Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jan 16.
Published in final edited form as: Int Psychogeriatr. 2012 May 22;24(10):1536–1552. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212000737

The problem with “problem behaviors”: a systematic review of the association between individual patient behavioral and psychological symptoms and caregiver depression and burden within the dementia patient–caregiver dyad

Katherine Ornstein 1, Joseph E Gaugler 2
PMCID: PMC5769475  NIHMSID: NIHMS932456  PMID: 22612881

Abstract

Background

Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSDs) are routinely cited as important predictors of caregiver burden and depression. Although BPSDs include a wide variety of patient behaviors, they are routinely grouped together as one construct to differentiate them from cognitive symptoms of dementia. Determining the specific BPSDs that result in increased depression and burden for caregivers may elucidate the stress process for caregivers and facilitate the development of effective interventions for caregivers.

Methods

We conducted a systematic review of English-language articles published from 1990 to 2010 to determine whether there are known symptoms or symptom clusters which exert undue negative impact on caregiver depression and burden. Additionally, we review systems used for classifying BSPD symptom clusters and determine whether there have been any mechanisms studied by which individual BPSD symptoms negatively affect caregivers. Finally, we examine how the role of timing of symptoms has been examined within the literature.

Results

Thirty-five original research articles examined the impact of an individual behavior symptom on caregiver burden or depression/depressive symptoms. The studies had no consistent system for categorizing symptoms. Although depression, aggression, and sleep disturbances were the most frequently identified patient symptoms to impact negatively on caregivers, a wide range of symptoms was associated with caregiver burden and depression.

Conclusions

The evidence is not conclusive as to whether some symptoms are more important than others. The studies reviewed were largely exploratory relative to the differential impact of individual BPSDs and did not focus on testing causal mechanisms by which specific symptoms exert more impact on caregiver mental health than others. Future research may benefit from the re-conceptualization of BPSDs from the perspective of their impact on the caregiver to examine hypothesis-driven differences among BPSD symptom clusters.

Keywords: caregiving, Alzheimer’s disease, BPSD, neuropsychiatric symptoms, behavior problems

Introduction

Family caregiving of dementia patients has well-documented negative effects on the health and well-being of caregivers (Schulz et al., 1995; Pinquart and Sorensen, 2003b). Caregiver burden refers to the caregiver’s perceived emotional, social, and financial consequences of care provision (Zarit et al., 1980). Research on the health effects of dementia caregiving has largely been guided by the stress process model (Pearlin et al., 1990; Aneshensel et al., 1995), which conceptualizes caregiving as the proliferation, or spread, of stress from primary stressors (e.g. measures of dementia severity) to increased risk of morbidity including major depression or depressive symptomatology.

Researchers have consistently demonstrated associations between the psychiatric or behavioral disturbances related to dementia (i.e. the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia, or BPSDs) and negative outcomes for caregivers of patients with dementia including increased caregiver burden, stress, or depressive symptoms (Pinquart and Sorensen, 2003a; Black and Almeida, 2004). BPSDs are often referred to as “problem behaviors” or simply “behavioral disturbances” and include verbal and physical aggression, agitation, psychotic symptoms (e.g. hallucinations and delusions), sleep disturbances, depression, oppositional behavior, and wandering. These behaviors are reported to be more stressful for caregivers than cognitive and functional problems in the patient, perhaps due to the capricious nature of BPSD. While the functional and cognitive trajectories for the dementia patient follow an expected steady decline, behavior problems can ebb and flow, which can leave the caregiver less prepared to handle such behaviors adequately. Additionally, because they effectively alter the patient’s personality, BPSDs may serve as more dramatic reminders of the major changes that have occurred in the patient and the loss experienced by the caregiver. BPSDs are also associated with more caregiver anger and resentment toward the patient than other aspects of the disease (e.g. cognitive decline; Croog et al., 2006) suggesting that there is a negative emotional response to BPSDs, which could increase caregiver stress and negatively affect caregiver psychological health.

While BPSDs are increasingly recognized as important aspects of dementia that are associated with disease outcomes and require treatment, they are difficult to measure because symptoms are episodic and subject to environmental provocations, and caregivers often report on the frequency or intensity of BPSDs. Moreover, various measures exist and are used to assess the presence of BPSDs (Jeon et al., 2011) resulting in a lack of clear consensus as to which symptoms constitute BPSDs or how these symptoms should be categorized (Finkel et al., 1996). BPSDs are most often measured in summative fashion, where the occurrence or frequency of behavior problems are summed and operationalized as a single construct of “behavior problems.” The vast majority of routinely cited references published in the past 20 years that provide evidence on the empirical relationships between BPSDs and caregiver negative outcomes did not examine or even review the effect of individual symptoms (e.g. delusions) or separate clusters of symptoms (e.g. psychotic behaviors) in favor of the summative approach to behavior problems (Schulz and Williamson, 1991; Aneshensel et al., 1995; Burns, 2000; Pinquart and Sorensen, 2003a). Specific BPSDs (individual symptoms or clusters) should be examined to determine whether there are components of BPSDs that differentially impact caregiver burden or depression. The failure to capture the complexity of unique behaviors or clusters of behaviors limits our understanding of whether there are particular aspects of dementia behavior that have more deleterious effects on caregiver burden and depression, especially given that individual symptoms clearly have value in understanding the nature and progression of dementia.

Certain individual patient symptoms or symptom clusters may result in more burden or depression for the caregiver than others because they may be more difficult to manage physically, may be taken more personally by the caregiver, or may serve as more dramatic reminders of loss of a loved one. Specific psychotic behaviors (e.g. delusions of abandonment, paranoid delusions) may be particularly disturbing to caregivers who are being accused of not helping the patient despite all their efforts, whereas other behaviors that do not involve the caregiver (e.g. auditory hallucinations) may be less threatening to the caregiver. Individual behaviors may ultimately affect the caregiver’s mood and ability to effectively care for the patient via different pathways, i.e. by differentially evoking more burden, fear, or sympathy. Understanding whether specific BPSD clusters have more negative impact on caregiver burden and depression and the mechanisms by which this occurs can help to target treatment and intervention efforts more effectively for patient–caregiver dyads.

Furthermore, as patient–caregiver relationships develop within the chronic disease context of dementia (which may last for years), considering the timing of behaviors (i.e. existence of sensitive time periods for onset of BPSD symptoms and caregiver adaptation to symptoms over time) relative to caregiver burden and depression is necessary. The dementia caregiver confronts challenges extending throughout the disease course, including patient cognitive and functional decline as well as variation in individual BPSDs, which are known to change with illness progression (Davis et al., 1997; Aalten et al., 2005). Establishing whether and how the timing of individual behaviors affects caregiver response is paramount to understanding the etiology of caregiver burden and depression and developing effective interventions appropriate to the changing course of disease. By studying the impact of timing of behaviors, we may, for example, be able to focus on specific stages of the patient–caregiver relationship in which intervention for caregivers would be most beneficial.

Our aim was to review the literature on the relationship between BPSDs and caregiver outcomes to determine whether there are known symptoms or symptom clusters which exert undue negative impact on caregiver burden and depression. Additionally, we reviewed scales used to measure BPSDs and approaches used for classifying BSPD symptom clusters to determine whether variations in measurement/scaling may also account for differences in caregiver burden and depression. Finally, we look to see how the role of timing of symptoms has been examined within the literature.

Methods

Materials for this review were primarily identified through searches of two electronic databases for peer-reviewed published articles: the MED-LINE/Pubmed and PsychInfo. Search terms were selected based on initial review of relevant keywords across databases that were likely to yield relevant results. Initial search terms included the following keywords and/or subject headings: (1) “dementia” or “Alzheimer,” (2) “caregivers” or “caregiving,” and (3) “behavior symptom” or “BPSD” or “psychiatric” or “neuropsychiatric” or “hallucination” or “delusion” or “aggression” or “agitation” or “wandering” or “psychosis” or “depression” or “behavior.” A search of related articles in these databases, references in review articles and other relevant articles, and consultation with recognized experts yielded additional articles for review.

A set of 708 unique abstracts or articles was reviewed through 2010. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) published in the last 20 years (i.e. after 1989, in order to focus the review on the most recent information regarding caregiving); (2) written in English; (3) peer-reviewed; (4) original research (i.e. no review articles); (5) participants were informal or family caregivers of older adults with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) or dementia in general; (6) included measure of caregiver depression, depressive symptoms, or caregiver burden as an outcome; and (7) included as exposure variable at least one specific dementia patient behavioral or psychological symptom/symptom cluster. Studies were included even if patient behavior–caregiver outcomes were not the primary research interest. Articles were excluded if they (1) were case reports, cases series, or contained 20 or fewer subjects; (2) assessed BPSD as a cumulative variable; (3) focused on a specific subtype of dementia other than AD; (4) did not perform a test to determine whether the association between individual patient behaviors and caregiver outcomes was statistically significant (e.g. baseline data from clinical trials that reported mean scores of caregiver distress outcomes for two or more symptoms); or (5) examined relationship between BPSD and caregiving among patients with unique neuropsychiatric profiles.

Of abstracts screened, 580 were excluded for not meeting initial eligibility criteria. Of the 128 studies retrieved, 55 were excluded because they did not study the association between individual BPSDs and caregiver burden or depression. Additionally, review studies (n = 6), clinical trials that did not measure impact of behaviors on caregivers (n = 9), descriptive studies that failed to quantify the association between BPSD and caregiver depression or burden (n = 17), and case reports or studies with n < 20 were also excluded (n = 6). The final number of articles included was 35.

Studies included in the analysis were reviewed to: (1) determine reasons for testing individual BPSDs or utilizing specific classification systems; (2) determine which individual symptoms or clusters were tested and had impact on caregiver outcomes; (3) identify any causal mechanisms hypothesized and tested; and (4) determine if and how the role of timing of symptoms was studied. As a part of this analysis, we extracted the following study data: design, setting, population, diagnosis, exposure and outcome measures, BPSD categorization schemas, analytical approaches, and control for potential confounding.

Impactful symptoms/symptom clusters were operationalized as follows: symptoms with effect estimates designated as statistically significant in adjusted analyses (or unadjusted when not available) based on p-value <0.05 or with 95% confidence intervals that did not include null values. In our review of findings across studies, we retained all original categorizations of symptoms by investigators and only combined symptoms when they represented the same construct but had different labels (e.g. depression and dysphoria were combined into one category referred to as depression). Symptoms that were combined by authors but consisted of more than one construct were separated when appropriate (e.g. we separated aggression/agitation, a single category within the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI), into two symptoms, namely, aggression and agitation; see Cummings et al., 1994). We also excluded memory problems as a type of BPSD as the focus of this paper is on non-cognitive behavioral and psychological symptoms.

We tallied which symptoms were reported as impactful across relevant studies and also examined how often symptoms were reported as impactful relative to how often the symptom was examined. Symptoms commonly examined (≥ three individual articles) were separately reviewed to determine those symptoms most often found to be impactful. All articles were examined to determine whether there were a priori hypotheses regarding which symptoms would have more severe effects for caregivers. Additionally, we noted whether studies included mediation analyses to determine causal mechanisms for patient–caregiver associations and reviewed study designs in detail to assess the role of timing of symptoms within longitudinal designs. Finally, we examined overall study quality (e.g. sample size, population source, statistical control for potential confounding) to determine whether poor quality factors such as small sample size contributed to our findings.

In addition to examining findings across all studies, we initially examined studies that assessed caregiver burden (n = 23) and caregiver depressive symptoms (n = 17) separately because caregiver burden and depression are different constructs. Five studies examined both caregiver burden and caregiver depression as outcomes (LoGiudice et al., 1995; Chappel and Penning, 1996; Donaldson et al., 1998; Victoroff et al., 1998; Berger et al., 2005). These outcomes were considered separately, for a total of 35 articles and 40 outcomes assessed. Information from each study was extracted and is presented in Tables 1 (N = 17) and 2 (N = 23). These data are organized by study author and list all symptoms tested and associated with the outcome of interest.

Table 1.

Publications that examine association between individual BPSD symptom clusters and caregiver depression/depressive symptoms published 1990–2010 (n = 17)

STUDY STUDY DESIGN SETTING POPULATION DIAGNOSIS SAMPLE
SIZE
SYMPTOM SCALE USED CG DEPRESSION
SCALE USED
SYMPTOMS EXAMINED
Asada et al. (2000) CS Asia Mixed AD 107 TBS GHQ Agitation*; hyperactivity; miscellaneous
Ballard et al. (1995) Case-control Europe Clinic sample Dementia 109 Carers Stress Scale Cornell Depression Scale Aggression; depression; restlessness; sleep problems; suspiciousness
Berger et al. (2005) Longitudinal Europe Clinic sample Dementia 45 BEHAVE-AD BDI; GDS Activity disturbances; aggression; affective disturbances; anxiety; psychotic symptoms; sleep disturbances*
Brodaty and Luscombe (1998) CS Australia Clinic sample Dementia 193 Problem Behavior Checklist (demanding behavior subscale); Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Ham-D) GHQ Demanding/difficult behaviors*; depression*
Caron et al. (1999) Longitudinal USA Mixed Dementia (mild to moderate) 72 BEHAVE –AD Zung Caregiver Depression (Zung) Activity disturbances*; anxiety*; depression; paranoia*
Chappell and Penning (1996) CS Canada Population sample Dementia 327 DBD CES-D Aggression; aimlessness; apathy*; inappropriate sexual behavior; lack of cooperation; restlessness
Covinsky et al. (2003) CS USA Population sample Dementia 5,627 Not stated GDS Anger/aggression*; danger to self or others*; wakes caregiver at night
Croog et al. (2006) CS USA Clinical trial AD (mild to moderate) 199 MBPC; SCAG General Well-Being Adjustment Scale (depression subscale) Destructive behaviors; emotional lability*; personal care problems
Danhauer et al. (2004) Longitudinal USA Clinic sample AD 90 Patient Behavior Checklist (select items) CES-D Agitation; aggression*
Donaldson et al. (1998) CS Europe Clinic sample AD 100 CSDD; Manchester and Oxford Scale for Psychopathology Assessment in Dementia GHQ Aggression; depression*; emotional lability; psychotic symptoms*; sleep disturbances*; various individual psychotic behaviors
Harwood et al. (1998) CS USA Clinic sample AD 653 Clinical diagnosis of depression and psychosis CES-D Depression*; psychosis*
Heok and Li (1997) CS Asia Clinic sample Dementia 50 BEHAVE-AD GHQ Depression*
Levesque et al. (1995) CS Canada Mixed Dementia 265 RMBPC BSI Depression; disruptive behaviors*
LoGiudice et al. (1995) CS Australia Clinic sample Dementia 24 CAMDEX GHQ Depression*; paranoia; personality changes*; sleep disturbances*
Mahoney et al. (2005) CS Europe Population AD 153 NPI, GDS HADS Aberrant motor behaviors; agitation/aggression; anxiety; apathy; delusions; depression; disinhibition/acting out; euphoria; hallucinations; irritability*
Neundorfer et al. (2001) Longitudinal USA Clinic sample Dementia 353 BRSD-DEP CES-D Depression*
Victoroff et al. (1998) CS USA Clinic sample Dementia (early to middle stages) 35 CMAI, CSDD, BEHAVE-AD (select items) Zung Agitation (various individual items)*; delusions; depression; hallucinations
*

Symptom determined to be impactful in study.

CS = Cross-sectional; AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CG = caregiver; BEHAVE-AD = Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease; CES-D = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; CAMDEX = Cambridge Mental Disorders of the Elderly Examination; NPI = Neuropsychiatric Inventory; CMAI = Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory; CSDD = Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia; TBS = Troublesome Behavior Scale; GHQ = General Health Questionnaire; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; DBD = Dementia Behavior Disturbance Scale; MBPC = Memory and Behavior Problem Checklist; SCAG = Sandoz Clinical Assessment-Geriatric; RMBPC = Revised Memory and Behavior Problem Checklist; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; BRSD-DEP = Behavior Rating Scale for Depression.

Table 2.

Publications that examine association between individual BPSD symptom clusters and caregiver burden published 1990–2010 (n = 23)

STUDY STUDY
DESIGN
SETTING POPULATION DIAGNOSIS SAMPLE
SIZE
SYMPTOM
INSTRUMENT USED
CG BURDEN
SCALE USED
SYMPTOMS EXAMINED
Allegri et al. (2006) CS South America Clinic sample AD 82 NPI ZBI Agitation/aggression; anxiety*; apathy; delusions; disinhibition/acting out; depression; euphoria; hallucinations*; irritability; sleep disturbances*; unusual motor behaviors*
Bédard et al. (2005) CS Canada Clinic sample AD 557 GDS; DBRI ZBI Depression; emotional behaviors*; demanding/difficult behaviors*; psychotic behaviors; repetitive behavior*
Berger et al. (2005)** Longitudinal Europe Clinic sample Dementia 45 BEHAVE-AD CBI Activity disturbances*; affective disturbances; aggression*; anxiety*; psychotic symptoms; sleep disturbances
Chappell and Penning (1996)** CS Canada Population Dementia 327 DBD ZBI Apathy*; aimlessness*; aggression*; restlessness*
Donaldson et al. (1998)** CS Europe Clinic sample Dementia 100 CSDD; Manchester and Oxford Scale for Psychopath Assessment in Dementia Gilleard’s Strain Scale Aggression; depression*; emotional lability; psychotic symptoms; sleep disturbances; various individual psychotic behaviors
Elmstahl et al. (1998) Longitudinal Europe Clinic sample Dementia 64 Confusion Scale; Hallucinations-Syncope Index; Lack of Vitality Index; Clinical Variations Index Not stated Clinical variations (mood etc); confusion; hallucinations; lack of vitality*; syncope
Gallagher-Thompson et al. (1992) Longitudinal USA Clinic sample Dementia 35 BDQ (sundowning only) PSS Sundowning*
Gonzalez-Salvador et al. (1999) Case-control Europe Clinic sample AD 67 BEHAVE-AD RSS Aggression*; activity disturbance; affective symptoms; anxiety; delusions; hallucinations; sleep disturbances
Grafstrom and Winblad (1995) Longitudinal Europe Population Dementia 219 CAMDEX Not stated Hiding things*; paranoia*; sleep disturbances*
Hadjistavropoulis et al. (1994) CS Canada Clinic sample AD 136 GDS; PFQ (dysphoria only) ZBI Depression*
Lim et al. (1999) CS Asia Clinic sample Dementia 93 Various individual behaviors Clinician assessment Aggression; agitation; apathy; repetitive behavior*; stripping; wandering*
Lim et al. (2008) CS Asia Clinic sample Dementia 83 Korean Wandering Scale Caregiver Worry, Overload, andRole Captivity Scale Wandering*
LoGiudice et al. (1995)** CS Australia Clinic sample Dementia 24 CAMDEX Family Burden Interview Depression*; paranoia*; personality changes*; sleep disturbances*
Magai and Cohen (1998) CS USA Clinic sample Dementia (middle to late stage) 168 BEHAVE -AD ZBI Activity disturbances; affective symptoms; anxiety; delusions; depression*; hallucinations; sleep disturbances
Molloy et al. (1996) CS Canada Clinic sample Cognitive impairment 108 GDS; DBRI ZBI Disinhibition/acting out*; delusions; depression; disruptive behavior*; frustration/crying*; hallucinations; repetitive behaviors
Onishi et al. (2005) CS Asia Clinic sample None (elderly patients) 116 Various individual behaviors ZBI Depression*; disturbing conversation* (33 additional individual symptoms examined)
Reis et al. (1994) CS Canada Mixed Dementia 213 MBPC; Ryden Aggression Scale ZBI Aggression*
Riello et al. (2002) CS Europe Clinic sample AD (mild) 43 NPI NPI -distress Delusions*
Robinson et al. (2001) CS USA Mixed Dementia 30 RMBPC Cost of Care Index Depression; disruptive behaviors*
Rymer et al. (2002) CS USA Clinic sample AD 41 Frontal Systems Behavior Scale ZBI Apathy; disinhibition/acting out*; executive dysfunction
Shaji et al. (2009) CS Asia Population Dementia 29 BEHAVE-AD BEHAVE-AD (caregiver distress rating) Activity disturbances*; affective symptoms; aggression*; anxiety; delusions*; hallucinations; sleep disturbances
Tun et al. (2008) CS USA Clinic sample AD 122 NPI Screen for Caregiver Burden Scale Affective symptoms*; apathy; highly symptomatic symptoms*
Victoroff et al. (1998)** CS USA Clinic sample Dementia (early to middle stage) 35 CMAI; CSDD; BEHAVE-AD (select items) ZBI Aggression*; agitation (various individual items); delusions; depression; hallucinations
*

Symptom determined to be impactful in study;

**

Study also listed in Table 1.

CS = Cross-sectional; AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CG = caregiver; NPI = Neuropsychiatric Inventory; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; BEHAVE-AD = Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease; DBD = Dementia Behavior Disturbance Scale; CSDD = Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia; CAMDEX = Cambridge Mental Disorders of the Elderly Examination; CMAI = Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory; ZBI = Zarit Burden Interview; DBRI = Dysfunctional Behavior Rating Instrument; CBI = Caregiver Burden Interview; BDQ = Behavioral Disturbance Questionnaire; PSS = Perceived Stress Scale; RSS = Relative’s Stress Scale; PFQ = Present Functioning Questionnaire; DBRI = Dysfunctional Behavior Rating Instrument.

Results

Approaches to measuring and categorizing individual symptoms

Across the 35 studies that met the inclusion criteria and investigated at least one individual symptom or symptom cluster, 25 different scales were used to measure BPSDs. Ten (29%) of the reviewed studies (Heok and Li, 1997; Magai and Cohen, 1998; Victoroff et al., 1998; Caron et al., 1999; Gonzalez-Salvador et al., 1999; Riello et al., 2002; Berger et al., 2005; Mahoney et al., 2005; Allegri et al., 2006; Shaji et al., 2009) made use of one of the following commonly used scales to study a broad range of behaviors: (1) the Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease (BEHAVE-AD) rating scale (Reisberg et al., 1996), which assesses behaviors occurrence over seven domains – delusions, hallucinations, activity disturbances, aggression, sleep, affective symptoms, and anxiety; and (2) the NPI (Cummings et al., 1994), which assesses ten individual behaviors – delusions, hallucinations, agitation/aggression, disinhibition, abberant motor behavior, irritability, dysphoria, anxiety, apathy, and euphoria – with follow-up questions on severity and frequency of behavior. Measures vary as to whether they assess and/or give weight to the frequency of behavior occurrence (e.g. in the Revised Memory and Behavior Problems Checklist (Teri et al., 1992), the caregiver notes how often behavior occurred on five-point Likert scale). Other studies employed one or more assessments used to measure more specific aspects of symptomatology: for example, four studies (Hadjistavropoulos et al., 1994; Molloy et al., 1996; Bédard et al., 2005; Mahoney et al., 2005) used the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; Brink et al., 1982), which measures patient depressive symptoms using 15 or 30 dichotomous (yes/no) questions. Additionally, not all studies made use of validated BPSD scales, instead relying on clinical diagnoses of behaviors such as psychosis (Harwood et al., 1998) or original lists of individual items to measure BPSD (Lim et al., 1999; Onishi et al., 2005).

There was no consistent approach to the categorization of symptoms across studies. Two studies did not provide any information on categorization schema (Molloy et al., 1996; Covinsky et al., 2003) and almost half (n = 17) did not employ any system of categorization, instead using select individual items or diagnoses (Gallagher-Thompson et al., 1992; Reis et al., 1994; Ballard et al., 1995; Grafstrom and Winblad, 1995; Heok and Li, 1997; Brodaty and Luscombe, 1998; Elmstahl et al., 1998; Harwood et al., 1998; Magai and Cohen, 1998; Gonzalez-Salvador et al., 1999; Lim et al., 1999; Neundorfer et al., 2001; Onishi et al., 2005; Allegri et al., 2006; Lim et al., 2008; Shaji et al., 2009). The remainder of studies either used (1) subscales of instruments or (2) subscales and select individual scale items. The use of subscales and/or individual items resulted in the testing of a range of symptoms/symptom clusters that often include overlapping constructs: for example, while some studies (Donaldson et al., 1998; Harwood et al., 1998) used the broader category of “psychosis,” others delineated between specific psychotic behaviors such as experiencing hallucinations and delusions (e.g. Allegri et al., 2006; Shaji et al., 2009). Finally, seven studies (20%) employed factor analytic techniques to categorize symptoms based on how they clustered within patients (Hadjistavropoulos et al., 1994; Chappel and Penning, 1996; Caron et al., 1999; Asada et al., 2000; Mahoney et al., 2005; Croog et al., 2006; Tun et al., 2008).

Symptoms that impact caregiver outcomes

No symptom/symptom cluster was consistently identified as having negative impact on either caregiver burden or caregiver depression by the majority of studies examined. For those studies assessing caregiver depression, 19 different patient symptoms/symptom clusters were cited as significantly impacting caregivers. Patient depression was the most frequently reported symptom associated with caregiver depression (35%; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Heok and Li, 1997; Brodaty and Luscombe, 1998; Donaldson et al., 1998; Harwood et al., 1998; Neundorfer et al., 2001), although sleep disturbances (18%; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Donaldson et al., 1998; Berger et al., 2005), anger/aggression (12%; Covinsky et al., 2003; Danhauer et al., 2004), psychosis (12%; Donaldson et al., 1998; Harwood et al., 1998), and agitation (12%; Victoroff et al., 1998; Asada et al., 2000) were also reported by multiple studies.

Twenty-eight different symptoms/symptom clusters were significantly associated with caregiver burden in at least one study. Anger/aggression (26%; Reis et al., 1994; Chappel and Penning, 1996; Victoroff et al., 1998; Gonzalez-Salvador et al., 1999; Berger et al., 2005; Shaji et al., 2009) and depression (17%; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Donaldson et al., 1998; Magai and Cohen, 1998; Onishi et al., 2005) were the most frequently cited patient symptoms associated with caregiver burden although sleep disturbances (13%; Grafstrom and Winblad, 1995; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Allegri et al., 2006) and repetitive behaviors (13%; Victoroff et al., 1998; Lim et al., 1999; Bédard et al., 2005) were also reported by multiple studies.

We ultimately included caregiver burden and depression studies together for analyses after finding no substantial differences between them. Combining the caregiver burden and depression studies (n = 35 articles that assessed n = 40 outcomes), we identified 36 different symptoms that had negative impact on caregivers. One study (Ballard et al., 1995) did not find any specific behavior patterns to be stressful. The most commonly reported symptoms for either caregiver burden or depression were: depression (25%; Hadjistavropoulos et al., 1994; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Heok and Li, 1997; Brodaty and Luscombe, 1998; Donaldson et al., 1998; Magai and Cohen, 1998; Asada et al., 2000; Neundorfer et al., 2001; Onishi et al., 2005), anger/aggression (20%; Reis et al., 1994; Chappel and Penning, 1996; Victoroff et al., 1998; Gonzalez-Salvador et al., 1999; Covinsky et al., 2003; Danhauer et al., 2004; Berger et al., 2005; Shaji et al., 2009), and sleep disturbances (15%; Grafstrom and Winblad, 1995; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Donaldson et al., 1998; Allegri et al., 2006). Other symptoms linked to caregiving outcomes by at least three studies were paranoia (Grafstrom and Winblad, 1995; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Caron et al., 1999), repetitive behaviors (Victoroff et al., 1998; Lim et al., 1999; Bédard et al., 2005), anxiety (Caron et al., 1999; Berger et al., 2005; Allegri et al., 2006), activity disturbances (Caron et al., 1999; Berger et al., 2005; Shaji et al., 2009), and disruptive behaviors (Levesque et al., 1995; Molloy et al., 1996; Robinson et al., 2001).

Because only those symptoms tested as predictors have the potential to have a demonstrated association with caregiver outcomes, we examined how often specific symptoms/symptom clusters were tested within studies. The following 13 symptoms were examined by at least three individual articles: affective symptoms, agitation, anger/aggression, activity disturbances, anxiety, delusions, depression, disinhibition/acting out, disruptive behaviors, hallucinations, paranoia, repetitive behavior, and sleep disturbances. The most commonly cited impactful symptoms were also those most frequently tested: 66% of reviewed studies tested the impact of depression, 43% tested the impact of anger/aggression, and 34% tested the impact of sleep disturbances. On the other hand, symptoms less frequently cited as having an impact, e.g. anxiety and paranoia, were each tested by only 9% of reviewed articles.

Limiting analyses to the 13 most commonly tested symptoms, we assessed the proportion of studies finding a symptom to be impactful using the number of studies in which the symptom was examined as the denominator. Six symptoms impacted caregivers in at least 75% of the studies in which they were examined: anxiety, paranoia, activity disturbances, disruptive behaviors, agitation and repetitive behavior. Among the three most commonly reported impactful symptoms, we found that patient depression impacted caregivers in 40% of the studies, anger/aggression impacted caregivers in 50% of the studies, and sleep disturbances impacted caregivers in 43% of the studies. On the other hand, while the effect of patient hallucinations was examined in six studies, only one found the symptom to affect caregiver burden (Donaldson et al., 1998); similarly, while delusions were tested seven times, in only two cases were they found to have negative impact on caregiver burden or depression (Riello et al., 2002; Shaji et al., 2009).

Causal mechanisms by which individual BPSD have an impact on caregivers

None of the studies tested causal mechanisms by which a specific symptom/symptom cluster resulted in caregiver depression or burden. Furthermore, none of the studies presented a priori hypotheses indicating one symptom would specifically result in increased caregiver depression or burden than other symptoms. The overwhelming majority of studies were either exploratory in nature (i.e. interested in assessing which symptoms were associated with caregiver burden or depression) or tested hypotheses about other issues within the caregiving relationship such as the role of gender (Bédard et al., 2005), attachment style (Magai and Cohen, 1998), or family boundary ambiguity (Caron et al., 1999). Several studies were aimed at testing the influence of only one particular symptom/symptom cluster on caregiver mental health – e.g. wandering (Lim et al., 2008), delusions (Riello et al., 2002), depression (Hadjistavropoulos et al., 1994; Neundorfer et al., 2001), and sundowning (Gallagher-Thompson et al., 1992) – and therefore could not make hypotheses about the relative effect of multiple symptoms. Investigators who tested the association of more than one symptom (e.g. aggression and agitation; Danhauer et al., 2004) with caregiver outcomes did not hypothesize as to whether one symptom would have a greater impact on caregivers than another.

Dynamic relationships over time

Although the overwhelming majority of studies were cross-sectional in nature, seven articles (20%) examined the relationship between individual symptoms and outcomes over multiple time points; three (Caron et al., 1999; Neundorfer et al., 2001; Danhauer et al., 2004) examined caregiver depression as an outcome, three (Gallagher-Thompson et al., 1992; Grafstrom and Winblad, 1995; Elmstahl et al., 1998) examined caregiver burden as an outcome, and one (Berger et al., 2005) examined both outcomes over time. No consistent patterns could be discerned from these studies due to the small number of longitudinal studies conducted, the studies’ disparate aims relative to the role of behavior timing, wide variation in the number of time points assessed, period of time studied, and stage of disease/caregiving relationship. Yet, these study findings suggest that the timing of behaviors may impact the relationship between BPSDs and caregiver outcomes. First, specific behaviors may have more negative effects for caregivers at certain points in the caregiver–patient relationship. For example, Berger et al. (2005) found that across five time points, specific BPSD had varying associations with caregiver burden and depression (e.g. patient sleep disturbance was only correlated with caregiver depression at 24 months). Second, there is some evidence to suggest that there may be sensitive time periods in which patients’ symptoms may have lasting impact on caregiver outcomes. Elmstahl et al. (1998) examined individual BPSD features at varying time points and found that lack of vitality (i.e. tiredness) during transition to group living facility affected caregiver burden 12 months later. Additionally, authors found that initial sundowning behaviors influenced increases in stress over time (Gallagher-Thompson et al., 1992). Danhauer et al. (2004), however, did not find baseline behaviors to impact subsequent caregiver depression. Finally, findings from two studies suggest that positive or negative changes in patient behaviors may similarly influence changes in caregiver behaviors (Caron et al., 1999; Neundorfer et al., 2001).

Study quality

We examined overall study quality to determine whether factors such as sample size and lack of control for potential confounding limited our ability to find consistently impactful BPSD symptoms. Half of the studies included sample sizes of 100 dyads or less, which limits ability to detect smaller effect sizes. The median sample size across the 35 studies was 107, and only four studies included more than 300 dyads (Chappel and Penning, 1996; Harwood et al., 1998; Neundorfer et al., 2001; Covinsky et al., 2003). Furthermore, most studies included in this review did not control for important confounders such as functional status or caregiver characteristics in part due to small sample size. Overall, there was a lack of statistical control for multiple covariates: 26% used unadjusted correlation analyses (n = 6; Gallagher-Thompson et al., 1992; LoGiudice et al., 1995; Caron et al., 1999; Robinson et al., 2001; Berger et al., 2005; Allegri et al., 2006), χ2 tests (n = 2; Riello et al., 2002; Shaji et al., 2009), or paired t-tests (n = 1; Grafstrom and Winblad, 1995) to examine patient– caregiver associations. Control for such factors would have weakened observed associations and may account for why so many individual symptoms were found to negatively impact both caregiver burden and depression. Furthermore, studies failed to simultaneously control for the full range of BPSD symptoms, even if they successfully adjusted for the effects of multiple covariates. For example, Covinsky et al. (2003) controlled for important patient and caregiver characteristics including patient functional status using data from a large population-based study. While this study found that anger/aggression and danger to self or others were important predictors of caregiver depression, it unfortunately only tested three individual patient behaviors in total, failing to simultaneously consider the range of behaviors that are important stressors for caregivers.

Discussion

The literature has successfully revealed that BPSDs are empirically associated with caregiver burden and depression. We undertook this review in order to determine if there is evidence to suggest that individual BPSD symptoms or symptom clusters differentially impact these critical caregiver outcomes. The vast majority of studies addressing the relationship between patient behavior problems and caregiver burden and depression did not examine individual symptoms or symptom clusters and instead aggregated behaviors into one measure of BPSD such that each symptom was operationalized as equally contributing to caregiver stressors or depressive symptoms. While it is clear that BPSDs in general and many individual symptoms are associated with negative outcomes for caregivers, the evidence remains inconclusive as to whether some symptoms are more important than others. While we did find specific symptoms such as depression, aggression, and sleep disturbances to be commonly cited as impactful, there were no individual symptom/symptom cluster(s) that were consistently tested and found to result in increased burden or depression for caregivers. In fact, most symptom types were associated with either caregiver burden or depression in at least one study.

Furthermore, the most commonly cited impactful symptoms were also those most frequently tested. While most studies tested a range of symptoms that dementia patients are known to exhibit, several studies were specifically designed to test the effect of one specific symptom/symptom cluster (e.g. delusions, Riello et al., 2002; wandering, Lim et al., 2008). While we included these studies in our review, they did not examine the impact of one symptom relative to others, and their inclusion makes the pool of symptoms examined less representative of all possible dementia behaviors. When we examined which symptoms were most often found as significantly associated with caregiver burden and depression relative to how often they were tested, we found that anxiety, activity disturbances, and disruptive behaviors were always statistically significantly associated with caregiver outcomes, whereas paranoia, repetitive behavior, and agitation had a significant impact 75% of the time. However, because none of these symptoms was examined by more than four studies, they were not commonly cited as impactful to caregivers among all articles reviewed. This discrepancy further substantiates our conclusion that research has yet to elucidate whether there are individual symptoms/symptom cluster(s) that result in the most negative outcomes for caregivers.

The lack of consistency in measurement of BPSDs ultimately limits our ability to reach a consensus as to which symptoms impact caregiver burden and depression. In addition to not measuring all aspects of BPSDs, scales do not consistently measure frequency of behaviors. How often the behavior occurs may affect the caregiver’s response to the behavior, especially over time. The use of valid and consistent measures of BPSDs will improve future research on BPSDs in general (Jeon et al., 2011), and specifically improve our knowledge as to which symptoms have greatest influence on caregiver burden and depression.

Finally, we examined a variety of study characteristics as potential explanations for the heterogeneity of findings. First, we examined study setting variation given the wide range of international settings in which study samples were drawn and potential cultural differences in caregiver response to behaviors. Only one-third of studies examined were US-based; the remainder were from Europe (n = 8, 23%), Asia (n = 6, 17%); Canada (n = 6, 17%), Australia (n = 2, 6%), and South America (n = 1, 3%). When limiting the sample to those studies with large, US-based samples (n > 100) with caregiver depression as an outcome (n = 4), we still find the following range of symptoms associated with caregiver outcomes: psychosis (Harwood et al., 1998), depression (Harwood et al., 1998; Neundorfer et al., 2001), anger/aggressiveness (Covinsky et al., 2003), and emotional lability (Croog et al., 2006). Although we reviewed studies published over a 20-year period, we did not find any variations in findings due to when the study was published. Furthermore, like most caregiving studies, the studies examined included samples largely recruited from memory clinics, hospitals, assisted living facilities, or caregiver support groups. When we restricted our review to studies with population samples (Grafstrom and Winblad, 1995; Chappel and Penning, 1996; Covinsky et al., 2003), we continued to find inconsistent patterns of findings. The majority of studies included patients with a mix of dementia diagnoses or unspecified dementia or cognitive impairment (see Tables 1 and 2). Among those studies that focused on patients with AD (n = 12), we did not find BPSDs that consistently influence caregiver burden or depression. Finally, the vast majority of studies did not consider effect modifiers such as race/ethnicity, which may affect the caregiver’s response to specific symptoms.

Limitations

We maintained the study authors’ original BPSD categorization schema (i.e. how symptoms are categorized within a study) for this analysis and did not systematically re-categorize symptoms across studies when counting which individual BPSDs were most often associated with caregiver burden and depression. Thus, the symptom categorization scheme used within each study (e.g. broad vs. more specific) may have influenced our findings. Even within individual studies, researchers reported different findings from models that incorporated broader subscales than models that included individual items. For example, when examining predictors of caregiver depression, Victoroff et al. (1998) found that non-aggressive agitation as a subscale was predictive of caregiver depression, but when examining 29 individual agitation items, specific aggressive behaviors (e.g. making sexual advances and destroying property) were most predictive. Depression and aggression are broader symptom categories than specific behaviors such as hiding objects and hitting. While the latter symptoms were analyzed in this review as separate items, they may in fact be referring to similar constructs. Re-categorizing more specific behaviors/symptoms into broader categories could have influenced these findings.

While we initially planned to limit this review to studies that focused on caregiver depression as an outcome, because of the limited number of published studies examining individual symptoms we included caregiver burden studies as well. While we first reviewed these outcomes separately, we ultimately grouped them together after finding no differences between them. A wide range of assessments was used to measure depression and burden (see Tables 1 and 2). Within our review, researchers used nine different measures of caregiver depression and at least 13 different measures of caregiver burden (including one clinical assessment of burden; see Lim et al., 1999). The use of a wide range of outcome measures may also contribute to inconsistent findings. Again, because of a lack of published studies, we were unable to limit this review to studies that measured depressive symptoms using identical assessments. We relied on a synthesis approach for this study, which effectively relies on a count of significant and non-significant effects in contrast to more advanced meta-analytic approaches that can empirically pool effect sizes. Because of the limited number of studies meeting inclusion criteria, and heterogeneity in study design and study measurements, meta-analysis techniques could not be employed to examine pooled estimates of effect. Similarly, due to a small number of studies that met our inclusion/exclusion criteria, we were not able to limit this analysis to review only the highest quality studies. However, we did attempt to examine different study subgroups to discern sources of heterogeneity in our findings. Finally, the search and categorization of articles was conducted by one individual (K.O) and the reliability of findings cannot be determined.

Implications and recommendations

An extensive body of research provides robust evidence suggesting that summed scores of BPSDs exert a negative impact on caregiver outcomes. The next step, not yet addressed in the literature, is to determine whether and how individual symptom clusters differentially impact the caregiver. Although individual BPSDs cluster together, increased specificity within the BPSD construct may help target interventions for caregivers since BPSDs as a whole are pervasive over the course of dementia. Based on this review, we recommend that future research should build upon existing knowledge of the relationship between BPSDs and caregiving and consider the following issues:

  1. Categorizing individual BPSD based on the caregiver’s perspective: Among studies reviewed, individual BPSDs were categorized based on how they manifest within the patient and not based on how they impact the caregivers, despite the fact that caregiver depression and burden were the outcomes of interest. Behaviors tend to be grouped together in order to describe clinical symptoms using similar domains used for non-dementia patients (e.g. psychotic vs. mood symptoms) or based on how they empirically cluster within the dementia patient. While these classifications may be clinically useful for understanding potential varying etiologies of BPSD syndromes (e.g. Aalten et al., 2003) and for assessing patient treatment options, they may not be fully capturing the impact of the behaviors on the caregiver. Each member of the patient–caregiver dyad is impacted by BPSDs, but not necessarily in the same way. Major events can differentially affect patients and caregivers; nursing home placement, for example, has positive effects for caregivers such as decreased stress but negative repercussions for patients including increased mortality. Therefore, researchers should begin to conceptualize BPSD components from the perspective of how they might impact the caregiver and consider how a behavior or symptom may differentially impact the caregiver and patient. For example, given the fact that psychotic behaviors are often unexpected and may be frightening to the caregivers who perceive them as non-normative, psychosis may have relatively little effect on quality of life for the patient, but may have huge implications for the well-being of the overwhelmed caregiver.

  2. Delineating causal mechanisms: Studies have not identified pathways for the association between an individual symptom and caregiver outcomes. Further research is necessary to understand if and why individual behaviors or symptoms may increase caregiver burden or depression by operating through different pathways. While research suggests that the association between BPSDs in general and mental and physical health outcomes of caregivers are mediated by subjective stress appraisal (Goode et al., 1998; Hooker et al., 2002; Son et al., 2007), it is not yet clear by which pathway individual symptoms ultimately result in negative caregiver outcomes. In addition to subjective stress, other pathways may be part of this process and should be further studied using mediation analysis.

  3. The role of time and timing: While the majority of studies were cross-sectional, and the longitudinal studies reviewed were highly heterogeneous in aims and methodology, some evidences suggest that timing of BPSDs over the course of the patient–caregiver relationship is important for understanding the relationship between individual BPSDs and caregiver burden and depression. While this review suggests that specific time periods over the patient–caregiver relationship may be important to focus on and that change in patient behavior is associated with change in caregiver burden and depression, the paucity of studies evaluating the issue highlights a clear need for further study.

  4. Emphasis on stage of illness: The vast majority of studies reviewed does not specify and control for stage of disease in assessing the relationship between symptoms and caregiver outcomes. Authors rarely examined stage of dementia except to systematically exclude institutionalized patients who typically have the most advanced dementia. We found that only five studies (Magai and Cohen, 1998; Victoroff et al., 1998; Caron et al., 1999; Riello et al., 2002; Croog et al., 2006) indicated that they restricted the sample to patients at a specific stage/stages of illness (e.g. mild to moderate or moderate to severe). Although controlling for cognitive status was a common technique used to address this issue, such an approach unfortunately may not succeed in capturing how long the patient has been suffering with illness or impairment or the actual stage of dementia progression. Grouping together dementia patients at all stages of disease progression may be problematic as disease stage affects caregiver outcomes (Ballard et al., 2000). Gonzalez-Salvador et al. (1999), for example, found that the associations between caregiver stress and individual behaviors in mild, moderate, and severe AD subgroups differ substantially. While the authors concluded that aggression was most important for all stages of AD, delusional ideation was independently associated with stress for those with mild and moderate AD but not for those with severe AD. Future research in this area should consider the impact of disease stage in analyses.

  5. Diagnostic context: Additionally, studies have not differentiated between dementia subtypes. While our study excluded samples that focused exclusively on less common subtypes of dementia (e.g. dementia with Lewy bodies and frontotemporal dementia), only one-third of studies were restricted to patient samples who exclusively had diagnoses of AD (e.g. Donaldson et al., 1998). Various BPSDs emerge in the context of different subtypes of dementia (Chiu et al., 2006). Etiology of dementia or diagnostic context may influence the way caregivers experience psychiatric features of dementia. According to attribution theory, perceptions of controllability of behaviors influence the emotional reaction of caregivers (Weiner, 1986). In other words, caregivers who perceive patients as having less control over their behaviors have a less emotional response and less negative effect. This relationship has been tested within the context of aging. Using vignette experiments, researchers found that disruptive behavior attributed to AD compared to aging alone or even depression was perceived as (1) more biological in nature, (2) less controllable by the patient, and (3) more likely to garner sympathy from the caregiver (Wadley and Haley, 2001). Diagnostic context may therefore play a role in explaining the relationship between BPSDs and caregiver depression such that those caregivers who attribute more behaviors to disease (based on their understanding of specific types of dementia) are less likely to experience negative outcomes. Testing these relationships across varied disease contexts can add to our understanding of how BPSDs affect caregivers and ultimately impact disease-specific design.

Conclusions

Our review finds that despite the consistency of the association between BPSDs and caregiver depression and burden, research has not focused on examining whether and how specific symptoms or groups of symptoms differentially impact caregiver outcomes. Moreover, very few studies have examined how these relationships change over time. In order to determine the key components of BPSDs that result in negative mental health for caregivers, studies are required that not only are designed to examine differences among BPSD symptom clusters but specifically categorize BPSDs based on how they negatively affect caregivers. Additionally, we recommend that studies examine casual mechanisms by which individual BPSDs impact caregivers and that subsequent research considers stage and subtypes of dementia. Such approaches will help to elucidate whether individual BPSDs differentially impact caregivers and allow for focused intervention and treatment efforts that benefit the patient–caregiver dyad.

Acknowledgments

K. Ornstein acknowledges the following members of her dissertation committee who assisted with this project: Sharon Schwartz, PhD, Ann Stueve, PhD, Yaakov Stern, PhD, and Bruce Link, PhD. This research was supported by NIMH T32 MH013043-36 (K. Ornstein)

Description of author’s roles

K. Ornstein was responsible for formulating the research study, analyzing the data, and preparation of the paper; J. E. Gaugler assisted in the writing of the paper and analysis of the data.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest

References

  1. Aalten P, et al. Behavioral problems in dementia: a factor analysis of the neuropsychiatric inventory. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2003;15:99–105. doi: 10.1159/000067972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aalten P, de Vugt ME, Jaspers N, Jolles J, Verhey FR. The course of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia. Part II: relationships among behavioural sub-syndromes and the influence of clinical variables. International Journal of Geriatic Psychiatry. 2005;20:531–536. doi: 10.1002/gps.1317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allegri RF, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms as a predictor of caregiver burden in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 2006;2:105–110. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aneshensel CS, Pearlin LI, Mullan JT, Zarit SH, Whitlach CJ. Profiles in Caregiving: The Unexpected Career. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  5. Asada T, Kinoshita T, Kakuma T. Analysis of behavioral disturbances among community-dwelling elderly with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2000;14:160–167. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200007000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ballard CG, et al. The aetiology of depression in the carers of dementia sufferers. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1995;35:59–63. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00041-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ballard C, Lowery K, Powell I, O’Brien J, James I. Impact of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia on caregivers. International Psychogeriatrics. 2000;12:93–105. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bédard M, Kuzik R, Chambers L, Molloy DW, Dubois S, Lever JA. Understanding burden differences between men and women caregivers: the contribution of care-recipient problem behaviors. International Psychogeriatrics. 2005;17:99–118. doi: 10.1017/s1041610204000857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Berger G, Bernhardt T, Weimer E, Peters J, Kratzsch T, Frolich L. Longitudinal study on the relationship between symptomatology of dementia and levels of subjective burden and depression among family caregivers in memory clinic patients. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry & Neurology. 2005;18:119–128. doi: 10.1177/0891988704273375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Black W, Almeida OP. A systematic review of the association between the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and burden of care. International Psychogeriatrics. 2004;16:295–315. doi: 10.1017/s1041610204000468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brink TL, Yesavage JA, Lum O, Heersema P, Adey MB, Rose TL. Screening tests for geriatric depression. Clinical Gerontologist. 1982;1:37–43. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brodaty H, Luscombe G. Psychological morbidity in caregivers is associated with depression in patients with dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 1998;12:62–70. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199806000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Burns A. The burden of Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;3:31–38. doi: 10.1017/S1461145700001905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Caron W, Boss P, Mortimer J. Family boundary ambiguity predicts Alzheimer’s outcomes. Psychiatry. 1999;62:347–356. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1999.11024882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chappel NL, Penning M. Behavioural problems and distress among caregivers of people with dementia. Ageing and Society. 1996;16:57–73. [Google Scholar]
  16. Chiu MJ, Chen TF, Yip PK, Hua MS, Tang LY. Behavioral and psychologic symptoms in different types of dementia. Journal of the Formosan Medical Association. 2006;105:556–562. doi: 10.1016/S0929-6646(09)60150-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Covinsky KE, et al. Patient and caregiver characteristics associated with depression in caregivers of patients with dementia. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2003;18:1006–1014. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2003.30103.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Croog SH, Burleson JA, Sudilovsky A, Baume RM. Spouse caregivers of Alzheimer patients: problem responses to caregiver burden. Aging and Mental Health. 2006;10:87–100. doi: 10.1080/13607860500492498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308–2314. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.12.2308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Danhauer SC, McCann JJ, Gilley DW, Beckett LA, Bienias JL, Evans DA. Do behavioral disturbances in persons with Alzheimer’s disease predict caregiver depression over time? Psychology and Aging. 2004;19:198–202. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Davis LL, Buckwalter K, Burgio LD. Measuring problem behaviors in dementia: developing a methodological agenda. Advances in Nursing Science. 1997;20:40–55. doi: 10.1097/00012272-199709000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Donaldson C, Tarrier N, Burns A. Determinants of carer stress in Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 1998;13:248–256. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1166(199804)13:4<248::aid-gps770>3.0.co;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Elmstahl S, Ingvad B, Annerstedt L. Family caregiving in dementia: prediction of caregiver burden 12 months after relocation to group-living care. International Psychogeriatrics. 1998;10:127–146. doi: 10.1017/s1041610298005249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Finkel SI, Costa e Silva J, Cohen G, Miller S, Sartorius N. Behavioral and psychological signs and symptoms of dementia: a consensus statement on current knowledge and implications for research and treatment. International Psychogeriatrics. 1996;8:497–500. doi: 10.1017/s1041610297003943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gallagher-Thompson D, Brooks JO, III, Bliwise D, Leader J, Yesavage JA. The relations among caregiver stress, “sundowning” symptoms, and cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1992;40:807–810. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01853.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gonzalez-Salvador MT, Arango C, Lyketsos CG, Barba AC. The stress and psychological morbidity of the Alzheimer patient caregiver. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 1999;14:701–710. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1166(199909)14:9<701::aid-gps5>3.0.co;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Goode KT, Haley WE, Roth DL, Ford GR. Predicting longitudinal changes in caregiver physical and mental health: a stress process model. Health Psychology. 1998;17:190–198. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.17.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Grafstrom M, Winblad B. Family burden in the care of the demented and nondemented elderly: a longitudinal study. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 1995;9:78–86. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199509020-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hadjistavropoulos T, Taylor S, Tuokko H, Beattie BL. Neuropsychological deficits, caregivers’ perception of deficits and caregiver burden. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1994;42:308–314. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1994.tb01757.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Harwood DG, Barker WW, Cantillon M, Loewenstein DA, Ownby R, Duara R. Depressive symptomatology in first-degree family caregivers of Alzheimer disease patients: a cross-ethnic comparison. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 1998;12:340–346. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199812000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Heok KE, Li TS. Stress of caregivers of dementia patients in the Singapore Chinese family. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 1997;12:466–469. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hooker K, et al. Behavioral change in persons with dementia: relationships with mental and physical health of caregivers. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2002;57:453–460. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.5.p453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jeon YH, et al. Recommended measures for the assessment of behavioral disturbances associated with dementia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2011;19:403–415. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181ef7a0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Levesque L, Cossette S, Laurin L. A multidimensional examination of the psychological and social well-being of caregivers of a demented relative. Research on Aging. 1995;17:332–360. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lim PPJ, Sahadevan S, Choo GK, Anthony P. Burden of caregiving in mild to moderate dementia: an Asian experience. International Psychogeriatrics. 1999;11:411–420. doi: 10.1017/s104161029900602x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lim YM, Son GR, Song JA, Beattie E. Factors affecting burden of family caregivers of community-dwelling ambulatory elders with dementia in Korea. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 2008;22:226–234. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2007.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. LoGiudice D, Waltrowicz W, McKenzie S, Ames D, Flicker L. Prevalence of dementia among patients referred to an aged care assessment team and associated stress in their carers. Australian Journal of Public Health. 1995;19:275–279. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-6405.1995.tb00443.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Magai C, Cohen CI. Attachment style and emotion regulation in dementia patients and their relation to caregiver burden. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 1998;53:147–154. doi: 10.1093/geronb/53b.3.p147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mahoney R, Regan C, Katona C, Livingston G. Anxiety and depression in family caregivers of people with Alzheimer disease: the LASER-AD study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2005;13:795–801. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.9.795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Molloy DW, Lever R, Bedard M, Guyatt G, Butt G. Burden in care-givers of older adults with impaired cognition: the relationship with dysfunctional behaviors, daily living and mood. Annals of the Royal College of Physicians and Surgeons of Canada. 1996;29:151–154. [Google Scholar]
  41. Neundorfer MM, McClendon MJ, Smyth KA, Stuckey JC, Strauss ME, Patterson MB. A longitudinal study of the relationship between levels of depression among persons with Alzheimer’s disease and levels of depression among their family caregivers. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2001;56:301–313. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.5.p301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Onishi J, Suzuki Y, Umegaki H, Nakamura A, Endo H, Iguchi A. Influence of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) and environment of care on caregivers’ burden. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 2005;41:159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2005.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pearlin LI, Mullan JT, Semple SJ, Skaff MM. Caregiving and the stress process: an overview of concepts and their measures. Gerontologist. 1990;30:583–594. doi: 10.1093/geront/30.5.583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Associations of stressors and uplifts of caregiving with caregiver burden and depressive mood: a meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2003a;58:112–128. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.p112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in psychological health and physical health: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging. 2003b;18:250–267. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Reis MF, Gold DP, Andres D, Markiewicz D, Gauthier S. Personality traits as determinants of burden and health complaints in caregiving. International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 1994;39:257–271. doi: 10.2190/6LYN-YFWQ-P87D-MKWX. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Reisberg B, Auer SR, Monteiro IM. Behavioral pathology in Alzheimer’s disease (BEHAVE-AD) rating scale. International Psychogeriatrics. 1996;8:301–308. doi: 10.1097/00019442-199911001-00147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Riello R, Geroldi C, Zanetti O, Frisoni GB. Caregiver’s distress is associated with delusions in Alzheimer’s patients. Behavioral Medicine. 2002;28:92–98. doi: 10.1080/08964280209596047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Robinson KM, Adkisson P, Weinrich S. Problem behaviour, caregiver reactions, and impact among caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2001;36:573–582. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.2001.02010.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Rymer S, Salloway S, Norton L, Malloy P, Correia S, Monast D. Impaired awareness, behavior disturbance, and caregiver burden in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2002;16:248–253. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200210000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schulz R, O’Brien AT, Bookwala J, Fleissner K. Psychiatric and physical morbidity effects of dementia caregiving: prevalence, correlates, and causes. Gerontologist. 1995;35:771–791. doi: 10.1093/geront/35.6.771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schulz R, Williamson GM. A 2-year longitudinal study of depression among Alzheimer’s caregivers. Psychology and Aging. 1991;6:569–578. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.6.4.569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Shaji KS, George RK, Prince MJ, Jacob KS. Behavioral symptoms and caregiver burden in dementia. Indian Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;51:45–49. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.44905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Son J, Erno A, Shea DG, Femia EE, Zarit SH, Stephens MA. The caregiver stress process and health outcomes. Journal of Aging Health. 2007;19:871–887. doi: 10.1177/0898264307308568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Teri L, Truax P, Logsdon R, Uomoto J, Zarit S, Vitaliano PP. Assessment of behavioral problems in dementia: the revised memory and behavior problems checklist. Psychology and Aging. 1992;7:622–631. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.7.4.622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tun SM, Murman DL, Colenda CC. Concurrent validity of neuropsychiatric subgroups on caregiver burden in Alzheimer disease patients. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;16:594–602. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318173f5fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Victoroff J, Mack WJ, Nielson KA. Psychiatric complications of dementia: impact on caregivers. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 1998;9:50–55. doi: 10.1159/000017022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Wadley VG, Haley WE. Diagnostic attributions versus labeling: impact of Alzheimer’s disease and major depression diagnoses on emotions, beliefs, and helping intentions of family members. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2001;56:244–252. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.4.p244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Weiner B. An Attributional Theory of Motivation and Emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  60. Zarit SH, Reever KE, Bach-Peterson J. Relatives of the impaired elderly: correlates of feelings of burden. Gerontologist. 1980;20:649–655. doi: 10.1093/geront/20.6.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES