Abstract
Memories of fearful events can be maintained throughout the lifetime of animals. Here we showed that lesions of the lateral nucleus (LA) performed shortly after training impaired the retention of long-term memories, assessed by the concomitant measurement of two dissociable defensive responses, freezing and avoidance in rats. Strikingly, when LA lesions were performed four weeks after training, rats did not show freezing to a learned threat stimulus, but they were able to direct their responses away from it. Similar results were found when the central nucleus (CeA) was lesioned four weeks after training, whereas lesions of the basal nucleus (BA) suppressed avoidance without affecting freezing. LA and BA receive parallel inputs from the auditory cortex, and optogenetic inhibition of these terminals hampered both freezing and avoidance. We therefore propose that, at variance with the traditional serial flow of information model, long-term fearful memories recruit two parallel circuits in the amygdala, one relying on the LA-to-CeA pathway and the other relying solely on BA, which operate independently and mediate distinct defensive responses.
Introduction
The association between a neutral sensory stimulus (the conditioned stimulus, CS, such as a tone) and a painful event (the unconditioned stimulus, US, for example a mild footshock) is rapidly formed and can be maintained throughout the lifetime of animals. The neural basis for the encoding of CS-US association is well studied. Information about the CS and US is thought to converge in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA), where neurons undergo learning-evoked changes. This information is then conveyed to the basal nucleus (BA) and the central nucleus (CeA), which coordinate the expression of defensive behaviors1–4. In particular, CeA outputs are crucial for the expression of passive autonomic responses to the CS (including freezing) while it is dispensable for the avoidance of threat stimuli5–8. Conversely, BA is not necessary for the expression of freezing5,9,10 but see11 while it plays a crucial role in the active responses, like “escape from fear” and avoidance5–8,12.
This model of serial information flow within the amygdala is based on anatomical tracing, lesion, pharmacological, electrophysiological and optogenetic studies performed during or shortly after CS-US association. On the other hand, it is less clear whether this serial flow of information processing is maintained in the course of consolidation and long-term maintenance of fearful memories. Several studies showed that the neural circuits underlying fearful memory maintenance gradually reorganize over time13–16. Based on evidence indicating that the retrieval of long-term fearful memories enhances LA activity17,18 but see15 and that combined lesions in both LA and BA impair fearful memories across the lifetime of animals19–21, it has been proposed that CS-US association is permanently stored in LA22. However, there have been no studies addressing the individual involvement of LA or BA nuclei in the long-term expression of fearful memories, and their individual contributions to this process remain poorly understood. In particular, it remains a matter of debate whether the LA persists in being a crucial locus of memory as time passes22–24.
Therefore, in the present study, we investigated the involvement of different amygdala nuclei during the formation versus the long-term expression of fearful memories assessed through the simultaneous examination of two dissociable defensive behaviors, freezing and avoidance responses.
Results
LA lesions differently affected the formation and the long-term expression of fearful memories
Because previous studies showed that different amygdala nuclei may be involved in the expression of different defensive responses5–8,12, we set-up a new behavioral paradigm which allows the concomitant measurement of two dissociable defensive responses, freezing and avoidance. Rats were trained to associate a tone (CS) with a footshock (US) in a standard conditioning chamber. One month later, the CSs were presented within a new apparatus divided into six subzones (see Methods) (Fig. 1a). The CS was delivered in zone 6. In this situation, the rats directed their actions to actively avoid the CS by running away from zone 6 towards the zone farthest from the CS (zone 1). Compared with naive (n = 13) rats, animals that underwent fear conditioning (n = 15) spent more time in zone 1 and less time in zone 6 (Fig. 1b). A 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA showed a significant main effect of the zone (F(1,26) = 8.31, P = 0.008). Student’s t tests revealed that naive animals differed from conditioned animals in zone 1 (t(26) = −2.82, P = 0.009) and in zone 6 (t(26) = 3.12, P = 0.004). To further characterize the overall level of avoidance, we calculated an avoidance index by subtracting the time spent in zone 6 from the time spent in zone 1 (Fig. 1c). Avoidance behavior was stronger in conditioned versus naive animals (t(26) = 3.00, P = 0.006). Naive rats spent approximately the same amount of time in the two different zones, showing no avoidance behavior. Upon arrival in zone 1, the conditioned rats began to freeze and the freezing response in conditioned rats was greater than in naive animals (t(26) = 6.57, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1d). Thus, our paradigm allows the assessment of fearful memory by simultaneously measuring two fear indices in the same animal: the time spent in zone 1 to avoid the CS and the time spent freezing. This procedure differs from those employed in previous studies in which fearful memory retention was assessed in a cage where the animals were only able to freeze, because alternative defensive behaviors, such as aversion or escape, were not viable.
We thus exploited this paradigm to investigate the involvement of LA in the formation or the long-term expression of fearful memories. To do this, LA was lesioned 15–20 mins after training or four weeks after training. In both conditions, memory was tested one month after CS-US association (Fig. 2a and e). Lesions entirely ablated the dorsal, medial and ventral subregions of LA but spared the BA and CeA nuclei (Fig. 2i,j and Supplementary Fig. S1). In keeping with previous studies1,5,8, LA lesions performed 15–20 mins after training (n = 11) impaired the formation of long-term memories, regardless of whether or not freezing (Fig. 2d) or avoidance (Fig. 2b and c) was used as a measure of memory retention. A mixed ANOVA model yielded a significant effect of the group in zone 1 (F(2,36) = 11.681, P < 0.001) and zone 6 (F(2,36) = 7.312, P = 0.002). Post hoc Bonferroni-corrected comparisons revealed that LA-lesioned animals spent less time in zone 1 (P < 0.001) and more time in zone 6 (P = 0.003) than conditioned ones, but they did not differ from naive ones (P > 0.05) in both zones. LA-lesioned animals showed a lack of avoidance behavior (one-way ANOVA, F(2,36) = 10.036, P < 0.001; conditioned versus LA-lesioned rats, P < 0.001, naive versus LA-lesioned rats, P > 0.05) and a freezing response lower than conditioned ones but similar to naive ones (one-way ANOVA, F(2,36) = 45.821, P < 0.001; conditioned versus LA-lesioned rats, P < 0.001, naive versus LA-lesioned rats, P > 0.05).
We then repeated similar experiments but by lesioning LA four weeks after training. In line with previous studies20,21, upon delivery of the CS, LA-lesioned rats (n = 12) did not display conditioned freezing responses (Fig. 2h). The freezing of LA-lesioned rats was low and similar to that of naive animals (one-way ANOVA, F(2,37) = 21.9, P < 0.001; conditioned versus LA-lesioned rats, P < 0.001, naive versus LA-lesioned rats, P > 0.05). Strikingly, however, the same animals were still able to bias their actions away from the CS and spent significantly more time in the zone farthest away (Fig. 2f). Mixed ANOVA revealed main effects of the zone (F(1,37) = 24.5, P < 0.001) and the group in zone 1 (F(2,37) = 5.60, P = 0.007) and zone 6 (F(2,37) = 7.29, P = 0.002). Pairwise Bonferroni-corrected comparisons indicated that the time spent in zone 1 by LA-lesioned rats was significantly greater than for naive animals (P = 0.026) and was reduced in zone 6 (P = 0.009) but did not differ from the time spent by conditioned rats (P > 0.05 in both instances). These data were further confirmed by the avoidance index (Fig. 2g). Conditioned animals and LA-lesioned rats spent more time displaying avoidance behavior (F(2,37) = 6.52, P = 0.004; naive versus LA-lesioned rats, P = 0.014; conditioned versus LA-lesioned animals, P > 0.05). These data revealed that during the retrieval of long-term fearful memories the LA is necessary for the expression of freezing, but not the avoidance of threatening CSs.
Thus, LA involvement in the processing of CS-US association changes over the course of the formation and the long-term maintenance of this information. LA is essential for the formation of fearful memories whereas, as time passes, it becomes necessary only for those memories which require the expression of the freezing response.
Two independent pathways within the amygdala support the expression of freezing and avoidance during the retrieval of long-term memories
Our findings raise the question of which neural pathway is recruited to actively avoid learned threat stimuli during the long-term expression of fearful memories. Previous studies showed that during the formation of fearful memories BA is required to learn to escape from fear5 and to avoid threats6–8,12. Thus, it is possible that BA covers this role also during the expression of long-term memories. However, in this case, it should become independent from information of LA, that our findings showed to not be necessary for the avoidance of CSs. In order to shed light on this issue, we lesioned BA four weeks after training and we tested lesioned rats as in the case of LA-lesioned ones. BA lesions included the basomedial and basolateral regions, and spared LA and CeA nuclei (Fig. 3f,g and Supplementary Fig. S2). In marked contrast to the results observed in LA-lesioned animals, BA-lesioned rats (n = 13) did not display any avoidance behavior in response to the CS (Fig. 3b and c). A mixed ANOVA showed a significant main effect of the group in zone 1 (F(2,38) = 4.45, P = 0.018) and in zone 6 (F(2,38) = 4.86, P = 0.013). Pairwise comparisons showed that the time spent in zones 1 and 6 was similar between naive and BA-lesioned rats (P > 0.05) and significantly differed between conditioned and BA-lesioned rats in zone 1 (P = 0.046) and in zone 6 (P = 0.021). Furthermore, the avoidance index analysis (F(2,38) = 4.82, P = 0.014) revealed differences between BA-lesioned and conditioned rats (P = 0.028). However, it is noteworthy that although the averaged time spent in zones 1 and 6 was similar between BA-lesioned and naive rats, the behavior of the two groups differed markedly. In fact, upon delivery of the CS, BA-lesioned rats moved to the nearest extremity of the cage, irrespective of the source of the CS, and then remained there. That is, some animals (n = 6) moved to zone 1 and then spent most of their time there, whereas others (n = 5) moved to zone 6 and remained there. To bring out these differences, we analysed the overall distance travelled by naive and BA-lesioned animals (Fig. 3e). The two groups differed significantly whilst BA-lesioned rats were similar to the conditioned animals (one-way ANOVA, F(2,38) = 5.34, P = 0.009; BA-lesioned versus naive rats, P = 0.022, BA-lesioned versus conditioned groups, P > 0.05). The difference between BA-lesioned rats and naive rats became evident when we considered freezing in response to the CS. While naive animals showed very little freezing, BA-lesioned rats showed higher levels of freezing, similar to those in the conditioned animals (one-way ANOVA, F(2,38) = 16.2, P < 0.001; BA-lesioned versus naive rats, P < 0.001, BA-lesioned versus conditioned group, P > 0.05) (Fig. 3d). Thus, rats with BA lesions were unable to direct their actions to avoid the CS. Upon delivery of the CS, they suddenly began to freeze. These results showed that at variance with LA lesions, BA lesions impaired the avoidance of the CS, but the conditioned freezing response remained intact. Therefore, several weeks after learning BA has become independent from LA information and the two nuclei mediate different behavioral responses.
These data raise the question of the involvement in the expression of long-term memories of the other nucleus of the amygdala that has been studied extensively in fear conditioning, namely the CeA. A recent study showed that this nucleus plays a crucial role in the retrieval of long-term fearful memories tested by analyzing the freezing response15. We therefore tested the effects of lesioning the CeA four weeks after training in our behavioral paradigm. Lesions encompassed the lateral, capsular and medial divisions of CeA, but spared LA and BA (Fig. 4e,f and Supplementary Fig. S3). Upon delivery of the CS, CeA-lesioned rats (n = 9) were able to bias their actions away from the CS and spent significantly more time in the zone farthest away (Fig. 4b and c). A 3 × 2 mixed ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of the zone (F(1,34) = 26.3, P < 0.001), significant effects of the group in zone 1 (F(2,34) = 6.38, P = 0.004) and in zone 6 (F(2,34) = 8.09, P = 0.001). Pairwise Bonferroni-corrected comparisons revealed that CeA-lesioned animals differed from naive animals in zone 1 (P = 0.012) and zone 6 (P = 0.005), whereas there were no differences between CeA-lesioned and conditioned rats (P > 0.05 in both zones). These data were confirmed by the avoidance analysis revealing significant CS avoidance in the CeA-lesioned rats. One-way ANOVA showed significant differences among groups (F(2,34) = 7.33, P = 0.002) and Bonferroni post hoc comparisons indicated differences between CeA-lesioned and naive rats (P = 0.007) but not between conditioned and CeA-lesioned rats (P > 0.05). At the same time, the conditioned freezing response was abolished in these rats (Fig. 4d). Freezing behavior in CeA-lesioned rats (F(2,34) = 24.3, P < 0.001) was similar to that in naive animals (P > 0.05) and different from that in the conditioned group (P < 0.001). This result suggests that CeA is necessary for the expression of freezing, but not avoidance behavior, in marked similarity to LA-lesioned rats and in contrast to BA-lesioned rats.
This double dissociation leads to the idea that during the expression of long-term memories there are two distinct and dissociable neural systems within the amygdala (LA-to-CeA and BA) that are not only independently capable of supporting the retention of memory information, but also independently mediate distinct types of defensive responses. The existence of these parallel circuits implies that the inhibition of the entire amygdala, encompassing the CeA, LA and BA, or even LA (which triggers CeA activity) and BA nuclei alone, should block the expression of both freezing and avoidance responses. Consistent with this assumption, animals with combined lesions of LA and BA (Fig. 4h–l) or of the entire amygdala (Supplementary Fig. S5) were totally impaired in both fear-related responses.
LA and BA nuclei receive parallel and independent inputs from the auditory cortex during the expression of long-term memories
Our findings raise the question of which inputs carry the mnemonic information about learned stimuli to BA and LA. The amygdala receives inputs from the auditory cortex, especially from the higher-order Te2 and Te3 areas25–27. Recent findings suggested that the Te2 cortex is an essential locus for the long-term maintenance of auditory fearful memories14,28–31, and that Te2 activity is highly correlated with amygdala activity during long-term memory expression32. Intriguingly, the Te2 cortex sends projections not only to LA but also to BA25–27. Therefore, this cortex may provide information about auditory fearful memories to both LA and BA. To test this hypothesis, we first investigated the impact of Te2 lesions on fearful memories using our behavioral paradigm. Te2-lesioned rats did not display the avoidance behavior (Fig. 5b and c) and freezing response towards the CS (Fig. 5d), suggesting that this region is essential for the long-term memory expression, irrespective of the fear index measured. However, the data do not demonstrate that Te2 is the source of information for either LA and BA. To address this question directly, we relied on an optogenetic approach to specifically silence Te2 terminals in LA and BA. In Te2, we administered an adeno-associated viral vector (AAV-5) expressing the light-sensitive chloride pump halorhodopsin combined with enhanced red fluorescent protein (mCherry). The virus was under the control of a CaMKIIα promoter (AAV5-CaMKIIa-eNpHR3.0-mCherry) that drives its expression within pyramidal neurons33, i.e. the neurons that are the major source of efferents to subcortical nuclei25–27. Several weeks after injection of the viral vector, we observed Te2 terminals labeled in both LA and BA (Fig. 5e and g). We then implanted optical fibers and, one week later, we delivered light in order to simultaneously inactivate Te2 terminals in LA and BA during the long-term memory retention test. The inhibition of Te2 terminals in LA and BA impaired both the avoidance of the CS (Fig. 5h and i) and the conditioned freezing response (Fig. 5j). Student’s t test showed that eNpHR-mCherry-injected animals (n = 8) spent significantly less time in zone 1 (t(14) = −2.36, P = 0.033) and significantly more time in zone 6 (t(14) = 2.64, P = 0.019) than mCherry controls (n = 8). Accordingly, animals injected with eNpHR-mCherry exhibited significantly lower avoidance than control animals (t(14) = −2.50, P = 0.025). Furthermore, animals injected with eNpHR-mCherry showed lower levels of freezing than mCherry controls (t(14) = −2.48, P = 0.026). Thus, these data suggested that Te2 axons carry information about fearful memories that is essential to LA for the expression of freezing responses and to BA for the regulation of avoidance behaviors. Alternatively, optogenetic manipulations may have impaired the basal activity of the amygdala, thereby preventing the expression of defensive responses. To rule out this possibility, we probed the effects of Te2 terminal inhibition during the presentation of an innate threat stimulus, such as the image of a predator, in the same rats. In this situation, the inhibition of Te2 terminals did not affect defensive responses (t(14) = 0.88, P > 0.05) (Fig. 5k). These data showed that optogenetic inhibition of Te2 terminals in LA and BA impaired the expression of fear-related behaviors towards learned auditory CSs while it did not affect amygdala activity in the presence of innate threats. In addition, these data showed that not only the lesioning procedure but also the optogenetic inactivation of LA + BA affected long-term memories.
Discussion
Here we showed that LA lesions resulted in different effects depending on the memory phase in which they were performed. LA lesions performed 15–20 mins after training impaired long-term fearful memories regardless of whether or not freezing or avoidance was used as a measure. Conversely, when lesions of LA were performed four weeks after training, only the freezing response, but not the avoidance, was impaired. At this late interval, the BA is able to support the expression of avoidance even in the absence of LA inputs. Both LA and BA require projections from the auditory cortex in order to emit conditioned defensive behaviors. Therefore, we propose that LA is essential for the formation of the CS-US association but, over time, the CS-US memory that drives avoidance depends on a cortical-basal amygdala circuit which operates independently from a cortical-lateral and central amygdala circuit that is essential for freezing.
The data that LA lesions performed 15–20 mins after training impaired the formation of memories, irrespective of the fear index analyzed, is in line with previous studies showing that LA blockade performed before or shortly after training impaired the subsequent expression of freezing34,35 and avoidance5,7,8. Overall, these data support the idea that LA is essential for the formation of fearful memories. Conversely, the fact that LA lesions performed several weeks after training did not affect the expression of all types of defensive reactions sheds a new light on the participation of LA to long-term fearful memories. Previous studies have addressed the involvement of LA (or, more frequently, LA + BA) in the long-term expression of CS-US association. All these studies showed a permanent impairment in the freezing and conditioned startle responses to the CS19–21, and therefore come to the conclusion that this nucleus is a key substrate supporting long-term memory storage of fearful memories22. This view was further supported by data showing that the retrieval of long-term memories enhances LA activity17,18 but see15. Our data confirmed that LA lesions impaired freezing to threatening CSs. However, through the simultaneous measurement of an alternative fear index in the same animals, our data showed that the involvement of the LA in the long-term expression of fearful memories is much more limited than previously hypothesized, suggesting that the LA is not an essential locus for all CS-US memories. Our results did not allow to discriminate whether during the expression of long-term memories the LA serves only for the regulation of the freezing response or, alternatively, whether it also maintains memory information essential to drive the freezing response, as previously claimed15,17–20, but see21. In the latter case, our results suggested that within the amygdala there may be multiple and independent memory representations of the CS-US association.
The other major finding coming from our study is that the BA may operate independently from LA inputs during the expression of long-term memories, in order to bias the choice of action away from the source of the threat. This finding does not support the idea that LA and BA form an anatomically and functionally unitary complex (the basolateral complex, BLA)4 nor that LA is the main pathway carrying sensory information about the CS to the amygdala1. With respect to previous studies5,6,36 our data strongly suggest that, within the amygdala, there are at least two parallel input pathways, the LA and the BA. Two dissociable circuits originate from these two nuclei, one relying on the LA-to-CeA pathway which serves to express freezing to threatening stimuli, and the other relying solely on the BA (not on LA or CeA), which drives the actions of the subject away from the source of the threatening stimulus. These two pathways may support memory expression independently.
On the other hand, it remains unclear the role of the BA in the formation of recent fearful memories. This structure may be involved in the memory formation process as the LA nucleus. In this case, a lesion would impair the expression of both freezing and avoidance behavior. Alternatively, its role may be specific for the expression of avoidance behavior. Notably, in our previous work14, where animals could display only freezing response, BA was indeed activated after recent but not remote fearful memory expression, maybe suggesting its involvement in fear memory formation. Future studies will better address this issue.
Overall, our findings lead to the idea that the flow of information that has been proposed to occur serially from LA to BA to CeA during the formation of a fearful memory trace (Fig. 6a), cannot be applied to the long-term expression of fearful memories in favor of a new model in which two parallel circuits operate independently and mediate distinct defensive responses (Fig. 6b).
Our study also revealed that both LA and BA require direct inputs from the auditory cortex, and particularly the Te2 area, in order to produce defensive responses. This data is in line with previous findings that during the retrieval of long-term fearful memories the activity of Te2 and LA + BA nuclei is highly synchronized in the theta frequency range, and a preponderant Te2-to-BLA directionality characterizes this dialogue32. In addition, here we showed that the auditory cortex may be a crucial site for the maintenance of fearful memories regardless of whether or not freezing or avoidance is used as a measure. As LA is necessary for the formation of all fearful memories, it may be proposed that upon memory encoding this nucleus enables the formation of new memory traces at the level of the auditory cortex. Accordingly, previous studies showed that during the formation of a memory trace LA promotes memory-related processes in the auditory cortex37–41. Subsequently, during memory retrieval, these cortical memory traces may drive the LA and BA activity in order to express conditioned defensive responses.
The existence of two parallel and independent pathways within the amygdala has important implications for the study of anxiety disorders. Understanding the role of these two independent circuits is important for the treatment of traumatic memories, since the treatment often occurs long after the initial trauma2. In addition, studies addressing the possible extinction of traumatic memories should take into consideration these two independent and parallel pathways. Moreover, different exacerbated fear responses, such as avoidance behavior in obsessive-compulsive disorders or immobility during panic attacks, may rely on different circuits in the amygdala.
Methods
Subjects
Male Wistar rats (age: 65–75 days, weight: 230–360 g) were used. All the animals were housed in plastic cages with food and water available ad libitum, under a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 A.M.) at a constant temperature of 22 ± 1 °C. All the experiments were conducted in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive 2010/63/EU and approved by the Italian Ministry of Health (Authorization No 265/2011) and by the local Bioethical Committee of the University of Turin.
Fear Conditioning
One auditory stimulus (pure tone, 8 s, 80 dB, 3.000 Hz, 22 s inter-trial interval) was administered repeatedly for seven times as conditioned stimulus (CS), each time co-terminating with a painful stimulation (unconditioned stimulus, US) consisting of an electric foot shock (1 s, 0.7 mA). We employed seven trials because previous studies using this protocol14,30,42 reported a strong fear memory (freezing levels ≥80%) lasting for 4–5 weeks. Tones were delivered by a loudspeaker located 20 cm above the grid floor. Rats were left in the chamber for 1 additional min, and then they returned to the home cage.
Fearful Memory Test
Long-term aversive memories were tested at five weeks after the conditioning session. Animals were handled for four days before memory retention. Both habituation and retention occurred in a completely different apparatus, which was located in a separate experimental room in order to avoid conditioned fear responses to contextual cues14.
Apparatus
The testing chamber was a long cage made of 2 cm thick fir wood (184 cm length x 40 cm height x 34 cm width, opened on the superior side), internally covered with wood texture laminated paper. The cage was divided in six adjacent zones (each 30 × 30 cm) by a 2 cm wide strip of green adhesive tape applied to the ground, for a total of 180 × 30 cm of accessible space (Fig. 1a). On the external side of the short wall in zone 6, a loudspeaker and a light bulb were respectively fixed 6 cm and 13 cm above the ground, horizontally centered. Both the loudspeaker and the light bulb were pointed into the accessible space through two holes and were not visible from inside the cage. An exhaust fan, which eliminated odorized air from the enclosure, provided background noise of 60 dB.
Procedure
Animals underwent a four-days habituation protocol and a memory retention test on the fifth day. On the first day rats were delicately placed into the long cage (zone 2) with the snout oriented towards the light and auditory sources (zone 6) and left undisturbed for 8 min, then they returned to their home cages. The same procedure was repeated on the second day. In the third day, in order to increase the exploration of the zone 6 (i.e., the zone where the CS will be administered), a child’s toy consisting in a light blue building block (6,5 cm length x 5,5 cm height x 3,5 cm width) was added into the cage (fixed on the short wall) in zone 6, and animals were left free to explore it for 5 min. The same procedure was repeated on the fourth day. On the fifth day, the control stimulus was removed, and memory retention was assessed through the presentation of the CS. As in the previous days, rats were placed into the apparatus (zone 2). To facilitate the identification of the source of the CS, a light stimulus was delivered from the bulb located above the loudspeaker. When rats were in zone 2 with the snout oriented towards the light and auditory sources (zone 6), the light turned on and, 2 s later, the first CS was delivered. All the successive CSs were simultaneously presented with the light, for a total amount of 6 tones-lights of 8 s. CSs were administered in a manner identical to that employed during the conditioning session (duration, intensity and inter-trial intervals). The innate threat stimulus consisted in an eagle-shaped child toy, which was presented 1 m above the cage for 3 times of 8 s each, with an inter-trial interval of 22 s. The rat behavior was recorded by means of a digital video camera, fixed to the ceiling of the room and pointed perpendicularly to the apparatus.
Avoidance and Freezing Analysis
Behavioral patterns used as fear indices were avoidance and freezing responses. Avoidance was defined as the difference between the time that rats spent in zone 1 and in zone 6, measured during the 180-s fear memory retention test. The time that rats spent in each zone of the apparatus was analyzed using Smart v. 3.0.04 (Panlab Harvard Apparatus). Freezing response was defined as the complete absence of somatic mobility, except for respiratory movements. Freezing was measured by means of a stopwatch and counted as the overall time of immobility within the overall accessible space (zones 1–6) of the cage. A further employed index was the travelled distance. The measurement was performed using Smart v. 3.0.04. The following formula was used: Travelled distance = [(d * 180)/l]/100, where d is the raw travelled distance (cm) calculated by the software, 180 is the constant real length (cm) of the cage, and l is the length (cm) of the cage estimated by the software. All the behavioral analyses were performed by two persons who did not know to which experimental group each animal belonged (E. M. and A. R.).
Surgery
Irreversible excitotoxic lesions
Rats were mounted in a stereotaxic apparatus, an incision of the skull was made, and small burr holes were drilled to allow the penetration of a 28 gauge infusion needle. A 10 µl Hamilton syringe mounted on an infusion pump was used to deliver infusions at a rate of 0.1 µl/min. Neuronal cell loss was induced by injecting the ibotenic acid43 or the N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA) bilaterally at the coordinates taken from Paxinos and Watson atlas44. The needle was left in place for additional 6 mins in the case of CeA, LA and Te2 lesions and of 10 mins in the case of BA lesions. Naive and conditioned sham rats underwent an identical procedure, except that no infusions were made. Because no differences were detected among sham-operated animals in CeA, LA (15–20 mins after as well as four weeks after training), BA or Te2 areas, they were collected altogether. For this reason, the same naive and conditioned animals were kept constant throughout all the experiments. Following the surgical procedures, the rats were kept warm and under observation until recovery from anesthesia. All behavioral procedures were made one week after surgery to allow for recovery.
Amygdala nuclei lesions
Central amygdala lesions were made by administering ibotenic acid (10 mg/ml, dissolved in sodium phosphate buffer, pH 7.4) at two points with the following coordinates: 1.9 mm posterior (AP) to bregma, ± 4.0 mm lateral (L) and 8.4 mm ventral (V), total volume 0.20 µl; 2.5 mm posterior to bregma, ± 4.3 mm lateral and 8.2 mm ventral, total volume 0.22 µl. Lateral amygdala lesions were made by administering NMDA (18 mg/ml, dissolved in physiological saline) at two points with the following coordinates: 2.3 mm posterior (AP) to bregma, ±5.4 mm lateral (L) and 7.9 mm ventral (V), total volume 0.18 µl; 3.3 mm posterior to bregma, ±5.4 mm lateral and 8.0 mm ventral, total volume 0.18 µl. Basal amygdala lesions were made by administering NMDA (16 mg/ml) at two points with the following coordinates: 2.28 mm posterior (AP) to bregma, ±4.7 mm lateral (L) and 9.65 mm ventral (V), total volume 0.15 µl; 3.3 mm posterior to bregma, ±4.7 mm lateral and 9.65 mm ventral, total volume 0.16 µl.
Secondary auditory cortex lesions
Secondary auditory cortex Te2 was lesioned by injecting NMDA (18 µg/µl) at the coordinates: AP = 5.8, L = ± 6.5–6.7, V = 6.0, 0.22 µl; AP = 6.8, L = ± 6.5–6.7, V = 6.0, 0.22 µl.
Histology
The extension of the damaged areas in the case of excitotoxic lesions was histologically verified at the end of the experiments with NeuN staining analysis. Rats were intracardially perfused with 4% PFA. The brain was incubated in 4% PFA overnight at 4 °C, transferred to 30% sucrose and finally sectioned at 30 μm on a cryostat. Sections were placed in NeuN antibody (1:2000) at room temperature (RT) for 24 h, transferred to biotinylated anti-mouse antibody IgG (1:1000) at RT for 1 h and finally placed in a solution of ABC on a shaker for 2 h. Sections were rinsed in Tris-HCl and incubated in DAB. The DAB was developed by the addition of 0.015% H202, and the development stopped by washing with Tris-HCl. Verification of lesions was made observing NeuN-stained tissue under a microscope magnified at 2x and 4x. The lack of staining was used as an indication of neuronal loss due to lesions. The extensions of the lesions were analyzed by two persons who did not know to which experimental group each animal belonged (L. M. and B. S.).
Virus-mediated gene expression
The adeno-associated viruses (AAV, serotype 5) were obtained from the University of North Carolina Vector Core (Chapel Hill, NC, USA). Viral titer was 4.7 × 1012 vg/ml for AAV5:CaMKIIα::eNpHR3.0-mCherry. The use of CaMKII promoter enables transgene expression favoring pyramidal neurons. Viruses were housed in an −80 °C freezer. Viral infusions targeting the Te2 cortex were performed at the two stereotaxic coordinates employed for the excitotoxic lesions at the volumes of 0.5 μl. The viruses were injected at a rate of 0.1 μl/min, and the needle was left in place for additional 10 min. These injections produced strong opsin expression in the Te2 cortex. Fibers from the Te2 cortex were found to innervate either the lateral and basal amygdala, in keeping with prior anatomy studies25–27. The optic fibers (200/230 μm core; 10 mm length, Plexon, Texas, USA) were implanted bilaterally in LA + BA (AP = −2.8, L = ± 5.4, V = 8.2 mm from bregma).
Illumination
Optogenetic inhibition of Te2 projections to LA + BA was obtained by using the PlexBright Optogenetic Stimulation System (Plexon, Texas, USA). Orange light (620 nm) was generated by Compact LED Modules (Plexon, Texas, USA). Light was passed through an optical fiber (2.40 m lenght) to reach the brain. The power density estimated at the tip of the optic fiber was of 3 mW for illumination of projection sites. Orange light was initiated 4 s prior to CS/innate threat stimulus onset, persisted throughout the 8 s CS/innate threat stimulus, and was stopped 4 s after the CS/innate threat stimulus offset. Rats were familiarized with the patchcord for 4 days before starting each behavioral session.
mCherry immunohistochemistry
Upon completion of experiments, rats were deeply anaesthetized and perfused intracardially with 4% PFA in order to examine the diffusion of the virus. The brains were dissected, stored overnight at 4 °C, and finally transferred to 30% sucrose. Coronal sections (50 μm) were cut on a cryostat and collected in PBS. Free-floating sections were incubated in a blocking solution (4% bovine serum albumin (BSA), 10% normal goat serum and 0.5% Triton X-100) for 1 h at RT. Then, they were incubated in primary monoclonal mouse antibody to mCherry (1: 500 dilution, Abcam, ab125096) in the blocking solution overnight at 4°. Subsequently, sections were washed with PBS and incubated for 1 h at RT with secondary fluorescent labels AlexaFluor-568-labeled anti-mouse antibody (1:400 dilution, Lifetechnologies, A11036) diluted in PBS for 1 h on a shaker at room temperature. Sections were washed in PBS, mounted with mounting media containing DAPI (Vector, H1200) and coverslipped.
Confocal microscope imaging
The presence of mCherry labeling was examined by using a Leica SP5 confocal microscope: two laser were used (488 and 570 nm), each corresponding to the peak emission spectrum for DAPI (Nissl stain for cell nuclei) and CY3 (mCherry), respectively. Representative photomicrographs of Te2 and LA + BA (Fig. 5g) are mosaic images (each single was acquired by using a 20 × objective).
In order to visualize Te2-LA and Te2-BA terminals, we acquired Z-stack images in both areas by using a 63 × objective. Thus, LA and BA images showed in Fig. 5g are maximum intensity projections of 5 optical sections spaced 1 μm apart.
Statistical analysis
Since all data passed the Levene’s test for equality of variances, parametric statistics were employed through all the experiments. In order to test the main effect of zones (and specifically zone 1 and 6) and the simple main effects of groups (naive, conditioned and lesioned or mCherry control and eNpHR-mCherry), a mixed-design ANOVA with group as a between-subjects variable and zone as a within-subjects variable was performed. Pairwise multiple comparisons for the simple main effects of groups were implemented using Fisher’s Least Significant Difference (LSD) tests, corrected with Bonferroni adjustments. In order to test the differences between groups on the avoidance response, and the differences between groups on the freezing response, a one-way ANOVA (or a Student’s t test, in the case of two groups) was performed. Post hoc analysis was implemented using Bonferroni-corrected t tests. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics 22 (IBM).
Data availability
The datasets generated during and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We thank also the other members of the laboratory, M. Cambiaghi, G. Concina and A. Grosso, for discussion and advices. This work was supported by the grant from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Program (FP7/2007–20013)/ERC grant agreement no. 281072.
Author Contributions
E.M. and A.R. devised, carried out and analysed behavioural experiments; L.M. performed histological, immunohistochemical and confocal microscopy analyses; B.S. helped to devise and analyse the experiments and wrote the manuscript with input from E.M., A.R. and L.M. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Eugenio Manassero and Annamaria Renna contributed equally to this work.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-017-19074-3.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mahan AL, Ressler KJ. Fear conditioning, synaptic plasticity and the amygdala: implications for posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Orsini CA, Maren S. Neural and cellular mechanisms of fear and extinction memory formation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012;36:1773–1802. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tovote P, Fadok JP, Lüthi A. Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:317–331. doi: 10.1038/nrn3945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Amorapanth P, LeDoux JE, Nader K. Different lateral amygdala outputs mediate reactions and actions elicited by a fear-arousing stimulus. Nature Neurosci. 2000;3:74–79. doi: 10.1038/71145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Killcross S, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Different types of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within amygdala. Nature. 1997;388:377–380. doi: 10.1038/41097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Choi JS, Cain CK, LeDoux JE. The role of amygdala nuclei in the expression of auditory signaled two-way active avoidance in rats. Learn. memory. 2010;17:139–147. doi: 10.1101/lm.1676610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lázaro-Muñoz G, LeDoux JE, Cain CK. Sidman instrumental avoidance initially depends on lateral and basal amygdala and is constrained by central amygdala-mediated Pavlovian processes. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;67:1120–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Herry C, et al. Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature. 2008;454:600–606. doi: 10.1038/nature07166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Anglada-Figueroa D, Quirk GJ. Lesions of the basal amygdala block expression of conditioned fear but not extinction. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:9680–9685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2600-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Amano T, Duvarci S, Popa D, Paré D. The fear circuit revisited: contributions of the basal amygdala nuclei to conditioned fear. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:15481–15489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3410-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ramirez F, Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE, Sears RM. Active avoidance requires a serial basal amygdala to nucleus accumbens shell circuit. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:3470–3477. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1331-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6:119–130. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sacco T, Sacchetti B. Role of secondary sensory cortices in emotional memory storage and retrieval in rats. Science. 2010;329:649–656. doi: 10.1126/science.1183165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Do Monte FH, Quinones-Laracuente K, Quirk GJ. A temporal shift in the circuits mediating retrieval of fear memory. Nature. 2015;519:460–463. doi: 10.1038/nature14030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Do Monte FH, Quirk GJ, Li B, Penzo MA. Retrieving fear memories, as time goes by…. Mol. Psychiatry. 2016;21:1027–1036. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kwon JT, Jhang J, Kim HS, Lee S, Han JH. Brain region-specific activity patterns after recent or remote memory retrieval of auditory conditioned fear. Learn. Memory. 2012;19:487–494. doi: 10.1101/lm.025502.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Schroeder BW, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear learning induces persistent facilitation of amygdala synaptic transmission. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005;22:1775–1783. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04343.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lee Y, Walker D, Davis M. Lack of a temporal gradient of retrograde amnesia following NMDA-induced lesions of the basolateral amygdala assessed with the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behav. Neurosci. 1996;110:836–839. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.110.4.836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Maren S. Neurotoxic basolateral amygdala lesions impair learning and memory but not the performance of conditional fear in rats. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:8696–8703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-19-08696.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gale GD, et al. Role of the basolateral amygdala in the storage of fear memories across the adult lifetime of rats. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:3810–3815. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4100-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Maren S, Fanselow MS. The amygdala and fear conditioning: has the nut been cracked? Neuron. 1996;16:237–240. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80041-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Vazdarjanova A, McGaugh JL. Basolateral amygdala is not critical for cognitive memory of contextual fear conditioning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1998;95:15003–15007. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cahill L, Weinberger NM, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Is the amygdala a locus of “conditioned fear”? Some questions and caveats. Neuron. 1999;23:227–228. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80774-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Romanski LM, LeDoux JE. Information cascade from primary auditory cortex to the amygdala: corticocortical and corticoamygdaloid projections of temporal cortex in the rat. Cereb. Cortex. 1993;3:515–532. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.6.515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Shi CJ, Cassell MD. Cortical, thalamic, and amygdaloid projections of rat temporal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 1997;382:153–175. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19970602)382:2<153::AID-CNE2>3.0.CO;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.McDonald AJ. Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Prog. Neurobiol. 1998;55:257–332. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Grosso A, Cambiaghi M, Concina G, Sacco T, Sacchetti B. Auditory cortex involvement in emotional learning and memory. Neuroscience. 2015;299:45–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.04.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Grosso, A. et al. Region- and layer-specific activation of the higher order auditory cortex Te2 after remote retrieval of fear or appetitive memories. Cerebr. Cortex. bhw159, 10.1093/cercor/bhw159 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 30.Grosso A, et al. The higher order auditory cortex is involved in the assignment of affective value to sensory stimuli. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:8886. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Apergis-Schoute AM, Schiller D, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Extinction resistant changes in the human auditory association cortex following threat learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2014;113:109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cambiaghi M, et al. Higher-order sensory cortex drives basolateral amygdala activity during the recall of remote, but not recently learned fearful memories. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:1647–1659. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2351-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gradinaru V, et al. Molecular and cellular approaches for diversifying and extending optogenetics. Cell. 2010;141:154–165. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.02.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.LeDoux JE, Cicchetti P, Xagoraris A, Romanski LM. The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 1990;10:1062–1069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-04-01062.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Memory consolidation of auditory pavlovian fear conditioning requires protein synthesis and protein kinase A in the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:RC96. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-18-j0003.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Balleine BW, Killcross S. Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:272–279. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.McGaugh JL. Memory–a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287:248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.McGaugh JL. Consolidating memories. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015;66:1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-014954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Chavez CM, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM. The basolateral amygdala modulates specific sensory memory representations in the cerebral cortex. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2009;91:382–392. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Chavez CM, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM. Activation of the basolateral amygdala induces long-term enhancement of specific memory representations in the cerebral cortex. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2013;101:8–18. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yang Y, et al. Selective synaptic remodeling of amygdalocortical connections associated with fear memory. Nature Neurosci. 2016;19:1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.4370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sacchetti B, Ambroqi LC, Baldi E, Tassoni G, Bucherelli C. Memorization of contextual and CS conditioned fear response (freezing) in a one-trial acquisition paradigm. Arch. Ital. Biol. 1999;137:235–248. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Mody P, Holland PC, Gallagher M. Central, but not basolateral, amygdala is critical for control of feeding by aversive learned cues. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:15205–15212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3656-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates (Academic Press Elsevier, 2007).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.