Skip to main content
PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases logoLink to PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases
. 2018 Jan 12;12(1):e0005524. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0005524

Cognitive deficits and educational loss in children with schistosome infection—A systematic review and meta-analysis

Amara E Ezeamama 1,2,*, Amaya L Bustinduy 3, Allan K Nkwata 2, Leonardo Martinez 2, Noel Pabalan 4, Michael J Boivin 1, Charles H King 5
Editor: Amadou Garba6
PMCID: PMC5766129  PMID: 29329293

Abstract

Background

By means of meta-analysis of information from all relevant epidemiologic studies, we examined the hypothesis that Schistosoma infection in school-aged children (SAC) is associated with educational loss and cognitive deficits.

Methodology/Principal findings

This review was prospectively registered in the PROSPERO database (CRD42016040052). Medline, Biosis, and Web of Science were searched for studies published before August 2016 that evaluated associations between Schistosoma infection and cognitive or educational outcomes. Cognitive function was defined in four domains—learning, memory, reaction time, and innate intelligence. Educational outcome measures were defined as attendance and scholastic achievement. Risk of bias (ROB) was evaluated using the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale. Standardized mean differences (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated to compare cognitive and educational measures for Schistosoma infected /not dewormed vs. uninfected/dewormed children. Sensitivity analyses by study design, ROB, and sequential exclusion of individual studies were implemented. Thirty studies from 14 countries, including 38,992 SAC between 5–19 years old, were identified. Compared to uninfected children and children dewormed with praziquantel, the presence of Schistosoma infection and/or non-dewormed status was associated with deficits in school attendance (SMD = -0.36, 95%CI: -0.60, -0.12), scholastic achievement (SMD = -0.58, 95%CI: -0.96, -0.20), learning (SMD = -0.39, 95%CI: -0.70, -0.09) and memory (SMD = -0.28, 95%CI: -0.52, -0.04) tests. By contrast, Schistosoma-infected/non-dewormed and uninfected/dewormed children were similar with respect to performance in tests of reaction time (SMD = -0.06, 95%CI: -0.42, 0.30) and intelligence (SMD = -0.25, 95%CI: -0.57, 0.06). Schistosoma infection-associated deficits in educational measures were robust among observational studies, but not among interventional studies. The significance of infection-associated deficits in scholastic achievement was sensitive to ROB. Schistosoma infection-related deficits in learning and memory tests were invariant by ROB and study design.

Conclusion/Significance

Schistosoma infection/non-treatment was significantly associated with educational, learning, and memory deficits in SAC. Early treatment of children in Schistosoma-endemic regions could potentially mitigate these deficits.

Trial registration

ClinicalTrials.gov CRD42016040052

Author summary

Empirical evidence for cognitive or educational benefits of anti-Schistosoma treatment is currently uncertain, despite the recommended practice of wide-scale deworming with praziquantel. We addressed this knowledge gap by synthesizing information from 30 relevant epidemiologic studies reporting on 38,992 children between 5–19 years old from 14 countries. In those studies, Schistosoma infection or non-dewormed status was associated with educational loss and cognitive deficits. Specifically, there were small to moderate deficits in both school attendance and scholastic achievement. Similarly, Schistosoma infection or non-dewormed status was associated with deficits in learning and memory domains of psychometrically tested cognitive function. However, there was no evidence of Schistosoma infection- or non-deworming-associated deficits on tests of innate intelligence or reaction-time. Overall, compared to Schistosoma-uninfected or to dewormed children, the presence of Schistosoma infection or non-dewormed status was associated with educational, learning, and memory deficits in school-aged children. The combined evidence suggests that early treatment of children in Schistosoma-endemic regions could mitigate these deficits.

Introduction

An estimated 800 million persons in tropical and sub-tropical countries are at risk of infection by one of three main human Schistosoma parasites–S. mansoni, S. haematobium, and S. japonicum [1]. As many as 240 million adults and children are actively infected [24] resulting in as much as 3.3 million disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) lost per annum due to overt and subclinical morbidities of Schistosoma infection [4, 5]. Sub-Saharan Africa is most affected; children from endemic areas are often infected by two years of age and many remain chronically infected throughout their school-age years [68]. Periodic mass drug administration (MDA) with praziquantel in school-aged children has been recommended for morbidity control by the World Health Organization [6]. However, Schistosoma-infected pre-school children are not routinely treated in such settings, and they constitute a potentially high risk group for accumulation of morbidity [9]. At present, there is no specific guidance for anti-Schistosoma drug treatment of preschool children, partly because of the lack of a child-friendly pediatric formulation [10].

Treatment with praziquantel has a demonstrated effectiveness in reducing infection intensity within individuals and in reducing the prevalence of infection within communities [11]. Such treatment results in clear-cut improvements with respect to advanced schistosomiasis-associated morbidities such as urinary tract fibrosis and hepatosplenic disease, including peri-portal fibrosis [12, 13]. Epidemiologic studies have associated Schistosoma infections with adverse impacts on anemia, growth [14], fitness [15], pediatric quality-of-life [16], and sub-optimal child development [17]. Definitively linking Schistosoma infections to these non-specific and sub-clinical morbidities is complicated in the context of poverty.[8, 12, 17, 18] However, plausible biologic mechanisms of these adverse impacts have been described [2, 19] and the likely underestimation of their morbidity-related health impact has been highlighted [3].

In helminth-endemic regions, the recommendation of periodic deworming of children is explained on the basis of its expected salutary impact on a range of child-health outcomes including anemia [12], nutritional status [14, 17, 18, 20], and overall well-being [16]. In addition, periodic deworming has been linked directly or indirectly to enhancement of school attendance and educational achievement among children enrolled in school [6, 14, 21, 22]. However, the empirical evidence-base for cognitive and educational benefits of deworming remains controversial [2327]. Recent criticism of the supposed benefits of deworming for educational enhancement has emphasized the undue influence of a single study in evidence reviews [28], which may have led to over-optimistic appraisals regarding the potential health and poverty alleviation benefits of deworming programs [29, 30].

Recent reviews of MDA effects have, thus far, focused on the impact of soil-transmitted helminth infections (STH), but health policy discussions–including those at the World Health Organization [30, 31], have tended to generalize findings to all helminths. Because different parasites can have dramatically different effects in terms of organ-specific and systemic pathologies, it is important now to distinguish the impact and potential benefits of individual anti-helminthic therapies [32]. To date, the evidence base for cognitive or educational benefits of anti-Schistosoma treatment has not undergone systematic review. The present systematic review and meta-analysis addresses the following questions: a) among school-aged children examined in the context of cross-sectional or case-control studies, is Schistosoma infection associated with worse performance in neurocognitive tests or with educational loss? b) among school-aged children enrolled in prospective studies with specific treatment for Schistosoma infection, is lack of treatment with praziquantel associated with worse performance in neurocognitive tests or with educational loss? For our current meta-analysis, we hypothesize that non-treatment or infection with Schistosoma infection is associated with educational loss and cognitive deficits in school-aged children from schistosomiasis-endemic regions.

Methods

Search strategy

This review, with pertinent information regarding our review protocol, was prospectively registered with the PROSPERO database as follows: http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.asp?ID=CRD42016040052 (see Supporting Information file S1 Text). We searched Medline, Web of Science, and Biosis electronic databases for original research articles, conference abstracts, or dissertations available as of August 22, 2016. Databases were searched with pre-specified keywords including “bilharzia”, “schistosomiasis”, “Schistosoma”, “school attendance”, “attention”, “impairment”, “memory”, “cognition”, among others. The complete search strategy is detailed in S2 Text.

Population, inclusion and exclusion criteria, study design

This systematic review and meta-analysis is focused on school-aged children five years and older. We did not restrict studies according to language, design, or publication date. Both interventional and observational studies were included in this review if they evaluated cognitive function in school-aged children using any psychometric test, or measured school attendance or achievement in relation to infection by Schistosoma parasites of any species. We excluded studies exclusively focused on pre-school aged children because of the absence of educational measures and the use of neurocognitive tests (e.g., the Mullen test) that were difficult to classify in terms of neurocognitive domains. We excluded meta-analyses and primary studies of soil-transmitted helminth infections where Schistosoma coinfection was absent.

Comparisons: Schistosoma infection vs. no infection & treatment with praziquantel vs. placebo

Schistosoma infection status was determined by microscopic examination of stool or urine as appropriate for species. Praziquantel was the primary deworming agent in interventional studies. The primary exposure for this meta-analysis included presence of infection or, operationally, infection was categorically defined based on study design as follows: 1) untreated/placebo versus praziquantel-treated in a randomized controlled trial, 2) any, versus no Schistosoma infection in cross-sectional studies, or 3) pre-, versus post-praziquantel treatment, or infection-free versus persistent infection, among Schistosoma-infected individuals in a longitudinal design study. “Untreated” refers to children determined to be infected but not dewormed.

Outcomes: Psychometrically assessed cognitive function

In the reviewed studies, psychometrically-assessed cognitive function was measured by a range of instruments that, for the purposes of this meta-analysis, were categorized into four domains: memory, learning/executive function, attention/reaction time, and intelligence (see S1 Table for details). The memory domain instruments included tests of working (short-term) memory as well as those of long-term memory. Attention/reaction time tests were those that measured the ability of a child to sustain concentration on a particular object, action, or thought, including their capacity to manage competing demands in their environments. The learning/executive function domain included tests to evaluate children’s performance in goal-oriented behavior, particularly components that are important for scholastic advancement. These test a cluster of cognitive processes that enable children to connect past experience with present action, and by so doing, engage in planning, organizing, strategizing, paying of attention to details, and to emotionally self-regulate, make necessary efforts to remember important details required for attainment of future goals [29]. We included in the ‘intelligence’ domain psychometric tests of intelligence quotient (IQ) that likely measures largely biologically-determined cognitive abilities, in contrast to cognitive performance measures that are environmentally pliable [33].

It was common for studies to use a suite of psychometric instruments to assess a single or multiple cognitive domains in enrolled children. When multiple instruments were used to measure the same cognitive domain, a grand mean of scores and a grand mean of standard deviation (SD) across all instruments were calculated. Thus, for each publication, one overall mean and SD value was determined for each domain. A study could contribute data to different cognitive domains if it used tools spanning across several cognitive domains. However, each instrument only contributed to one single domain of function, as shown in S1 Table.

Outcomes: Educational loss

Two dimensions of educational loss were tabulated–school attendance and scholastic achievement for children enrolled in school.

School attendance: For children enrolled in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, attendance rate was respectively defined as the number of days children attended school over the past month or over the study period. In case-control studies, the percentage of children enrolled vs. not enrolled in school was calculated for Schistosoma-infected and non-infected children.

School achievement was assessed across studies based on: i) children’s pass rate on standardized teacher-generated tests; ii) the percent of children who were in appropriate class for age; iii) their enrollment in elite vs. non-elite schools; iv) their scores in the school function domain of pediatric quality-of-life inventory; v) their change in class position after treatment for Schistosoma infection; vi) an above average vs. average/below average scholastic performance as rated by a teacher; or vii) their pass rate in any kind of educational test, whether teacher-administered or not.

Study selection, data extraction and management

Two researchers (LM and AK) independently screened individual articles by title and abstract, after which eighty-eight full text articles were assessed for eligibility for inclusion in this review. Studies were excluded on the following basis: no outcome measure reported (n = 39), non-primary literature or a review article (n = 7), absence of both infection and outcome measures (n = 6), no variation in exposure (n = 2), a limited meeting abstract duplicated by later full publication (n = 3) and a nonhuman study (n = 1) Disagreements between reviewers on inclusion of a given study were resolved by consensus between LM and AK. If no consensus was reached, the article was further evaluated by an additional reviewer (AEE). Thereafter, two researchers (AEE and NP) independently extracted relevant data for meta-analyses. Where differences in approach to standard error (SE) estimation were noted, discrepancy was resolved by consensus between AEE and NP. When a potentially relevant publication did not present needed information for meta-analysis, the authors were contacted to request additional data. If the dataset was publicly available, we obtained needed values directly [28].

Statistical analysis

The method for deriving SD from respective studies depended on how data were presented in the original reports. Some papers presented median (m) and range (a to b) instead of means and SD. These measures were converted into approximate mean and SD as follows: x¯a+2m+b4,S2112((a2m+b)24+(ba)2), see [34], where x¯ and S2 refer to the values of mean and variance, respectively. Some studies reported mean of respective measures and 95% confidence intervals. For such studies, SDs were derived as follows: SD = sqrt(N)*(upper limitlower limit)/3.92. Other studies presented data on means and their standard errors (SE), and SD was estimated as SD = SE*the square root of N, the study size. For studies presenting data on differences in mean scores between two time points for treated/infected vs. untreated/uninfected groups, appropriate SD for mean difference was calculated using the approach recommended by the Cochrane Collaboration [35].

For the meta-analysis, studies were grouped into six categories of outcomes used to measure cognitive or school-based function: school attendance, school achievement, memory, learning, IQ, or attention. Standardized mean difference (SMD) estimates and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated for each test. SMD estimates were classified as robustly statistically different if their confidence intervals excluded zero. SMD estimates were interpreted based on thresholds described by Cohen [36], as follows: ‘trivial’ (< |0.20|), ‘small’ (≥| 0.20| to < |0.50|), ‘moderate’ (≥ |50| to < |0.80|) or ‘large’ (≥ |0.80|) effects, according to standard practice in social science research.

All analyses and plots were implemented in STATA, versions 11 or 12. Heterogeneity between studies was measured with Higgins’s and Thompson’s I2 statistic and chi-square p-values [37]. Where between-study heterogeneity was high, random effects modelling was used to estimate a pooled summary effect across studies [38, 39]. In the absence of heterogeneity, fixed effects modeling was performed [39]. Publication bias was assessed using the Egger test [40]. In sensitivity analysis, we evaluated potential heterogeneity in pooled impact estimates based on: i) observational vs. intervention study design; ii) the quality of original studies based on the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale; and iii) by Schistosoma species. For sensitivity analyses by species, we distinguished between urogenital schistosomiasis (S. haematobium), which is often obvious to affected children, and intestinal/hepatosplenic schistosomiasis (S. mansoni/S. japonicum). The latter two infections are similarly diagnosed by stool exam and infection is seldom obvious to most children. We examined the potential for overly influential publications using the ‘metabias’ function in STATA to evaluate robustness of our pooled estimate, based on sequential removal of individual publications from the calculation of summary estimates. Lastly, we evaluated the impact of year of publication on the stability of the pooled estimate by iteratively including studies based on year of publication–i.e. starting from the earliest to the most recent publication using the ‘metacum’ function in STATA.

Quality assessment

Our investigation was guided by recommendations of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) initiative and the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) guidelines for observational studies [41]. Quality ranking of each study was implemented using an adapted version of the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale (NOQAS) [42, 43] to derive a quality score for each investigation with respect to: i) representativeness of the infected population sample or the selection cases and controls (this yielded a score with range 0 to 3*); ii) comparability with respect to known correlates of cognitive function/educational attainment (score range 0 to 6*); iii) the absence of bias in relation to outcome assessment in prospective cohort studies (0 to 3*) or exposure assessment in cross-sectional and case-control studies (0 to 3*). We adapted the comparability segment of this scale to account for confounding effects of age, sex, nutrition, and socioeconomic status in the relation between Schistosoma infection and educational/cognitive outcomes. Comparability with respect to these factors was either achieved by design (i.e. age or sex restriction for observational studies, or randomization for RCTs), or analytically, via stratified analyses or multivariable adjustment in regression models. Scores were assigned for attainment of comparability with respect to these factors as follows: age (score = 1*), sex (score = 1*), nutritional status (score = 2*) and socioeconomic status (score = 2*). For each study, the initial raw quality score (max = 12*) was rescaled to match the scale of 9* and then classified as low, high, or very high risk of bias based on precedents in prior literature [42].

Results

Our database search yielded a total 2914 unique records. The screening of titles and abstracts resulted in the exclusion of 2846 records leaving 78 unique papers for full text review. We identified an additional set of ten relevant studies from the bibliographies of relevant articles. Of these 88 articles, 58 were excluded based on full text review for reasons pecified in Fig 1.

Fig 1. Flow diagram for search and selection of included studies.

Fig 1

A total of 30 epidemiologic studies that assessed differences in cognitive test scores (based on psychometric tests) and/or educational status (measured as scholastic achievement or school attendance rate) in relation to Schistosoma infection or treatment were selected for inclusion (Table 1). Of these, 21 studies were cross-sectional [16, 4456] or case-control [5763] design where children were classified based on presence vs. absence of Schistosoma infection. Seven were longitudinal studies or pre-post intervention studies that featured screening and treatment for Schistosoma infections at the time of first assessment [28, 6468]. In these studies, outcome contrasts were made according to: a) the duration of persistent infection vs. duration of an infection-free interval, and b) the number of children for whom intensity of infection at last follow-up remained lower vs. the number of children for whom there was no change from baseline infection status after treatment with praziquantel.

Table 1. Characteristics of 30 eligible studies of educational or cognitive loss in relation to Schistosoma species infectiona.

Author Design Age (years) N for Infected, Not Treated, or Pre-treatment N for Uninfected, Treated, or Post-treatment Outcome Domain(s) Evaluated by Schistosoma Species Involved Country
Loveridge 1948 CC 11–18 91 108 Achievement Attendance of elite vs. non-elite school S. haematobium South Africa
Jordan 1962 Cohort 12–19 58 58 Achievement Class Rank Improvement S. haematobium Kenya
Goldin 1972 CS 9–12 112 80 Achievement Teacher ranking of scholastic ability S. haematobium Zambia
Bell 1973 Cohort 8–12 69 69 IQ Ravens Progressive Matrix S. haematobium & S. mansoni South Africa
Castle 1974 CS 11–18 26 308 Achievement, Learning, Memory Thurstone Mental Abilities Test S. haematobium South Africa
Epstein 1974 CS 13–14 26 to 43 132 to 224 Learning, Achievement, Attendance Nelson Reading Test, Class Rank, Attendance Rate S. mansoni St. Lucia
Ejezie 1981 CS 6–15 164 517 Achievement, Attendance % Passing in last Year, % Attendance S. haematobium Nigeria
Haycock 1983 CS 6–15 481 353 Achievement % in age-appropriate class S. haematobium South Africa
el-Hawy 1990 CS 13–17 300 300 Achievement, Attendance Pass rate, Attendance S. haematobium S. mansoni Egypt
Kimura 1992 Cohort 9–19 49 49 Attention Tanaka Binet Intelligence Test S. haematobium Kenya
Ekanem 1994 CS 5–15 177 285 Achievement, Attendance Teacher Given Test, % attendance S. haematobium Nigeria
Hussein 1996 CS 6–18 6471 (Upper)
11080 (Lower)
5149 (Upper), 3006 (Lower) Enrollment rate % Attendance S. haematobium Egypt (Upper & Lower regions)
Fentiman 1997 CC 6–18 130 239 Attendance % enrolled in school S. haematobium Ghana
de Clerq 1998 CS 6–11 203 263 Attendance % Attendance S. haematobium Mali
Nazel 1999 CC 9–12 80 40 Memory, IQ, Attendance, Achievement VF, WISC, %Attendance, Standardized Test S. mansoni Egypt
Nokes 1999 RCT 5–16 89 92 Memory, Attention Fluency, FR, DSF, CB, Picture Search S. japonicum China
Useh 1999 CC 6–12 243 254 Attendance % enrolled in school S. haematobium Nigeria
Beasley 2000 CC 7–12 167 274 Attendance % Attendance S. haematobium Tanzania
Meremikwu 2000 Cohort 8–9 210 203 Achievement, Attendance Teacher Administered Test, % attendance S. haematobium Nigeria
Tiruneh 2001 CC 6–15 597 518 Enrollment rate % Attendance S. mansoni Ethiopia
Jukes 2002 CS 9–15 241 97 Memory, Learning, Reaction Time, Achievement DS, WF, CB, SWLT, Stroop, CRT, PBT, SS, Reading, Spelling, Math S. haematobium +/- Hookworms Tanzania
Miguel 2004 Cohort 6–18 92 Persistently Infected 407 Uninfected both years Enrollment rate, Achievement % Attendance, Change Scores over 2 years S. mansoni Kenya
Ezeamama 2005 CS 6–18 244 75 Memory, Learning, IQ VF/WRAML Memory, WRAML Learning, PNIT S. japonicum The Philippines
Mekheimar 2005 CC 6–12 57 42 Attendance % enrolled in school S. mansoni Egypt
Grigorenko 2006 RCT 11–13 92 Screened Not Treated 74 Screened & Treated Memory, Learning, Reaction Time, Achievement DS, WF, CB S. haematobium + hookworms Tanzania, Africa
Berhe 2009 CS 5–18 219 114 Attention % symptoms leading to distraction in class S. mansoni Ethiopia
Ezeamama 2012 Cohort 6–18 214 Not Cured/re-infected 39 Not re-infected Memory, Learning, IQ VF/WRAML Memory, WRAML Learning, PNIT S. japonicum The Philippines
Terer 2013 CS 5–18 352 450 Achievement Score in school functioning S. haematobium Kenya
Hurlimann 2014 Cohort 5–14 130 89 Memory, Attention Digit Span Test, Code Transmission Test S. mansoni + STH Côte D’Ivoire
Rasoamanamihaja 2016 CS 7–10 684 1274 Enrollment rate % regular vs. non-regular attendance S. haematobium + S. mansoni Madagascar

aAbbreviations: CC, Case-Control; CS, Cross-sectional; RCT, Randomized controlled trial; VF, verbal fluency; WRAML, wide range assessment of memory and learning; PNIT, Philippine non-verbal intelligence test; DS, Digit Span; WF, word fluency; CB, Corsi Block; SWLT, Spanish word learning task; FR, Free recall; DSF, Digit Span Forwards; CRT, choice reaction time; PBT, peg board task (dominant & non-dominant hand); SS = silly sentencies.

Two studies were randomized controlled studies. Only one included study was a classic placbo-controlled randomized-trial intervention in which children with Schistosoma infection were randomized to praziquantel vs. placebo/no treatment [69]. The other study randomized children to screening vs. non-screening for Schistosoma infections [70]. Children in the non-screened arm remained untreated (although that sample was subsequently tested for infection to distinguish infected from uninfected children). In that study, among children randomized to screening, those found to be Schistosoma-infected were given treatment, and we were thus able to derive differences in cognitive test scores between Schistosoma-infected/treated vs. Schistosoma-infected/not-treated children (Table 2). The median follow-up duration across the nine logitudinal studies was 12 months. Minimum follow-up duration was one month and maximum follow-up duration was 36 months. In four of the longitudinal studies, follow-up duration was 6 months or less. In another four studies, follow-up duration was more than 12 months. One study had a 12 month follow-up.

Table 2. Pooled estimates of Schistosoma infection/non-treatment effects on educational/cognitive loss–Evaluation of heterogeneity and publication bias.

Cognitive Domain # Studies SMD (95%CI) Heterogeneity test Publication bias
Studies Included
P-valueφ I2 (%) P-valueα
Memory 8 -0.28 (-0.52, -0.04) 0.0001 78.6 0.786 [42, 48, 52, 58, 62, 67, 76]
Learning 6 -0.39 (-0.70, -0.09) 0.0001 79.4 0.793 [42, 47, 48, 52, 67, 76]
Intelligence Quotient Based Assessments 4 -0.25 (-0.57, 0.06) 0.008 74.8 0.450 [48, 58, 76, 77]
Reaction Time 6 -0.06 (-0.42, 0.30) 0.030 88.5 0.142 [41, 52, 62, 64, 66, 67]
Educational Loss Assessments
Achievement 16 -0.58 (-0.96, -0.20) 0.0001 97.9 0.595 [15, 25, 4247, 49, 50, 52, 56, 58, 63, 65, 67]
School Attendance 16 -0.36 (-0.60, -0.12) <0.001 98.7 0.991 [15, 25, 4347, 4952, 5460, 63, 65, 67]

SMD < 0 suggests a negative effect of infection/non-treatment on the indicated outcome; SMD > 0 indicates a positive effect of infection on respective outcomes.

φ: measures the extent to which there is heterogeneity across studies in terms of underlying results.

α: evaluates the tendency for increased publication of studies that show a statistically robust finding; here, a P < 0.05 suggests presence of publication bias.

In all, a total of 38,992 children between the ages of 5 to 19 years from 14 countries in three continents–Africa, Asia and North America, were included in this review. The vast majority of studies were of children from Africa–Nigeria (n = 4), Egypt (n = 4), South Africa (n = 4), Tanzania (n = 3), Kenya (n = 4), Mali (n = 1), Côte D’Ivoire (n = 1), Zambia (n = 1); Ghana (n = 1) and Ethiopia (n = 1). Three studies were conducted in Southeast Asia or Asia [The Philippines (n = 2) and China (n = 1)], one study was conducted in St. Lucia (Caribbean), and another study was implemented in Madagascar.

A total of 36,626 children were studied in the context of cross-sectional or case control studies. Of these, cognitive test scores and/or indicators of educational loss were measured in 23,126 (59.3%) children infected with one of three schistosomiasis species (S. haematobium (20 studies), S. japonicum (3 studies), or S. mansoni (10 studies)). These infected children were compared to 13,835 (40.6%) children without Schistosoma infection. A total of 2,366 children were studied in the context of randomized intervention studies including praziquantel vs. placebo, or prospective treatment studies that included baseline and follow-up assessments of cognitive function (Table 2). Of the 30 studies included in this review, three (10%) and 16 (53.3%) were judged to be at very high or high risk of bias, respectively, per the NOQAS (Table 3).

Table 3. Quality of evidence from individual studies included in the meta-analysisa.

Newcastle Ottawa Quality Assessment Scale
Study ID Design Description Selection max = 3* Comparability max = 6* Exposure
/Outcome
max = 3*
Scaled Quality Score
max = 9
Risk of Bias
Grigorenko 2006 RCT Randomized 254 Tanzanian children 11–13 years old to screening vs. no screening with 16 months of FU. Screened, infected children were treated with ALB + PZQ. All randomized to no screening were untreated but study distinguished "infected not treated" from uninfected/untreated. Analyzed for association between infected/untreated status, infected/treated vs. uninfected/not treated status on cognitive function with statistical control for multiple confounders. *** ****** *** 9 Low
Ezeamama 2012 Cohort Treatment reinfection study of 253 schisto infected Filipino children 6–18 years old followed for 18 months with repeated assessment for infection and cognitive function. Evaluated association between infection free duration and performance in four cognitive tests. Controlled for: age, sex, nutritional status, socioeconomic status, coincident STH, and other factors. *** ****** *** 9 Low
Nazel 1999 CC 120 Egyptian children 9–12 years old. Infected cases (mild & moderate/high intensity, n = 80) matched to uninfected classmate controls (n = 40). Analyses controlled for age. Data on socioeconomic status of both parents, nutritional status, crowding index, and number of siblings did not differ by infection category. *** ****** ** 8 Low
Nokes 199967 RCT Placebo controlled 2x2 intervention trial among 181 Chinese children 5–16 years old with allocation to treatment with: PZQ with ALB-placebo, ALB with PZQ-placebo, PZQ and ALB, or PZQ-placebo and ALB-placebo; FU duration = 3 months. *** ****** ** 8 Low
Jukes2002 CS 338 Tanzanian children 9–15 years old. Included uninfected, moderate, or heavy schistosome infection, with or without coinfection with moderate intensity hookworm. Multivariate control for multiple confounders including SES, nutritional indices, inflammation, and malaria coinfections. *** ****** ** 8 Low
Miguel 2004 Cohort Prospective investigation of scholastic achievement and attendance by infection status over 12 months in 499 Kenyan children 6–18 years old. Sample restricted to those present at both FU periods. Robust control for confounding covariates. *** ****** ** 8 Low
Ezeamama 2005 CS 319 children 6–18 years Filipino children. Controlled for age, sex, hemoglobin status, nutritional status, socio-economic status, and coincident STH infections *** ****** ** 8 Low
Berhe 2009 CS Included 333 Ethiopian children 5–18 years old. Multivariable investigation of infection-related differences in psychometric tests. Controlled analytically for several confounders including SES, nutritional status. The surrogate for attention "severe cramps distracting class attentiveness" is inherently subjective. ** ****** ** 8 Low
Terer 2013 CS Compared school functioning for schistosome-infected and uninfected Kenyan children 5–18 years old. Controlled for age, sex, nutritional, and socioeconomic confounders via multivariable analyses. *** ****** ** 8 Low
Hurlimann 2014 Cohort 219 Ivorian children 5–14 years old. Repeated treatment for schistosome and STH infection with 5 months follow-up. Controlled for age, sex, socioeconomic, and nutritional status *** ****** * 8 Low
Epstein 1974 CS Enrolled 267 St. Lucian children 13–14 years old. Compared outcomes for children with infection and uninfected. Age, sex, and SES adjusted for in multivariable analysis. *** **** ** 7 Low
Bell 1973 Cohort 138 South African Children 8–12 years old. Analyses compared infected and uninfected children with respect to change in IQ test over 12 months of repeat testing. Authors state that each child was paired (pairing factors unspecified) to eliminate variation due to age, sex, grade, and school. Mostly descriptive analyses reported. *** *** ** 6 High
Fentiman 1997 CC Enrolled 352 Ghanaian children 6–18 years old. Compared schistosome infection in enrolled and unenrolled school aged children matched for age and sex or class (if age-inappropriate for class). No evidence of multivariable analysis but confounding by age, sex, and to some extent SES, is addressed by matching factors. *** *** ** 6 High
Jordan 1962 Cohort Enrolled 116 boys 12–19 years old from hyper endemic area around Lake Victoria. Treated with Lucanthone hydrochloride or TwSb; untreated boys served as controls. Scholastic ability was assessed at enrollment and 6 months later. Boys in treatment group improved their class position over six months vs. those uninfected and/or infected but not treated. *** * *** 5 High
Kimura 1992 Cohort Included 49 Kenyan children 9–19 years old confirmed to be schisto infected. Allocated to PZQ or no treatment without randomization. There was matching by grade level and pre- vs. post-enrollment assessment of cognition over 1 month. No difference in groups by age, sex, infection intensity, or scores at enrollment. Scores improved for treated but not for untreated children. No evidence of control for sex, SES, or nutritional status in multivariable analysis. *** * ** 5 High
Ekanem 1994 CS 462 infected and uninfected Nigerian children 5–15 years old. Infected children were matched to uninfected children by age and sex. No multivariable analyses; no control for socioeconomic or nutritional status. *** ** ** 5 High
de Clerq 1998 CS Study of 466 infected & uninfected Malian children, 6–11 years old. Association age- and sex-adjusted in multivariable analysis. *** ** ** 5 High
Meremikwu 2000 Cohort 210 schistosome infected Nigerian children, all 8–9 years old, were treated with PZQ, followed for 36 months, and screened yearly for reinfection. Re-infected children were retreated. Retention was high. Did not control for sex, SES, or malnutrition. *** * *** 5 High
Rasoamana-mihaja 2016 CS Enrolled 1958 children 7 to 10 years old from Madagascar as part of a cluster randomized study including 29 sentinel sites. 20 school attending and 4 non-school attending children from each cluster were randomly selected analyzed for relationship of infection (prevalence and intensity) to school attendance. No difference between attendees vs. non-attendees with respect to infection. However, non-attendees had higher intensity of infection. *** ** ** 5 High
Castle 1974 CS Included 334 South African children 11–18 years old with or without subclinical schistosome infection. Measured data on father occupation as SES surrogate, behavioral risk factors, and pupil factual knowledge of infection cause and prevention, but no evidence of multivariable analysis. *** ** 4 High
Ejezie 1981 CS Included 681 Nigerian children 6–15 years old. Descriptive analyses of educational loss by infection status. No evidence of adjustment for SES, age, sex, or nutrition. *** ** 4 High
el-Hawy 1990 CS Enrolled 600 Egyptian boys 13–17 years old. Descriptive analysis of school performance by schistosome infection status. No control for age, SES, or nutritional status. *** ** 4 High
Hussein 1996 CS Comparison of infection prevalence among enrolled and unenrolled school children in upper Egypt (n = 11,620) and lower Egypt (n = 14,806). Infection prevalence and cultural practice with respect to education of children differ by Northern vs. Southern Egypt. All analyses region-stratified, hence we maintain Upper and Lower Egypt as distinct regions contributing unique data points in this meta-analysis. Children were 6–18 years old. *** ** 4 High
Useh 1999 CC Enrolled 560 Nigerian children 6–12 years old. School attendance rate and non-enrollment rate were determined based on head of household recall for index child. Potential misclassification of enrollment due to recall bias. * ** ** 4 High
Beasley 2000 CC Enrolled 441 Tanzanian children 7–12 years old. Comparison of enrollment rate in infected and uninfected children. Mostly descriptive analysis presented but information on SES, nutritional status and other factors evaluated by infection status. *** ** 4 High
Tiruneh 2001 CC Enrolled 1,115 Ethiopian children, 6–15 years old. Comparison of schistosome infection prevalence among enrolled and non-enrolled children. Descriptive analysis with high potential for residual confounding by SES, nutritional status, etc. *** ** 4 High
Mekheimar 2005 CC Enrolled 99 Egyptian children 6–12 years old. Comparison of enrollment rate by infection status via descriptive analyses. *** ** 4 High
Loveridge 1948 CC Included 199 South African children, 11–18 years old. Descriptive comparison of enrollment in elite vs. non-elite school by schistosome infection status. ** ** 3 Very high
Goldin 1972 CS Enrolled 192 Zambian children 9–12 years old. Descriptive analysis of subjective teacher ranking of index student as above or below average scholastic achievement by schistosome infection status. ** ** 3 Very high
Haycock 1983 CS 834 South African children 6–15 years old. Measured whether students were at age appropriate classes or not by schistosome infection status. No information on confounders beyond age. No multivariable analysis. ** ** 3 Very high

aAbbreviations: CC, Case-Control; CS, Cross-sectional; RCT, Randomized controlled trial; FU, Follow up; ALB, albendazole; PZQ, praziquantel; STH, soil-transmitted helminths; SES, socioeconomic standing; TwSb, Stibophen

Schistosoma infection and its associations with school attendance and educational attainment

Sixteen studies evaluated Schistosoma infection-related differences in school attendance (Table 2). Schistosoma infection-associated attendance deficits varied in magnitude and direction by study design. Specifically, we did not find any evidence of association between Schistosoma infection and school attendance among the two interventional studies (SMD = 0.03, 95%CI: -0.73, 0.78; Table 4); however, the observed infection-associated deficit in school-attendance was robust for the pooled estimate of 14 observational studies (SMD = -0.42, 95%CI: -0.70, -0.14). Compared to uninfected or praziquantel-treated children, the magnitude and direction of infection-associated deficit in school attendance was similar for children infected with S. haematobium or S. mansoni. Within strata of study quality, the association between infection and scholastic achievement was directionally consistent and statistically robust (Table 4). Across all studies, regardless of design or ROB, a deficit in school-attendance was evident for Schistosoma-infected or non-praziquantel-treated children compared to uninfected or praziquantel treated children (n = 15 studies; SMD = -0.36, 95% CI: -0.60, -0.12).

Table 4. Pooled estimate of Schistosoma infection or non-treatment on educational and cognitive loss in school-aged children from schistosomiasis-endemic regions: Stratified by study design, Schistosoma species, and study qualitya,b.

  Test of Association Test of Heterogeneity within stratum
STRATUM K SMD 95% CI PA PB I2 AM
Memory
Interventional Design 4 -0.36 [-0.81, 0.09] 0.12 <0.001 90 R
Observational Design 4 -0.17 [-0.33, 0.01] 0.058 0.61 61 F
S. haematobium 3 -0.45 [-1.07, 0.17] 0.15 <0.001 88 R
S. mansoni/japonicum 5 -0.19 [-0.41, 0.04] 0.10 0.02 65 R
Low ROB 7 -0.27 [-0.53, -0.004] <0.001 <0.001 81.5 R
All Studies Included 8 -0.28 [-0.52, -0.04] <0.001 <0.001 81 R
Learning
Interventional Design 2 -0.79 [-1.19, -0.39] <0.001 0.062 71 F
Observational Design 4 -0.18 [-0.34, -0.01] 0.04 0.576 0 F
S. haematobium 3 -0.46 [-1.15, 0.23] 0.19 <0.001 90 R
S. mansoni /japonicum 3 -0.36 [-0.54, -0.18] < 0.001 0.14 49 F
Low ROB 5 -0.41 [-0.75, -0.06] <0.001 <0.001 83 R
All Studies Included 6 -0.4 [-0.70, -0.09] 0.001 0.05 79 R
Reaction time
Interventional Design 4 0.13 [-0.03, 0.29] 0.12 0 58.2 F
Observational Design 2 -0.39 [-1.19, 0.40] 0.25 <0.001 84 R
S. haematobium 3 0.11 [-0.07, 0.29] 0.24 0.38 0 F
S. mansoni/ japonicum 3 -0.20 [-0.82, 0.42] 0.53 <0.00001 93 R
Low ROB 5 -0.07 [-0.49, 0.35] 0.26 <0.001 89 R
All Studies Included 6 -0.06 [-0.42, 0.30] 0.30 <0.001 88.5 R
Intelligence
All 4 -0.25 [-0.57, 0.06] 0.11 0.008 74 R
Low ROB 3 -0.29 [-0.73, 0.15] 0.19 0.003 84 R
Achievement
Interventional Design 4 -0.35 [-0.71, 0.01] 0.06 <0.001 85 R
Observational Design 12 -0.65 [-1.12, -0.17] <0.001 <0.001 98.3 R
S. haematobium 12 -0.62 [-1.09, -0.14] 0.01 < 0.001 98 R
S. mansoni 3 -0.22 [-0.40, -0.05] 0.01 0.32 11 F
Low ROB 6 -0.08 [-0.21, 0.02] 0.114 0.216 29 F
High ROB 7 -0.84 [-1.52, -0.16] <0.001 <0.001 95 R
Very High ROB 3 -0.92 [-2.1, 0.28] 0.185 <0.001 98.5 R
All Studies Included 16 -0.58 [-0.95, -0.20] <0.001 <0.001 98 R
Attendance
Interventional Design 2 0.03 [-0.73, 0.78] 0.277 <0.001 96 R
Observational Design 14 -0.36 [-0.64, -0.08] <0.001 <0.001 99 R
S. haematobium 10 -0.29 [-0.59, 0.01] 0.06 < 0.001 99 R
S. mansoni 4 -0.26 [-0.42, -0.01] 0.001 0.28 22 F
Low ROB 3 -0.24 [-0.42, -0.07] 0.006 0.286 20 F
High ROB 13 -0.34 [-0.63, -0.05] <0.001 <0.001 99 R
All Studies Included 16 -0.31 [-0.57, -0.05] <0.001 <0.001 98 R

aAbbreviations: K, number of studies; SMD, standard mean difference; CI, confidence interval; PA, P value for association; PB, P value for heterogeneity; AM, analysis model: R, Random-effects; F, Fixed-effects; I2, measure of variability expressed in %

bValues in bold indicate significant associations. SMD < 0 suggests a negative effect of infection/non-treatment on the indicated outcome; SMD > 0 indicates a positive effect of infection on the respective outcome.

The overall finding of schistosomiasis-associated achievement deficit (n = 16 studies; SMD = -0.58, 95%CI: -0.96, -0.20) was directionally consistent but varied in magnitude by study design and study quality (Tables 3 and 4). Specifically, achievement deficit with Schistosoma infection was noted in the pooled estimate derived from four intervention studies, but the magnitude of this association was lower and not statistically robust (SMD = -0.35, 95%CI: -0.71, 0.01; Table 4). Compared to uninfected or PZQ-treated children, the infection or non-treatment associated deficit in scholastic attainment was directionally consistent across different Schistosoma species, although the magnitude of effect was higher for infection with S. haematobium (SMD = -0.62; 95% CI:-1.09, -0.14) than for infection with S. mansoni (SMD = -0.22; 95% CI:-0.40, -0.05). Among observational study designs, scholastic achievement deficit was statistically robust and of larger magnitude (n = 12 studies; SMD = -0.65, 95% CI: -1.12, -0.17, Table 4). Similarly, Schistosoma infection was not associated with deficits in scholastic achievement among studies identified as low risk of bias (n = 6 studies; SMD = -0.08, 95%CI: -0.21, 0.02; Table 4). The estimates of infection-related deficit in scholastic achievement increased when study quality was lower: for studies with high risk of bias (n = 7) the SMD = -0.84 (95% CI:-1.52, -0.16); for studies with very high risk of bias (n = 7) SMD = -0.92 95% CI: -2.1, 0.28) although the estimated difference was statistically imprecise (i.e., with a wider CI) among the studies with greatest risk of bias.

Impact of Schistosoma infection on psychometrically evaluated cognitive domains

We found Schistosoma infection-associated deficits in memory tests (n = 8 studies; SMD = -0.28, 95% CI: -0.52, -0.04; Table 2). Similarly, Schistosoma infection was associated with small-to-moderate deficits in learning tests (n = 6 studies; SMD = -0.39, 95%CI: -0.70, -0.09; Table 2). The Schistosoma infection-related deficits in memory tests were directionally consistent by study design; although separate pooled estimates for interventional studies (n = 4 studies; SMD = -0.36, 95% CI: -0.61, 0.09) and observational studies (n = 4 studies; SMD = -0.15, 95% CI: -0.34, 0.03) were not statistically robust. For Schistosoma infection-related association with learning, pooled estimates suggested the presence of deficits for infected/non-dewormed vs. uninfected/praziquantel-treated children (n = 6 studies; SMD = -0.39, 95% CI: -0.70, -0.09). However, the magnitude of this association differed according to study design. Here, larger pooled standardized differences were realized for interventional studies (n = 2 studies; SMD = -0.79, 95% CI:-1.19, -0.39) than for observational studies (n = 4 studies; SMD = -0.18, 95% CI: -0.35, -0.01). Schistosoma infection was not significantly associated with performance on tests of reaction time (n = 6 studies; SMD = -0.06, 95% CI: -0.42, 0.30) or performance in tests of innate intelligence (n = 4 studies; SMD = -0.25, 95% CI: -0.57, 0.06). For all psychometrically-assessed cognitive domains, the overall findings were not sensitive to study quality. Of note, the majority of publications with psychometrically-evaluated endpoints had low risk of bias and produced pooled estimates similar in magnitude and direction to the overall results (Table 4).

Sensitivity analyses

High levels of heterogeneity were observed across included studies (P ≤ 0.03, I2 ≥ 74.8%; Tables 2 and 4) for all outcome measures, but there was no evidence of undue influence by individual studies or of publication bias among the included studies (Egger’s test, all P-value ≥ 0.142, Table 2). Overall, inferences based on pooled estimates were generally insensitive to differences in study design (Table 4), to the exclusion of individual studies (S2 Table), or to the influence of publication year (S1 Fig).

Discussion

Principal findings and interpretations

This systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive and educational impact of Schistosoma infection in school-aged children supports the hypothesis that infection is associated with reduced school-attendance, with deficits in scholastic achievement and deficits in memory and learning domains of psychometrically evaluated cognitive function. It has previously been conjectured that Schistosoma infection may affect school attendance, scholastic achievement, and cognitive function, either directly through via deposition of Schistosoma eggs within the central nervous system, via physical discomfort and subsequent distraction due to the presence of the worms, or indirectly, via iron-deficiency and malnutrition [3, 71, 72]. However, Schistosoma infection or non-treatment was not associated with performance in tests of innate intelligence or reaction time. Inferences based on most pooled estimates for psychometrically assessed endpoints were generally insensitive to study design, Schistosoma species, and risk of study bias. However, associations between infection and educational outcomes were sensitive to study design and study quality–especially estimates for impact on scholastic achievement. The association between infection and scholastic achievement was directionally similar and statistically robust regardless of Schistosoma species; however, average effects were substantially larger for S. haematobium compared to S. mansoni infection. Cohen’s criteria for effect size suggest that the average Schistosoma infection-related deficits in education, learning, and memory performance range from ‘small’ to ‘moderate’. S. haematobium infection was associated with relatively larger deficits in scholastic achievement.

Prior meta-analysis of four randomized controlled trials that evaluated cognitive impacts of STH infections–which sometimes co-occur with schistosomiasis, have reached a different conclusion about the cognitive and scholastic effects of STH infection and the impact of interval treatments for STH [25, 26]. Those reviews concluded that there was substantial evidence that deworming for soil-transmitted helminth infections does not yield a cognitive or educational benefit. We note that our approach differed from these prior STH-based meta-analyses on several grounds: a) we included both interventional and observational studies to take advantage of all research data available on this question, b) we evaluated Schistosoma-associated impacts on two domains of educational loss (attendance and achievement), c) we further defined domains of cognitive function based on psychometrically-assessed testing to include the following: learning, memory, attention, and intelligence and d) the intervention, as defined in this meta-analysis, denoted treatment for Schistosoma infection, whether or not the study was randomized.

We intentionally included data from all available epidemiologic studies–whether interventional or observational in design, in this first systematic review and meta-analysis of Schistosoma infection-related differences in cognitive and educational outcomes. The inclusion of all available evidence reflects current standards for clinical evidence-gathering to inform health policy, in order to shape clinical practice based on the ‘best available’ relevant information [23, 24]. Future randomized-controlled trials to address this question are expected to be limited in scope and may be considered ethically objectionable given the current widespread adoption of deworming for schistosomiasis and STH. The current adoption of ‘preventive chemotherapy’ guidelines has been based on helminth-associated adverse effects on anemia and child growth. In consequence, meta-analysis of available evidence, as performed in the present study, remains the most practical strategy to inform current policy. By this approach, we have identified ‘small to moderate’ infection-related deficits in education, learning, and memory performance using the Cohen’s criteria of effect size. However, such numerically small deficits (per Cohen’s criteria) may significantly underestimate clinical significance of infection for childhood development, as Schistosoma infection is an exposure affecting millions of children in endemic regions. Hence, small to moderate deficits at the individual level may amount to large and important differences in disease burden at the population level [73, 74]. Our interpretation of SMD estimates is ultimately grounded in the importance of summary measures for clarifying existing knowledge gaps regarding the relationship of Schistosoma infections to respective outcomes, and the expected benefit of systematically lowering infection-related deficits in the millions of children at risk.

As a limitation of our approach, we acknowledge the possibility of residual confounding and bias in the primary literature, given that majority of included studies were observational or non-randomized intervention trials. Only two of the 30 included studies used an RCT design, thus a sensitivity analysis based on RCT vs. non-RCT study design was not possible. However, as part of sensitivity analysis we evaluated the potential for differences in pooled estimates based on our expanded definition of intervention as including longitudinal studies that included praziquantel treatment. Investigation of pooled estimate sensitivity by region of study, the year of publication, and by risk of bias did not result in materially different statistical inferences. Specific investigation of publication bias suggests that any possible greater likelihood of publishing positive studies did not unduly influence observed findings. Our approach of including both interventional and non-interventional studies is consistent with theoretical and empirical evidence that meta-analyses based on observational studies generally produce estimates of effect similar to those from meta-analyses based on randomized controlled trials, and that a priori exclusion of observational studies in systematic reviews is inappropriate and inconsistent with the evidence-based medical decision-making approach [75, 76]. In addition, the often restrictive inclusion criteria and short follow-up duration in RCTs could easily result in outcomes largely different from when the same interventions are applied to a general population.

Despite the intuitive appeal of our cognitive domain based evaluation, we acknowledge the critique that our classification of psychometric instruments by domain required some level of subjectivity, especially for tools that capture performance across multiple domains. We explicitly identified the instruments used in each study and, based on literature description of the major cognitive domain assessed by each tool, combined related tools into four separate domains. Ultimately, each instrument was assigned to one cognitive domain only. We have described the logic for our choices in the supplementary information (S1 Table) to provide a basis for further discussion and give sufficient context for critical evaluation of our approach in developing future studies.

Our decision to combine psychometric evaluations of cognitive functions in four domains (based on the primary capacity being tested) is a strength of our empirical approach. By so doing, we recognize that Schistosoma infection may not have equal impact on all cognitive domains. For example, to the extent that innate intelligence is strongly influenced by fixed or heritable factors, we did not anticipate infection related differences on tests of ‘intelligence quotient’. Unlike intelligence tests, we considered the other tests of memory, learning, and reaction time to be more sensitive to infection, and thus modifiable by presence/non-treatment vs. absence/treatment of Schistosoma infection. Unexpectedly, reaction time was not associated with Schistosoma infections. However, our findings of infection-related reductions in learning and memory tests are consistent with our hypotheses that these cognitive domains are sensitive to adverse environmental perturbations–including Schistosoma infection.

Remaining gaps, and recommendations for future research

The finding of infection-related cognitive deficits and educational loss reported here is clinically and health policy-relevant for mitigating the cognitive and functional morbidities of Schistosoma spp. infection in children. The ‘small-to-moderate’ effects demonstrated may, in reality, be an underestimate of the lifetime impact of Schistosoma infection on personal performance (as affected via ultimately irreversible educational and cognitive losses). Typical epidemiologic studies necessarily include only a constrained portion of the relevant etiologic period. Among school-age children, Schistosoma infection is often effectively already chronic and/or recurrent, with reinfection rates extremely high in the absence of meaningful environmental interventions to reduce re-infection following treatment. The cumulative cognitive and educational impact of persistent infection may not be adequately captured by the relatively short-term investigations of treatment impact included in this meta-analysis.

In schistosomiasis-endemic regions, many children are infected by age two and remain chronically infected through school-age and late adolescence [9]. Under current national Schistosoma control treatment guidelines, preschool-age children are not treated as part of routine deworming programs for STH or schistosomiasis [1, 6, 30]. These children may therefore suffer cumulative damage to their health and function that is currently not reflected in most short-term study outcomes (or this meta-analysis). Of note, recent investigations have demonstrated the safety and efficacy of praziquantel for treatment of Schistosoma infection in preschool children [9]. The existence of an adverse developmental impact of Schistosoma infection on cognitive/educational domains would be a major justification for expanding the age-bracket of children who should be treated with praziquantel. Currently, evidence suggests that the timing of infection across a life path is especially consequential in terms of the severity of cognitive and physiologic impairments experienced [77, 78]. Future investigations evaluating the relative differences in cognitive outcomes for pre-school children with and without Schistosoma infection will be important for understanding the magnitude of potential impact of better prevention of Schistosoma infection.

It is currently unknown whether the cognitive and educational loss associated with Schistosoma infection can be reversed with treatment alone. The fact that infection often occurs in the context of malnutrition, coincident parasitic infections, and extreme poverty suggests that cognitive remediation efforts will need to be multi-faceted, using an integrated disease management framework. We expect that educational and cognitive interventions will be most effective if initiated earlier in life and that the package of interventions may need to include remedial instruction, the prevention of reinfection for treated/cured children, management of comorbid health conditions, and interventions for improvement of nutritional status.

Policy implications

Our investigation suggests that Schistosoma infection/non-treatment is associated with educational and cognitive loss. Our findings further suggest a definite cognitive and educational benefit of anti-schistosomal deworming among school-age children. Future complex intervention studies of early childhood interventions, focused on improving child well-being and cognitive potential, are needed to determine to what extent these observed deficits are preventable or reversible. Interventions that employ an integrated disease management framework will likely identify cost-efficiencies for leveraging existing disease and nutrition treatment programs in helminth-affected regions.

Supporting information

S1 Text. PROSPERO protocol CRD42016040052 registered for this study.

(PDF)

S2 Text. Schematic of search strategy for the systematic review.

(DOCX)

S3 Text. Summary of supplemental data.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Classification of individual study assessments into domains based on psychometric evaluation of cognitive function & education-related assessments.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. The impact of Schistosoma infection/non-treatment on educational loss, memory, and learning domains with serial exclusion of individual studies–a sensitivity analysis quantifying the influence of individual studies.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. PRISMA checklist.

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Sensitivity analysis of the impact of publication year on pooled estimates for educational and cognitive loss domains.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the invaluable contribution of Dr. Daniel Colley from the University of Georgia, Department of Microbiology and the Center for Tropical and Emerging Global Diseases in reviewing early drafts of this work.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by Schistosomiasis Consortium for Operational Research and Evaluation (SCORE), University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.WHO, Prevention and control of schistosomiasis and soil-transmitted helminthiasis. WHO Technical Report Series, 2002(912). [PubMed]
  • 2.Colley D.G., et al. , Human schistosomiasis. Lancet, 2014. 383(9936): p. 2253–64. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61949-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.King C.H., Dickman K., and Tisch D.J., Reassessment of the cost of chronic helmintic infection: a meta-analysis of disability-related outcomes in endemic schistosomiasis. Lancet, 2005. 365(9470): p. 1561–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66457-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Steinmann P., et al. , Schistosomiasis and water resources development: systematic review, meta-analysis, and estimates of people at risk. Lancet Infect Dis, 2006. 6(7): p. 411–25. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(06)70521-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Murray C.J., et al. , Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 291 diseases and injuries in 21 regions, 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet, 2012. 380(9859): p. 2197–223. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61689-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Colley D.G., Morbidity control of schistosomiasis by mass drug administration: how can we do it best and what will it take to move on to elimination? Trop Med Health, 2014. 42(2 Suppl): p. 25–32. doi: 10.2149/tmh.2014-S04 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.King C.H., Parasites and poverty: the case of schistosomiasis. Acta Trop, 2010. 113(2): p. 95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.actatropica.2009.11.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.van der Werf M.J., et al. , Quantification of clinical morbidity associated with schistosomiasis infection in sub-Saharan Africa. Acta Trop, 2003. 86(2–3): p. 125–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Nalugwa A., et al. , Single versus double dose praziquantel comparison on efficacy and schistosoma mansoni re-Infection in preschool-age children in Uganda: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS Negl Trop Dis, 2015. 9(5): p. e0003796 doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0003796 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Stothard J.R., et al. , Schistosomiasis in African infants and preschool children: let them now be treated! Trends Parasitol, 2013. 29(4): p. 197–205. doi: 10.1016/j.pt.2013.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Erko B., et al. , Efficacy and side effects of praziquantel in the treatment of schistosomiasis mansoni in schoolchildren in Shesha Kekele Elementary School, Wondo Genet, Southern Ethiopia. Asian Pac J Trop Biomed, 2012. 2(3): p. 235–9. doi: 10.1016/S2221-1691(12)60049-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Friedman J.F., Kanzaria H.K., and McGarvey S.T., Human schistosomiasis and anemia: the relationship and potential mechanisms. Trends Parasitol, 2005. 21(8): p. 386–92. doi: 10.1016/j.pt.2005.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Andrade G., et al. , Decline in infection-related morbidities following drug-mediated reductions in the intensity of Schistosoma infection: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Negl Trop Dis, 2017. 11(2): p. e0005372 doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0005372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Coutinho H.M., et al. , Nutritional status improves after treatment of schistosoma japonicum-infected children and adolescents. J Nutr, 2006. 136(1): p. 183–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bustinduy A.L., et al. , Measuring fitness of Kenyan children with polyparasitic infections using the 20-meter shuttle run test as a morbidity metric. Plos Neglected Tropical Diseases, 2011. 5(7): p. e1213 doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0001213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Terer C.C., et al. , Evaluation of the Health-related Quality of Life of children in Schistosoma haematobium-endemic communities in Kenya: A cross-sectional study. Plos Neglected Tropical Diseases, 2013. 7(3): p. e2106 doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0002106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.King C.H. and Dangerfield-Cha M., The unacknowledged impact of chronic schistosomiasis. Chronic Illn, 2008. 4(1): p. 65–79. doi: 10.1177/1742395307084407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Olveda D.U., et al. , Bilharzia in the Philippines: past, present, and future. Int J Infect Dis, 2014. 18: p. 52–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ijid.2013.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Asahi H. and Stadecker M.J., Analysis of egg antigens inducing hepatic lesions in schistosomiasis infection. Parasitol Int, 2003. 52(4): p. 361–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Papier K., et al. , Childhood malnutrition and parasitic helminth interactions. Clin Infect Dis, 2014. 59(2): p. 234–43. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Brooker S., et al. , Epidemiology of single and multiple species of helminth infections among school children in Busia District, Kenya. East Afr Med J, 2000. 77(3): p. 157–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bundy D.A., et al. , Deworming and development: asking the right questions, asking the questions right. PLoS Negl Trop Dis, 2009. 3(1): p. e362 doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0000362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Davey C., et al. , Re-analysis of health and educational impacts of a school-based deworming programme in western Kenya: a statistical replication of a cluster quasi-randomized stepped-wedge trial. Int J Epidemiol, 2015. 44(5): p. 1581–92. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hicks J.H., Kremer M., and Miguel E., Commentary: Deworming externalities and schooling impacts in Kenya: a comment on Aiken et al. (2015) and Davey et al. (2015). Int J Epidemiol, 2015. 44(5): p. 1593–6. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Taylor-Robinson D.C., Jones A.P., and Garner P., Deworming drugs for treating soil-transmitted intestinal worms in children: effects on growth and school performance. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 2007(4): p. CD000371 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD000371.pub3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Taylor-Robinson D.C., et al. , Deworming drugs for soil-transmitted intestinal worms in children: effects on nutritional indicators, haemoglobin and school performance. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 2012. 7: p. CD000371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Taylor-Robinson D.C., et al. , Deworming drugs for soil-transmitted intestinal worms in children: effects on nutritional indicators, haemoglobin, and school performance. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 2015(7): p. CD000371 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD000371.pub6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Miguel E. and Kremer M., Worms: Identifying impacts on education and health in the presence of treatment externalities. Econometrica, 2004. 72(1): p. 159–217. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Blair C., School readiness. Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. Am Psychol, 2002. 57(2): p. 111–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.WHO, Strategy Development and Monitoring for Parasitic Diseases and Vector Control Team. Deworming: The Millennium Development Goals. The evidence is in: deworming helps meet the Millennium Development Goals. 2005, World Health Organization: Geneva. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hotez P.J., New antipoverty drugs, vaccines, and diagnostics: a research agenda for the US President's Global Health Initiative (GHI). PLoS Negl Trop Dis, 2011. 5(5): p. e1133 doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0001133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bustinduy A.L., et al. , Population Pharmacokinetics and Pharmacodynamics of Praziquantel in Ugandan Children with Intestinal Schistosomiasis: Higher Dosages Are Required for Maximal Efficacy. MBio, 2016. 7(4). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Deary I.J., Differences in mental abilities. BMJ, 1998. 317(7174): p. 1701–3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hozo S.P., Djulbegovic B., and Hozo I., Estimating the mean and variance from the median, range, and the size of a sample. BMC Med Res Methodol, 2005. 5: p. 13 doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-5-13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Imputing standard deviations for changes from baseline. [December 20, 2017]; Available from: http://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org/chapter_16/16_1_3_2_imputing_standard_deviations_for_changes_from_baseline.htm.
  • 36.Cohen J., Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. rev. ed. 1977, New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Higgins J.P. and Thompson S.G., Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med, 2002. 21(11): p. 1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Berkey C.S., et al. , A random-effects regression model for meta-analysis. Stat Med, 1995. 14(4): p. 395–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Schmidt F.L., Oh I.S., and Hayes T.L., Fixed- versus random-effects models in meta-analysis: model properties and an empirical comparison of differences in results. Br J Math Stat Psychol, 2009. 62(Pt 1): p. 97–128. doi: 10.1348/000711007X255327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sterne J.A., Gavaghan D., and Egger M., Publication and related bias in meta-analysis: power of statistical tests and prevalence in the literature. J Clin Epidemiol, 2000. 53(11): p. 1119–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Stroup D.F., et al. , Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA, 2000. 283(15): p. 2008–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lo C.K., Mertz D., and Loeb M., Newcastle-Ottawa Scale: comparing reviewers' to authors' assessments. BMC Med Res Methodol, 2014. 14: p. 45 doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-45 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Stang A., Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur J Epidemiol, 2010. 25(9): p. 603–5. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Berhe N., Myrvang B., and Gundersen S.G., Gastro-intestinal symptoms associated with intense Schistosoma mansoni infection affect class-attentiveness of schoolchildren in Ethiopia. Acta Tropica, 2009. 110(1): p. 52–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Castle W.M., Clarke V.D.V., and Hendrikz E., Effects of subclinical bilharziasis on mental-ability in schoolchildren. South African Medical Journal, 1974. 48(48): p. 2035–2038. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.de Clercq D., et al. , The relationship between Schistosoma haematobium infection and school performance and attendance in Bamako, Mali. Ann Trop Med Parasitol, 1998. 92(8): p. 851–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ejezie G.C. and Ade-Serrano M.A., Schistosoma haematobium in Ajara community of Badagry, Nigeria. A study on prevalence, intensity and morbidity from infection among primary school children. Trop Geogr Med, 1981. 33(2): p. 175–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ekanem E.E., et al. , Effect of Schistosoma haematobium infection on the physical growth and school performance of Nigerian children. Cent Afr J Med, 1994. 40(2): p. 38–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Epstein E.H. and Weisbrod B.A., Parasitic diseases and academic performance of schoolchildren. Social and Economic Studies, 1974. 23(4): p. 551–570. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ezeamama A.E., et al. , Helminth infection and cognitive impairment among Filipino children. American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 2005. 72(5): p. 540–548. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Goldin D. and Barclay R., Schistosomiasis in Rural Zambia. Annals of Tropical Medicine and Parasitology, 1972. 66(2): p. 193–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Haycock D.C. and Schutte C.H.J., Schistosoma-haematobium infection and scholastic attainment amongst black schoolchildren. South African Journal of Science, 1983. 79(9): p. 370–373. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Husein M.H., et al. , Who misses out with school-based health programmes? A study of schistosomiasis control in Egypt. Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 1996. 90(4): p. 362–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Jukes M.C., et al. , Heavy schistosomiasis associated with poor short-term memory and slower reaction times in Tanzanian schoolchildren. Trop Med Int Health, 2002. 7(2): p. 104–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Rasoamanamihaja C.F., et al. , Baseline prevalence and intensity of schistosomiasis at sentinel sites in Madagascar: Informing a national control strategy. Parasit Vectors, 2016. 9: p. 50 doi: 10.1186/s13071-016-1337-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.El-Hawey A.M., et al. , Impacts of schistosomiasis on scholastic achievement of school children in two Egyptian rural communities. Egypt J Comm Med., 1990. 7: p. 27–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Beasley N.M., et al. , The health of enrolled and non enrolled children of school age in Tanga, Tanzania. Acta Trop, 2000. 76(3): p. 223–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Fentiman A., Hall A., and Bundy D., Health and cultural factors associated with enrolment in basic education: a study in rural Ghana. Soc Sci Med, 2001. 52(3): p. 429–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Loveridge F.G., Ross W.F., and Blair D.M., Schistosomiasis; the effect of the disease on educational attainment. S Afr Med J, 1948. 22(7): p. 260–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Mekheimar S.I. and Talaat M., School non-enrollment and its relation with health and schistosomiasis knowledge, attitudes and practices in rural Egypt. East Mediterr Health J, 2005. 11(3): p. 392–401. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Nazel M.W., et al. , Schistosoma mansoni infection and cognitive functions of primary school children, in Kafr El Sheikh, Egypt. J Egypt Public Health Assoc, 1999. 74(1–2): p. 97–119. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Tiruneh M., et al. , Schistosomiasis mansoni in school attenders and non-attenders in Northwest Ethiopia. Ethiop J. Health Dev., 2001. 15(2): p. 117–123. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Useh M.F. and Ejezie G.C., School-based schistosomiasis control programmes: a comparative study on the prevalence and intensity of urinary schistosomiasis among Nigerian school-age children in and out of school. Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 1999. 93(4): p. 387–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Ezeamama A.E., et al. , Treatment for Schistosoma japonicum, reduction of intestinal parasite load, and cognitive test score improvements in school-aged children. Plos Neglected Tropical Diseases, 2012. 6(5): p. e1634 doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0001634 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hurlimann E., et al. , Effect of deworming on school-aged children's physical fitness, cognition and clinical parameters in a malaria-helminth co-endemic area of Cote d'Ivoire. BMC Infect Dis, 2014. 14: p. 411 doi: 10.1186/1471-2334-14-411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Jordan P. and Randall K., Bilharziasis in Tanganyika—Observations on Its effects and effects of treatment in schoolchildren. Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 1962. 65(1): p. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kimura E., et al. , Effects of Schistosoma haematobium infection on mental test scores of Kenyan school children. Trop Med Parasitol, 1992. 43(3): p. 155–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Meremikwu M.M., et al. , Treatment of schistosomiasis haematobium with praziquantel in children: Its effect on educational performance in rural Nigeria. Trop Med, 2000. 42(1): p. 39–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Nokes C., et al. , Evidence for an improvement in cognitive function following treatment of Schistosoma japonicum infection in Chinese primary schoolchildren. Am J Trop Med Hyg, 1999. 60(4): p. 556–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Grigorenko E.L., Sternberg R.J., Jukes M., Alcock K., Lambo J., Ngorosho D., Nokes C., and Bundy D.A., Effects of antiparasitic treatment on dynamically and statically tested cognitive skills over time. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2006. 27: p. 499–526. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Kvalsvig J.D., The effects of Schistosomiasis haematobium on the activity of school children. J Trop Med Hyg, 1986. 89(2): p. 85–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Watkins W.E. and Pollitt E., "Stupidity or worms": do intestinal worms impair mental performance? Psychol Bull, 1997. 121(2): p. 171–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Ellis P.D. Thresholds for interpreting effect sizes. 2009. [cited 2015 09/06]; Available from: http://www.polyu.edu.hk/mm/effectsizefaqs/thresholds_for_interpreting_effect_sizes2.html. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Glass G.V., McGaw B., and Smith M.L., Meta-Analysis in Social Research. 1981, Beverly Hills: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Manchikanti L., et al. , Evidence-based medicine, systematic reviews, and guidelines in interventional pain management: part 6. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses of observational studies. Pain Physician, 2009. 12(5): p. 819–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Shrier I., et al. , Should meta-analyses of interventions include observational studies in addition to randomized controlled trials? A critical examination of underlying principles. Am J Epidemiol, 2007. 166(10): p. 1203–9. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hagberg H., Gressens P., and Mallard C., Inflammation during fetal and neonatal life: implications for neurologic and neuropsychiatric disease in children and adults. Ann Neurol, 2012. 71(4): p. 444–57. doi: 10.1002/ana.22620 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Hagberg H., Peebles D., and Mallard C., Models of white matter injury: comparison of infectious, hypoxic-ischemic, and excitotoxic insults. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev, 2002. 8(1): p. 30–8. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.10007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Text. PROSPERO protocol CRD42016040052 registered for this study.

(PDF)

S2 Text. Schematic of search strategy for the systematic review.

(DOCX)

S3 Text. Summary of supplemental data.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Classification of individual study assessments into domains based on psychometric evaluation of cognitive function & education-related assessments.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. The impact of Schistosoma infection/non-treatment on educational loss, memory, and learning domains with serial exclusion of individual studies–a sensitivity analysis quantifying the influence of individual studies.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. PRISMA checklist.

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Sensitivity analysis of the impact of publication year on pooled estimates for educational and cognitive loss domains.

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


Articles from PLoS Neglected Tropical Diseases are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES