Abstract
Modulation of stimulus-response gain and stability of spontaneous (unstimulated) firing are both important for neural computation. However, biologically plausible mechanisms that allow these distinct functional capabilities to coexist in the same neuron are poorly defined. Low-threshold, inactivating (A-type) K+ currents (IA) are found in many biological neurons and are historically known for enabling low-frequency firing. By performing simulations using a conductance-based model neuron, here we show that biologically plausible shifts in IA conductance and inactivation kinetics produce dissociated effects on gain and intrinsic firing. This enables IA to regulate gain without major changes in intrinsic firing rate. Tuning IA properties may thus represent a previously unsuspected single-current mechanism of silent gain control in neurons.
1. Introduction
The ability of neurons to alter their gain, that is, to change the steepness of the relationship between input stimulus and output firing rate, is considered crucial for neural computation (Salinas & Thier, 2000; Salinas & Sejnowski, 2001). However, effective neural processing is also thought to rely on the ability to maintain stable electrical activity in the absence of stimuli (Turrigiano & Nelson, 2000; Davis & Bezprozvanny, 2001; Marder & Prinz, 2002). Thus, it is of interest to elucidate biologically feasible mechanisms that allow modulation of stimulus-response gain independently of intrinsic neuronal activity. Hitherto proposed mechanisms of achieving such “silent” gain modulation (i.e., changing stimulus responsiveness without changing neural activity in the absence of stimuli) rely on elaborate balancing of electrically opposite sets of synaptic inputs (Chance, Abbott, & Reyes, 2002) or somatic voltage-gated currents (Burdakov, 2005). While this can, in theory, achieve silent gain modulation, there is little evidence to suggest that such balanced changes in coincident and opposing sets of currents can be triggered by biologically relevant stimuli.
On the other hand, there is a large body of experimental evidence showing that endogenous stimuli (such as second messengers and neuromodulators) can tune the properties of individual voltage-gated currents. However, little is known about whether changes in a single current can enable silent gain modulation. Here we have focused on a subthreshold current expressed in diverse types of neurons, the low-voltage-activated (LVA) transient A-type K+ current (IA) (Birnbaum et al., 2004). We explored the extent to which experimentally reported variations in LVA IA properties can produce silent gain modulation.
2. Methods
To study the effects of IA on neuronal gain, we used a previously published single-compartment, Hodgkin-Huxley-type model neuron that comprises an LVA IA, six other membrane currents (INa, ICaS, IKCa, IKd, IH, and Ileak), and an intracellular calcium buffer (Prinz, Thirumalai, & Marder, 2003). All maximal conductances, channel kinetics, and reversal potentials (Erev) were as in the tonically firing model neuron in Prinz et al. (2003), except those for the LVA IA, which were changed as indicated. The membrane area was constant at 0.628 × 10−3 cm2 for all simulations. For simplicity of analysis, changes to parameters were constrained to keep the firing pattern tonic and regular and to avoid silence or bursts. Simulations were performed using Matlab stiff systems numerical integrator ode23s at a time resolution of 25 μs.
Input-output gain was measured as the average gradient of the relationship between neuronal firing rate and driving current (as presented in Hz per nA) in the range of 0 to 0.4 nA driving current. In practice, this captured the steepest part of the tuning curve. The firing frequency plotted in these graphs was assessed 1 second after a change in the driving stimulus to avoid any confounding effects of adaptation.
We focused on two types of input stimulus: a sustained driving stimulus of variable intensity in the form of current, as occurs in a large class of biological neurons (Cowley et al., 2001; Burdakov et al., 2006) (see Figures 1 and 2), and a current-based synaptic input model (see Figure 3). The synaptic model was designed to introduce a series of impulses of 0.75 nA magnitude, each lasting 2 ms, at random intervals distributed by the Poisson distribution (see Figure 3A). The intensity of synaptic input was varied by changing the mean interpulse interval (λ) between 1 and 30 ms.
3. Results
We examined three types of biologically plausible changes in LVA IA properties. First, we varied IA conductance (gA). Increasing gA progressively reduced both the gain and the spontaneous firing rate (see Figures 1A–D). Given that the speed (τh) and voltage dependence of LVA IA inactivation can vary in biological neurons (Lozovaya, Vulfius, Ilyin, & Krasts, 1993; Muller, Hallermann, & Swandulla, 1999; Burdakov & Ashcroft, 2002), we next examined the relationship between gA, gain, and firing rate at (1) intermediate and voltage dependent τh (from Prinz et al., 2003) (see Figure 1B); (2) slow voltage-independent τh (150 ms) (see Figure 1C); and (3) fast voltage-independent τh (25 ms) (see Figure 1D). Increasing gA progressively reduced both gain and baseline unstimulated firing rate under all of these conditions. Thus, gA can modulate gain, but this modulation is not entirely silent, as the spontaneous firing rate is also affected.
Second, we explored the effects of changes in the speed of IA inactivation. For simplicity, in these simulations we made τh voltage independent, as reported for some biological neurons (Burdakov & Ashcroft, 2002). Varying τh within a biologically plausible range of values had little effect on our measure of gain or on tonic firing rate, irrespective of whether the V1/2 of inactivation was depolarized (see Figure 1E) or hyperpolarized (see Figure 1F).
Third, we varied the voltage dependence of IA inactivation. Shifting the voltage at which inactivation is half-maximal (V1/2) toward positive potentials reduced both gain and baseline firing rate (see Figures 2B–2E; see Figure 2A for an example of how the neuron processed the same input for different values of V1/2). However, the changes in gain and intrinsic firing rate occurred at different ranges of V1/2. In the hyperpolarized range (V1/2 < −75 mV), as reported experimentally for some neuronal subtypes (Song et al., 1998; Starodub & Wood, 2000; Schone, Venner, Knowles, Karnani, & Burdakov, 2011), there were large effects on gain but not baseline firing (see Figures 2C–2E). This differential modulation of gain versus firing rate persisted at the three different types of τh, as described above: (1) intermediate and voltage-dependent τh (from Prinz et al., 2003) (see Figure 2C); (2) slow voltage-independent τh (150 ms) (see Figure 2D); and (3) fast voltage-independent τh (25 ms) (see Figure 2E). This suggests that changing the voltage dependence of IA inactivation can lead to near-silent gain modulation regardless of the speed of inactivation within a biologically plausible range of inactivation V1/2 (see Figure 2A for an example of raw data).
To explore whether these findings hold true under a noisy input, as well as under sustained current inputs, as examined in Figures 1 and 2, we also simulated the effects of changing gA and V1/2 of inactivation on firing driven by a fluctuating, synaptic-like, excitatory input (see Figure 3A and section 2). Similar to the simulations using a sustained driving input (see Figure 1), these simulations showed that increasing gA progressively reduced both gain and baseline firing rate, but this was not entirely silent (see Figures 3B and 3C). On the other hand, again in concordance with the sustained-input simulations (see Figure 2), shifting the V1/2 of inactivation to more hyperpolarized potentials increased gain with little or no effect on baseline firing (see Figures 3D and 3E).
In view of these results, we next analyzed IA current data in order to determine whether we could elucidate a mechanism of differential gain modulation. We measured the average IA current per action potential, (averaged over 10 action potentials) under three physiological conditions: IA inactivation V1/2 of −55 mV (baseline), −80 mV, and −105 mV (see Figure 4A). We found that the relationship between average IA current per spike and firing rate was considerably steeper for depolarized V1/2 than for hyperpolarized V1/2 (see Figure 4A). In contrast, the current-frequency relationship for IH (an electrophysiological opponent of IA) was similarly steep for all three values of V1/2 (see Figure 4B). Thus, depolarizing shifts in inactivation V1/2 would affect high-frequency firing more than lower frequency firing. This would account for the silent-like gain modulation we observe in Figures 2A and 2B.
4. Discussion
Our results show that both gA and IA inactivation V1/2 are capable of modulating gain and intrinsic (baseline) firing differentially, to produce either non-silent (gA; see Figures 1A–1D), or near-silent (V1/2 of inactivation; see Figure 2) gain modulation. These effects were consistent across a range of biologically plausible shifts in the voltage dependence and speed of inactivation (Lozovaya et al., 1993; Muller et al., 1999; Burdakov & Ashcroft, 2002), and also for two types of driving stimulus: a sustained current input, such as that triggered by certain neuromodulators (Cowley et al., 2001; Burdakov et al., 2006) (see Figures 1 and 2), and a fluctuating synaptic-like input (see Figure 3). We found τh itself incapable of controlling the gain or baseline firing rate at either depolarized or hyperpolarized potentials (see Figures 1E and 1F). This suggests that while the speed of IA inactivation presumably modulates temporal summation of phasic inputs, it modulates neither firing nor gain under the conditions studied here (tonic firing, tonic driving current).
We propose that the differential effects of changes in IA parameters (demonstrated for inactivation V1/2) on gain versus intrinsic firing arise because the effects of IA on firing depend on the average membrane potential of the neuron, which becomes more positive as the firing rate increases from a low (intrinsic) value to higher (stimulus-driven) values. Depolarizing IA V1/2 of inactivation affects the stimulus-driven (higher frequency) firing more that the intrinsic (lower frequency) firing (see Figure 4A). In our model, this steepening of the frequency dependence of IA (see Figure 4A) was not accompanied by a corresponding steepening in its interspike opponent, IH (see Figure 4B), showing that the kinetics of the two currents do not necessarily ensure coupled changes in their frequency dependencies.
We speculate that the IA V1/2 of inactivation could be more capable of silent gain modulation than gA because these parameters cause different transformations in the IA “permissive voltage window” (see Figure 4C). Shifts in gA (1 in Figure 4C) will enhance IA at all potentials at which it is active. However, shifts in the IA V1/2 of inactivation (2 in Figure 4C) would differentially enhance IA at positive, but not negative, potentials because it moves the right-side (inactivation-set) boundary but not the left-side (activation-set) boundary of the IA voltage window (see Figure 4C).
In summary, these results suggest that changes in IA kinetics, especially in IA inactivation V1/2, can produce silent-like gain modulation without a requirement for any balancing changes in other currents. To the best of our knowledge, such a cellular mechanism of silent gain control has not previously been described. This mechanism is biologically plausible and could be of general physiological importance, especially considering that the LVA IA is widely expressed in many biological neurons and that its voltage dependence of inactivation has been reported to be tunable by a number of neuromodulators (Lozovaya et al., 1993; Muller et al., 1999; Birnbaum et al., 2004).
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the ERC (FP7 grant to D. B.), the Wellcome Trust TMAT program, and the MB/PhD program at the University of Cambridge (PhD studentship to A.P.).
References
- Birnbaum SG, Varga AW, Yuan LL, Anderson AE, Sweatt JD, Schrader LA. Structure and function of Kv4-family transient potassium channels. Physiol Rev. 2004;84:803–833. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00039.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdakov D. Gain control by concerted changes in IA and IH conductances. Neural Comp. 2005;17:991–995. doi: 10.1162/0899766053491841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdakov D, Ashcroft FM. Cholecystokinin tunes firing of an electrically distinct subset of arcuate nucleus neurons by activating A-type potassium channels. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6380–6387. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06380.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdakov D, Jensen LT, Alexopoulos H, Williams RH, Fearon IM, O’Kelly I, et al. Tandem-pore K+ channels mediate inhibition of orexin neurons by glucose. Neuron. 2006;50:711–722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD. Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron. 2002;35:773–782. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00820-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowley MA, Smart JL, Rubinstein M, Cerdan MG, Diano S, Horvath TL, et al. Leptin activates anorexigenic POMC neurons through a neural network in the arcuate nucleus. Nature. 2001;411:480–484. doi: 10.1038/35078085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis GW, Bezprozvanny I. Maintaining the stability of neural function: A homeostatic hypothesis. Annu Rev Physiol. 2001;63:847–869. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.63.1.847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lozovaya NA, Vulfius CA, Ilyin VI, Krasts IV. Intracellular ATP modifies the voltage dependence of the fast transient outward K+ current in Lymnaea stagnalis neurones. J Physiol. 1993;464:441–455. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder E, Prinz AA. Modeling stability in neuron and network function: The role of activity in homeostasis. Bioessays. 2002;24:1145–1154. doi: 10.1002/bies.10185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller W, Hallermann S, Swandulla D. Opioidergic modulation of voltage-activated K+ currents in magnocellular neurons of the supraoptic nucleus in rat. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81:1617–1625. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prinz AA, Thirumalai V, Marder E. The functional consequences of changes in the strength and duration of synaptic inputs to oscillatory neurons. J Neurosci. 2003;23:943–954. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00943.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salinas E, Sejnowski TJ. Gain modulation in the central nervous system: Where behavior, neurophysiology, and computation meet. Neuroscientist. 2001;7:430–440. doi: 10.1177/107385840100700512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salinas E, Thier P. Gain modulation: A major computational principle of the central nervous system. Neuron. 2000;27:15–21. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00004-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schone C, Venner A, Knowles D, Karnani MM, Burdakov D. Di-chotomous cellular properties of mouse orexin/hypocretin neurons. J Physiol. 2011;589:2767–2779. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.208637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song WJ, Tkatch T, Baranauskas G, Ichinohe N, Kitai ST, Surmeier DJ. Somatodendritic depolarization-activated potassium currents in rat neostriatal cholinergic interneurons are predominantly of the A type and attributable to coexpression of Kv4.2 and Kv4.1 subunits. J Neurosci. 1998;18:3124–3137. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-09-03124.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starodub AM, Wood JD. A-type potassium current in myenteric neurons from guinea-pig small intestine. Neuroscience. 2000;99:389–396. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00196-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB. Hebb and homeostasis in neuronal plasticity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2000;10:358–364. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00091-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]