Highlights
-
•
First investigation of the role of NMNAT1 in the early stage of tauopathies in mice.
-
•
htau mice display a robust deficit in food burrowing prior to onset of cognitive symptoms.
-
•
NMNAT1 overexpression rescues this behavioral abnormality.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease (AD), htau, NMNAT1, NAD, Food burrowing, MRS
Abstract
NAD metabolism and the NAD biosynthetic enzymes nicotinamide nucleotide adenylyltransferases (NMNATs) are thought to play a key neuroprotective role in tauopathies, including Alzheimer’s disease. Here, we investigated whether modulating the expression of the NMNAT nuclear isoform NMNAT1, which is important for neuronal maintenance, influences the development of behavioral and neuropathological abnormalities in htau mice, which express non-mutant human tau isoforms and represent a model of tauopathy relevant to Alzheimer’s disease. Prior to the development of cognitive symptoms, htau mice exhibit tau hyperphosphorylation associated with a selective deficit in food burrowing, a behavior reminiscent to activities of daily living which are impaired early in Alzheimer’s disease. We crossed htau mice with Nmnat1 transgenic and knockout mice and tested the resulting offspring until the age of 6 months. We show that overexpression of NMNAT1 ameliorates the early deficit in food burrowing characteristic of htau mice. At 6 months of age, htau mice did not show neurodegenerative changes in both the cortex and hippocampus, and these were not induced by downregulating NMNAT1 levels. Modulating NMNAT1 levels produced a corresponding effect on NMNAT enzymatic activity but did not alter NAD levels in htau mice. Although changes in local NAD levels and subsequent modulation of NAD-dependent enzymes cannot be ruled out, this suggests that the effects seen on behavior may be due to changes in tau phosphorylation. Our results suggest that increasing NMNAT1 levels can slow the progression of symptoms and neuropathological features of tauopathy, but the underlying mechanisms remain to be established.
1. Introduction
NAD metabolism is increasingly implicated in a variety of biological functions, including regulation of gene transcription, lifespan, cell death, circadian rhythm and glucose homeostasis [1], [2]. The three mammalian isoforms of the central NAD biosynthetic enzyme, nicotinamide mononucleotide adenylyltransferases (NMNATs), have recently emerged as crucial players in neuronal maintenance and protection [3], [4], [5], [6]. They share the same enzymatic activity but a different subcellular distribution [7], [8]. Cytoplasmic NMNAT2 has been recently described as an axon survival factor, a role which is linked to its enzymatic activity, while NMNAT1 appears to have a similar function in neuronal cell bodies [6]. The discovery that loss-of-function mutations in the nuclear isoform NMNAT1 cause Leber Congenital Amaurosis (LCA), a human nervous system disorder characterized by degeneration of retinal neurons and blindness [9], [10], [11], directly links NMNAT1 to neuron survival. Nuclear NMNAT1 has also been associated with neuroprotection in models of human disorders, such as PolyQ toxicity and hypoxic ischemia, with a mechanism correlated to the enzymatic activity and its reported chaperone function [12], [13]. In Drosophila a single, homogenously distributed NMNAT isoform has been reported, whose loss causes neuronal and axonal degeneration [3].
Studies in Drosophila and mice also highlight NMNAT protective function in models of tauopathies. Neurodegenerative diseases, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD), Pick’s disease and frontotemporal dementia with Parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17 (FTDP-17), are characterized by the presence of hyperphosphorylated tau protein that aggregates and forms neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) [14]. Tau is a microtubule-associated protein (MAP) expressed in neurons and normally localized in axons, where it promotes microtubule assembly and stabilization, depending on its level of phosphorylation [15], [16]. Conversely, hyperphosphorylated tau, as seen in tauopathies, is unable to stabilize microtubule assemblies and sequesters normal tau and other microtubule stabilizing protein such as MAP1A/MAP1B and MAP2, leading to disruption of microtubules and axonal transport, finally resulting in retrograde degeneration and cell death [17]. Furthermore, abnormally hyperphosphorylated tau leads to the formation of NFTs, which are associated with cellular toxicity and dementia [17]. Overexpression of Drosophila NMNAT reduces hyperphosphorylated tau load and rescues cognitive and locomotor deficits caused by overexpression of human wild type or mutant tau in Drosophila [18]. Importantly, NMNAT neuroprotection was also confirmed in a mouse model of tauopathy, where NMNAT2 was downregulated in both cortex and hippocampus of mice overexpressing the FTDP-17 associated mutant tau P301L. In this model, hippocampal-specific overexpression of either NMNAT1or NMNAT2 significantly reduced hippocampal neuronal loss [19]. Furthermore, it has been recently shown that overexpression of NMNAT1, when redistributed in the cytoplasm (cytNMNAT1), preserves cortical functional connectivity and reduces tau aggregation in the brain of P301S mutant human tau transgenic mice [20]. However, little is known on the role of NMNAT1 in tauopathies, when its nuclear subcellular localization is maintained.
We therefore investigated whether modulating the levels of nuclear NMNAT1 alters the progression of early behavioral dysfunctions and pathological abnormalities, in the htau mouse model of tauopathy which has particular relevance to AD [21] by crossing htau mice with Nmnat1 transgenic and knockout mice [22], [23].
The htau mouse is characterized by the expression of non-mutant human tau, coding for all six human tau isoforms, including three (3R) and four (4R) microtubule binding domain repeats [21]. This results in an AD-like pathology characterized by early tau hyperphosphorylation, redistribution into cell bodies and dendrites as well as accumulation to form neurofibrillary tangle-like assemblies [21]. We hypothesized that reducing NMNAT1 levels will accelerate, while overexpressing NMNAT1 will slow, the progression of behavioral and neurological features in htau mice. We have extensively characterized the behavioral phenotype of htau mice with aging and found that they display specific early defects in ‘daily life activities’, assessed in the food burrowing test, from after onset of tau hyperphosphorylation[24].
Here we found that total cellular NAD levels were unaltered by NMNAT1 modulation in htau mice although downregulating or overexpressing NMNAT1 significantly reduced or increased NMNAT enzyme activity, respectively. We found that while overexpressing nuclear NMNAT1 reversed early behavioral deficits characteristic of htau mice and attenuated tau hyperphosphorylation, downregulating NMNAT1 had subtle effects on these features and did not cause neurodegenerative changes. Our findings therefore suggest that an increased amount of nuclear NMNAT1 is protective during the early stages of tauopathy in htau mice.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Mice and genotyping
All experimental animals used were bred and housed in groups at the University of Nottingham Biomedical Service Unit. The animal work was carried out in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act of 1986 (under project licences: PPL 40/3482 and 40/3601), approved by the University of Nottingham Ethical Review Committee and is reported according to the ARRIVE guidelines [25]. All mice were maintained at the University of Nottingham and humanely killed by cervical dislocation. Heterozygous Nmnat1 knockout (KO) and transgenic mice have been previously described [22], [23]. htau mice (STOCK Mapttm1(EGFP)Klt Tg(MAPT)8cPdav/J) were originally purchased from the Jackson Laboratories (Bar Harbor, ME, USA) as described previously [24] and crossed with Nmnat1 mutant mice to produce experimental htau mice with either increased or reduced levels of nuclear NMNAT1.
Mice were genotyped by PCR using the following primers:
Nmnat1+/−
Forward 5′-CCCAGTCACTAAGACATTCAA-3′
Reverse 5′-CCTTCTTGCTTCCCACGAGG-3′;
Nmnat1 tg
Forward 5′-ACTTCGGCTCACAGCGCG-3′
Reverse 5′-TCCTTGGCCAGCTCGAACA-3′;
Human tau
Forward 5′-ACTTTGAACCAGGATGGCTGAGC-3′
Reverse 5′-CTGTGCATGGCTGTCCCTACCTT-3′;
Murine tau
Forward 5′-CCAGTTGTGTATGTCCACCC-3′
Reverse 5′-CTCAGCATCCCACCTGTAAC-3′
Disrupted murine tau
Forward 5′-AAGTTCATCTGCACCACCG-3′,
Reverse 5′-TGCTCAGGTAGTGGTTGTCG-3′ [20].
Nmnat1+/− and Nmnat1 tg were crossed with htau mice producing the genotypes of interest that were all heterozygous for murine tau (mtau+/−): htau, Nmnat1 tg/htau, Nmnat1+/−/htau. Control experiments were first carried out to verify the extent of tau pathology in htau/mtau+/−mice. Behavioral data were further collected from relevant control mice (mtau+/−, Nmnat1 tg and Nmnat1+/− mice), in comparison to wild type mice, as explained below and reported in the supplementary information.
2.2. Study design
12 mice of each genotype (n = 6/sex) were subjected to a range of tests relevant to AD measuring activities of daily living, learning and memory. Locomotor activity and anxiety-related behavior were also tested. Activities of daily living were measured using the food burrowing test. Memory was assessed using spontaneous alternation in the Y-maze, novel object location/discrimination test and contextual fear conditioning. Open-field was used to test locomotor and anxiety-related behavior. A timeline of the experimental plan and behavioral experiments is illustrated in Fig. 1A. The food burrowing test was repeated from two to six months of age in a monthly interval, the spontaneous alternation and open-field tests were repeated at the age of 4 and 6 months and contextual fear conditioning was performed at the age of six months only, as this test cannot be repeated. Behavioral assessment was followed by in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to assess metabolic and structural changes in the hippocampus, after which tissue was collected for post-mortem analyses.
2.3. Food-burrowing test
The protocol was adapted from Deacon et al. [26]. Mice were individually placed for one night in a novel cage with ad libitum access to food and water. A glass jar containing 30 g of food pellet broken into small pieces was added to the cage. The amount of food displaced from the jar was measured. Data was transformed by square root in order to conform them to a normal distribution [27] and analyzed using 3-way ANOVA with sex and genotype and repeated measures over age, followed by post-hoc planned comparisons.
2.4. Spontaneous alternation task
Spontaneous alternation was used to test spatial working memory as previously described [28], [29]. The Y-shaped maze comprised three identical transparent Plexiglas® arms at a 120° angle from each other (41.5 cm in length and 6 cm in width surrounded by 15 cm high transparent Perspex walls). The start point (6 cm x 7.5 cm) was located in the center of the maze, and the animal was allowed to freely explore the three arms over five minutes. The number of alternations was recorded and expressed as a percentage of alternation to estimate spatial working memory performance [28], [29]. Data was analyzed using 3-way ANOVA with sex and genotype and repeated measures over age, followed by post-hoc planned comparisons.
2.5. Open-field
Open field was used to assess locomotor and anxiety-related behavior as previously described [29]. Mice were placed in Perspex activity boxes (30 cm × 35 cm × 25 cm) for 30 min during which total distance moved (cm) and percentage of time spent in the center (s) were recorded using Ethovision software (Noldus, Wagenigen, Netherlands). Data was analyzed using 3-way ANOVA with sex and genotype and repeated measures over age, followed by post-hoc planned comparisons.
2.6. Novel object location and recognition
Novel object location and novel object recognition were used to measure spatial and recognition memory respectively, as previously described [29]. Briefly, this test consists of three separated trials: in the habituation trial each mouse was allowed to explore two identical objects next to each other along one arena wall. After a 10-min-long interval each mouse was placed into the same arena for the location trial, with one of the two objects being moved across to the opposite wall. After a 20-min inter-trial interval, each mouse was allowed to explore the objects for a third time (discrimination trial) in which one object had been exchanged. Total object exploration times, object location and object discrimination times were recorded using Ethovision software (Noldus, Wagenigen, Netherlands). Data was analyzed using 3-way ANOVA with sex and genotype as between subject factors and repeated measures over age, followed by post-hoc planned comparisons. For each group, preference indices in the object tests were compared to chance levels (50%) using one-sample t-tests, in order to determine if the mice were able to discriminate the novel location or novel object.
2.7. Contextual fear conditioning (CFC)
Associative learning and memory were assessed using contextual fear conditioning following a previously described protocol [29]. Briefly, this test consisted of three trials conducted on three consecutive days using the same chamber (25 cm × 25 cm × 38 cm): (1) in the acquisition trial, mice were exposed to a series of five foot-shocks (1s-long, 0.4 mA), each separated by a 60-s interval. (2) In the retention trial, mice were placed in the same chamber for one minute without the application of foot shocks. (3) In the one-minute-long extinction trial foot shocks were also omitted. Analysis of the time that the mice spent immobile was conducted in blind twice, and the data were averaged for statistical analysis. Data from the acquisition trial were analyzed using 3-way ANOVA with sex and genotype as between subject factors and repeated measures over time (minutes separating each shock application), followed by post-hoc planned comparisons; data from retention and extinction trials were analyzed using two-way ANOVA, followed by post-hoc planned comparisons.
2.8. In vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy and imaging
Animals were anaesthetized with 1% − 1.75% isoflurane in a nitrous oxide:oxygen = 2:1 gas mixture and placed in a custom built holder inside the MR scanner to minimize head motion. Throughout the study, breathing rate and body temperature were monitored continuously and kept constant through adjustment of anesthesia levels and warm water circulation.
In vivo MRI and MRS data were acquired from mice of all four genotypes (male and female, age = 6 months, Suppl. Table 1) using a horizontal 7 T MR imaging system (Avance Biospec 70/30 USR; Bruker, Karlsruhe, Germany) equipped with a volume transmit/surface receive radiofrequency coil combination.
MR spectroscopy data were acquired from a 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 voxel (volume of interest) centered on the left hippocampus and positioned with multi slice rapid acquisition with relaxation enhancement (RARE) images [30] (Fig. 3A). A line-width of 10–15 Hz for the water peak was obtained after adjustment of first- and second-order shims with FASTMAP [31]. A Point-Resolved Spin echo sequence (PRESS) [32] was used to acquire the MR spectra (repetition time, TR = 2000 ms, echo time, TE = 14 ms, 1024 signal averages) after water suppression with variable pulse power and optimized relaxation delays (VAPOR) [33]. The total acquisition time was 34 min. All spectra processing was conducted blinded to group assignment. Spectra were analyzed using LCModel (Version 6.3-0I) [34]. Resonances corresponding to myo-inositol (Ins), taurine, glutamate (Glu), N-acetylaspartate (NAA), glutamine (Gln), glutathione (GSH), glycerophosphocholine and phosphatidylcholine (PCh + GPC), and creatine and phosphocreatine (Cr + PCr) were quantified relative to the total metabolic content (Glu + PCh + GSH + Ins + NAA + Gln + Taurine + Cr + pCr). Only results with the Cramer–Rao lower bounds ≤15% were included in the analysis. Data was further analyzed using ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc test.
For anatomical MRI, 30 coronal slices were acquired using a RARE sequence (256 × 256 matrix size, field of view, FOV = 1.66 × 1.66 cm, 0.3 mm slice thickness; TR = 7500 ms, effective TE = 29.4 ms, RARE factor = 8, number of averages = 8). The total acquisition time per volume was 32 min. What is referred to as hippocampal areas (covered in 10+/− 1 coronal slices) in the context of this work included fields CA1, CA2 and CA3, the pyramidal cell and lacunosum moleculare layers, the stratum lucidum and radiatum of the hippocampus as well as the dentate gyrus including its molecular, polymorph and granular layers as well as the subiculum relating to Figures. 39 –67 in The Mouse Brain Atlas by Paxinos & Franklin [35]. Hippocampal volumes were determined by manually drawing regions of interest (Fig. 3D) in blind using a software [Medical Image Processing, Analysis and Visualization (MIPAV); v. 7.0.1; Center for Information Technology (CIT), National Institutes of Health (NIH)]. The mean of the hippocampal volume of the left and right hemispheres was expressed as percentage of the total brain volume as outlined in The Mouse Brain Atlas [35]. Since the hippocampal and whole-brain volume did not differ across the two sexes they were combined within each genotype for the statistical analysis of genotype differences. Data was analyzed using ANOVA followed by Fisher’s LSD post-hoc test.
2.9. Tissue processing and staining
Mice were humanely killed using an approved Schedule 1 method (cervical dislocation) at six-months-of-age, immediately after the MRS procedure. Half brains from htau/mice, Nmnat1 tg/htau/mtau+/−, Nmnat1+/−/htau/mtau+/− and mtau+/− mice were post-fixed in 4% PFA for a minimum of 48 h at room temperature. Brains were dehydrated by an ascending alcohol series and then embedded in paraffin. Paraffin blocks were cut into coronal microtome sections (thickness 8 μm) and drawn up on microscope slides.
For evaluation of gross hippocampal morphology, 8 μm coronal sections were mounted on Superfrost slides (BDH), air dried and stained with Haematoxylin and Eosin (H&E). (n = 5/genotype). H&E stained sections were imaged by low power (Plan-NEOFLUOAR 1.25X/0.035) light microscopy (Zeiss Axioplan). Sections were matched between all genotypes and used for the analysis and hippocampal areas were measured using ImageJ software. Data were analyzed using one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc test.
For immunohistochemistry, cut sections were deparaffinized in xylene and rehydrated through decreasing concentrations of ethanol before the immunohistochemical procedure. Sections were then rinsed in water and subsequently incubated at room temperature in a solution, containing 0.3% H2O2 diluted in PBS (0.1 mol/L, pH 7.4) for 30 min in order to block endogenous peroxidases. After rinsing, sections were treated with 10 mM sodium citrate buffer pH 6 for 20 min at 100 °C for antigen retrieval. Sections were then incubated with mouse immunoglobulin G blocking reagent (mouse on mouse [MOM] Kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) for 1 h in a humid chamber at room temperature, subsequently incubated with monoclonal antibody CP13 (1:25, generously provided by Prof. Peter Davies, Bronx, NY, USA [21]) and developed with 3,3′-diaminobenzidine (DAB). Sections were scanned and viewed using NanoZoomer digital slide scanner (Hamamatsu, Japan).
2.10. Western immunoblotting
Freshly frozen hippocampi and cortices from all genotypes (n = 3-5/genotype) were homogenized in RIPA lysis buffer containing protease and phosphatase inhibitors (Complete mini, Roche). The protein samples were then loaded on a 10% SDS polyacrylamide gel and subsequently transferred to nitrocellulose membrane using a semi-dry blotting apparatus (BioRad). Membranes were blocked for 1 h in 5% BSA in PBS plus 0.2% Tween-20 (PBST), incubated overnight with primary antibody in 5% BSA at 4 °C and incubated for 1 h at RT with HPRT-linked secondary antibody in 3% BSA in PBST. The following primary antibodies were used: mouse anti-synaptophysin (Dako clone Y38, 1:10000), Phospho-PHF-tau pSer202 + Thr205 (AT8, Pierce, 1:500). As loading controls, mouse anti-β-tubulinIII (Sigma, 1:5000) and mouse anti-β-actin (Abcam, 1:5000) were used. Bands were visualized on film using a chemiluminescent detection kit (GE Healthcare). Data was analyzed using one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc test.
2.11. NMNAT enzyme activity assay and NAD level determination
Cortices and hippocampi of 6-month-old mice, collected at the end of the MRS procedure, were snap-frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at −80 °C. On the day of use, tissues were homogenized in 0.5 M HEPES buffer pH 7.5 and immediately processed for NAD level and NMNAT enzyme activity determination. NAD was then quantified with the HPLC-based assay as described previously [36] (n = 4/group). Data was analyzed using one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc test.
2.12. Control experiments
2.12.1. Tau pathology in htau mice heterozygous for murine tau (htau/mtau+/−)
These were compared to control wild type C57BL/6J mice (wild type, Charles River UK). Previous characterization of htau mice has been in the context of the homozygous deletion of murine tau as removal of murine tau was found necessary for the tau pathology to occur in htau mice [21]. However, we have found that murine tau knockout mice develop a behavioral phenotype with aging which bears some similarities to the behavioral phenotype of htau mice but progresses more slowly [24] and, as reported previously, is associated with the occurrence of systemic pathologies [37]. Since there is no evidence that complete removal of murine tau is necessary for the tau pathology to occur in htau mice, we first tested whether a heterozygous murine tau knockout background, which would prevent adverse effects of murine tau deletion, is sufficient for the pathology to occur by carrying out western immunoblotting of total (Tau46), early (CP13) and late (PHF1) pathological stage tau phosphorylation on cortical tissue from three-four 6-month-old mice per genotype, as described previously [24]. These preliminary findings, reported in Suppl. Fig. 1, reveal similar levels of total (Tau46, Suppl. Fig. 1. A&B) and phosphorylated (CP13: Suppl. Fig. 1. C&D; PHF1: Suppl. Fig. 1. E&F) tau in htau mice, regardless of whether they were bred on a full or heterozygous murine tau knockout background. In htau mice, hyperphosphorylated tau was found to redistribute from its axonal position to the cell body and dendrites from 3 months of age [21]. In order to assess whether a similar redistribution was also evident in htau mice heterozygous for murine tau, immunohistochemistry was performed with CP13 antibody [21]. We also found that tau is more abundant in the axons of wild type mice (Suppl. Fig. 1G and I), while it redistributes to a perinuclear location in htau mice which are heterozygous KO for murine tau (htau/mtau+/− Suppl. Fig. 1H and J), similar to previous reports in htau mice homozygous for the murine deletion [21]. These observations indicate that reducing murine tau level by half is sufficient for the tau pathology to develop in htau mice.
2.12.2. Behavioral characteristics of mtau+/−, nmnat1 tg and Nmnat1+/− mice
We have previously reported that full murine tau knockout mice (mtau−/−) display significant hyperactivity from 4 months of age and deficits in food burrowing from 9 months of age, compared to wild type C57BL/6J mice [24]. We therefore compared the performance of 4- and 6- months-old mtau+/− and wild type mice (n = 12/genotype) for these two features and found no differences (Suppl. Table 2 and Suppl. Fig. 2A and C). This indicates that potentially confounding effects of murine tau deletion in htau mice can be avoided with a heterozygous murine tau knockout background without affecting the occurrence of tau pathology. Likewise, we assessed these behaviors sensitive to tau dysfunction in Nmnat1 tg and Nmnat1+/− mice of the same ages (n = 12/genotype) and found unaltered food burrowing performance (Suppl. Fig. 2B) but Nmnat1 tg developed an hyperactive phenotype with age (Suppl. Table 1 and Suppl. Fig. 2C).
3. Results
The effect of sex/genotype/age interactions and results of the ANOVA for behavioral variables are reported in Table 1.
Table 1.
Num. df | Den. df | F-value | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Food burrowing test | ||||
sex | 1 | 64 | 13.96 | <0.001 |
genotype | 3 | 64 | 4.63 | 0.005 |
age | 4 | 237 | 13.65 | <0.001 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 64 | 0.37 | 0.778 |
sex X age | 4 | 237 | 0.93 | 0.449 |
genotype X age | 12 | 237 | 1.82 | 0.046 |
sex X genotype X age | 12 | 237 | 0.71 | 0.741 |
Acquisition of contextual fear | ||||
sex | 1 | 30 | 0.11 | 0.737 |
genotype | 3 | 30 | 0.04 | 0.988 |
minute | 4 | 120 | 135.69 | < 0.001 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 30 | 0.04 | 0.987 |
sex X minute | 4 | 120 | 1.82 | 0.13 |
genotype X minute | 12 | 120 | 0.4 | 0.962 |
sex X genotype X minute | 12 | 120 | 0.74 | 0.71 |
Retention of contextual fear | ||||
sex | 1 | 8.19 | 0.08 | 0.775 |
genotype | 3 | 74.47 | 0.76 | 0.527 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 51.48 | 0.52 | 0.67 |
Extinction of contextual fear | ||||
sex | 1 | 2.05 | 0.08 | 0.775 |
genotype | 3 | 18.62 | 0.76 | 0.527 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 12.87 | 0.52 | 0.67 |
Novel object habituation | ||||
sex | 1 | 64 | 0.54 | 0.466 |
genotype | 3 | 64 | 0.62 | 0.603 |
age | 1 | 64 | 2.1 | 0.152 |
sex X age X genotype | 3 | 64 | 0.07 | 0.976 |
sex X age | 1 | 64 | 1 | 0.321 |
genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 1.03 | 0.386 |
sex X genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 0.25 | 0.858 |
Novel object location | ||||
sex | 1 | 64 | 0.09 | 0.771 |
genotype | 3 | 64 | 0.3 | 0.827 |
age | 1 | 64 | 7.17 | 0.009 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 64 | 0.58 | 0.633 |
sex X age | 1 | 64 | 5.81 | 0.019 |
genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 1.11 | 0.35 |
sex X genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 1.06 | 0.372 |
Novel object discrimination | ||||
sex | 1 | 64 | 0.31 | 0.58 |
genotype | 3 | 64 | 1.19 | 0.319 |
age | 1 | 64 | 0.37 | 0.548 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 64 | 1.73 | 0.17 |
sex X age | 1 | 64 | 1.8 | 0.184 |
genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 0.77 | 0.516 |
sex X genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 1.1 | 0.358 |
Open field (locomotor activity) | ||||
sex | 1 | 64 | 2.75 | 0.102 |
genotype | 3 | 64 | 5.72 | 0.002 |
age | 1 | 64 | 53.11 | < 0.001 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 64 | 2.28 | 0.088 |
sex X age | 1 | 64 | 0.86 | 0.358 |
genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 2.94 | 0.04 |
sex X genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 0.7 | 0.558 |
Open field (anxiety index) | ||||
sex | 1 | 64 | 1.42 | 0.238 |
genotype | 3 | 64 | 0.96 | 0.417 |
age | 1 | 64 | 0 | 0.966 |
sex X genotype | 3 | 64 | 0.52 | 0.668 |
sex X age | 1 | 64 | 1.72 | 0.194 |
genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 2.75 | 0.05 |
sex X genotype X age | 3 | 64 | 1.95 | 0.131 |
3.1. NMNAT1 overexpression rescues an early impairment in food burrowing performance in htau mice
Subtle behavioral abnormalities, preceding cognitive dysfunction, are among early signs in AD [38]. In htau mice, cognitive decline has been detected by 12 months of age [39], but we have found earlier non-cognitive behavioral abnormalities in the food burrowing test emerging by 4 months of age, after onset of tau pathology [24]. This test has been used to assess activities of daily living and the inability to perform these activities is an early sign of AD [27]. We therefore subjected the mice to the food burrowing test from 2 months to 6 months of age. There were significant genotype (F(3.64) = 4.63; p = 0.005) and age X genotype interaction (F(12,237) = 1.82; p = 0.046) effects, regardless of sex, but females overall performed better in this task (F(1.64) = 13.96; p < 0.001). htau/mtau+/− mice showed a marked impairment in food burrowing (p < 0.05 compared to wild type at 2, 3, 4 and 6 months of age, Fig. 1B–F). Strikingly, we found that increasing NMNAT1 levels improved the food burrowing performance from 3 months of age and at all later points (Fig. 1C–F), but not at 2 months of age. However, reducing NMNAT1 levels did not further decrease the htau/mtau+/− mice’s impaired performance (Fig. 1B–F). Thus, NMNAT1 deficiency does not contribute to food burrowing impairment in htau/mtau+/− mice, but NMNAT1 overexpression rescues this deficit.
3.2. Locomotor activity is increased in htau/mtau+/− mice overexpressing NMNAT1
We assessed locomotor activity and anxiety-related behavior in mice of all genotypes using the open field. Anxiety-related behavior did not differ between the genotypes at both four and six months of age (Table 1 and Fig. 2A and D). Interestingly, there were significant genotype (F(3.64) = 5.72; p = 0.002) and age X genotype interaction (F(3.64) = 2.94; p = 0.04) effects for locomotor activity (Table 1) which was higher in six month old htau/mtau+/− mice overexpressing NMNAT1 than in the other genotypes (Fig. 2A). This age-dependent higher activity is linked to NMNAT1 overexpression as it was also observed in Nmnat1 tg mice compared to wild type mice (Suppl. Fig. 2D and Suppl. Table 2).
3.3. htau/mtau+/− mice do not show learning and memory deficits at six-months-of-age
In the spontaneous alternation test, mice of all genotypes alternated arms with a frequency higher than chance without statistically significant differences across the genotypes (Table 1, Fig. 2C and F). Likewise, mice of all genotypes displayed a comparable performance in object recognition and location both at 4 (Suppl. Fig. 3A, C and E) and 6 (Suppl. Fig. 3B, D and F) months, as tested by the novel object paradigm. In the contextual fear conditioning test, during the acquisition trial, all mice were able to acquire contextual fear (F(4;120) = 135.69; p < 0.001. Table 1 and Fig. 2G). No significant differences were detected between the different genotypes in contextual memory retention and extinction (Table 1 and Fig. 2G–H).
3.4. Absence of neurodegenerative changes in the htau mouse brain
We then asked whether any sign of neurodegeneration in the htau/mtau+/− mouse brains could explain the specific defect seen in the food burrowing test (Fig. 1), which has been found to be linked to hippocampal and cortical integrity [40], [41]. First, we measured non-invasively the levels of Ins, taurine, Glu, NAA, Gln, GSH, PCh + GPC, and total creatine (Cr + PCr) in vivo using MRS in the hippocampus of six-month-old mice, as the level of these metabolites was found to change during neurodegenerative processes [42], [43]. We did not observe any statistically significant difference in metabolite levels, assessed by MRS across the four genotypes at the time point studied (Fig. 3C). We also measured the volume of the hippocampus in vivo (Fig. 3E) and post-mortem (Fig. 4A and B) and found no differences across the genotypes. Finally, we determined the levels of the synaptic marker synaptophysin in the hippocampus (Fig. 4E) and cortex (Fig. 5F) and no significant changes were detected in all genotypes.
Taken together, these data show that downregulation of NMNAT1 levels does not cause metabolic or neurodegenerative changes in the brain of htau/mtau+/− mice up to six months of age and that rescue of the early behavioral abnormality, i.e. the reduced performance in the food burrowing test, by NMNAT1 overexpression may not be due to its neuroprotective property.
3.5. NAD levels are not affected by changes in NMNAT enzyme activity
We determined NMNAT enzyme activity and NAD levels in the cortex and hippocampus of htau/mtau+/− mice. Overexpression or downregulation of NMNAT1 in htau/mtau+/− mice had expected corresponding effect on enzymatic activity, which was reduced in Nmnat1+/−/htau/mtau+/− mice and increased in Nmnat1tg/htau/mtau+/− mice (Fig. 5A-B). Changes in enzymatic activity were not associated with alterations in steady-state NAD levels (Fig. 5C–D). This indicates that behavioral effects of NMNAT1 overexpression in htau/mtau+/− are not caused by detectable changes in NAD levels.
3.6. Levels of hyperphosphorylated tau in brains of htau mice are reduced by NMNAT1 overexpression
We showed previously that deficits in food burrowing develop after onset of tau hyperphosphorylation in htau mice [24]. Therefore in order to test whether behavioral effects of NMNAT1 overexpression are associated with changes in hyperphosphorylated tau, western immunoblotting of AT8 were performed in the cortex (Fig. 5E) and hippocampus (Fig. 5F), as both regions were found to mediate food burrowing performance [40], [41]. AT8 levels were significantly increased in the cortex of htau/mtau+/− mice and this was prevented by overexpressing NMNAT1 in Nmnat1tg/htau/mtau+/− mice (Fig. 5 E). In contrast, in the hippocampus, AT8 levels in htau/mtau+/− mice were significantly increased only when NMNAT1 was downregulated (Fig. 5F). These results first suggest that tau pathology develop faster in the cortex than hippocampus, as seen in other models of tauopathy (mtau and rTg4510 lines) [44], [45]. They also suggest that overexpressing NMNAT1 in htau/mtau+/− mice may protect against tau hyperphosphorylation, possibly contributing to alleviation of early behavioral deficits.
4. Discussion
NMNAT1 plays an important role in neuron maintenance and its loss of function causes neurodegeneration [11]. In addition, NMNAT1 is neuroprotective in several animal models of neurodegenerative disorders [19]. Our study extends these observations to a mouse model of tauopathy relevant to AD as we found that overexpressing NMNAT1 in mice which express wild type human tau rescues an early behavioral deficit associated with the development of tau pathology [24]. However, cerebral morphometry and cognitive deficits, which are not yet present at this stage of the pathology, were not induced by NMNAT1downregulation. The lack of differences in cognitive deficits is in contrast with previous findings that reported an impairment in visuospatial learning and memory in Mapttm1(EGFP)Klt Tg(MAPT)8cPdav/J mouse (htau mice), at six months of age [46]. However, in this pilot study, only three mice were used and tested with different behavioral tests, based on operant conditioning while, in contrast, our tasks aimed to explore spontaneous exploration.
The htau mouse provides a model to study tau pathology relevant to AD through the expression of non-mutant human tau that develops a neuropathology which is reminiscent of that occurring in the early stages of AD [21], [24], [39]. The food burrowing test exploits a natural rodent behavior which requires the integrity of the hippocampus and frontal cortex (brain areas particularly sensitive to AD-like pathology) and is affected following lesions of these brain areas [26], [41]. The first evident symptom in AD is a deterioration in the ability to perform ‘daily-life activity’ which, in mouse models, correspond to burrowing and nest construction [27]. Although behavioral screening in AD patients are mainly focused on cognitive tests, it may be important to assess more specific behaviors which, as suggested by our results, could be modified at early stage. We found that increasing NMNAT1 levels in htau/mtau+/− mice progressively rescued the specific food burrowing deficit at three, four and six months of age, which may be related to a protective effect of NMNAT1 on tau hyperphosphorylation in the cortex. This protective effect might develop with age, which could explain the lack of protection at the first time point tested. Reducing NMNAT1 levels did not further decrease the food burrowing performance of htau mice; this may be due to the fact that the deficit of the htau mouse was already pronounced producing a ceiling effect, or to the fact that NMNAT1 overexpression has a greater impact on NMNAT activity (+200%) than its downregulation (-75%), since baseline NMNAT activity is moderate. However, the fact that NMNAT1 downregulation appear to have opposite effects on tau phosphorylation in the cortex and the hippocampus could also contribute to the lack of exacerbation of burrowing behavior which is mediated by both brain regions [40], [41].
Interestingly, we found that htau/mtau+/− mice overexpressing NMNAT1 retain the significant increase in locomotor activity also evident in six-months-old Nmnat1 tg mice compared to their wild-type littermates. Overexpression of NMNAT1 has been proposed to delay aging and improve metabolism ([47] and Rossi, Conforti et al., unpublished), an observation that could explain why Nmnat1 tg mice are more active.
To gain understanding of the mechanisms underlying the modulation of features characteristic of tauopathy in htau mice by alteration in NMNAT1 levels, we compared brain metabolite levels between genotypes in vivo, using MRS. This method has been widely used to study cellular mechanisms preceding complex pathological conditions [48] such as in models of amyloidosis and Pick disease [49], [50]. Changes in specific metabolites may indicate the onset or the course of specific neurodegenerative processes, therefore measuring brain metabolism might be a sensitive method to screen for early biomarkers of pathology. For example, NAA, a neuronal marker, is reduced in the medial temporal lobe in AD patients that show only mild cognitive impairments [51]. Taurine, which plays a role in osmoregulation and neuroprotection in the central nervous system, is also reduced in a murine mouse model of amyloidosis [52]. Several lines of evidence suggest that other metabolites, such as myo-inositol, a glial marker, Glu, Gly, and choline, are changed in AD [53], [54]. In the present study, the metabolic contents of the hippocampus in six month old htau/mtau+/− mice remained unchanged. Likewise, no gross structural changes were detected in the presence of human tau as determined by in vivo MRI and post-mortem tissue. However, these sensitive methods were able to detect changes in other more aggressive models of tauopathies [55] and the lack of changes is therefore consistent with the lack of aggravation of tau pathology by downregulation of NMNAT1.
NMNAT enzyme activity in htau/mtau+/− mice was similar to that of their control littermate mtau+/, while it was increased when NMNAT1 was overexpressed and decreased when NMNAT1 was downregulated, in agreement to corresponding changes in activity in Nmnat1 tg and Nmnat1+/− mice [22], [23]. Despite the modulation of NMNAT enzyme activity, which correlated to its levels of expression, steady-state NAD levels were not significantly different in all genotypes. Genetic overexpression of NMNAT in muscle was also found to have modest effects on NAD concentrations, which were thought to be maintained at a steady-state levels through a negative feedback mechanism [56]. We, however, cannot rule out that the increased NMNAT activity could cause localized changes in NAD availability and therefore affect the activity of downstream NAD dependent enzymes. In support of this, recent observations correlate nuclear NAD changes sensitive to the levels of NMNAT1 with sirtuin 1 (SIRT1), a NAD-dependent deacethylase, and disruption of nuclear-mitochondrial communication [47]. A number of mitochondrial proteins, involved in oxidative phosphorylation and other metabolic pathways were found altered in the cerebral cortices of 6 month old males 3xTg-AD which develop tau pathology as well as amyloid deposition [57]. Importantly, these alterations were found to precede amyloid deposition and neurofibrillary tangles formation, suggesting mitochondrial dysregulation as an early event in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease [58]. Although muscle data failed to observe alterations in mitochondrial NAD content following genetic overexpression of NMNAT [56], possible changes in mitochondrial homeostasis amongst the different NMNAT1 genotypes would support the hypothesis of localized variations of NAD availability and could be associated with more subtle neurodegenenerative changes. This will need to be investigated in further studies.
4.1. The protective effects of NMNAT1 overexpression in htau mice could be mediated by SIRT1, regardless of changes in NAD concentrations
SIRT1 is ubiquitously present in areas of the brain especially susceptible to age-related neurodegenerative states (e.g., the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and basal ganglia) [59] and was found able to reduce the level of tau aggregates by promoting deacetylation and clearance of toxic tau species, a mechanism thought to be dependent upon nuclear NAD availability [60]. But SIRT1 activation could also be mediated by an increase in the cellular ratio of NAD to its reduced form NADH [56], [61] which is known to occur in response to an increased in NMAPT expression and activity [62] and can be associated with changes in enzymatic activity independently from changes in NAD concentrations [61], [62].
SIRT1 may also be involved in the delayed protection by NMNAT1 overexpression of the food burrowing deficit characteristic of htau mice. While NMNAT1 is located into the nucleus [7], SIRT1 shows dynamic changes in its subcellular localization during development and as a function of stress status [63]. In the mouse heart, SIRT1 is exclusively expressed in the nucleus at embryonic stages, but found in both the nucleus and cytoplasm at adulthood [64]. Although SIRT1 subcellular localization remains predominantly nuclear in the adult healthy brain [59], where it facilitates tau clearance [60], this redistribution could contribute to onset of tau pathology and associated early deficits in food burrowing behavior. Cellular stress, as is induced by tau accumulation [65], was found to trigger nuclear transport of SIRT1 [63], as well as alternative splicing of neuronal NMNAT towards the cytoplasmic variant which has robust neuroprotective capacity [66]. As such, the onset of tau pathology could switch NMNAT1 towards enhanced neuroprotective functions, in part via dynamic changes in SIRT1 subcellular localization. This mode of action could also possibly explain the differential effects of NMNAT1 downregulation on phosphorylated tau levels in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex, in which tau pathology develops at a different rate as. Insufficient nuclear SIRT1 levels is a possible mechanism whereby reducing NMNAT1 level by half could accelerate the onset of tau hyperphosphorylation. Our hippocampus data are indeed in agreement with previous observation in a Drosophila model of tauopathy in which it was found that downregulation of NMNAT accelerates the development of tau pathology [18]. It is however unclear why downregulation of NMNAT1 attenuated tau phosphorylation in the cortex, where the pathology developed earlier. Although one limitation of our study is that we only assessed tau hyperphosphorylation using AT8 immunoblotting, it is possible that reducing NMNAT1 level by half is not sufficient to prevent a neuroprotective response to tau accumulation when the pathology progresses. Altogether, this suggests that neuroprotective effects of NMNAT1 on behavioral symptoms, and possibly tau pathology, would only occur after disease onset, as was also seen in a Drosophila model of tauopathy [18]. The implication of SIRT1 in the age-dependency of the beneficial effects of nmnat1will need to be addressed in future studies.
In addition, protective effects of NMNAT could also be linked to its chaperone-like activity as seen in models a of polyQ toxicity [64] and in a Drosophila model of tauopathy where the chaperone function of NMNAT was found to contribute to tau clearance [18]. A similar explanation was proposed for the neuroprotective effect of lentiviral expression of NMNAT1 or NMNAT2 in the hippocampus of a mouse model of tauopathy [19].
5. Conclusion
In summary, we show that the reported early, specific behavioral deficit in food burrowing observed in htau mice [24] can be alleviated when NMNAT1 is overexpressed. The capability of NMNAT1 to notably ameliorate the food burrowing performance in the htau mouse model at very early stage highlights the potential for NMNAT1 to be a target for early stage AD and other pathologies, but the underlying mechanisms need to be addressed in future studies.
Authors contribution
LC and MCP conceived and planned the study; LC, FR, PCG and MCP designed most experiments, with input from HF and MP; FR, PCG and WM performed most experiments; AHS, AL, MP, MRB and MYL conducted some experiments; data were collected and analyzed by FR, PCG and MRB, with input from WM, MYL and HF. LC and MCP supervised and coordinated the research with help from HF. FR, MCP and LC co-wrote the manuscript with additional input from PCG.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by a University of Nottingham “Bridging the Gaps: Next Generation Feasibility Award” to LC and PCG, and by a Non clinical senior fellowship of the Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences to LC. FR was supported by EURES and the Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences. This work was also supported by the Medical Research Council [grant number MR/K017853/1] through a MRC-CASE Studentship award with Eisai Ltd (MRB,MCP). We thank Prof Dorothee Auer for helpful advice with the MRS and MRI aspects of the study, David Tooth for his technical support and BSU staff for their help with the in vivo studies. We thank Prof Peter Davies for kindly providing the CP13 antibody and for helpful advice on the CP13 immunostaining, and The University of Nottingham Medical Engineering unit for custom built equipment.
Footnotes
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.11.030.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following are Supplementary data to this article:
References
- 1.Imai S., Yoshino J. The importance of NAMPT/NAD/SIRT1 in the systemic regulation of metabolism and ageing. Diabetes, Obes. Metabol. 2013;15(Suppl (3)):26–33. doi: 10.1111/dom.12171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Magni G., Orsomando G., Raffelli N., Ruggieri S. Enzymology of mammalian NAD metabolism in health and disease. Frontiers in bioscience. J. Virtual Lib. 2008;13(May (01)):6135–6154. doi: 10.2741/3143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Zhai R.G., Cao Y., Hiesinger P.R., Zhou Y., Mehta S.Q., Schulze K.L. Drosophila NMNAT maintains neural integrity independent of its NAD synthesis activity. PLoS Biol. 2006;4(November (12)):e416. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gilley J., Adalbert R., Yu G., Coleman M.P. Rescue of peripheral and CNS axon defects in mice lacking NMNAT2. J. Neurosci. 2013;33(August (33)):13410–13424. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1534-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hicks A.N., Lorenzetti D., Gilley J., Lu B., Andersson K.E., Miligan C. Nicotinamide mononucleotide adenylyltransferase 2 (Nmnat2) regulates axon integrity in the mouse embryo. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47869. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gilley J., Coleman M.P. Endogenous nmna is an essential survival factor for maintenance of healthy axons. PLoS Biol. 2010;8(January (1)) doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Berger F., Lau C., Dahlmann M., Ziegler M. Subcellular compartmentation and differential catalytic properties of the three human nicotinamide mononucleotide adenylyltransferase isoforms. J. Biol. Chem. 2005;280(October (43)):36334–36341. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M508660200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Magni G., Amici A., Emanuelli M., Orsomando G., Raffaelli N., Ruggieri S. Enzymology of NAD+ homeostasis in man. Cell. Mol. Life Sci.: CMLS. 2004;61(January (1)):19–34. doi: 10.1007/s00018-003-3161-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kaplan J., Perrault I., Hanein S., Dollfus H., Rozet J.M. [Mutations in NMNAT1 cause Leber congenital amaurosis with severe macular and optic atrophy] Med. Sci.: M/S. 2013;29(January (1)):26–27. doi: 10.1051/medsci/2013291008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Koenekoop R.K., Wang H., Majewski J., Wang X., Lopez I., Ren H. Mutations in NMNAT1 cause Leber congenital amaurosis and identify a new disease pathway for retinal degeneration. Nat. Genet. 2012;44(September (9)):1035–1039. doi: 10.1038/ng.2356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Perrault I., Hanein S., Zanlonghi X., Serre V., Nicouleau M., Defoort-Delhemmes S. Mutations in NMNAT1 cause Leber congenital amaurosis with early-onset severe macular and optic atrophy. Nat. Genet. 2012;44(September (9)):975–977. doi: 10.1038/ng.2357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Verghese P.B., Sasaki Y., Yang D., Stewart F., Sabar F., Finn M.B. Nicotinamide mononucleotide adenylyl transferase 1 protects against acute neurodegeneration in developing CNS by inhibiting excitotoxic-necrotic cell death. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2011;108(November (47)):19054–19059. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107325108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ocampo A., Liu J., Barrientos A. NAD+ salvage pathway proteins suppress proteotoxicity in yeast models of neurodegeneration by promoting the clearance of misfolded/oligomerized proteins. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2013;22(May (9)):1699–1708. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddt016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Huang Y., Mucke L. Alzheimer mechanisms and therapeutic strategies. Cell. 2012;148(March (6)):1204–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.02.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Espinoza M., de Silva R., Dickson D.W., Davies P. Differential incorporation of tau isoforms in Alzheimer's disease. J. Alzheimer's Dis.: JAD. 2008;14(March (1)):1–16. doi: 10.3233/jad-2008-14101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sergeant N., Delacourte A., Buee L. Tau protein as a differential biomarker of tauopathies. Bba-Mol Basis Dis. 2005;1739(January (2–3)):179–197. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2004.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Iqbal K., Alonso A.D.C., Chen S., Chohan M.O., El-Akkad E., Gong C.X. Tau pathology in Alzheimer disease and other tauopathies. Bba-Mol Basis Dis. 2005;1739(January (2–3)):198–210. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2004.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ali Y.O., Ruan K., Zhai R.G. NMNAT suppresses Tau-induced neurodegeneration by promoting clearance of hyperphosphorylated Tau oligomers in a Drosophila model of tauopathy. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2012;21(January (2)):237–250. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddr449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ljungberg M.C., Ali Y.O., Zhu J., Wu C.S., Oka K., Zhai R.G. CREB-activity and nmnat2 transcription are down-regulated prior to neurodegeneration, while NMNAT2 over-expression is neuroprotective, in a mouse model of human tauopathy. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2012;21(January (2)):251–267. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddr492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Musiek E.S., Xiong D.D., Patel T., Sasaki Y., Wang Y.N., Bauer A.Q. Nmnat1 protects neuronal function without altering phospho-tau pathology in a mouse model of tauopathy. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neur. 2016;3(January (6)):434–442. doi: 10.1002/acn3.308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Andorfer C., Kress Y., Espinoza M., de Silva R., Tucker K.L., Barde Y.A. Hyperphosphorylation and aggregation of tau in mice expressing normal human tau isoforms. J. Neurochem. 2003;86(August (3)):582–590. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.01879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Conforti L., Janeckova L., Wagner D., Mazzola F., Cialabrini L., Di Stefano M. Reducing expression of NAD(+) synthesizing enzyme NMNAT1 does not affect the rate of Wallerian degeneration. FEBS J. 2011;278(August (15)):2666–2679. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-4658.2011.08193.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Conforti L., Fang G., Beirowski B., Wang M.S., Sorci L., Asress S. NAD(+) and axon degeneration revisited: nmnat1 cannot substitute for Wld(S) to delay Wallerian degeneration. Cell Death Differ. 2007;14(January (1)):116–127. doi: 10.1038/sj.cdd.4401944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Geiszler P.C., Barron M.R., Pardon M.C. Impaired burrowing is the most prominent behavioral deficit of aging htau mice. Neuroscience. 2016;239(August):98–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kilkenny C., Browne W.J., Cuthill I.C., Emerson M., Altman D.G. Improving bioscience research reporting: the ARRIVE guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol. 2010;8(January (6)) doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Deacon R.M. Burrowing: a sensitive behavioural assay, tested in five species of laboratory rodents. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;200(January (1)):128–133. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Deacon R. Assessing burrowing, nest construction, and hoarding in mice. J. Visualized Exp.: JoVE. 2012;(January (59)):e2607. doi: 10.3791/2607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Pardon M.C., Kendall D.A., Perez-Diaz F., Duxon M.S., Marsden C.A. Repeated sensory contact with aggressive mice rapidly leads to an anticipatory increase in core body temperature and physical activity that precedes the onset of aversive responding. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004;20(August (4)):1033–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03549.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bonardi C., de Pulford F., Jennings D., Pardon M.C. A detailed analysis of the early context extinction deficits seen in APPswe/PS1dE9 female mice and their relevance to preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;222(September (1)):89–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hennig J., Nauerth A., Friedburg H. RARE imaging: a fast imaging method for clinical MR. Magn. Reson. Med. 1986;3(December (6)):823–833. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910030602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Automatic Gruetter R. Localized invivo adjustment of all 1st-Order and 2nd-Order shim coils. Magn. Reson. Med. 1993;29(June (6)):804–811. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910290613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bottomley P.A. Spatial localization in NMR spectroscopy in vivo. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1987;508:333–348. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1987.tb32915.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tkac I., Starcuk Z., Choi I.Y., Gruetter R. In vivo H-1 NMR spectroscopy of rat brain at 1 ms echo time. Magn. Reson. Med. 1999;41(April (4)):649–656. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1522-2594(199904)41:4<649::aid-mrm2>3.0.co;2-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Provencher S.W. Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in-Vivo proton nmr-Spectra. Magn. Reson. Med. 1993;30(December (6)):672–679. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910300604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Paxinos G., Franklin K.B.J. The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Acad. Press. 2001:39–167. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Balducci E., Emanuelli M., Raffaelli N., Ruggieri S., Amici A., Magni G. Assay-Methods for nicotinamide mononucleotide adenylyltransferase of wide applicability. Anal. Biochem. 1995;228(June (1)):64–68. doi: 10.1006/abio.1995.1315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Levine S., Saltzman A., Levy E., Ginsberg S.D. Systemic pathology in aged mouse models of Down's syndrome and Alzheimer's disease. Exp. Mol. Pathol. 2009;86(February (1)):18–22. doi: 10.1016/j.yexmp.2008.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wild K.V., Kaye J.A., Oken B.S. Early noncognitive change in Alzheimer's disease and healthy aging. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 1994;7(October–December (4)):199–205. doi: 10.1177/089198879400700401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Polydoro M., Acker C.M., Duff K., Castillo P.E., Davies P. Age-dependent impairment of cognitive and synaptic function in the htau mouse model of tau pathology. J. Neurosci. 2009;29(August (34)):10741–10749. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1065-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Deacon R.M., Penny C., Rawlins J.N. Effects of medial prefrontal cortex cytotoxic lesions in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2003;139(February (1–2)):139–155. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00225-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Deacon R.M., Croucher A., Rawlins J.N. Hippocampal cytotoxic lesion effects on species-typical behaviours in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2002;132(May (2)):203–213. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00401-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Dominy J., Thinschmidt J.S., Peris J., Dawson R., Papke R.L. Taurine-induced long-lasting potentiation in the rat hippocampus shows a partial dissociation from total hippocampal taurine content and independence from activation of known taurine transporters. J. Neurochem. 2004;89(June (5)):1195–1205. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2004.02410.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kanamori K., Ross B.D., Kondrat R.W. Glial uptake of neurotransmitter glutamate from the extracellular fluid studied in vivo by microdialysis and C-13 NMR. J. Neurochem. 2002 Nov;83(November (3)):682–695. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2002.01161.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Adams S.J., Crook R.J.P., DeTure M., Randle S.J., Innes A.E., Yu X.Z. Overexpression of wild-Type murine tau results in progressive tauopathy and neurodegeneration. Am. J. Pathol. 2009 Oct;175(October (4)):1598–1609. doi: 10.2353/ajpath.2009.090462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Holmes H.E., Colgan N., Ismail O., Ma D., Powell N.M., O'Callaghan J.M. Imaging the accumulation and suppression of tau pathology using multiparametric MRI. Neurobiol. Aging. 2016;39(March):184–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Phillips M., Boman E., Osterman H., Willhite D., Laska M. Olfactory and visuospatial learning and memory performance in two strains of Alzheimer's disease model mice?a longitudinal study. PLoS One. 2011;6(May (5)):e19567. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gomes A.P., Price N.L., Ling A.J., Moslehi J.J., Montgomery M.K., Rajman L. Declining NAD(+) induces a pseudohypoxic state disrupting nuclear-mitochondrial communication during aging. Cell. 2013;155(December (7)):1624–1638. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Geiszler P.C., Auer D.P., Daykin C.A. The journey from metabolic profiling to biomarkers: the potential of NMR spectroscopy based metabolomics in neurodegenerative disease research. Curr. Metab. 2013;1:160–179. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dedeoglu A., Choi J.K., Cormier K., Kowall N.W., Jenkins B.G. Magnetic resonance spectroscopic analysis of Alzheimer's disease mouse brain that express mutant human APP shows altered neurochemical profile. Brain Res. 2004;1012(June (1–2)):60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.02.079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cheng L.L., Ma M.J., Becerra L., Ptak T., Tracey I., Lackner A. Quantitative neuropathology by high resolution magic angle spinning proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1997;94(June (12)):6408–6413. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.12.6408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Block W., Traber F., Flacke S., Jessen F., Pohl C., Schild H. In-vivo proton MR-spectroscopy of the human brain: assessment of N-acetylaspartate (NAA) reduction as a marker for neurodegeneration. Amino Acids. 2002;23(1-3):317–323. doi: 10.1007/s00726-001-0144-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Salek R.M., Xia J., Innes A., Sweatman B.C., Adalbert R., Randle S. A metabolomic study of the CRND8 transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Neurochem. Int. 2010;56(July (8)):937–947. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2010.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Firbank M.J., Harrison R.M., O'Brien J.T. A comprehensive review of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies in dementia and Parkinson's disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. 2002;14(2):64–76. doi: 10.1159/000064927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Klunk W.E., Xu C., Panchalingam K., McClure R.J., Pettegrew J.W. Quantitative 1H and 31P MRS of PCA extracts of postmortem Alzheimer's disease brain. Neurobiol. Aging. 1996;17(May–June (3)):349–357. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(96)00035-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Yang D., Xie Z., Stephenson D., Morton D., Hicks C.D., Brown T.M. Volumetric MRI and MRS provide sensitive measures of Alzheimer's disease neuropathology in inducible Tau transgenic mice (rTg4510) Neuroimage. 2011;54(February (4)):2652–2658. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Frederick D.W., Davis J.G., Davila A., Agarwal B., Michan S., Puchowicz M.A. Increasing NAD synthesis in muscle via nicotinamide phosphoribosyltransferase is not sufficient to promote oxidative metabolism. J. Biol. Chem. 2015;290(January (3)):1546–1558. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M114.579565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Chou J.L., Shenoy D.V., Thomas N., Choudhary P.K., Laferla F.M., Goodman S.R. Early dysregulation of the mitochondrial proteome in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. J. Proteomics. 2011;74(April (4)):466–479. doi: 10.1016/j.jprot.2010.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Reddy P.H. Abnormal tau, mitochondrial dysfunction, impaired axonal transport of mitochondria, and synaptic deprivation in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res. 2011;30(September (1415)):136–148. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.07.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Zakhary S.M., Ayubcha D., Dileo J.N., Jose R., Leheste J.R., Horowitz J.M. Distribution analysis of deacetylase SIRT1 in rodent and human nervous systems. Anat. Rec. 2010;293(June (6)):1024–1032. doi: 10.1002/ar.21116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Min S.W., Cho S.H., Zhou Y.G., Schroeder S., Haroutunian V., Seeley W.W. Acetylation of tau inhibits its degradation and contributes to tauopathy. Neuron. 2010;67(September (6)):953–966. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lin S.J., Ford E., Haigis M., Liszt G., Guarente L. Calorie restriction extends yeast life span by lowering the level of NADH. Gene Dev. 2004;18(June (1)):12–16. doi: 10.1101/gad.1164804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fulco M., Cen Y., Zhao P., Hoffman E.P., McBurney M.W., Sauve A.A. Glucose restriction inhibits skeletal myoblast differentiation by activating SIRT1 through AMPK-mediated regulation of Nampt. Dev. Cell. 2008;14(May (5)):661–673. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Nogueiras R., Habegger K.M., Chaudhary N., Finan B., Banks A.S., Dietrich M.O. Sirtuin 1 and sirtuin 3: physiological modulators of metabolism. Physiol. Rev. 2012;92(July (3)):1479–1514. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00022.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Tanno M., Sakamoto J., Miura T., Shimamoto K., Horio Y. Nucleocytoplasmic shuttling of the NAD+-dependent histone deacetylase SIRT1. J. Biol. Chem. 2007;282(March (9)):6823–6832. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M609554200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ozdemir A.Y., Rom I., Kovalevich J., Yen W., Adiga R., Dave R.S. PINCH in the cellular stress response to tau-Hyperphosphorylation. PLoS One. 2013;8(March (3)) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ruan K., Zhu Y., Li C., Brazill J.M., Zhai R.G. Alternative splicing of Drosophila Nmnat functions as a switch to enhance neuroprotection under stress. Nat. Commun. 2015;6(November) doi: 10.1038/ncomms10057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.