-Stress). Dunn’s multiple comparisons test found that the ethanol-naïve, nonstress group was significantly different than all other groups regarding the acute effect of ethanol on DA release (p < 0.001). However, no difference was found between EN-Stress, EE-Nonstress and EE-Stress groups (p > 0.5).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Prior ethanol exposure and social-defeat stress significantly attenuate the effects of acutely applied ethanol on electrically-evoked DA release in rat nucleus accumbens. The inserted bars above each ethanol concentration along the x-axis represent the averaged DA efflux observed during the final 20 min of each concentration as a percent of pre-ethanol values (dashed line = 100% of baseline). All data are presented as mean ± SEM and analyzed by a Friedman test. A single slice per rat was used for FSCV in every experimental group. The number of subjects for naïve nonstressed: n = 7; for naïve stressed: n = 10; for ethanol drinking and nonstressed: n = 6; for ethanol drinking and stressed: n = 5. ***p < 0.001.

Stress and ethanol self-administration do not modify evoked DA release recovery following an acute ethanol challenge

After the final ethanol concentration was perfused, we analyzed the recovery of the DA signal by washing aCSF over the slice (Fig. 5). The amplitudes of DA release from the final 3 time points of ethanol application at the 220 mM concentration were averaged to calculate a new baseline (100%). This approach allowed us to more accurately to compare the recovery of evoked DA release following acute ethanol exposure across all experimental conditions. Due to unequal variances between groups, a nonparametric test was used to analyze these results. According to the Friedman test, there was no significant difference between conditions (χ2(3) = 4.96, p > 0.05) during the recovery period. Therefore, neither defeat stress nor ethanol drinking experience or their combination resulted in any changes in DA signal recovery from the acute effect of ethanol applied locally in the nucleus accumbens.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Stress and ethanol exposure do not affect electrically-evoked DA signal recovery during aCSF wash following ethanol application. After the final ethanol concentration (220 mM), the brain slices were perfused with aCSF containing no ethanol. There was no significant difference in the recovery of electrically-stimulated DA efflux between any groups after replacing the ethanol solution with aCSF. Data are presented as mean ± SEM and analyzed by a Friedman test. The number of subjects (a single slice per rat) for naïve nonstressed: n = 7; for naïve stressed: n = 10; for ethanol drinking and nonstressed: n = 6; for ethanol drinking and stressed: n = 5.

Discussion

The most fascinating finding of this study was that a relatively mild stress exposure (three social defeat episodes without any physical interaction) significantly altered evoked DA release and consequent uptake in accumbal brain slices. Moreover, we also found that a history of moderate ethanol self-administration through the two-bottle choice paradigm significantly increased the rate of DA uptake and that this drinking experience blunted the effects of social defeat stress on accumbal DA dynamics. Finally, we demonstrated that stress and ethanol self-administration decreased the acute effects of ethanol on evoked DA efflux in the nucleus accumbens and that ethanol drinking occluded additional effects of stress on this measure. No group differences were observed in the time course of the recovery of the DA response following acute ethanol exposure.

This study extends our earlier report that social defeat stress can increase the frequency of burst activity in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and therefore increase phasic DA release in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats22. It is important to highlight that these changes remained after the confrontation terminated and the defeated animal was returned to its home cage22. Here we demonstrated that the consequences of this stressor on DA dynamics can also be detected in an ex vivo preparation, where DA efflux is triggered by electrical stimulation of terminals which are disconnected from the DA cell bodies. Therefore, relatively brief stress episodes can result in significant alterations in presynaptic DA activity. These results are in good agreement with previous ex vivo findings demonstrating enhanced evoked DA efflux and reuptake rate following adolescent social isolation, a well-established model of chronic early-life stress9,20,23. Studies using the social isolation model revealed that the increased DA uptake was accompanied by an increase in expression of the DA transporter. Together, these data demonstrate that different stressful events may promote analogous maladaptive changes in presynaptic DA dynamics.

Previous findings revealed a reduction in dynorphin peptide and dynorphin mRNA levels following social isolation and social defeat stress, respectively9,24. Importantly, activation of kappa opiod receptors (KORs), which are found on DA terminals, suppresses DA release in the nucleus accumbens9,25,26. Consequently, decreased levels of endogenous agonist could result in reduced inhibition of DA release through the activation of KORs. In addition to this possible mechanism, accumbal DA release can be increased via co-activation of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) 1 and CRF2 receptors27.

Dynorphin-DA interactions may potentially explain why prior ethanol exposure completely prevented the stress-induced DA increases. There is evidence for enhanced levels of dynorphin in rat brain regions, including the nucleus accumbens, following repeated ethanol administration28,29. Moreover, a recent study demonstrated that chronic ethanol exposure increased KOR responsiveness to agonist30,31. The combination of these changes might result in suppression of DA release, since KOR activation has clear inhibitory actions on accumbal DA release26,30. In fact, a reduction in electrically-evoked DA release following chronic ethanol vapor exposure was observed in mouse19,30, but not rat brain slices17. However, electrically-evoked DA release measured in the nucleus accumbens in vivo was not different between ethanol-naïve rats and an adolescent binge alcohol group32 and intermittent ethanol drinking did not alter accumbal DA efflux in the current study. The difference between species combined with the model of ethanol exposure and the time point when measurements were performed may account for these discrepancies. Perhaps, this effect of chronic ethanol exposure through two-bottle choice on electrically-evoked DA can be blunted in rats to a greater extent than in mice. For example, reduced accumbal D2 autoreceptor regulation in rats3335 could obstruct this effect. Alternatively, chronic ethanol-induced supersensitivity of D2 autoreceptors combined with the increased reuptake rate observed in mouse nucleus accumbens19,30, but not adaptations in the KOR system, can be preferentially responsible for the DA decrease following chronic ethanol19,30.

This study investigated the influence of stress and chronic ethanol exposure through the two-bottle choice procedure on the acute effects of ethanol on DA release in male rats. Therefore, future studies using females are necessary to explore a possible sex difference in stress and alcohol interaction, since women are more susceptible to AUD than men. It should also be noted that we focused on the effects of relatively high ethanol concentrations, as in vitro FSCV studies can only detect the inhibitory effects of ethanol on DA release dynamics16,3639, since DA cell bodies are not present in these preparations. In contrast, lower doses of ethanol have a well-characterized excitatory effect on DA transmission when administered in vivo. This occurs through an increase in neuronal firing rate in the ventral tegmental area40,41, thereby elevating DA release in the nucleus accumbens4245. Therefore, any changes observed in the acute effects of ethanol in brain slices may preferentially reflect the development of tolerance to the inhibitory effect of ethanol on accumbal DA release. In fact, prior stress and chronic ethanol drinking, either alone or together, significantly reduced the acute effects of ethanol on electrically-evoked DA efflux in the rat nucleus accumbens in this study. These findings are consistent with other findings in the literature. For example, exposure to binge levels of ethanol during adolescence blunted ethanol inhibition of electrically-evoked DA release measured in the nucleus accumbens of adult rats in vivo 32. Similarly, a complete tolerance to the inhibitory effect of 150 mM ethanol on DA release was observed in caudate slices from monkeys which drank ethanol for 18 months16, although the ability of lower ethanol concentrations (80 and 120 mM) to decrease electrically-evoked DA release following seven months of the same drinking model was unchanged46. Together, these data suggest that the time required for the development of tolerance to the acute inhibitory effects of ethanol on accumbal DA release may vary in different species.

To our knowledge, this study is the first to report on the effects of stress exposure on ethanol inhibition of accumbal DA release. On the other hand, prior work has shown that social stress boosted the excitatory effects of ethanol on DA release in the rat nucleus accumbens47. The combination of enhanced excitation (at low to moderate ethanol doses) and blunted inhibition (at higher concentrations) observed in the present experiment may lead to decreased aversive and increased rewarding properties of ethanol. This shift could potentially accelerate ethanol drinking behavior and perhaps the development of earlier stage of addiction. In agreement with this suggestion, exposing mice and rats to social defeat episodes or social isolation during adolescence promotes increases in ethanol intake510. Together, these results suggest that social defeat stress and chronic ethanol drinking through the two-bottle choice paradigm trigger overlapping adaptations at DA terminals in the nucleus accumbens, which may promote maladaptive escalations in ethanol drinking behavior associated with alcohol addiction.

Materials and Methods

Animals and behavioral procedures

Single-housed male Long Evans rats (400–600 g; Envigo) were used in this study. Animals were kept on a 12-hr light/dark cycle (lights off at 6 pm). Food and water were available ad libitum. Subjects were assigned to either ethanol-naïve (EN) or ethanol-exposed (EE) and nonstress or stress groups. For the EE subjects, ethanol (20%) and water were available for 24 hrs through a two-bottle choice paradigm48 on each Monday, Wednesday, and Friday for 6–8 consecutive weeks. Water and ethanol bottles were alternated to account for side preference and weighed to quantify daily consumption. There was no difference in ethanol intake values measured for the entire drinking period between the nonstress and stress groups (3.1 ± 0.1 vs 3.5 ± 0.2 g/kg/day, respectively). The average ethanol intake for the week prior to voltammetric experiments for all subjects was 3.7 ± 0.3 g/kg/day. Notably, previous studies have revealed that this level of ethanol consumption generated pharmacologically relevant blood ethanol concentrations49. Ethanol was removed from the cages twenty-four hours before sacrificing the animals for neurochemical analyses.

Ethanol-naïve and ethanol-exposed subjects were stressed three times via a social defeat procedure on separate days. We used an open top, clear Plexiglas container (h = 18″; l = 54″; w = 24″) divided into two equal chambers with an opening (7″ × 7″) in the dividing wall connecting the two halves. “Intruder” stressed rats were confined within an immobile cylindrical metal cage (h = 8.25″; diameter = 10.75″) in the middle of one chamber while the “aggressor” rat was placed in the empty chamber. The social defeat stress bouts were conducted during the light phase around 9 a.m. and lasted for 30 min each. The “aggressor” rat was able to move freely around the testing apparatus, including up to the metal cage confining the “intruder” rat, but physical contact between the rats could not occur. The exposure of intruders was counterbalanced between different groups to minimize possible effects of habituation of the used aggressive residents. The same “aggressor” rat was used for all stress bouts for all “intruder” rats to control for variations in aggressive behaviors. Observations noted the “aggressor” would often stand up against the containment cylinder or place his front paws on top of the cylinder. Although direct physical contact between the “intruder” and “aggressor” was prevented50, these postures were sufficiently anxiogenic, as the “intruder” rat exhibited species-specific stress behaviors (e.g. hunched posture, decreased movement)11,51. The chambers were cleaned with a mild detergent and bleach solution between each stress procedure. The “intruder” rats (EN and EE stress groups) were further used for voltammetric experiments.

Animal handling and all procedures were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. All protocols were approved by the Wake Forest University School of Medicine Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Fast-scan cyclic voltammetry

The “intruder” rats (EN, n = 10 and EE, n = 5 per group) were sacrificed 1 h following the third stress event. Nonstressed (EN, n = 7 and EE, n = 6) animals, matched by age and housing conditions, were taken at a similar time of day to be sacrificed, with nonstressed EN subjects serving as the control. Briefly, rats were anesthetized with isoflurane, decapitated and the brain was rapidly removed and immediately placed in ice-cold oxygenated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF): 126 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 1.2 mM NaH2PO4 (monobasic), 2.4 mM CaCl2, 1.2 mM MgCl2, 0.4 mM L-ascorbic acid, 11 mM C6H12O6, 25 mM NaHCO3, and adjusted pH to 7.40. Coronal slices 400 µm thick containing the nucleus accumbens were obtained using a vibrating tissue slicer (Vibratome 1000 Plus, The Vibratome Company, St. Louis, MO, USA) and then placed in oxygenated aCSF at room temperature and allowed 30 min to equilibrate. The slices were taken and placed on a submersion recording chamber with room-temperature oxygenated aCSF flowing at a rate of 1 mL/min. Carbon fiber electrodes (diameter: 7 µm; Goodfellow Cambridge Ltd., Huntington, UK) were constructed as previously described52 and the exposed carbon fiber was trimmed to 90–145 µm with a scalpel under a microscope (Leica, Buffalo Grove, IL, USA). The carbon fiber electrode was connected to a voltammetric amplifier (UNC Electronics Design Facility, Chapel Hill, NC, USA) and placed into the nucleus accumbens core while a twisted bipolar stimulating electrode (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA, USA) was placed on the surface of the tissue ~200 µm from the recording electrode and connected to a voltage output box. DA release was evoked by a single, rectangular, electrical pulse (350 µA, 4 ms/phase, monophasic) applied every 5 min. Extracellular DA was recorded at the carbon fiber electrode every 100 ms for 15 s by applying a triangular waveform (−0.4 V to +1.3 V and back to −0.4 V vs Ag/AgCl, 400 V/s). DA was identified by observing background-subtracted cyclic voltammograms characterized by oxidation and reduction peaks occurring at ~+0.6 and ~−0.2 V, respectively (vs. Ag/AgCl reference). Following acquisition of a stable baseline DA signal (3 recordings within 10% variability), ethanol was washed over the slice at increasing concentrations (20 mM, 80 mM, 160 mM, and 220 mM ethanol in oxygenated aCSF) for 30 min each. Oxygenated aCSF was washed over the slice at the conclusion of the 220 mM exposure to measure DA signal recovery for 40 min. Data were digitized (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) and stored on a computer. The response of the carbon fiber microelectrode was calibrated in a flow injection analysis system after each experiment53. The calibrations were performed in triplicate using a known concentration (1 µM) of DA (Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) that was dissolved in calibration buffer adjusted pH to 7.40 at room temperature. The voltammetric current was measured at the peak potential. The averaged calibration factor was 14 ± 3 nA per 1 µM of DA.

FSCV kinetic analysis

DA release parameters (i.e., DA release per stimulus pulse ([DA]p) and uptake (V max)) of recordings prior to ethanol administration were calculated using kinetic modeling (LVIT software, UNC, Chapel Hill, NC, USA). These parameters were modeled assuming to follow Michaelis-Menten kinetics17,54. DA concentration changes with respect to time were estimated by equation (1):

d[DA]/dt=(f)[DA]p(Vmax/{(Km/[DA])+1}) 1

where f is the stimulation frequency (Hz), [DA]p is the concentration of DA released per stimulus pulse, and V max and K m are Michaelis-Menten rate constants for DA uptake. The baseline value of K m was set to 0.16–0.20 µM, a value determined in rat brain synaptosomes55.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software version 6.05, San Diego, CA, USA). Two-way ANOVAs and Tukey’s multiple comparisons tests (where appropriate) were used to analyze DA release and uptake data. Due to unequal variances between groups, evoked DA efflux data were analyzed as a percent of baseline using nonparametric Friedman tests and Dunn’s multiple comparisons test (where appropriate). Data are presented as mean ± SEM and the criterion for significance was set at p < 0.05.

Data availability

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

The research for this study was funded by NIH grants AA022449 (E.A.B.), AA21099, AA 17531 (J.L.W.), P50 AA026117-01 (E.A.B., J.L.W.), T32AA007565 (A.L.D.) and supported in part by the Russian Science Foundation grant 14-50-00069. We would also like to acknowledge Valentina Grinevich for help with behavioral experiments and Benjamin Rowland for statistical assistance.

Author Contributions

E.A.B., J.L.W., and A.L.D. designed the experiments. A.L.D. and J.K.K. collected the data for the experiments. A.L.D. conducted the statistical analyses. A.L.D., J.L.W., and E.A.B. contributed to writing the paper and all authors reviewed the manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology. 2001;158:343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tamers SL, et al. The impact of stressful life events on excessive alcohol consumption in the French population: findings from the GAZEL cohort study. PLoS One. 2014;9:e87653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Thomas SE, Bacon AK, Randall PK, Brady KT, See RE. An acute psychosocial stressor increases drinking in non-treatment-seeking alcoholics. Psychopharmacology. 2011;218:19–28. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2163-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Higley JD, Hasert MF, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M. Nonhuman primate model of alcohol abuse: effects of early experience, personality, and stress on alcohol consumption. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1991;88:7261–7265. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.16.7261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Caldwell EE, Riccio DC. Alcohol self-administration in rats: Modulation by temporal parameters related to repeated mild social defeat stress. Alcohol. 2010;44:265–274. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2010.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lopez MF, Laber K. Impact of social isolation and enriched environment during adolescence on voluntary ethanol intake and anxiety in C57BL/6J mice. Physiol. Behav. 2015;148:151–156. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Norman KJ, et al. Social stress and escalated drug self-administration in mice I. Alcohol and corticosterone. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:991–1001. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3733-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Skelly MJ, Chappell AE, Carter E, Weiner JL. Adolescent social isolation increases anxiety-like behavior and ethanol intake and impairs fear extinction in adulthood: Possible role of disrupted noradrenergic signaling. Neuropharmacology. 2015;97:149–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Karkhanis AN, Rose JH, Weiner JL, Jones SR. Early-Life Social Isolation Stress Increases Kappa Opioid Receptor Responsiveness and Downregulates the Dopamine System. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;41:2263–2274. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Panksepp JB, Rodriguez ED, Ryabinin AE. Sweetened ethanol drinking during social isolation: enhanced intake, resistance to genetic heterogeneity and the emergence of a distinctive drinking pattern in adolescent mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2017;16:369–383. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Miczek KA, Yap JJ, Covington HE., 3rd Social stress, therapeutics and drug abuse: preclinical models of escalated and depressed intake. Pharmacol. Therapeut. 2008;120:102–128. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2008.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wook Koo J, et al. Essential Role of Mesolimbic Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor in Chronic Social Stress-Induced Depressive Behaviors. Biol. Psychiat. 2016;80:469–478. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Orru A, et al. Contingent and non-contingent recreational-like exposure to ethanol alters BDNF expression and signaling in the cortico-accumbal network differently. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233:3149–3160. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4358-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Holly EN, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Increased mesocorticolimbic dopamine during acute and repeated social defeat stress: modulation by corticotropin releasing factor receptors in the ventral tegmental area. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:4469–4479. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4082-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ostroumov A, et al. Stress Increases Ethanol Self-Administration via a Shift toward Excitatory GABA Signaling in the Ventral Tegmental Area. Neuron. 2016;92:493–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Budygin EA, et al. Chronic ethanol exposure alters presynaptic dopamine function in the striatum of monkeys: a preliminary study. Synapse. 2003;50:266–268. doi: 10.1002/syn.10269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Budygin EA, et al. Effects of chronic alcohol exposure on dopamine uptake in rat nucleus accumbens and caudate putamen. Psychopharmacology. 2007;193:495–501. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0812-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Carroll MR, Rodd ZA, Murphy JM, Simon JR. Chronic ethanol consumption increases dopamine uptake in the nucleus accumbens of high alcohol drinking rats. Alcohol. 2006;40:103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2006.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Karkhanis AN, Rose JH, Huggins KN, Konstantopoulos JK, Jones SR. Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure reduces presynaptic dopamine neurotransmission in the mouse nucleus accumbens. Drug Alcohol Depen. 2015;150:24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yorgason JT, Espana RA, Konstantopoulos JK, Weiner JL, Jones SR. Enduring increases in anxiety-like behavior and rapid nucleus accumbens dopamine signaling in socially isolated rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2013;37:1022–1031. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Diana M, Pistis M, Carboni S, Gessa GL, Rossetti ZL. Profound decrement of mesolimbic dopaminergic neuronal activity during ethanol withdrawal syndrome in rats: electrophysiological and biochemical evidence. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1993;90:7966–7969. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.17.7966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Anstrom KK, Miczek KA, Budygin EA. Increased phasic dopamine signaling in the mesolimbic pathway during social defeat in rats. Neuroscience. 2009;161:3–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Yorgason JT, et al. Social isolation rearing increases dopamine uptake and psychostimulant potency in the striatum. Neuropharmacology. 2016;101:471–479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.10.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Donahue RJ, et al. Effects of acute and chronic social defeat stress are differentially mediated by the dynorphin/kappa-opioid receptor system. Behav. Pharmacol. 2015;26:654–663. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Werling LL, Frattali A, Portoghese PS, Takemori AE, Cox BM. Kappa receptor regulation of dopamine release from striatum and cortex of rats and guinea pigs. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1988;246:282–286. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ebner SR, Roitman MF, Potter DN, Rachlin AB, Chartoff EH. Depressive-like effects of the kappa opioid receptor agonist salvinorin A are associated with decreased phasic dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology. 2010;210:241–252. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1836-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lemos JC, et al. Severe stress switches CRF action in the nucleus accumbens from appetitive to aversive. Nature. 2012;490:402–406. doi: 10.1038/nature11436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lindholm S, Ploj K, Franck J, Nylander I. Repeated ethanol administration induces short- and long-term changes in enkephalin and dynorphin tissue concentrations in rat brain. Alcohol. 2000;22:165–171. doi: 10.1016/S0741-8329(00)00118-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Marinelli PW, Lam M, Bai L, Quirion R, Gianoulakis C. A microdialysis profile of dynorphin A(1-8) release in the rat nucleus accumbens following alcohol administration. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2006;30:982–990. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00112.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Karkhanis AN, Huggins KN, Rose JH, Jones SR. Switch from excitatory to inhibitory actions of ethanol on dopamine levels after chronic exposure: Role of kappa opioid receptors. Neuropharmacology. 2016;110:190–197. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.07.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rose JH, et al. Supersensitive Kappa Opioid Receptors Promotes Ethanol Withdrawal-Related Behaviors and Reduce Dopamine Signaling in the Nucleus Accumbens. Int. J. Neuropsychop. 2016;19:1–10. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyv127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Shnitko TA, Spear LP, Robinson DL. Adolescent binge-like alcohol alters sensitivity to acute alcohol effects on dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens of adult rats. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233:361–371. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4106-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Engleman EA, McBride WJ, Li TK, Lumeng L, Murphy JM. Ethanol drinking experience attenuates (−)sulpiride-induced increases in extracellular dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens of alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2003;27:424–431. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000056618.57931.A5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Thielen RJ, et al. Ethanol drinking and deprivation alter dopaminergic and serotonergic function in the nucleus accumbens of alcohol-preferring rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2004;309:216–225. doi: 10.1124/jpet.103.059790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Franklin KM, et al. A single, moderate ethanol exposure alters extracellular dopamine levels and dopamine d receptor function in the nucleus accumbens of wistar rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2009;33:1721–1730. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01009.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Budygin EA, Phillips PE, Wightman RM, Jones SR. Terminal effects of ethanol on dopamine dynamics in rat nucleus accumbens: an in vitro voltammetric study. Synapse. 2001;42:77–79. doi: 10.1002/syn.1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Budygin EA, Mathews TA, Lapa GB, Jones SR. Local effects of acute ethanol on dopamine neurotransmission in the ventral striatum in C57BL/6 mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2005;523:40–45. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Schilaty ND, et al. Acute ethanol inhibits dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens via alpha6 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2014;349:559–567. doi: 10.1124/jpet.113.211490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Yorgason JT, Ferris MJ, Steffensen SC, Jones SR. Frequency-dependent effects of ethanol on dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2014;38:438–447. doi: 10.1111/acer.12287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Brodie MS, Shefner SA, Dunwiddie TV. Ethanol increases the firing rate of dopamine neurons of the rat ventral tegmental area in vitro. Brain Res. 1990;508:65–69. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91118-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mrejeru A, Marti-Prats L, Avegno EM, Harrison NL, Sulzer D. A subset of ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons responds to acute ethanol. Neuroscience. 2015;290:649–658. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.12.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Imperato A, Di Chiara G. Preferential stimulation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats by ethanol. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1986;239:219–228. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tang A, George MA, Randall JA, Gonzales RA. Ethanol increases extracellular dopamine concentration in the ventral striatum in C57BL/6 mice. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2003;27:1083–1089. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000075825.14331.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cheer JF, et al. Phasic dopamine release evoked by abused substances requires cannabinoid receptor activation. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:791–795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4152-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Robinson DL, Howard EC, McConnell S, Gonzales RA, Wightman RM. Disparity between tonic and phasic ethanol-induced dopamine increases in the nucleus accumbens of rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2009;33:1187–1196. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00942.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Siciliano CA, et al. Chronic ethanol self-administration in macaques shifts dopamine feedback inhibition to predominantly D2 receptors in nucleus accumbens core. Drug Alcohol Depen. 2016;158:159–163. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.10.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Karkhanis AN, Locke JL, McCool BA, Weiner JL, Jones SR. Social isolation rearing increases nucleus accumbens dopamine and norepinephrine responses to acute ethanol in adulthood. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2014;38:2770–2779. doi: 10.1111/acer.12555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bass CE, et al. Optogenetic stimulation of VTA dopamine neurons reveals that tonic but not phasic patterns of dopamine transmission reduce ethanol self-administration. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2013;7:173. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Samson HH, Czachowski CL. Behavioral measures of alcohol self-administration and intake control: rodent models. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2003;54:107–143. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(03)54004-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Miczek KA. Intraspecies aggression in rats: effects of d-amphetamine and chlordiazepoxide. Psychopharmacologia. 1974;39:275–301. doi: 10.1007/BF00422968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Grant E, Mackintosh J. A comparison of the social postures of some common laboratory rodents. Behaviour. 1963;21:246–259. doi: 10.1163/156853963X00185. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kawagoe KT, Zimmerman JB, Wightman RM. Principles of voltammetry and microelectrode surface states. J. Neurosci. Meth. 1993;48:225–240. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(93)90094-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Phillips PE, et al. Presynaptic dopaminergic function is largely unaltered in mesolimbic and mesostriatal terminals of adult rats that were prenatally exposed to cocaine. Brain Res. 2003;961:63–72. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03840-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Jones SR, Mathews TA, Budygin EA. Effect of moderate ethanol dose on dopamine uptake in rat nucleus accumbens in vivo. Synapse. 2006;60:251–255. doi: 10.1002/syn.20294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Near JA, Bigelow JC, Wightman RM. Comparison of uptake of dopamine in rat striatal chopped tissue and synaptosomes. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1988;245:921–927. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES