Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2017 Sep 5;113(1):91–106. doi: 10.1111/add.13929

The relationship between gambling expenditure, socio‐demographics, health‐related correlates and gambling behaviour—a cross‐sectional population‐based survey in Finland

Sari Castrén 1,2,3,, Jukka Kontto 4, Hannu Alho 1,5, Anne H Salonen 1,2
PMCID: PMC5763410  PMID: 28667828

Abstract

Aims

To investigate gambling expenditure and its relationship with socio‐demographics, health‐related correlates and past‐year gambling behaviour.

Design

Cross‐sectional population survey.

Setting

Population‐based survey in Finland.

Participants

Finnish people aged 15–74 years drawn randomly from the Population Information System. The participants in this study were past‐year gamblers with gambling expenditure data available (n = 3251, 1418 women and 1833 men).

Measurements

Expenditure shares, means of weekly gambling expenditure (WGE, €) and monthly gambling expenditure as a percentage of net income (MGE/NI, %) were calculated. The correlates used were perceived health, smoking, mental health [Mental Health Inventory (MHI)‐5], alcohol use [Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)‐C], game types, gambling frequency, gambling mode and gambling severity [South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS)].

Findings

Gender (men versus women) was found to be associated significantly with gambling expenditure, with exp(β) = 1.40, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.29, 1.52 and P < 0.005 for WGE, and exp(β) = 1.39, 95% CI = 1.27, 1.51 and P < 0.005 for MGE/NI. All gambling behaviour correlates were associated significantly with WGE and MGE/NI: gambling frequency (several times a week versus once a month/less than monthly, exp(β) = 30.75, 95% CI = 26.89, 35.17 and P < 0.005 for WGE, and exp(β) = 31.43, 95% CI = 27.41, 36.03 and P < 0.005 for MGE/NI), gambling severity (probable pathological gamblers versus non‐problem gamblers, exp(β) = 2.83, 95% CI = 2.12, 3.77 and P < 0.005 for WGE, and exp(β) = 2.67, 95% CI = 2.00, 3.57 and P < 0.005 for MGE/NI) and on‐line gambling (on‐line and land‐based versus land‐based only, exp(β) = 1.35, 95% CI = 1.24, 1.47 and P < 0.005 for WGE, and exp(β) = 1.35, 95% CI = 1.24, 1.47 and P < 0.005 for MGE/NI).

Conclusions

In Finland, male gender is associated significantly with both weekly gambling expenditure and monthly gambling expenditure related to net income. People in Finland with lower incomes contribute proportionally more of their income to gambling compared with middle‐ and high‐income groups.

Keywords: Alcohol use, gambling expenditure, health, mental health, population survey, problem gambling, socio‐demographics

Introduction

Gambling opportunities are expanding rapidly world‐wide, and gambling has become increasingly normalized with the continuing growth of advertising and development of new platforms 1, 2. At the same time, it is known that individuals, families and communities are affected by gambling‐related harms 3, 4. It is important in this situation to know how much of government gaming revenue is generated by certain groups of individuals. In particular, an ethically and socially responsible gambling policy requires a clear picture of the breakdown of expenditure by individuals in different age groups, from different socio‐economic backgrounds and particularly by individuals with gambling problems. Expenditure is interchangeable with gaming revenue or the gaming operator's gross profit: it is the amount of money that players spend or lose. The theory of total consumption implies that gambling expenditure is associated positively with gambling‐related harm, as pointed out in studies from Nordic countries 5, 6 and elsewhere 7, 8. From this it is possible to draw the further inference that problem gambling is a public health issue 5, 9.

Finland has one of the highest rates of gambling expenditure in the European Union (EU) 10. Most of the gambling revenue generated in the country goes to good purposes: profits and tax revenue are channelled via public and private organizations to support the arts and sciences, youth work, health and wellbeing, social and veteran programmes as well as horse breeding and designated research projects.

In Finland, the prevalence of past‐year problem gambling in 2015 was 3.3% while evaluated using the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS ≥ 3). From 2011 to 2015, this rate showed a tendency to increase among women, and at the same time attitudes towards gambling became more permissive 11.

Socio‐demographics

On average, women spend less on gambling than do men 12, 13. Overall, it seems that gambling expenditure tends to increase with age 14, 15, although it starts to drop after 44 years of age 16. Furthermore, lower educational level, blue‐collar status and unemployment are associated with a higher level of gambling participation and thus with higher gambling expenditure 14, 17. Giroux and colleagues 18 reported that problem gamblers have a lower annual income than non‐problem gamblers. Conversely, being married or cohabiting seems to have a protective effect against high gambling expenditure 14. Higher net incomes have been found to increase gambling expenditure 15, 19, but it has also been shown that lower income receivers gamble more 17, 20, 21, 22. Among the socio‐demographic factors reported to correlate with problem gambling are male gender, lower education, younger age, being single or divorced, being unemployed or laid off from work, sick leave, retirement on pension for health reasons and lower income 11, 17, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27. Some of these undesirable societal outcomes may be linked to income inequality, which may put individuals at risk of problem gambling if they decide try to get ahead in society by gambling. This may spill over into excessive expenditure on gambling and cause even more anxiety and stress among poorer individuals which, in turn, may increase gambling as a relief or escape, as discussed by Bol and colleagues 22.

Health‐related factors

Comorbidities of problem gambling, such as mental illness and substance abuse, are common in both men and women 28, 29. There is a scarcity of research on the association between gambling expenditure and perceived health and comorbid problems. Problematic gambling behaviour is known to have an impact upon health determinants that contribute to negative health outcomes 3. Furthermore, it is known that mental health and addiction problems and various associated harms tend to cluster in individuals who are already socio‐economically vulnerable 30, 31, 32, thus contributing to an accumulation of social inequality. It is therefore important to investigate how different health‐related factors are related to gambling expenditure.

Gambling behaviour

A high frequency of gambling, gambling several different games and problem gambling are correlated with higher overall gambling expenditure 33, 34, 35, 36. Overall expenditure in on‐line gambling is higher than expenditure in land‐based gambling 37, 38. Women spend less on on‐line gambling than men 13. High gambling expenditure is associated clearly with problem gambling 26, 34, 39, as is high gambling frequency and increased experienced harms 24, 40. Many aspects of women's and men's gambling behaviour appear to differ considerably 41. Two recently published Finnish register‐based studies also indicate that on‐line gambling expenditure differs significantly between Finnish men and women 42, 43. Edgren and colleagues 44 found that female on‐line gambling may be related to higher relative expenditure and at‐risk and problem gambling. It is possible that females experience a greater stigma attached to gambling than males 45, 46. There is as yet only limited research into gender‐specific gambling expenditure and its correlates. Therefore, in this study we have chosen to analyse men and women separately 47, 48, 49.

Excessive gambling has many potentially serious adverse effects, including financial, relationship, emotional and psychological, health, work/study, cultural, criminal activity and life‐course harms 3, 9, 50, 51. All these undesirable outcomes cause inequality in society. It is important, therefore, to create greater awareness of the associations of socio‐demographic and socio‐economic factors with excessive gambling. We hope that our analysis will contribute to these efforts and increase awareness among policymakers and gambling providers of gambling‐related problems, and ultimately provide them with tools for more effective and better‐targeted gambling‐related harm minimization, prevention and intervention strategies.

Our study explores gambling expenditure among Finnish women and men. This study aims to examine (1) the socio‐demographic correlates of gambling expenditure by gender; (2) the health correlates of gambling expenditure by gender; and (3) the gambling behaviour correlates of gambling expenditure by gender.

Methods

Design and participants

The data were drawn from the nationally representative cross‐sectional Finnish Gambling 2015 survey 4, 11. A total of 7400 Finns were selected randomly from the Population Information System. The inclusion criteria were: (1) age 15–74 years; (2) mother tongue Finnish or Swedish; and (3) residence in mainland Finland.

Data collection

The Finnish Gambling 2015 survey was designed by researchers from the National Institute for Health and Welfare. The data were collected by Statistics Finland between 3 March and 8 June 2015 by computer‐assisted telephone interview 4, 11. Potential participants were informed that the survey concerned their ‘opinions and attitudes towards gambling’. They received written information about the study, including the principles of confidentiality and voluntary participation. On average, each interview lasted 18 minutes.

In the gross sample, 103 people were non‐eligible 2, 9. A total of 4515 interviews were conducted, giving a response rate of 62% of eligible participants (n = 7297). Reasons for respondent attrition were established for 1594 people (22%): no telephone number (n = 1125) and failure to contact respondent (n = 469). Furthermore, 275 people were unwilling to respond to the interviewer's call, 896 declined to participate and 17 represented other net loss. The male response rate was somewhat higher than the female response rate in all age groups. Similarly, the older participants' response rate was higher than that from younger participants. People living in rural areas participated more actively than those living in urban and densely populated areas. In order to count as a past‐year gambler (n = 3617) and to be included into the study, respondents were to have gambled on at least one type of game during the past year.

Measures

Gambling expenditure

Gambling expenditure (GE) was measured with only one question inquiring the overall expenditure: ‘Roughly how much money do you spend on gambling in a typical week (€)?’. In cases where the respondent did not gamble weekly, the interviewer was instructed to encourage the respondent to give an estimate of their spending when they did gamble. Net income (NI) was assessed with the question: ‘What is your monthly income after tax (i.e. disposable income). Please take into account all your sources of income (e.g. wages or salary, property income, pension and other social benefits)’. Past‐year weekly gambling expenditure (WGE), yearly gambling expenditure (YGE) and monthly gambling expenditure as a percentage of net income (MGE/NI) were derived based on past‐year (including 365.25 days) gambling frequency (F), as follows: (1) WGE, if past‐year gambling frequency was at least once a week, WGE = GE. If past‐year gambling frequency was less than once a week, WGE = F × GE/365.25 × 7, where (a) F = 30, if past‐year gambling frequency was two to three times a month, (b) F = 12, if past‐year gambling frequency was once a month and (c) F = 6, if past‐year gambling frequency was less than monthly; (2) YGE = WGE/7 × 365.25; and (3) MGE/NI = 100 × YGE/12 × NI, when NI > 0.

The highest WGE in the data set (€50 000) was replaced by the second highest WGE (€1500): it was more than 10 times the corresponding reported weekly net income and considered to be an outlier. Gambling expenditure was examined from three perspectives: expenditure share (in %), WGE (in €) and MGE/NI (in %).

Socio‐demographic variables

Socio‐demographic variables including age, marital status and education were derived from the population register, while data on employment status and net income were obtained from the participants (Table 1).

Table 1.

Relationship between gambling expenditure and socio‐demographics by gender.

Females Males
n P% Expenditure share (CI) Mean WGE (CI) Mean MGE/NI (CI) n P% Expenditure share (CI) Mean WGE (CI) Mean MGE/NI (CI)
Age (years)
15–17 16 1 0.3 (0.2, 0.4) 1.06 (0.48, 1.76) 0.98 (0.27, 1.88) 33 2 0.3 (0.3, 0.4) 2.29 (1.07, 4.16) 17.32 (4.63, 34.70)
18–24 95 9 4.4 (2.9, 5.9) 2.47 (1.59, 3.65) 1.60 (1.01, 2.47) 177 12 7.3 (6.1, 8.8) 7.94 (5.77, 10.41) 5.54 (3.68, 7.68)
25–34 204 15 7.5 (5.0, 10.1) 2.60 (1.97, 3.35) 0.93 (0.71, 1.17) 255 17 26.9 (22.3, 32.3) 19.88 (10.00, 34.58) 4.41 (2.33, 7.27)
35–44 191 16 12.1 (8.0, 16.3) 4.14 (3.00, 5.38) 1.47 (0.89, 2.29) 314 17 12.5 (10.4, 15.0) 9.35 (7.03, 12.04) 1.95 (1.42, 2.53)
45–54 243 17 12.6 (8.3, 16.9) 3.85 (3.23, 4.55) 1.01 (0.82, 1.24) 344 18 16.1 (13.3, 19.3) 11.36 (9.17, 14.10) 2.53 (2.01, 3.20)
55–64 357 23 43.8 (28.9, 58.8) 10.18 (4.76, 20.66) 2.66 (1.42, 4.86) 387 19 22.6 (18.7, 27.1) 14.77 (12.70, 17.01) 3.44 (2.89, 4.10)
65–74 312 18 19.3 (12.7, 25.9) 5.84 (5.04, 6.72) 2.08 (1.71, 2.46) 323 14 14.2 (11.8, 17.1) 12.59 (10.55, 15.01) 3.07 (2.62, 3.59)
Marital status
Single/not in a
registered relationship 434 35 43.6 (28.8, 58.5) 6.75 (3.15, 15.05) 2.02 (1.15, 3.56) 696 42 51.0 (42.2, 61.2) 15.57 (10.82, 21.23) 4.88 (3.74, 6.31)
Married 703 47 40.6 (26.8, 54.5) 4.58 (4.07, 5.13) 1.55 (1.27, 1.89) 934 48 38.0 (31.4, 45.5) 10.07 (8.92, 11.41) 2.26 (1.91, 2.67)
Separated/divorced 198 13 9.7 (6.4, 13.0) 3.94 (3.28, 4.67) 1.15 (0.94, 1.40) 187 9 10.3 (8.6, 12.4) 14.02 (10.98, 17.42) 3.33 (2.59, 4.26)
Widow 83 5 6.1 (4.0, 8.2) 6.56 (4.22, 9.98) 2.21 (1.30, 3.46) 16 1 0.7 (0.6, 0.8) 11.67 (6.81, 17.09) 3.19 (1.65, 5.05)
Education
Master's or equivalent 160 12 4.0 (2.6, 5.3) 1.82 (1.38, 2.34) 0.39 (0.28, 0.50) 178 10 4.6 (3.8, 5.5) 5.99 (3.25, 9.55) 1.04 (0.48, 1.93)
Bachelor's or equivalent 253 19 11.0 (7.3, 14.8) 3.17 (2.57, 3.83) 0.84 (0.65, 1.04) 206 12 7.6 (6.3, 9.2) 8.32 (5.93, 11.76) 1.79 (1.22, 2.54)
Short cycle tertiary education 263 18 37.4 (24.7, 50.2) 11.14 (4.36, 23.99) 2.92 (1.24, 6.26) 273 14 21.8 (18.0, 26.1) 19.42 (10.62, 34.61) 3.02 (2.14, 4.21)
Upper secondary 86 7 3.1 (2.1, 4.2) 2.34 (1.45, 3.62) 1.06 (0.60, 1.77) 158 10 10.9 (9.0, 13.1) 13.93 (7.38, 21.77) 3.92 (2.51, 5.55)
Basic vocational qualification 425 30 28.1 (18.5, 37.7) 5.03 (4.39, 5.79) 1.71 (1.46, 1.99) 679 37 38.9 (32.2, 46.7) 13.52 (11.22, 16.46) 4.09 (2.98, 5.57)
Up to lower secondary education 225 14 16.0 (10.5, 21.4) 5.90 (4.62, 7.37) 2.68 (2.04, 3.37) 329 17 14.9 (12.4, 17.9) 11.26 (9.40, 13.29) 4.84 (3.86, 6.14)
Other or missing 6 0 0.4 (0.3, 0.6) 5.38 (2.00, 9.67) 1.46 (0.34, 3.56) 10 1 1.2 (1.0, 1.4) 29.21 (11.27, 50.84) 3.25 (0.40, 6.76)
Employment status
Working 773 57 64.4 (42.5, 86.3) 6.01 (3.75, 11.07) 1.52 (0.97, 2.44) 1096 62 63.5 (52.5, 76.1) 13.10 (10.26, 16.68) 2.10 (1.84, 2.39)
Unemployed or laid off 74 5 5.7 (3.8, 7.6) 5.63 (3.49, 8.85) 2.57 (1.72, 3.75) 117 7 10.6 (8.8, 12.8) 20.53 (10.23, 35.81) 12.19 (6.47, 20.53)
Retired based on age or service years 362 21 22.6 (14.9, 30.3) 5.77 (5.05, 6.60) 2.00 (1.69, 2.33) 385 18 16.7 (13.9, 20.1) 12.07 (10.25, 14.18) 3.08 (2.65, 3.54)
Student 94 8 1.7 (1.1, 2.3) 1.14 (0.85, 1.47) 1.22 (0.61, 2.12) 142 9 4.2 (3.5, 5.0) 5.95 (3.77, 8.64) 7.93 (4.94, 11.74)
Retired based on illness/
chronic illness 56 4 3.6 (2.4, 4.8) 5.23 (3.69, 6.84) 1.94 (1.32, 2.68) 67 3 3.9 (3.2, 4.6) 14.58 (10.17, 20.37) 5.45 (3.52, 7.48)
Homemaker, carer 53 4 1.8 (1.2, 2.4) 2.31 (1.41, 3.32) 1.91 (1.09, 2.83) 1 0 0.0 (NA) 1.20 (NA) 0.21 (NA)
Other or missing 6 0 0.2 (0.1, 0.2) 2.25 (1.17, 3.44) 0.92 (0.40, 1.45) 25 1 1.1 (0.9, 1.3) 9.36 (2.59, 18.95) 2.69 (0.92, 4.72)
Net income
€0 23 2 0.5 (0.3, 0.7) 1.39 (0.81, 2.04) NA 58 3 0.9 (0.8, 1.1) 3.34 (1.59, 6.13) NA
≤ €500 64 5 1.8 (1.2, 2.4) 1.79 (1.22, 2.47) 3.38 (1.64, 6.04) 81 5 3.1 (2.6, 3.8) 7.86 (4.17, 12.36) 12,39 (7.66, 17.85)
€501–1000 202 14 12.0 (7.9, 16.1) 4.61 (3.71, 5.63) 2.35 (1.86, 2.87) 147 8 7.5 (6.2, 9.0) 11.86 (7.74, 17.72) 7.09 (4.14, 11.63)
€1001–1500 317 21 21.8 (14.4, 29.3) 5.54 (4.57, 6.71) 1.84 (1.52, 2.21) 208 11 10.1 (8.4, 12.1) 12.24 (9.85, 14.69) 4.03 (3.26, 4.89)
€1501–2000 411 29 26.9 (17.8, 36.1) 4.95 (4.04, 6.08) 1.19 (0.96, 1.48) 408 22 23.9 (19.8, 28.6) 13.67 (10.34, 17.58) 3.17 (2.42, 4.02)
€2001–2500 181 13 26.5 (17.5, 35.6) 10.82 (2.48, 33.48) 2.11 (0.47, 6.60) 334 19 18.1 (15.0, 21.7) 12.40 (9.85, 15.57) 2.28 (1.81, 2.86)
> €2500 142 10 6.8 (4.5, 9.1) 3.52 (2.56, 4.56) 0.49 (0.36, 0.65) 487 26 29.3 (24.3, 35.1) 14.23 (8.43, 23.49) 1.33 (1.04, 1.67)
Missing 78 5 3.6 (2.4, 4.8) 3.63 (2.61, 4.74) NA 110 6 7.1 (5.9, 8.5) 15.86 (9.53, 22.57) NA

P% = weighted population proportion; n = non‐weighted number of participants; CI = 95% confidence interval; WGE = past‐year weekly gambling expenditure (€); MGE/NI = monthly gambling expenditure as a percentage of net income; WGE data, n = 3251 non‐weighted (1418 females and 1833 males); MGE/NI data, n = 2982 non‐weighted (1317 females and 1665 males); weighted based on gender, age and region of residence; NA = not available. Estimate is shown in bold type if the corresponding estimate for the opposite gender is smaller and the CI of the difference of the estimates do not contain value 0.

Health‐related correlates

Perceived general health was assessed with the question: ‘How is your general health at present?’. Five response options were recoded into three groups: (1) good or rather good, (2) average and (3) poor or rather poor (Table 2). Mental health was assessed using the five‐item Mental Health Inventory 52. MHI‐5 measures nervousness, downheartedness and feeling sad, jollity, calmness and happiness using a Likert scale (range 1–6). Total MHI‐5 scores were calculated by summing up the scores for each item. Total scores (range 4–30) were rescaled to 0–100, with a score of 52 or less indicating clinically significant mental health problems 53. Cronbach's alpha for MHI‐5 was 0.786. Loneliness was measured with the question: ‘Do you feel lonely?’, with five response options collapsed into two categories: (1) never or very rarely and (2) sometimes, often or all the time.

Table 2.

Relationship between gambling expenditure and health‐related correlates and gambling behaviours by gender.

Females Males
n P% Expenditure share (CI) Mean WGE (CI) Mean MGE/NI (CI) n P% Expenditure Share (CI) Mean WGE (CI) Mean MGE/NI (CI)
Perceived general health
Good or rather good 1184 85 61.5 (40.5, 82.5) 3.87 (3.45, 4.29) 1.29 (1.11, 1.51) 1472 82 78.5 (65.0, 94.1) 12.27 (9.92, 15.30) 3.00 (2.53, 3.51)
Average 187 12 33.5 (22.1, 44.9) 14.87 (5.24, 32.75) 4.10 (1.85, 8.01) 278 14 13.8 (11.4, 16.5) 12.30 (10.42, 14.41) 3.64 (2.89, 4.55)
Poor or rather poor 45 3 5.0 (3.3, 6.7) 8.98 (4.35, 16.11) 3.40 (1.62, 6.06) 76 4 6.7 (5.5, 8.0) 22.63 (13.26, 36.30) 11.28 (4.80, 21.95)
Missing 2 0 0.1 (0.1, 0.1) 3.89 (0.11, 8.00) 1.76 (NA) 7 0 1.1 (0.9, 1.3) 36.36 (14.91, 58.78) 10.03 (NA)
Loneliness
Never, rarely 1108 78 62.7 (41.3, 84.1) 4.29 (3.81, 4.78) 1.48 (1.27, 1.72) 1525 83 82.8 (68.6, 99.3) 12.69 (10.31, 15.96) 3.14 (2.72, 3.63)
Sometimes, often, all the time 308 22 37.2 (24.5, 49.9) 9.13 (3.73, 19.33) 2.48 (1.24, 4.70) 300 16 16.2 (13.4, 19.4) 12.65 (9.59, 16.28) 4.80 (2.91, 7.62)
Missing 2 0 0.1 (0.1, 0.2) 3.44 (0.69, 6.00) 0.17 (NA) 8 0 1.0 (0.8, 1.2) 29.32 (9.93, 55.61) NA
Perceived mental healtha
No problems 1376 97 78.1 (51.5, 104.8) 4.30 (3.86, 4.78) 1.47 (1.28, 1.70) 1760 96 95.1 (78.7, 114.0) 12.62 (10.53, 15.46) 3.30 (2.82, 3.90)
Problems 35 3 21.7 (14.3, 29.1) 46.29 (3.43, 135.90) 9.75 (1.19, 27.25) 50 3 3.3 (2.7, 4.0) 15.05 (8.22, 23.38) 7.17 (3.37, 12.18)
Missing 7 1 0.3 (0.2, 0.3) 2.64 (0.85, 4.50) 0.35 (0.13, 0.66) 23 1 1.6 (1.3, 1.9) 18.33 (9.23, 29.59) 3.91 (1.78, 6.53)
Risky alcohol consumptionb
No 1239 86 68.2 (45.0, 91.5) 4.23 (3.82, 4.69) 1.45 (1.27, 1.66) 1282 69 66.0 (54.7, 79.2) 12.26 (9.46, 16.21) 3.03 (2.42, 3.89)
Yes 178 14 31.6 (20.9, 42.4) 12.30 (3.92, 28.45) 3.19 (1.18, 7.66) 540 31 32.9 (27.3, 39.5) 13.64 (11.53, 16.02) 4.29 (3.43, 5.16)
Missing 1 0 0.1 (NA) 6.00 (NA) NA 11 1 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) 23.33 (7.71, 41.69) NA
Daily smoking
No 1181 83 61.9 (40.8, 83.0) 3.98 (3.50, 4.49) 1.33 (1.13, 1.56) 1411 77 69.3 (57.4, 83.1) 11.52 (9.07, 15.01) 2.82 (2.36, 3.36)
Yes 236 17 38.0 (25.1, 51.0) 11.97 (5.31, 24.56) 3.40 (1.82, 6.32) 415 23 29.7 (24.6, 35.6) 16.52 (13.42, 19.97) 5.43 (3.91, 7.55)
Missing 1 0 0.1 (NA) 6.0 (NA) NA 7 0 1.0 (0.8, 1.2) 34.73 (13.45, 61.20) NA
Past‐year gambling frequency
Once a month / less than monthly 620 46 5.2 (3.4, 7.0) 0.61 (0.54, 0.68) 0.22 (0.19, 0.27) 504 28 2.0 (1.7, 2.4) 0.91 (0.79, 1.02) 0.27 (0.22, 0.35)
2–3 times a month 190 14 8.1 (5.3, 10.9) 3.19 (2.68, 3.79) 1.01 (0.83, 1.22) 334 19 5.5 (4.6, 6.6) 3.67 (3.35, 4.00) 1.00 (0.87, 1.15)
Once a week 487 33 42.1 (27.8, 56.5) 6.91 (6.36, 7.49) 2.33 (1.98, 2.79) 699 37 36.4 (30.2, 43.7) 12.57 (11.36, 14.02) 3.62 (2.98, 4.34)
Several times a week 121 8 44.6 (29.4, 59.8) 29.35 (13.96, 56.93) 8.33 (4.78, 14.80) 296 16 56.1 (46.4, 67.2) 45.51 (33.74, 61.61) 11.27 (8.85, 14.36)
Game types gambled, past year
1 game type 413 28 12.1 (8.0, 16.2) 2.29 (1.96, 2.66) 0.71 (0.60, 0.83) 441 23 10.3 (8.5, 12.4) 5.80 (4.26, 8.29) 2.04 (1.03, 3.85)
2 game types 473 33 20.8 (13.7, 28.0) 3.39 (2.97, 3.86) 1.23 (0.94, 1.63) 384 20 9.3 (7.7, 11.1) 5.77 (5.00, 6.74) 2.09 (1.30, 3.27)
3 game types 260 19 16.3 (10.7, 21.8) 4.68 (3.87, 5.60) 1.45 (1.15, 1.80) 324 17 13.7 (11.3, 16.4) 10.00 (8.05, 12.28) 2.87 (2.28, 3.57)
4–5 game types 218 16 39.8 (26.3, 53.4) 13.12 (5.67, 29.87) 3.68 (1.89, 7.24) 410 23 24.5 (20.3, 29.4) 13.62 (11.69, 15.60) 3.59 (2.95, 4.37)
≥ 6 game types 54 4 11.0 (7.3, 14.8) 14.11 (9.43, 20.50) 4.89 (3.36, 6.63) 274 16 42.2 (34.9, 50.6) 32.69 (21.74, 49.19) 7.15 (5.55, 9.00)
Gambling mode, past year
Land‐based only 1066 74 67.2 (44.3, 90.1) 4.85 (3.20, 7.91) 1.50 (1.05, 2.20) 1181 63 42.3 (35.0, 50.7) 8.62 (7.59, 9.68) 2.48 (2.12, 2.95)
On‐line and land‐based 352 26 32.8 (21.6, 44.0) 6.72 (5.48, 8.11) 2.22 (1.81, 2.70) 652 37 57.7 (47.8, 69.2) 19.67 (14.65, 26.48) 4.90 (3.77, 6.25)
Past‐year gambling severity
SOGS = 0 1142 80 51.2 (33.8, 68.7) 3.43 (3.12, 3.77) 1.19 (1.03, 1.40) 1345 72 44.8 (37.1, 53.7) 7.91 (6.95, 9.13) 2.06 (1.80, 2.37)
SOGS = 1 167 12 13.6 (8.9, 18.2) 5 .99 (4.58, 7.71) 1.89 (1.45, 2.44) 312 18 26.5 (21.9, 31.8) 19.04 (11.54, 33.16) 3.78 (2.98, 4.63)
SOGS = 2 62 5 6.8 (4.5, 9.1) 7.59 (4.96, 10.51) 2.73 (1.68, 3.99) 86 5 8.0 (6.6, 9.6) 20.70 (15.04, 26.64) 9.50 (5.20, 14.99)
SOGS = 3–4 27 2 5.4 (3.5, 7.2) 15.00 (6.48, 27.73) 4.52 (2.16, 8.01) 56 3 11.6 (9.6, 13.8) 46.22 (24.84, 76.71) 15.53 (6.16, 30.38)
SOGS ≥ 5 20 1 23.1 (15.3, 31.0) 82.40 (8.18, 234.38) 19.56 (3.21, 51.32) 33 2 9.2 (7.6, 11.0) 61.06 (40.82, 85.02) 15.91 (9.91, 23.37)
Missing 0 NA NA NA NA 1 0 0.0 (NA) 1.15 (NA) 0.95 (NA)

P% = weighted population proportion; n = non‐weighted number of participants; CI = 95% confidence interval; WGE = past‐year weekly gambling expenditure (€); MGE/NI = monthly gambling expenditure as a percentage of net income.

a

MHI‐5 = Mental Health Inventory, scaled 0–100, clinically significant problem ≤52;

b

Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT‐C), score for risk consumption ≥ 5 among females and ≥ 6 among males; WGE data, n = 3251 non‐weighted (1418 females and 1833 males); MGE/NI data, n = 2982 non‐weighted (1317 females and 1665 males); weighted based on gender, age and region of residence; SOGS = South Oaks Gambling Screen; NA = not available. Estimate is shown in bold type if the corresponding estimate for the opposite gender is smaller and the CI of the difference of the estimates do not contain value 0.

Smoking was determined with the question: ‘Have you smoked cigarettes, a pipe or cigars during the past 12 months?’. The three response options were dichotomized in (1) daily smoking and (2) occasionally or not at all. Alcohol consumption was measured using a three‐item version of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT‐C) 54. The total AUDIT‐C score was counted by summing the points (range 0–3) for each item and using the cut‐off points to define risky drinking among males (≥ 6 points) and females (≥ 5 points) 55. Cronbach's alpha for AUDIT‐C was 0.607.

Past‐year gambling behaviour

Past‐year gambling was defined using a list of 18 game types, including games offered by the Finnish gambling monopoly, gambling with friends, gambling on cruises to Sweden, Estonia and the Åland Islands and non‐monopoly on‐line gambling overseas. Frequency of gambling was classified as no gambling, less than monthly, once a month, two to three times a month, once a week, several times a week and daily. Gambling mode was a dichotomous variable (land‐based only/on‐line).

Gambling severity was measured using the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS 56). A recent problem was defined as one occurring within the past 12 months. Total SOGS scores (range 0–20) were categorized as follows: (1) non‐gamblers, (2) SOGS = 0 (non‐problem gamblers), (3) SOGS = 1, (4) SOGS = 2, (5) SOGS = 3–4 (problem gamblers) or SOGS ≥ 5 (probable pathological gamblers). Cronbach's alpha for SOGS was 0.857.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed for past‐year gamblers based on the expenditure data available. Respondents providing no net income information or reporting zero euros were excluded from the MGE/NI analysis. The data were weighted based on gender, age and region of residence.

Ninety‐five per cent confidence intervals (CI) of total gambling expenditures, expenditure shares, means and the differences of gender‐specific estimates were calculated using an ordinary, non‐parametric bootstrap with 1000 replicates using the percentile method 57. A difference was considered statistically significant if the 95% CI did not contain the value 0.

Total gambling expenditures in Finland in 2015 was calculated using the information that weekly gambling expenditure was available for 84% of women and 94% of men. Otherwise it was assumed that the gambling expenditure was similar between those who reported weekly gambling expenditure and those who had no weekly gambling expenditure available.

Multiple log‐linear regression was used to identify the association between gambling expenditure and the correlates. Two separate models were constructed with WGE and MGE/NI as response variables. All correlates were used as predictor variables in the models, except for loneliness and past‐year game types gambled, which were removed from the models to avoid multi‐collinearity. All respondents with at least one correlate subcategory of ‘missing’ or ‘other’ were removed from the model. Exponentiations of beta coefficients [exp(β)], along with 95% CIs for each correlate subcategory, were reported, and exp(β) was interpreted as a percentage difference between the mean of a subcategory and the mean of a reference category. P‐values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant. The likelihood ratio test was used to assess effect modifications between gender and the correlates. All analyses were performed using R 58, and bootstrapping was conducted using the package ‘boot’ 59.

Results

Our sample comprised 1418 female and 1833 male gamblers with gambling expenditure available (n = 3251) aged 15–74 years. The mean age of women was 47.56 years, 95% CI = 46.70, 48.41 and the mean age of men was 45.10 years, 95% CI = 44.35, 45.81. Women's monthly net income (mean = €1697, 95% CI = 1646, 1751) was lower than men's (mean = €2219, 95% CI = 2120, 2324), representing a difference of 30.7%.

Gambling expenditure

Our estimate of total gambling expenditure in Finland in 2015 for women was €394 million, CI = 293, 565, and for men €1062 million, CI = 891, 1285, totalling €1456 million, CI = 1245, 1733. Women reported lower weekly gambling expenditure (mean = €5.34, 95% CI = 3.99, 7.63) than men (mean = €12.75, CI = 10.75, 15.68). On average, women spent 1.69%, 95% CI = 1.33, 2.34 and men 3.42%, 95% CI = 2.90, 3.98 of their net income on gambling. The mean annual spending for women was €278.70, 95% CI = 208.89, 397.65, and for men €665.37, CI = 558.61, 803.11).

Socio‐demographics

Women aged 55–74 years accounted for 63.1% of women's total WGE. The corresponding figure for men aged 55–74 years was 36.8% (Table 1). Women aged 25–34 years accounted for 7.5% of women's total WGE, while the corresponding figure for men was 26.9%. Mean WGE was significantly higher among men than women in all but two age groups, i.e. 15–17 and 55–64 years. Furthermore, women aged 55–74 years had a higher mean MGE/NI than other female age groups. Among men, the highest mean MGE/NI was found for the age group 15–34 years.

Widowed women accounted for 6.1% of women's total WGE, while widowed men accounted for 0.7% of men's total WGE. Mean WGE was significantly higher among married and divorced/separated men than women with the same marital status, and men also had a significantly higher mean MGE/NI than women in all subgroups except for widows/widowers.

Women with short cycle tertiary education accounted for 37.4% of women's total WGE, while the corresponding proportion for men was 21.8%. Men had a significantly higher mean WGE and mean MGE/NI than women in all groups, except for short cycle tertiary education.

Unemployed or laid‐off men and male students accounted for significantly larger proportions of men's total WGE than the corresponding employment status groups of women. Mean WGE and mean MGE/NI were significantly higher among men than women in all but one group, i.e. for mean MGE/NI among employed respondents.

Women earning more than €2500/month accounted for 6.8% of women's total WGE. The corresponding figure for men was 29.3%. Furthermore, mean WGE was significantly higher among men than women in all but one income group, i.e. €2001–2500/month. However, mean MGE/NI was highest in the two lowest net income categories among both men and women.

Health‐related factors

Women with average perceived health accounted for a significantly larger proportion of women's total WGE than the corresponding proportion among men (Table 2). Also, men had a significantly higher mean MGE and mean MGE/NI than women in the groups reporting good or rather good and poor or rather poor perceived general health; 21.7% of women's total WGE came from gamblers with mental health problems and 37.2% from gamblers who considered themselves lonely at least sometimes. The corresponding figures for men were 3.3 and 16.2%. Mean WGE and mean MGE/NI were significantly higher among men than women with no mental health problems, who reported no experiences of loneliness, who did not have risky alcohol consumption and who did not smoke daily.

Past‐year gambling behaviour

Mean WGE was significantly higher among men gambling once a month or less and men gambling once a week compared with women with the same gambling frequencies. By type of gambling, women who gambled four to five different types of games accounted for the largest proportion (39.8%) of women's total WGE. Among men, those who gambled more than five games accounted for the largest proportion (42.2%) of men's total WGE. Furthermore, mean WGE was significantly higher among men who gambled one to three or more than five game types than among women with similar gambling patterns. Again, mean WGE and mean MGE/NI were significantly higher among men than women in both gambling mode groups, i.e. land‐based only and on‐line and land‐based. Women who only gambled land‐based accounted for 67.2% of women's total WGE, while men who gambled on‐line and land‐based accounted for 57.7% of men's total WGE.

Women who were probable pathological gamblers (PGs; i.e. SOGS ≥ 5 points) accounted for 23.1% of women's total WGE, while male PGs accounted for 9.2%. Men scoring one SOGS point and men identified as problem gamblers (i.e. SOGS 3–4 points) accounted for a significantly larger share of men's total WGE than women with the same status. Nevertheless, mean WGE and mean MGE/NI were significantly higher among men than women at all levels of gambling severity, excluding probable pathological gamblers.

Multiple log‐linear regression

Socio‐demographic correlates associated significantly with WGE were gender (male versus female), age (45–74 years versus 18–24 years), marital status (single versus married), education (all education categories versus master's or equivalent), employment status (student versus working) and net income (> €2500/month versus ≤ €500/month) (Table 3). Perceived general health (poor or rather poor versus good or rather good), risky alcohol consumption and daily smoking were significant health‐related correlates, while all correlates measuring gambling behaviour were also associated significantly with WGE. The combined effect of socio‐economic status, health‐related correlates and gambling behaviour explained 69% of the variation in WGE. The likelihood ratio test showed significant interactions between gender and employment status, net income, gambling frequency and gambling severity.

Table 3.

Combined effect of gambling expenditure and the correlates.

WGE MGE/NI
n exp(β) (CI) P‐value n exp(β) (CI) P‐value
Gender
Female 1323 1301
Male 1690 1.40 (1.29, 1.52) < 0.005 1636 1.39 (1.27, 1.51) < 0.005
Age (years)
15–17 45 0.90 (0.63, 1.29) 0.58 13 1.98 (1.12, 3.49) 0.02
18–24 248 211
25–34 447 1.06 (0.90, 1.26) 0.47 445 1.01 (0.85, 1.20) 0.90
35–44 483 1.18 (0.98, 1.42) 0.08 483 1.12 (0.92, 1.35) 0.25
45–54 544 1.25 (1.03, 1.50) 0.02 541 1.13 (0.93, 1.37) 0.21
55–64 677 1.39 (1.14, 1.68) < 0.005 676 1.25 (1.02, 1.52) 0.03
65–74 569 1.53 (1.18, 1.99) < 0.005 568 1.35 (1.04, 1.77) 0.03
Marital status
Married 1514 1510
Single/not in a registered relationship 1057 1.11 (1.01, 1.22) 0.04 986 1.11 (1.01, 1.23) 0.03
Separated/divorced 352 0.98 (0.86, 1.11) 0.75 351 0.97 (0.86, 1.11) 0.69
Widow 90 0.95 (0.75, 1.21) 0.69 90 0.94 (0.73, 1.20) 0.60
Education
Master's or equivalent 320 320
Bachelor's or equivalent 442 1.37 (1.18, 1.59) < 0.005 440 1.44 (1.24, 1.68) < 0.005
Short cycle tertiary education 500 1.41 (1.21, 1.64) < 0.005 499 1.46 (1.26, 1.71) < 0.005
Upper secondary 226 1.41 (1.17, 1.71) < 0.005 213 1.40 (1.16, 1.70) < 0.005
Basic vocational qualification 1030 1.52 (1.31, 1.75) < 0.005 1022 1.61 (1.40, 1.86) < 0.005
Up to lower secondary education 495 1.43 (1.21, 1.70) < 0.005 443 1.55 (1.30, 1.84) < 0.005
Employment status
Working 1765 1762
Unemployed or laid off 180 0.94 (0.79, 1.13) 0.54 171 1.03 (0.85, 1.24) 0.75
Retired based on age or service years 669 0.99 (0.82, 1.19) 0.92 668 1.06 (0.88, 1.28) 0.56
Student 227 0.74 (0.60, 0.91) < 0.005 165 0.87 (0.70, 1.09) 0.22
Retired based on illness/chronic illness 118 0.94 (0.75, 1.17) 0.57 118 1.01 (0.81, 1.27) 0.91
Homemaker, carer 54 1.03 (0.77, 1.38) 0.85 53 1.07 (0.79, 1.43) 0.67
Net income
€0 76 1.20 (0.89, 1.61) 0.24 0 NA NA
≤ €500 139 139
€501–1000 338 1.16 (0.93, 1.44) 0.18 338 0.46 (0.37, 0.57) < 0.005
€1001–1500 514 1.06 (0.84, 1.33) 0.63 514 0.27 (0.21, 0.34) < 0.005
€1501–2000 812 1.17 (0.92, 1.48) 0.20 812 0.22 (0.18, 0.29) < 0.005
€2001–2500 512 1.24 (0.96, 1.59) 0.09 512 0.19 (0.15, 0.24) < 0.005
> €2500 622 1.33 (1.03, 1.71) 0.03 622 0.14 (0.11, 0.18) < 0.005
Perceived general health
Good or rather good 2476 2404
Average 430 0.94 (0.84, 1.05) 0.27 427 0.94 (0.84, 1.06) 0.30
Poor or rather poor 107 1.31 (1.05, 1.64) 0.02 106 1.27 (1.01, 1.59) 0.04
Perceived mental healtha
No problems 2931 2855
Problems 82 0.82 (0.65, 1.03) 0.09 82 0.84 (0.66, 1.06) 0.14
Risky alcohol consumptionb
No 2347 2294
Yes 666 1.14 (1.04, 1.25) 0.01 643 1.12 (1.02, 1.23) 0.02
Daily smoking
No 2409 2345
Yes 604 1.12 (1.01, 1.23) 0.03 592 1.12 (1.01, 1.24) 0.03
Past‐year gambling frequency
Once a month / less than monthly 1046 1008
2–3 times a month 494 5.67 (5.08, 6.33) < 0.005 471 5.80 (5.17, 6.50) < 0.005
Once a week 1087 14.30 (13.02, 15.71) < 0.005 1077 14.74 (13.40, 16.22) < 0.005
Several times a week 386 30.75 (26.89, 35.17) < 0.005 381 31.43 (27.41, 36.03) < 0.005
Gambling mode, past year
Land‐based only 2061 2002
On‐line and land‐based 952 1.35 (1.24, 1.47) < 0.005 935 1.35 (1.24, 1.47) < 0.005
Past‐year gambling severity
SOGS = 0 2304 2255
SOGS = 1 449 1.20 (1.08, 1.34) < 0.005 431 1.21 (1.08, 1.34) < 0.005
SOGS = 2 134 1.61 (1.35, 1.92) < 0.005 126 1.58 (1.31, 1.90) < 0.005
SOGS = 3–4 76 1.91 (1.50, 2.42) < 0.005 75 1.75 (1.37, 2.23) < 0.005
SOGS ≥ 5 50 2.83 (2.12, 3.77) < 0.005 50 2.67 (2.00, 3.57) < 0.005
R 2 0.69 0.71

WGE = past‐year weekly gambling expenditure (€); MGE/NI = monthly gambling expenditure as a percentage of net income; n = non‐weighted number of participants; exp(β) = exponentiation of beta coefficient of multiple log‐linear regression model; CI = 95% confidence interval; † = reference category; R 2 = the coefficient of multiple determination.

a

MHI‐5 = Mental Health Inventory, scaled 0–100, clinically significant problem ≤ 52;

b

Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT‐C), score for risk consumption ≥ 5 among females and ≥ 6 among males; WGE data, n = 3013 non‐weighted; MGE/NI data, n = 2937 non‐weighted; weighted based on gender, age and region of residence; NA = not available. Estimate is shown in bold type if the corresponding P‐value is smaller than 0.05.

In the MGE/NI model, gender (male versus female), age (15–17 and 55–74 years versus 18–24 years), marital status (single versus married), education (all education categories versus master's or equivalent) and net income (all net income categories versus ≤ €500/month) were associated significantly with MGE/NI (Table 3). Health‐related correlates perceived general health (poor or rather poor versus good or rather good), risky alcohol consumption and daily smoking as well as all gambling behaviour correlates were also significant correlates in the MGE/NI model. The combined effect of all correlates in the model explained 71% of the variation in MGE/NI. There were no significant interactions between gender and the correlates.

Discussion

In 2015 men in Finland spent more on gambling than women. This finding is consistent with earlier results 12, 13, 26. Women aged 55–74 years accounted for 63.1% women's total WGE. The corresponding share for men was approximately one‐third. Men aged 25–34 accounted for one‐quarter of men's total WGE, compared with just 7.5% among women in this age bracket.

Overall, women's gambling and at‐risk and problem gambling (SOGS ≥ 1) have increased in Finland between 2011 and 2015 11. It is possible that women's on‐line gambling is related to higher relative expenditure 44. There is no evidence of an increase in women's gambling in the other Nordic countries 46, 60, 61, 62. Internationally, there is some evidence of a feminization of gambling: more women are gambling and showing an interest in on‐line gambling 43 developing gambling problems and seeking help for gambling‐related problems than before 41. In the Finnish context, the reasons for this particular fashion may lie in women's increasingly lenient attitudes towards gambling in certain age groups 63 and in the launch of on‐line games tailored specifically to females.

In our data set, problem and pathological gamblers accounted for 28.5% of women's total gambling expenditure. The corresponding proportion for men was 20.8%. These figures are in line with previous findings 31, 32, 64. From a public health viewpoint it is important that we continue to monitor trends in gambling prevalence, particularly with a view to identifying gender‐specific harms. Gambling expenditure is significant factor in moderate‐risk and problem gambling 36. Problem and pathological gamblers spend more money than they intend to, lose control over their gambling and take out loans to continue gambling despite the negative consequences 65, and often find themselves in a vicious circle of chasing losses 66, 67, 68, 69.

In our model, all gambling behaviours were associated significantly with MGE/NI, which again supports previous findings on gambling expenditure 4, 24, 33, 34, 35, 40, 70, 71. On‐line gambling accounted for almost 60% of men's and for one‐third of women's total WGE. It is possible that men's higher spending on gambling is explained by the larger amount they spend on‐line. The use of digital money (e.g. credit cards, electronic bank transfers and e‐wallets) seems to encourage intensified gambling behaviour and to lead to greater losses, predominantly in the case of problem gamblers, as gamblers seem to feel that they are not spending ‘real’ money 38. Men continue to outnumber women in on‐line gambling, which is associated furthermore with a higher income 72. Both non‐strategic games (slot machines, bingo, scratch cards) and strategic forms of gambling (blackjack, cards, sports betting, race wagering) are available on‐line 11, 73, 74. As men tend to prefer strategic games, which are often available 24/7 75, 76, they also spend more money on‐line than women.

Significant socio‐demographic correlates of MGE/NI among men were aged lower than 18 years and more than 55 years, single marital status, net monthly income of more than €500 and lower than master's education. Among women, the only significant socio‐demographic correlates were education and net income—despite the fact that women aged 55–74 accounted for a high proportion of total WGE. Both women's lower educational level 77, 78 and lower income level 41 can be seen as a sign of socio‐economic vulnerability for women. In our study, women's income was 30% lower than men's, a much higher figure than the national average of 20% 79. This experience of being socio‐economically disadvantageous may have become a trigger for gambling as a way to level up lower socio‐economic status. Increasing numbers of older adults are now gambling 80, which has been linked with life transitions such as retirement, lack of opportunities to socialize and loss of spouse 81. One particularly vulnerable group may be that of widowed women, as noted in our results. Loneliness, social isolation and disconnectedness may put women in a vulnerable position, as the need to take part in an acceptable activity such as gambling may lead to gambling‐related problems 41. In Finland, the transition into old‐age retirement takes place at age 63–68 years 82. It is assumed, therefore, that people in older age groups are relatively active and still in employment. These age groups may also have more time and money to spend on leisure activities such as gambling. Conversely, it is known that retired women are among the poorest 83, 84 (i.e. Sweden, Finland), thus the need for connectedness via gambling, that may sometimes develop somewhat quickly to problematic 85, placing them socio‐economically in an even weaker position.

Among men, we found that mean WGE increased with higher net income, lending support to the earlier finding that expenditure seems to intensify as a function of income 32. However, an increased income led to decreased MGE/NI. Lower education is known to be associated with problem gambling among both men and women 86, 87.

Women experiencing loneliness and mental health problems account for a larger proportion of women's total WGE than men. Our model showed that, for men, significant health‐related correlates were perceived health and mental health problems, while there were no significant health correlates for women. A few previous studies have found that poor health correlates with problem gambling 88, and perceived mental health problems, most notably depression and anxiety, are well documented among female problem gamblers 23, 28, 29, 88. We found no evidence of such a significant association.

All in all, the key finding of our study is that lower‐income individuals contribute proportionally more of their income to gambling compared to middle‐ and high‐income groups 17, 31, 32. It is noteworthy that people who are already in precarious social and financial situations tend to live in neighbourhoods with a high density of gambling opportunities 89, 90, 91. The associated risk of gambling‐related problems and experienced harms gives rise to inequality, as pointed out by Selin and colleagues 92. Based on an Australian study, it has also been reported that proportionally more gambling tax revenue is generated in socio‐economically disadvantaged areas 93. Therefore, more research is needed to explore regional differences and specific game types, particularly in disadvantaged areas, in order to prevent the accumulation of problems in certain areas.

Strengths and weaknesses

Our estimate of total gambling expenditure in Finland in 2015 at €1456 million is slightly lower than the figure reported by the Finnish monopoly operator. In 2013, the monopoly generated revenue of €1693 million 94. The coverage rate is 78% which is, in fact, an excellent figure by international standards, as gambling expenditure is typically much underestimated 95, 96, 97, or the amount of exact spending is difficult to recall when asked 95, 96, 97. The unusually high coverage rate is one of the strengths of our study, and a reflection of the clearly formulated question on expenditure 95. The use of a weekly time‐frame also has the advantage of reducing response variation and inaccuracy 98.

The response rate in our study (62%) was higher than the international average 99. The most under‐represented age group was 15–34 years and the most over‐represented age group 65–74 years 4, 11. Even though we had a very large overall sample size, the results must be interpreted with caution as the age groups are relatively small, which affects CIs particularly in the age group 15–17 years. As this study was based on self‐reported data, it is possible that the information collected has been subject to participant biases, especially as far as the expenditure data are concerned 96. Although the reported expenditure versus revenue coverage rate was excellent, indicating that the measure of expenditure was appropriate 95, the question did not specify whether it concerned net or gross. Our study did not allow us to explore household income, which has important implications for both the distribution of harms and public policy. Overall, some of the group differences observed were statistically significant, but the corresponding effect sizes implied that the magnitudes of these differences were not significant. The current study used previously validated instruments, including MHI‐5, AUDIT‐C and SOGS 53, 54, 55, 56.

Conclusions

This is one of the few studies 8, 100 to examine both gambling expenditure and its relationship to gamblers' net income (NI). Male gender was associated significantly with both WGE and MGE/NI. Overall, it seems that people with a higher mean gambling expenditure related to net income are lower‐income individuals. This may have undesirable societal outcomes and may engender many risk‐taking behaviours, such as problem and pathological gambling.

Our results indicate that harm prevention at both population and individual levels is important, and thus may reduce inequality. These efforts must include steps to more monitor closely the prevalence of gambling and related harms so that we can identify the occurrence of gambling harm in different population groups, as well as regional differences. In addition, there is a need for secondary prevention (brief and early interventions) and tertiary prevention (specialized interventions). Our findings provide useful guidance for public policies on gambling and debate on sources of funding for the public good.

Ethical approval

The Ethics Committee of the National Institute for Health and Welfare approved the research protocol (Statement: THL/1122/6.02.01/2014). The ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and its amendments or comparable ethical standards were followed.

Declaration of interests

The authors do not hold any position, receive ongoing or significant funding and are not engaged in any business or with any organization that creates a real or perceived conflict of interest in their work on this manuscript. For the last 3 years, senior researchers S.C., A.H.S. and statistician J.K. have worked full‐time at the National Institute for Health and Welfare. S.C. and A.H.S. as visiting researchers at the University of Helsinki, Finland and S.C. as an adjunct professor at the University of Turku, Finland. In addition, both S.C. and A.H.S. have received a travel grant from the Finnish Foundation for Gaming Research. For the last 3 years, H.A. has worked full‐time at the University of Helsinki and part‐time at the National Institute for Health and Welfare, Finland. H.A. and J.K. have not received any additional funding for gambling research.

Acknowledgements

The Ministry of Social Affairs and Health, Helsinki, Finland, funded the study (appropriation under section 52 of the Lotteries Act). However, it had no role in the study design, data analysis, data interpretation or in preparing the manuscript.

Castrén, S. , Kontto, J. , Alho, H. , and Salonen, A. H. (2018) The relationship between gambling expenditure, socio‐demographics, health‐related correlates and gambling behaviour—a cross‐sectional population‐based survey in Finland. Addiction, 113: 91–106. doi: 10.1111/add.13929.

References

  • 1. Derevensky J. L. Teen Gambling: Understanding a Growing Epidemic. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc.; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Abarbanel B., Gainsbury S. M., King D., Hing N., Delfabbro P. H. Gambling games on social platforms: how do advertisements for social casino games target young adults? Policy Int 2016; https://doi.org/10.1002/poi3.135/epdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Langham E., Thorne H., Browne M., Donaldson P., Rose J., Rockloff M. Understanding gambling related harm: a proposed definition, conceptual framework and taxonomy of harms. BMC Public Health 2016; 16: 80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Salonen A. H., Alho H., Castrén S. The extent and type of gambling harms for concerned significant others: a cross‐sectional population study in Finland. Scand J Public Health 2016; 44: 799–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Lund I. The population mean and the proportion of frequent gamblers: is the theory of total consumption valid for gambling? J Gambl Stud 2008; 24: 247–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Hansen M., Rossow I. Adolescent gambling and problem gambling: does the total consumption model apply? J Gambl Stud 2008; 24: 135–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Grun L., McKeigue P. Prevalence of excessive gambling before and after introduction of a national lottery in the United Kingdom: another example of the single distribution theory. Addiction 2000; 95: 959–966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Markham F., Young M., Doran B. Gambling expenditure predicts harm: evidence from a venue‐level study. Addiction 2015; 109: 1509–1516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Messerlian C., Derevensky J. L. Youth gambling: a public health perspective. J Gambl Issues 2005; https://doi.org/0.4309/jgi.2005.14.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Economist . The house wins. 2014. Available at: http://www.economist.com/blogs/graphicdetail/2014/02/daily‐chart‐0 (accessed 6 June 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX6dVCNK on 7 August 2017).
  • 11. Salonen A., Raisamo S. Suomalaisten rahapelaaminen 2015. Rahapelaaminen, rahapeliongelmat ja rahapelaamiseen liittyvät asenteet ja mielipiteet 15–74‐vuotiailla [Finnish gambling 2015. Gambling, gambling problems, and attitudes and opinions on gambling among Finns aged 15–74]. Report 16/2015. Helsinki National Institute for Health and Welfare (THL); 2015.
  • 12. Statens folkhälsoinstitut . Swedish longitudinal gambling study: en studie om spel och hälsa [Swedish longitudinal gambling study: a study of gambling and health]. Östersund: Statens folkhälsoinstitut; 2010. Available at: https://www.folkhalsomyndigheten.se/pagefiles/12668/R2011‐19‐Spel‐Kunskapsunderlag‐for‐folkhalsopolitisk‐rapport‐2010.pdf (accessed 8 May 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX78QtXU on 7 August 2017).
  • 13. McCormack A., Shorter G. W., Griffths M. D. An empirical study of gender differences in online gambling. J Gambl Stud 2014; 30: 71–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Worthington A.C., Brown K., Crawford M., Pickernell D. Socioeconomic and demographic determinants of household gambling in Australia. Discussion paper no. 156. School of Economics and Finance. Brisbane, Qld: Queensland University of Technology; 2003. Available at: http://eprints.qut.edu.au/329/1/Discussion_Paper_Worthington,_Brown,_Crawford,_Pickernell_‐_No_156.pdf (accessed 16 June 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX6Qco0V on 7 August 2017).
  • 15. Tan A. K. G., Yen S. T., Nayga R. M. Jr. Socio‐demographic determinants of gambling participation and expenditures: evidence from Malaysia. Int J Consum Stud 2010; 34: 316–325. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Mikesell J. L. Lottery expenditure in non‐lottery state. J Gambl Stud 1991; 7: 89–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Davidson T., Rodgers B., Markham F., Taylor‐Rodgers E. Gambling expenditure in the act of (2014): by level of problem gambling, type of activity, and socioeconomic and demographic characteristics. Final Report 2016. Available at: http://sociology.cass.anu.edu.au/sites/default/files/documents/2014_Gambling_Expenditure.pdf (accessed 1 February 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX6lSk0I 7 August 2017).
  • 18. Giroux I., Jacques C., Ladouceur R., Leclerc M., Brochu P. Prevalence des habitudes de jeu en Gaspésie et aux Isle‐de‐la‐Madeleine and 2009 [Prevalence of gambling habits in Gaspésie and Îles‐de‐la‐Madeleine in 2009]. Can J Psychiatry 2012; 57: 192–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. MacDonald M., McMullan J. L., Perrier D. C. Gambling households in Canada. J Gambl Stud 2004; 20: 187–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Breen H., Hing N., Weeks P. Machine gaming in Sydney clubs: characteristics of the supporting resident populations. J Gambl Stud 2002; 18: 293–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Beckert J., Lutter M. The inequality of fair play: lottery gambling and social stratification in Germany. Eur Sociol Rev 2009; 25: 475–488. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Bol T., Lancee B., Steijn S. Income inequality and gambling: a panel study in the United States (1980–1997). Sociol Spectr 2014; 1: 61–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Castrén S., Basnet S., Salonen A. H., Pankakoski M., Ronkainen J.‐E., Alho H. et al. Factors associated with disordered gambling in Finland. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy 2013; 8: 24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Heiskanen M., Toikka A. Clustering Finnish gambler profiles based on the money and time consumed in gambling activities. J Gamb Stud 2015; 32: 363–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Faregh N., Leth‐Steensen C. The gambling profiles of Canadians young and old: game preferences and play frequencies. Int Gamb Stud 2011; 11: 23–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Hing, N. , Russell A., Tolhard B., Nower L. A comparative study of men and women gamblers in Victoria. Victoria, Australia: Victorian Responsible Gambling Foundation 2014. Available at: https://www.responsiblegambling.vic.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0006/14289/A‐comparative‐study‐of‐men‐and‐women‐gamblers.pdf (accessed 6 July 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX7Lnu8m on 7 August 2017).
  • 27. Dowling N. A., Merkouris S. S., Greenwood C. J., Oldenhof E., Tornbourou J. W., Youssef G. J. Early risk and protective factors for problem gambling: A systematic review and meta‐analysis of longitudinal studies. Clin Psychol Rev 2017; 51: 109–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Lorains F. K., Colishaw S., Thomas S. H. Prevalence of comorbid disorders in problem and pathological gambling: systematic review and meta‐analysis of population surveys. Addiction 2011; 106: 490–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Abdollahnejad M., Delfabbro P., Denson L. The clustering of psychiatric disorders in high‐risk gambling populations. J Gambl Stud 2013; 30: 933–947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Reith G. Techno economic systems and excessive consumption: a political economy of ‘pathological’ gambling. Br J Sociol 2013; 64: 717–738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Williams R., Wood R. T. The proportion of gaming revenue derived from problem gamblers: examining the issues in Canadian context. Anal Soc Issues Publ Policy 2004; 4: 33–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Williams R.J., Rehm J., Stevens, R.M.G. The social and economic impacts of gambling. Final report prepared for the Canadian consortium for gambling research. March 11, 2011. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10133/1286 (accessed 15 May 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX7R3hy3 on 7 August 2017).
  • 33. Hing N., Lamont M., Vitartas P., Fink E. Sports bettors' responses to sports‐embedded gambling promotions: implications for compulsive consumption. J Bus Res 2015; 68: 2057–2066. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Quilty L. C., Murati D., Bagby R. M. Identifying indicators of harmful and problem gambling in a Canadian sample through receiver operating characteristic analysis. Psychol Additct Behav 2014; 28: 229–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Young M., Stevens M. Player preferences and social harm: an analysis of relationships between player characteristics, gambling modes, and problem gambling. Int J Ment Heal Addict 2009; 7: 262–279. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Currie S.R., Casey D.M., Hodgins D.C. Improving the psychometric properties of the problem gambling severity index. The Canadian Consortium for Gambling Research. Available at: http://www.ccgr.ca/wp-content/uploads/2013/03/Improving-the-Psycho 2010. (accessed 15 May 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX7Y4YOW on 7 August 2017).
  • 37. Wood R., Williams R. Internet gambling: prevalence, patterns, problems, and policy options. Guelph, Ontario: Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre; 2010. Available at: http://www.abc.net.au/mediawatch/transcripts/0909_originalreport.pdf (accessed 15 June 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX7hqVgJ on 7 August 2017).
  • 38. Gainsbury S. M. Online gambling addiction: the relationship between internet gambling and disordered gambling. Curr Addict Rep 2015; 2: 185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Shen Y., Kairouz S., Nadeau L., Robillard C. Comparing problem gamblers with moderate‐risk gambler in a sample of university students. J Behav Addict 2015; 4: 53–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Raisamo S., Mäkelä P., Salonen A. H., Lintonen T. The extent and distribution of gambling harm in Finland as assessed by the problem gambling severity index (PGSI). Eur J Public Health 2014; 25: 716–722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Holdsworth L., Hing N., Breen H. Exploring women's problem gambling: a review of the literature. Int Gambl Stud 2012; 12: 199–213. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Koivula A., Koiranen I., Räsänen P. Digitalisaatio ja verkkorahapelaamisen väestöryhmittäisetmuutokset 2006–2014 [English abstract: Digitalization and changes in online gambling behaviours in different population groups 2006–2014]. Yhteiskuntapolitiikka 2016; 4: 371–383. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Suhonen N., Kainulainen T. Ravivedonlyöjien verkkopelaaminen Suomessa. Erot kulutus‐ ja pelikäyttäytymisessä sukupuolen ja iän mukaan [English abstract: Online horse race betting in Finland. An empirical study on age and gender differences in consumer and betting behaviour]. Yhteiskuntapolitiikka 2016; 4: 395–406. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Edgren R., Castrén S., Alho H., Salonen A. H. Gender comparison of online and land‐based gamblers from a nationally representative sample: does gambling online pose elevated risk? Comput Hum Behav 2017; https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.033. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. McMillen J., Marshall D., Murphy L., Lorenzen, S. , Waugh B. Help‐seeking by problem gamblers, friends and families: a focus on gender and cultural groups. Canberra: Centre for Gambling Research, Australian National University Canberra; 2004. Available at: http://www.gamblingandracing.act.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0006/745062/Help‐Seeking‐by‐Problem‐Gamblers.pdf (accessed 2 February 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX7nD5Sf on 7 August 2017).
  • 46. Romild U., Svensson J., Volberg R. A gender perspective on gambling clusters in Sweden using longitudinal data. Nordic Stud Alcohol Drugs 2016; 33: 43–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Afifi T., LaPlante D., Tallieu T., Dowd D., Shaffer H. Gambling involvement: considering frequency of play and the moderating effects of gender and age. Int J Ment Heal Addict 2014; 12: 283–294. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Blanco C., Hasin D. S., Petry N., Stinson F. S., Grant B. F. Sex differences in subclinical and DSM‐IV pathological gambling: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on alcohol and related conditions. Psychol Med J Res Psychiatry Allied Sci 2006; 36: 943–953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Svensson J., Romild U. Problem gambling features and gendered gambling domains amongst regular gamblers in a Swedish population‐based study. Sex Roles 2014; 70: 240–254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Browne M., Langham E., Rawat V., Greer N., Li E., Rose J. et al Assessing gambling‐related harm in Victoria: a public health perspective. Victorian responsible gambling foundation, 2016. Melbourne. Available at: https://www.responsiblegambling.vic.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0007/28465/Browne_assessing_gambling‐related_harm_in_Vic_Apr_2016‐REPLACEMENT2.pdf (accessed 15 June 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX7rsgEq on 7 August 2017).
  • 51. Abbott M., Bellinger M., Garrett N., Mundy‐Mc‐Pherson S. New Zealand 2012 National Gambling Study: gambling harm and problem gambling. Ministry of Health, Final Report, July 2014. Available at: https://niphmhr.aut.ac.nz/research‐centres/gambling‐and‐addictions‐research‐centre/research‐reports (accessed 12 May 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX7vZkRY on 7 August 2017).
  • 52. Veit C. T., Ware J. E. The structure of psychological distress and well‐being in general populations. J Consult Clin Psychol 1983; 51: 730–734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Berwick D. M., Murphy J. M., Goldman P. A., Ware J. E. Jr., Barsky A. J., Weinstein M. C. Performance of a five‐item mental health screening test. Med Care 1991; 29: 169–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Bush K., Kivlahan D. R., McDonell M. B., Fihn S. D., Bradley K. A. The audit alcohol consumption questions (AUDIT‐C): an effective brief screening test for problem drinking. Arch Intern Med 1998; 158: 1789–1795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Seppä K. Potilaan haastatteleminen: Kolmen kysymyksen AUDIT‐C [Interviewing a patient: three‐question AUDIT‐C]. Suomen lääkäriseura [the Finnish Medical Society]. Duodecim 2010. Available at: http://www.kaypahoito.fi/web/kh/suositukset/naytaartikkeli/.../nak04611 (accessed 7 April 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX811gOm on 7 August 2017).
  • 56. Lesieur H. R., Blume S. B. The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): a new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. Am J Psychiatry 1987; 144: 1184–1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Davison A. C., Hinkley D. V. Bootstrap Methods and Their Applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 58. R Core Team R . A language and environment for statistical computing . Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. Available at: http://www.R‐project.org/ (accessed 16 June 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX85QwIZ on 7 August 2017). [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Canty A., Ripley B. Boot: bootstrap R (S‐plus) functions 2013. R package version 1.3–9. Available at: https://cran.r‐project.org/web/packages/boot/ (accessed 13 April 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8AYmKM on 7 August 2017).
  • 60. Brunborg G. S., Hanss D., Mentzoni R. A., Molde H., Pallesen S. Problem gambling and the five‐factor model of personality: a large population‐based study. Addiction 2016; 111: 1428–1435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Olasson D. T., Hayer T., Brosowski T., Meyer G. Gambling in the mist of economic crisis: results from three National Prevalence Studies from Iceland. J Gambl Stud 2015; 31: 759–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Ekholm O., Eiberg S., Davidsen M., Holst M., Larsen C. V., Juel K. The prevalence of problem gambling in Denmark in 2005 and 2010: a sociodemographic and socioeconomic characterization. J Gambl Stud 2014; 30: 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Salonen A. H., Alho H., Castrén S. Attitudes towards gambling, gambling participation, and gambling‐related harm: cross‐sectional Finnish population studies in 2011 and 2015. BMC Public Health 2017; 17: 122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Orford J., Wardle H., Griffiths M. What proportion of gambling is problem gambling? Estimates from the 2010 British gambling prevalence survey. Int Gambl Stud. 2013; 13: 4–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 65. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM‐5, 5th edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Brosowski T., Meyer G., Hayer T. Analyses of multiple types of online gambling within one provider: an extended evaluation framework of actual online gambling behaviour. Int Gambl Stud 2012; 12: 405–419. [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Currie S. R., Hodgins D. C., Wang J., El‐Guebaly N., Wynne H., Chen S. et al. Risk of harm from gambling in the general population as a function of level of participation in gambling activities. Addiction 2006; 101: 570–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Braverman J., LaPlante D. A., Nelson S. E., Shaffer H. J. Using cross‐game behavioral markers for early identification of high‐risk internet gamblers. Psychol Addict Behav 2013; 27: 868–877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Braverman J., Shaffer H. J. How do gamblers start gambling: identifying behavioural markers for high‐risk internet gambling. Eur J Public Health 2012; 22: 273–278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Wardle H., Sproston K., Orford J., Erens B., Griffiths M., Constantine R., Pigott S. British gambling prevalence survey 2007. Nat Cen; 2007. Available at: http://www.gamblingcommission.gov.uk/pdf/britsh%20gambling%20prevalence%20survey%202007%20‐%20sept%202007.pdf (accessed 15 August 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8HbsJk on 7 August 2017).
  • 71. LaPlante D. A., Nelson S. E., LaBrie R. A., Shaffer H. J. Disordered gambling, type of gambling and gambling involvement in the British gambling prevalence survey 2007. Eur J Public Health 2011; 21: 532–537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Gainsbury S. M., Russell A., Hing N., Wood R., Blaszczynski A. The impact of internet gambling on gambling problems: a comparison of moderate‐risk and problem internet and non‐internet gamblers. Psychol Addict Behav 2013; 27: 1092–1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Odlaug B. L., Chamberlain S. R., Kim S. W., Schreiber L. R., Grant J. E. A neurocognitive comparison of cognitive flexibility and response inhibition in gamblers with varying degrees of clinical severity. Psychol Med 2011; 41: 2111–2119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Stevens M., Young M. Independent correlates of reported gambling problems amongst indigenous Australians. Soc Indic Res 2010; 98: 147–166. [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Wood R., Williams R., Parke J. The relationship between internet gambling and problem gambling In: Williams R. J., Wood R. T., Parke J., editors. The Routledge International Handbook of Internet Gambling. London: Routledge; 2012, pp. 200–211. [Google Scholar]
  • 76. McCormack A., Griffiths, M . The effects of problem gambling on quality of life and wellbeing: a qualitative comparison of online and offline problem gamblers. Gambl Res 2011; 23: 63. [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Hing N., Russell A., Tolchard B., Nower L. A comparative study of men and women gamblers in Victoria. Victoria, Australia: Victorian Responsible Gambling Foundation; 2014. Available at: https://www.responsiblegambling.vic.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0006/14289/A‐comparative‐study‐of‐men‐and‐women‐gamblers.pdf (accessed 5 May 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8Lhd1G on 7 August 2017).
  • 78. Romild U., Svensson J., Volberg R. A gender perspective on gambling clusters in Sweden using longitudinal data. Nordic Stud Alcohol Drugs 2016; 4: 43–59. Available at: http://scholarworks.umass.edu/bioepi_faculty_pubs/4 (accessed 5 May 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8kVegU on 7 August 2017). [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Statistics Finland 2017. Available at: http://tilastokeskus.fi/tup/suoluk/suoluk_palkat.html. R. (accessed 3 February 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8Oo20Z on 7 August 2017).
  • 80. Subramaniam M., Wang P., Soh P., Vaingankar J., Chong S., Browning C. et al. Prevalence and determinants of gambling disorder among older adults: a systematic review. Addict Behav 2015; 41: 119–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. McNeilly D. P., Burke W. J. Late life gambling: the attitudes and behaviors of older adults. J Gambl Stud 2000; 16: 393–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Rapo M. Ikärakenne pakottaa keskustelemaan eläkeiästä [The changing age structure: implications for retirement age]. Published by Statistics Finland <Hyvinvointikatsauksessa 2/2014>. Available at: http://www.stat.fi/artikkelit/2014/art_2014‐05‐26_001.html (accessed 16 July 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8Sapqq on 7 August 2017).
  • 83. Organization for Economic Co‐operation and Development (OECD) 2011. Closing the gender gap. Sweden. Women's strong participation in paid work has not been enough to close gender gaps in other labour market outcomes. Available at: https://www.oecd.org/sweden/Closing%20the%20Gender%20Gap%20‐%20Sweden%20FINAL.pdf (accessed 8 May 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8WIcK8 on 7 August 2017).
  • 84. Ahonen K., Bach‐Othman J. Tracing old‐age poverty—the significance of the household structure on gender differences in the poverty rate in eight EU countries. Finnish Centre for Pensions Working Papers 2010:7:1–50. Available at: http://www.etk.fi/wp‐content/uploads/2015/10/wp%20710.pdf (accessed 12 May 2017) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8ZtwBp on 7 August 2017).
  • 85. Grant J. E., Odlaug B. L., Mooney M. E. Telescoping phenomenon in pathological gambling: Association with gender and comorbidities. J Nerv Ment Dis 2012; 200: 996–998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Castrén S., Basnet S., Pankakoski M., Ronkainen J.‐E., Helakorpi S., Uutela A. et al. An analysis of problem gambling among the Finnish working‐age population: a population survey. BMC Public Health 2013; 13: 519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Johansson A., Grant J. E., Kim S. W., Odlaug B. L., Gotestam K. G. Risk factors for problematic gambling: a critical literature review. J Gambl Stud 2009; 25: 67–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Afifi T. O., Brownridge D. A., MacMillan H., Sareen J. The relationship of gambling to intimate partner violence and child maltreatment in a nationally representative sample. J Psychiatr Res 2010; 44: 331–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Storer J., Abbott M., Stubbs J. Access or adaptation? A meta‐analysis of surveys of problem gambling prevalence in Australia and New Zealand with respect to concentration of electronic gaming machines. Int Gambl Stud 2009; 9: 225–244. [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Slutske W. S., Deauch A. R., Statham D. B., Martin N. G. Local area disadvantage and gambling involvement and disorder: evidence for gene–environment correlation and interaction. J Abnorm Psychol 2015; 124: 606–622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Young M., Markham F., Doran B. Too close home? The relationships between residential distance to venue and gambling outcomes. Int Gambl Stud 2012; 12: 257–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Selin J., Raisamo S., Murto A. Alueelliset erot subjektiivisesti koetussa rahapeliongelmassa [Regional differences in subjective perception of problem gambling]. Yhteiskuntapolitiikka 2016; 4: 429–436. [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Rintoul A., Livingstone C., Mellor A., Jolley D. Modelling vulnerability to gambling related harm: how disadvantage predicts gambling losses. Addict Res Theory 2013; 21: 329–338. [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Murto A., Mustalampi S. Rahapeliongelmien yhteiskunnallinen merkitys [Gambling problems in society] In: Alho H., Heinälä P., Kiianmaa K., Lahti T., Murto A., editors. Rahapeliriippuvuus [Gambling Addiction]. Helsinki: Duodecim; 2015, pp. 17–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Wood R. T., Williams R. J. How much money do you spend on gambling? The comparative validity of question wordings used to assess gambling expenditure. Int J Soc Res Methodol Theory Pract 2007; 10: 63–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Auer M., Griffiths M. D. Self‐reported losses versus actual losses in online gambling: an empirical study. J Gambl Stud 2016; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899‐016‐9648‐0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Braverman J., Tom M. A., Shaffer H. J. Accuracy of self‐reported versus actual online‐gambling wins and losses. Psychol Assess 2014; 26: 865–877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98. Blaszczynski A., Ladouceur R., Goulet A., Savard C. How much do you spend gambling? Ambiguities in questionnaire items assessing expenditure. Int Gambl Stud 2006; 6: 123–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 99. Williams R.J., Volberg R.A., Stevens R.M.G. The population prevalence of problem gambling: Methodological influences, standardized rates, jurisdictional differences, and worldwide trends. Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long Term Care. May 8, 2012. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10133/3068 (accessed 12 May 2016) (Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/6sX8e6Esb on 7 August 2017).
  • 100. Smith J. Gambling taxation: public equity in the gambling business. Aust Econ Rev 2000; 33: 120–144. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Addiction (Abingdon, England) are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES