Skip to main content
Aging and Disease logoLink to Aging and Disease
. 2018 Feb 1;9(1):90–101. doi: 10.14336/AD.2017.0128

Cardiovascular Risk Factors Are Correlated with Low Cognitive Function among Older Adults Across Europe Based on The SHARE Database

Joana Lourenco 1,2,3, Antonio Serrano 1, Alice Santos-Silva 4, Marcos Gomes 4, Claudia Afonso 2, Paula Freitas 5,6, Constanca Paul 3,7, Elisio Costa 4,*
PMCID: PMC5772862  PMID: 29392084

Abstract

Increased life expectancy is associated with a high prevalence of chronic, non-communicable diseases including cognitive decline and dementia. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the prevalence of cognitive impairment using three cognitive abilities (verbal fluency, numeracy and perceived memory) and their association with cardiovascular risk factors in seniors across Europe. Data from participants in wave 4 of the SHARE (Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe) database was used. Cognitive performance in perceived memory, verbal fluency and numeracy was evaluated using simple tests and a memory complaints questionnaire. Clinical and sociodemographic variables were also studied for potential associations. Standardised prevalence rates of cognitive impairment based on age and gender were calculated by country. The prevalence of cognitive impairment was 28.02% for perceived memory, 27.89% for verbal fluency and 20.75% for numeracy throughout the 16 evaluated countries. Years of education, being a current or former smoker, number of chronic diseases, diabetes or hyperglycemia, heart attack and stroke were all independent variables associated with impairment in the three studied cognitive abilities. We also found independent associations between physical inactivity and verbal fluency and numeracy impairment, as well as hypertension and perceived memory impairment. Lower performance in the evaluated cognitive abilities and higher memory complaints are highly prevalent, have a heterogeneous distribution across Europe, and are associated with multiple factors, most of which are potentially preventable or treatable, especially cardiovascular risk factors.

Keywords: cognitive function, ageing, chronic diseases, cardiovascular risk factors


Given worldwide population projections indicate that the number of persons aged 60 or above will double by 2050 and triple by 2100 [1]. Increased life expectancy is associated with an increased prevalence of chronic diseases, cognitive decline and dementia [2]. The cellular and molecular mechanisms associated with cognitive impairment are not well-established [3]; however, recent studies suggest that the pathological processes starts 10-20 years before the clinical onset of dementia [4]. The most commonly diagnosed cause of cognitive impairment is Alzheimer’s disease (AD), but vascular diseases including subclinical brain injury, stroke and silent brain infarction are also important causes and contributors to cognitive decline [5]. In fact, an association between brain health and cardiovascular risk factors was established in middle-aged adults using magnetic resonance imaging [6]. Another study also suggested a link between neurocognitive function and systemic vascular health through the evaluation of arterial stiffness and reactivity [7]. Moreover, silent strokes are more common than strokes with clinical manifestations and constitute an important risk factor as precursors of future strokes and cognitive impairment [3]. It is estimated that more than one in 10 adults over 60 years of age has had a stroke resulting in the loss of cognitive ability [8].

The impact of cardiovascular risk factors on cognitive function is receiving heightened attention as modifications of these factors may reduce the risk of dementia later in life. Therefore, we first aimed to evaluate the prevalence of low cognitive function by assessing perceived memory, verbal fluency and numeracy in people over 50 years of age across 16 European countries. Secondly, we assessed the association between cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function for the three cognitive abilities listed above. The identification of factors associated with impaired cognitive abilities in older adults can be used to develop effective interventions that prevent declines in the cognitive function of seniors across Europe.

MATERIAL AND METHODS

In this work, we used data from the SHARE (Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe) project, wave 4. SHARE is an international European database containing detailed information pertaining to the demographics, health, and social and economic status from representative samples of community-based populations over 50 years of age from 16 European countries (Austria, Belgium, France, Germany, Netherlands, Switzerland, the Czech Republic, Hungary, Poland, Denmark, Sweden, Estonia, Spain, Portugal, Italy, Slovenia) [9]. Modelled after the US Health and Retirement Study and the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, SHARE contains data from a very large population. The harmonised, cross-national design allows for consistent international comparisons of a large number of factors simultaneously, providing a dynamic picture of ageing in Europe. A detailed description of the SHARE data and methodology has been published and is available to registered users on the SHARE website (http://www.share-project.org).

The wave from 2010 contains data from 58,489 individuals aged 23-103 years. To evaluate the prevalence of and the variables associated with poorer cognitive performance in three cognitive abilities (perceived memory, verbal fluency and numeracy), our sample included non-institutionalised individuals aged 50 years or more, with a body mass index (BMI) of 18.5 kg/m2 or higher, and who answered all of the questions included in this analysis (dependent and independent variables). We excluded data from three countries (Germany, Sweden and Poland) in our association analysis due to the low number of individuals that remained after exclusion of cases with missing values (less than 5%).

Dependent variables - cognitive abilities

Two domains of cognitive function, i.e. verbal fluency (a test of executive function) and numeracy (arithmetical calculations) were evaluated in this work. The third cognitive domain, perceived memory, was assessed by a memory complaint questionnaire. In the SHARE project, cognitive tasks were based on standard dementia screening tools, i.e. the Mini-Mental-State and Dementia Detection Scale [10].

Perceived memory

The participant’s memory complaints in SHARE were assessed through the questions “How would you rate your memory at the present time? Would you say it is ‘excellent’, ‘very good’, ‘good’, ‘fair’ or ‘poor’?” This variable was dichotomised as excellent, very good or good, and as fair or poor [11].

Numeracy

The participant’s numeracy was assessed through the following described mathematical tasks. Participants were asked, “If the chance of getting a disease is 10 percent, how many people out of one thousand would be expected to get the disease?” The participant was then asked a second question: “In a sale, a shop is selling all items at half price. Before the sale, a sofa costs 300 (local currency). How much will it cost in the sale?” If the participant answered the first question incorrectly, the numeracy test was stopped after the second question, irrespective of whether the participant answered the second question correctly. If the first question was answered correctly, a third question was posed: “A second hand car dealer is selling a car for 6,000 (local currency). This is two-thirds of what it costs new. How much did the car cost new?” If two questions were answered correctly, the participant was invited to answer the last question: “Let’s say you have 2,000 (local currency) in a savings account. The account earns ten percent interest each year. How much would you have in the account at the end of two years?” The numeracy score ranged from 1 (the participant did not answer correctly any of the math questions) to 5 (the participant answered all of the math questions correctly). This variable was dichotomised into two categories, a score of 3 or less (indicative of impairment) or a score of more than 3 [12].

Verbal fluency

This test measured executive function and language abilities. Participants were told: “Now, I would like you to name as many different animals as you can think of. You have one minute to do this. Ready, go.” Valid answers were any members of the animal kingdom, real or mythical, specific species’ names, any accompanying breeds within the species, as well as male, female, and infant names within the species. Proper nouns were considered incorrect. The verbal fluency score was the number of valid animal names the participant was able to state. This result was dichotomised into two categories: a score of 15 or less, suggestive of verbal fluency impairment, or a score of more than 15 [13].

Explanatory variables

The wide scope of information in the SHARE project allowed us to include a large number of putative explanatory variables, such as sociodemographic (age, gender and years of education) and health variables.

Age was calculated as the difference between the year 2010 and the date of birth, and four age classes were set (50-54, 55-64, 65-74 and more than 75 years old). The gender response generated a dichotomous variable, male or female. The education variable resulted from their response to the question “years of education”, which was dichotomised as ≤ 12 years or as > 12 years of education. The self-reported presence or absence of physician-diagnosed vascular diseases (“heart attack”, stroke, diabetes or hyperglcemia, and high blood pressure or hypertension) as well as smoking status (current or former smoker) were recorded. The continuous variable BMI was transformed into a discrete variable with three classes (18.5-24.9, 25-29.9 and 30 kg/m2 or higher). The variable “number of chronic diseases” was based on the number of chronic diseases reported by each individual, dichotomised by ≤ 2 or >2 chronic diseases. Physical inactivity was assessed on the basis of the following questions: “How often do you engage in activities that require a moderate level of energy such as gardening, cleaning the car, or going for a walk?” and “We would like to know about the type and amount of physical activity you do in your daily life. How often do you engage in vigorous physical activity, such as sports, heavy housework, or a job that involves physical labor?” These questions addressed their levels of moderate and vigorous physical activity, respectively. Physical inactivity was defined as never, or almost never, engaging in moderate or vigorous physical activity.

Statistical analysis

A descriptive analysis of the outcomes was performed in order to obtain an estimate of the proportion of individuals with cognitive impairment in the 16 European countries. Age and gender standardised prevalence of cognitive impairment, i.e. in perceived memory, numeracy and verbal fluency, was calculated by country and the 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were calculated. We used direct age-adjusted standardisation based on the standard European population of 2013 [14].

With individuals nested by country, a multilevel logistic regression approach was used considering the three cognitive abilities as dependent variables: perceived memory (0: “poor” or “fair”; 1: “good”, “very good,” “excellent”), verbal fluency (0: ≤ 15 correct words; 1: > 15 correct words), and numeracy (0: “bad” 1-3, 1: “good” 4-5). In the first approach, multilevel, univariate, logistic regression models were used considering each covariate to identify potential factors associated with the outcome variables. Significant covariates retained in the first step (p < 0.05) were included in a multilevel, multivariable, logistic regression model. Country was considered to be a random effect. The final model was composed only of significant covariates, as determined by a backward selection method. Odds ratios and their 95% CI are reported. All the analyses were performed using the software IBM SPSS (version 23) and p < 0.05 was considered significant.

RESULTS

From a total of 58,489 individuals that participated in wave 4 of the SHARE survey, we selected non-institutionalised individuals aged 50 years or older, who answered all of the questions included in this work. Accordingly, we included 33,580 individuals in this analysis. Participants had a mean age of 65.4 (sd = 10.0) years; 56.4% were female. Table I shows the entire sample set characterised by gender, country and number of education years. The standardised prevalence of impaired cognitive performance in tests of cognitive abilities are shown in Table II. The prevalence of impairment was of 28.02%, 27.89% and 20.75% for perceived memory, verbal fluency, and numeracy, respectively, in the 16 evaluated countries. The geographic distribution was heterogeneous between the evaluated countries (Table II). Regarding memory complaints, the highest prevalence was found in Estonia [46.18 (45.71-46.66%)] and Portugal [44.41 (43.94-44.88%)], and the lowest prevalence was found in Denmark [8.82 (8.62-9.03%)]. For numeracy impairment, the highest prevalence was found in Spain [46.02 (45.55-46.50%)] and Portugal [36.92 (36.50-37.35%)], and the lowest prevalence was found in Switzerland [9.04 (8.83-9.25%)]. For verbal fluency impairment, the highest prevalence was found in Portugal [63.11 (62.55-63.67%)] and Italy [62.57 (62.01-63.12%)], and the lowest prevalence was found in Denmark [6.48 (6.31-6.67)]. The prevalence of cognitive impairment found in participants from Germany, Sweden and Poland must be evaluated with caution due to the small number of individuals included in the analysis (Table I).

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of the sample population included in this study by gender and age group.

Country Total
SHARE Population (n)
Selected
Population (n)
Selected
Population (%)
Total Male
(n)
Selected
Male (n)
Selected
Male (%)
Total Female
(n)
Selected
Female (n)
Selected
Female (%)
50-54 Male
(n) 55 - 64
65-74 >75 years 50-54 Female
(n) 55-64
65-74 >75 years
Austria 5286 3953 74.78 2230 1706 76.5 3056 2247 73.53 345 607 517 237 382 843 679 343
Germany* 1572 16 1.02 736 6 0.82 836 10 1.2 2 2 0 2 1 7 1 1
Sweden* 1951 65 3.33 894 31 3.47 1057 34 3.22 4 8 14 5 4 13 10 7
Netherlands 2762 756 27.37 1220 342 28.03 1542 414 26.85 64 127 86 65 74 144 129 67
Spain 3570 1316 36.86 1606 622 38.73 1964 694 35.34 132 213 148 129 149 237 154 154
Italy 3583 1346 37.57 1605 613 38.19 1978 733 37.06 97 263 175 78 107 309 201 116
France 5857 3298 56.31 2512 1455 57.92 3345 1843 55.1 246 534 405 270 291 682 524 346
Denmark 2276 431 18.94 1036 210 20.27 1240 221 17.82 28 85 48 49 35 77 52 57
Switzerland 3750 2371 63.23 1682 1121 66.65 2068 1250 60.44 151 405 310 255 172 453 370 255
Belgium 5300 2695 50.85 2363 1237 52.35 2937 1458 49.64 77 431 445 284 75 551 524 308
Czech 6118 4310 70.45 2576 1843 71.55 3542 2467 69.65 240 635 519 449 338 814 688 627
Poland* 1724 42 2.44 753 20 2.66 971 22 2.27 0 8 9 3 4 4 7 7
Hungary 3076 2843 92.43 1322 1223 92.51 1754 1620 92.36 122 570 320 211 182 707 441 290
Portugal 2080 1606 77.21 895 711 79.44 1185 895 75.53 108 233 214 156 136 292 243 224
Slovenia 2756 2518 91.36 1196 1108 92.64 1560 1410 90.38 149 363 362 234 201 464 426 319
Estonia 6828 6014 88.08 2748 2392 87.05 4080 3622 88.77 499 807 640 446 740 1263 941 678
*

These countries were not evaluated for an association between cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function.

Table 2.

Standardised prevalence of impaired cognitive performance by country and gender.

Perceived memory impairment Numeracy impairment Verbal fluency impairment
Overall prevalence (95% CI) Standardised incidence rates (95% CI) Overall prevalence (95% CI) Standardised incidence rates (95% CI) Overall prevalence (95% CI) Standardised incidence rates (95% CI)
Male Female Male Female Male Female
Austria 14.66
(14.40-14.94)
15.33
(14.94-15.72)
14.00
(14.63 -14.38)
12.60
(12.35-12.85)
9.61
(9.30-9.92)
15.60
(15.21- 15.99)
18.92
(18.62-19.23)
18.28
(17.86-18.71)
19.56
(19.12-20.01)
Germany* 13.55
(13.30 -13.81)
0.00
(0.00-0.01)
27.11
(26.59-27.63)
21.52
(21.20-21.85)
11.54
(11.20-11.88)
31.50
(30.95-32.06)
14.93
(14.66-15.20)
11.54
(11.20-11.88)
18.32
(17.89-18.74)
Sweden* 22.74
(22.40-23.07)
31.86
(31.30-32.42)
13.61
(13.25-13.98)
5.82
(5.65-5.99)
3.85
(3.65-4.05)
7.78
(7.51-8.07)
14.26
(14.00-14.53)
18.88
(18.45-19.31)
9.64
(9.34-9.96)
Netherlands 9.79
(9.58-10.02)
8.72
(8.43-9.02)
10.86
(10.54-11.20)
12.85
(12.60-13.10)
7.22
(6.96-7.50)
18.47
(18.05-18.91)
17.76
(17.46-18.05)
17.35
(16.94-17.77)
18.16
(17.74-18.59)
Spain 37.93
(37.50-38.37)
34.91
(34.32-35.50)
40.96
(40.33-41.60)
46.02
(45.55-46.50)
38.28
(37.67-38.90)
53.76
(53.04-54.49)
58.91
(58.37-59.45)
55.07
(54.34-55.81)
62.75
(61.97-63.54)
Italy 27.34
(26.98-27.71)
24.81
(24.32-25.31)
29.87
(29.33-30.42)
28.81
(28.44-29.19)
20.99
(20.54-21.45)
36.63
(36.03-37.24)
62.57
(62.01-63.12)
60.31
(59.54-61.08)
64.82
(64.03-65.63)
France 26.54
(26.18-26.91)
25.43
(24.93-25.93)
27.66
(27.14-28.19)
23.84
(23.50-24.19)
18.42
(18.00-18.85)
29.26
(28.73-29.80)
33.82
(33.42-34.23)
32.51
(31.95-33.08)
35.13
(34.55-35.72)
Denmark 8.82
(8.62-9.03)
7.05
(6.79-7.32)
10.60
(10.28-10.93)
10.89
(10.66-11.12)
7.19
(6.92-7.46)
14.59
(14.21-14.97)
6.48
(6.31-6.67)
6.67
(6.42-6.93)
6.30
(6.05-6.55)
Switzerland 12.85
(12.60-13.11)
13.75
(13.39-14.13)
11.95
(11.61-12.30)
9.04
(8.83-9.25)
6.67
(6.42-6.94)
11.41
(11.07-11.75)
22.91
(22.57-23.24)
24.25
(23.77-24.75)
21.56
(21.10-22.03)
Belgium 20.34
(20.03-20.66)
19.52
(19.09-19.97)
21.16
(20.70-21.62)
16.82
(16.54-17.11)
11.99
(11.65-12.34)
21.66
(21.20-22.12)
20.91
(20.59-21.23)
20.10
(19.66-20.55)
21.71
(21.25-22.18)
Czech 24.42
(24.08-24.77)
24.30
(23.81-24.79)
24.55
(24.06-25.05)
17.01
(16.72-17.30)
12.99
(12.64-13.35)
21.03
(20.58-21.49)
16.95
(16.66-17.24)
15.78
(15.39-16.18
18.11
(17.69-18.54)
Poland* 15.95
(15.67-16.23)
14.00
(13.63 -14.37)
17.90
(17.49-18.33)
29.99
(29.60-30.37)
31.36
(30.80-31.92)
28.62
(28.09-29.15)
41.20
(40.75-41.65)
46.37
(45.69-47.05)
36.03
(35.44-36.64)
Hungary 32.70
(32.30-33.11)
31.33
(30.78-31.90)
34.07
(33.49-34.65)
18.54
(18.24-18.84)
15.93
(15.54-16.33)
21.14
(20.69-21.61)
39.01
(38.57-39.45)
38.83
(38.21-39.45)
39.19
(38.57-39.81)
Portugal 44.41
(43.94-44.88)
38.35
(37.74-38.97)
50.46
(49.76-51.17)
36.92
(36.50-37.35)
26.89
(26.38-27.41)
46.95
(46.27-47.64)
63.11
(62.55-63.67)
59.65
(58.88-60.42)
66.57
(65.76-66.38)
Slovenia 25.38
(25.03-25.74)
23.89
(23.41-24.38)
26.88
(26.37-27.40)
29.83
(29.45-30.22)
25.05
(24.55-25.55)
34.62
(34.03-35.21)
24.07
(23.73-24.42)
23.74
(23.26-24.23)
24.40
(23.92-24.90)
Estonia 46.18
(45.71-46.66)
46.68
(46.00-47.36)
45.69
(45.02-46.37)
18.79
(18.48-19.09)
15.87
(15.48-16.27)
21.70
(21.24-22.17)
19.28
(18.97-19.59)
19.84
(19.40-20.28)
18.73
(18.30-19.16)
*

Germany, Sweden and Poland have a small number of cases.

To evaluate variables associated with the cognitive abilities identified in this study, we excluded data from three countries (Germany, Sweden and Poland) from this analysis due to the small number of individuals that remained after exclusion of cases with missing values (less than 5%). The remaining 33,126 individuals included in this analysis had a mean age of 65.4 (SD = 10.0) years; 56.5% were female. Via analysis of all countries (except Germany, Sweden and Poland) using unadjusted models, we found a significant association among all studied explanatory variables with the three cognitive abilities, i.e. perceived memory (Table 3), verbal fluency (Table 4), and numeracy (Table 4). However, from adjusted models we found that fewer years of education, being a smoker or former smoker, having more than two chronic diseases, and having diabetes, hyperglycemia or a history of heart attack or stroke were independent variables associated with perceived memory, verbal fluency and numeracy impairment (Tables 3, 4 and 5). We also found independent associations between physical inactivity with verbal fluency and numeracy impairment, females had more complaints about perceived memory and poorer performance in numeracy, older participants had more complaints about perceived memory and poorer performance in verbal fluency, and participants with hypertension had more complaints about memory.

Table 3.

Association of explanatory variables with memory impairment; unadjusted and adjusted models.

n n (%) of fair or poor Unadjusted model Adjusted model *
33126 OR CI 95 p OR CI 95 p
Female 18709 5430 (29.0) 1 - - 1 - -
Male 14417 3867 (26.8) 0.896 0.854-0.941 <0.001 0.927 0.879-0.977 0.005
Age, years
>75 6460 1951 (30.2) 1 - - 1 - -
65-74 9504 2658 (28.0) 0.897 0.837-0.962 0.002 0.903 0.840-0.970 0.005
55-64 12048 3225 (26.8) 0.845 0.790-0.903 <0.001 0.849 0.793-0.910 <0.001
50-54 5114 1463 (28.6) 0.926 0.854-1.004 0.062 0.936 0.861-1.018 0.121
Education, years
>12-y 9376 1689 (18.0) 1 - - 1 - -
≤12-y 23750 7608 (32.0) 2.145 2.022-2.276 <0.001 1.968 1.852-2.091 <0.001
BMI, kg/m2
≥30 7652 2386 (31.2) 1 - - - - -
25-29.9 13634 3808 (27.9) 0.855 0.805-0.909 <0.001 - - -
18.5-24.9 11840 3103 (26.2) 0.784 0.736-0.835 <0.001 - - -
Smoking
Smoker* 18156 5247 (28.9) 1 - - 1 - -
No 14970 4050 (27.1) 0.912 0.869-0.958 <0.001 0.928 0.881-0.978 0.005
Physical inactivity
No 29013 8116 (28.0) 1 - - -
Yes 4113 1181 (28.7) 0.964 0.897-1.036 0.323 - - -
Number of chronic diseases
>2 9050 3819 (42.2) 1 - - 1 - -
≤2 24076 5478 (22.8) 0.403 0.383-0.425 <0.001 0.574 0.537-0.614 <0.001
Comorbidities
Heart attack
Yes 4805 2096 (43.6) 1 - - 1 - -
No 28321 7201 (25.4) 0.441 0.414-0.469 <0.001 0.653 0.609-0.701 <0.001
Stroke
Yes 1485 804 (54.1) 1 - - 1 - -
No 31641 8493 (26.8) 0.311 0.280-0.345 <0.001 0.479 0.429-0.536 <0.001
Diabetes or high blood sugar
Yes 4135 1583 (38.3) 1 - - 1 - -
No 28991 7714 (26.6) 0.584 0.546-0.626 <0.001 0.900 0.834-0.970 0.006
High blood pressure
Yes 13241 4533 (34.2) 1 - - 1 - -
No 19885 4764 (24.0) 0.605 0.577-0.635 <0.001 0.877 0.829-0.928 <0.001
*

Country random effect: estimate = 0.006; standard error = 0.040; p = 0.881.

Table 4.

Association of explanatory variables with verbal fluency impairment; unadjusted and adjusted models.

n n (%) of fair or poor Unadjusted model Adjusted model *
33126 OR CI 95 p OR CI 95 p
Female 18709 5430 (29.0) 1 - - 1 - -
Male 14417 3867 (26.8) 0.896 0.854-0.941 <0.001 0.927 0.879-0.977 0.005
Age, years
>75 6460 1951 (30.2) 1 - - 1 - -
65-74 9504 2658 (28.0) 0.897 0.837-0.962 0.002 0.903 0.840-0.970 0.005
55-64 12048 3225 (26.8) 0.845 0.790-0.903 <0.001 0.849 0.793-0.910 <0.001
50-54 5114 1463 (28.6) 0.926 0.854-1.004 0.062 0.936 0.861-1.018 0.121
Education, years
>12-y 9376 1689 (18.0) 1 - - 1 - -
≤12-y 23750 7608 (32.0) 2.145 2.022-2.276 <0.001 1.968 1.852-2.091 <0.001
BMI, kg/m2
≥30 7652 2386 (31.2) 1 - - - - -
25-29.9 13634 3808 (27.9) 0.855 0.805-0.909 <0.001 - - -
18.5-24.9 11840 3103 (26.2) 0.784 0.736-0.835 <0.001 - - -
Smoking
Smoker* 18156 5247 (28.9) 1 - - 1 - -
No 14970 4050 (27.1) 0.912 0.869-0.958 <0.001 0.928 0.881-0.978 0.005
Physical inactivity
No 29013 8116 (28.0) 1 - - -
Yes 4113 1181 (28.7) 0.964 0.897-1.036 0.323 - - -
Number of chronic diseases
>2 9050 3819 (42.2) 1 - - 1 - -
≤2 24076 5478 (22.8) 0.403 0.383-0.425 <0.001 0.574 0.537-0.614 <0.001
Comorbidities
Heart attack
Yes 4805 2096 (43.6) 1 - - 1 - -
No 28321 7201 (25.4) 0.441 0.414-0.469 <0.001 0.653 0.609-0.701 <0.001
Stroke
Yes 1485 804 (54.1) 1 - - 1 - -
No 31641 8493 (26.8) 0.311 0.280-0.345 <0.001 0.479 0.429-0.536 <0.001
Diabetes or Hyperglycemia
Yes 4135 1583 (38.3) 1 - - 1 - -
No 28991 7714 (26.6) 0.584 0.546-0.626 <0.001 0.900 0.834-0.970 0.006
Hypertension
Yes 13241 4533 (34.2) 1 - - 1 - -
No 19885 4764 (24.0) 0.605 0.577-0.635 <0.001 0.877 0.829-0.928 <0.001
*

Country random effect: estimate = 0.363; standard error = 0.273; p = 0.184.

Table 5.

Association of explanatory variables with numeracy impairment; unadjusted and adjusted models.

n n (%) ≤15 correct words Unadjusted model Adjusted model *
33126 OR CI 95 p OR CI 95 p
Female 18709 5285 (28.2) 1 - - - - -
Male 14417 3959 (27.5) 0.962 0.916-1.009 0.113 - - -
Age, years
>75 6460 1898 (29.4) 1 - - 1 - -
65-74 9504 2617 (27.5) 0.913 0.852-0.980 0.011 0.937 0.871-1.008 0.082
55-64 12048 3392 (28.2) 0.942 0.881-1.007 0.078 0.974 0.908-1.046 0.467
50-54 5114 1337 (26.1) 0.851 0.784-0.924 <0.001 0.870 0.798-0.949 0.002
Education, years
>12-y 9376 1280 (13.7) 1 - - 1 - -
≤12-y 23750 7964 (33.5) 3.191 2.991-3.405 <0.001 3.075 2.881-3.283 <0.001
BMI. Kg/m2
≥30 7652 2162 (28.3) 1 - - - - -
25-29.9 13634 3827 (28.1) 0.955 0.895-1.020 0.172 - - -
18.5-24.9 11840 3255 (27.5) 0.928 0.867-0.993 0.031 - - -
Smoking
Smoker 18156 5583 (30.8) 1 - - 1 - -
No 14970 3661 (24.5) 0.729 0.694-0.766 <0.001 0.718 0.683-0.755 <0.001
Physical inactivity
No 29013 7997 (27.6) 1 - - 1 - -
yes 4113 1247 (30.3) 0.875 0.814-0.939 <0.001 0.906 0.839-0.977 0.011
Number of chronic diseases
>2 9050 3063 (33.8) 1 - - 1 - -
≤2 24076 6181 (25.7) 0.675 0.641-0.711 <0.001 0.920 0.863-0.981 0.011
Comorbidities
Heart attack
Yes 4805 1717 (35.7) 1 - - 1 - -
No 28321 7527 (26.6) 0.651 0.610-0.694 <0.001 0.785 0.730-0.844 <0.001
Stroke
Yes 1485 653 (44.0) 1 - - 1 - -
No 31641 8591 (27.2) 0.475 0.427-0.528 <0.001 0.574 0.513-0.642 <0.001
Diabetes or Hyperglycaemia
Yes 4135 1542 (37.3) 1 - - 1 - -
No 28991 7702 (26.6) 0.608 0.568-0.651 <0.001 0.712 0.660-0.768 <0.001
Hypertension
Yes 13241 4058 (30.6) 1 - - - - -
No 19885 5186 (26.1) 0.798 0.760-0.838 <0.001 - - -
*

Country random effect: estimate = 0.178; standard error = 0.147; p = 0.226

DISCUSSION

In this work, a high and heterogeneous prevalence of impairment in cognitive function was found in 16 evaluated European countries. Southern European countries (Spain, Portugal, Italy and Slovenia) as well as Eastern Europe (Hungary, Poland, Czech Republic) showed the highest prevalence of cognitive impairment in the evaluated abilities. Conversely, Western European countries (Austria, Belgium, Switzerland, France, and the Netherlands) as well as Northern Europe (Denmark, Sweden) showed the lowest rates of cognitive impairment. These results are in accordance with previous studies that showed significant variation in cognitive performance across countries [15,16].

The findings suggest that older ages, being female, and having less education are associated with poorer cognitive performance. Not surprisingly, increased age is significantly related to poorer cognitive performance. Indeed, the influence of ageing on cognitive function has been extensively described, especially regarding processing speed, memory and language [17]. Evidence shows that cognitive decline during the ageing process corresponds with structural brain changes such as cerebral atrophy, ventricular enlargement and the loss of neuronal networks [18-20].

Education has also been shown to be strongly associated with cognitive function, with higher education levels (more than 12 years) having a protective effect by significantly decreasing the risk of developing cognitive impairment. A positive association between cognition and education in the elderly has been reported previously [21]. Data from the English Longitudinal Study on Ageing supports our results, showing that among English seniors, education increases memory in both genders as well as executive function in males. This result could explain some of the differences observed between countries, since participants from Central and Northern Europe with better cognitive function are also in regions with relatively high education levels, as has been reported previously [15,19]. Moreover, cognitive tests may reflect the quality of education as well as socioeconomic status, social interactions, labor force participation or cognitive reserve [22]. There is also evidence that improvements in mental performance are related to changes in brain structure, affecting synaptic density, hippocampal volume and cortical thickness [18].

Our analyses show that lower performance in perceived memory, verbal fluency, and numeracy were strongly related to comorbidities represented by more than two self-reported chronic diseases. This is in accordance with other studies suggesting that individuals with poor health status are at a higher risk for significant cognitive impairment or dementia [23,24]. Existing data support the notion that the number of chronic diseases could be a marker of cumulative exposure to vascular risk factors [8]. The underlying mechanism seems to be related to peripheral inflammatory markers, a potential indicator of global inflammatory processes that may increase the risk for dementia through their role in the development and progression of cardiovascular disease [25].

Cardiovascular risk factors, including smoking, physical inactivity, hypertension and a previous diagnosis of diabetes, hyperglycemia, stroke or heart attack were independent variables associated with cognitive impairment. Stroke is the major risk factor for impaired cognitive function in numeracy, perceived memory and verbal fluency. A history of strokes almost doubled the risk, which agrees with other studies [26]. Although the underlying process of brain damage requires more research, it is known that vascular dysfunction and endothelial injury are important mediators of atherosclerosis development, damage to medium-large arteries, and probably to smaller brain arteries as well [27]. Studies investigating asymptomatic or “silent” strokes suggest that the development of AD may be positively associated with underlying vascular problems [28]. These findings suggest that strokes play a role in the etiology of dementia, which is supported by a recent meta-analysis that demonstrated a positive association between the risk for AD and stroke [29]. Growing evidence has identified cardiovascular risk factors, such as diabetes and hypertension, as predictors of cognitive decline post-stroke, especially after the occurrence of a second stroke [8]. High cardiovascular risk patients may be a target subpopulation in which to improve screening to prevent another cardiovascular event or cognitive decline [3].

Coronary heart disease (“heart attack”) increased the risk of having lower cognitive performance in all three cognitive domains evaluated, which is in accordance with recent research that identified an association between cognitive decline and small, clinically silent infarcts and coronary artery disease [30]. In the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study [31], in women over 65 years old with a history of infarction or invasive vascular procedures, the risk of cognitive decline was double that of the subgroup without myocardial infarction. Moreover, this study suggested that cognitive impairment was positively associated with heart and aortic atherosclerosis, particularly in patients with angina pectoris, carotid endarterectomy, peripheral vascular disease, and coronary bypass surgery [31]. However, the relationship between coronary artery disease and cognitive impairment or AD remains unclear, with conflicting reports [32,33].

Diabetes or hyperglycemia was independently associated with impaired cognitive performance in the three cognitive domains: numeracy, perceived memory and verbal fluency. This is in line with recent research that supports a positive association between type 2 diabetes and a higher incidence of cognitive decline [34]. Potential mechanisms involved in the pathogenesis of cognitive dysfunction and neurodegeneration include acute and chronic hyperglycemia and insulin resistance [35,36]. Although secreted peripherally, insulin also plays a particular role in cognitive function, learning, and memory [37]. However, more studies are needed to clarify the association between high glucose levels and dementia. Hypertension has been recognised as an important risk factor for cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment [38]. It is well-established that hypertension pressure can increase the risk of stroke and heart disease and decrease life expectancy. Many studies have associated the decline in cognitive function with hypertension in the elderly [39-41]. High blood pressure during middle age has been associated with future cognitive decline and dementia, including AD. Although the mechanisms underlying hypertension and cognitive dysfunction are not yet well understood, cerebral vessel disorder, which is pulsatile pressure changes in the cerebral microvasculature and possibly increased production of amyloid-beta, seems to be involved [42]. Although increased evidence suggests a contribution of hypertension to cognitive decline during middle age, data on its role later in life is not consistent. Some studies reported a significant positive effect of hypertension on cognitive decline [43] while others found a U-shaped relationship between hypertension and cognitive performance [44].

The increased risk of cognitive decline and dementia in smokers compared to non-smokers is supported by several prospective studies. These observations were corroborated by the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study in which a dose-response relationship was established between the number of pack-years and the deposition of amyloid substance in the brain [45]. It has been widely recognised that tobacco has important effects on the cardiovascular system, including the development of atherosclerosis and small vessel brain disease [8]. In addition to the direct neuronal damage caused by neurotoxins, data from the Whitehall II study showed a selective risk for certain cognitive domains [46], possibly because nicotine has psychoactive effects and stimulates acetylcholine receptors in the brain [8].

There is a wealth of literature on the association between physical activity and improved cognitive performance. Numerous underlying hypotheses have tried to explain how physical activity might protect brain health. The prominent theories are that physical activity leads to increased cerebral blood flow, neurogenesis, angiogenesis, and synaptic plasticity while at the same time reducing inflammation and improving resistance to brain insults [47-49].

Some limitations of this study require discussion. Firstly, the impact of other variables that might influence cognitive performance was not evaluated. This is a cross-sectional study while the associations being measured happened over a long period, which could be a limitation of this study. Additionally, even though this work is cross-sectional, SHARE data collection is ongoing and future observations over an extended period will allow for analyses between cognitive decline and its associated cardiovascular risk factors. The sampling procedure could also influence some of the results. In Northern and Western European countries, there is a higher proportion of the elderly population that is institutionalised, which could have excluded an unhealthier population from our sample. The sample from Southern Europe (Spain, Portugal and Italy), where old people often live with their families, has a higher chance of being included in the SHARE survey. Moreover, some variables presented high non-response ratios, which might affect the results. Finally, there are some contradictory studies on self-reported measures, a practice criticised by some authors but considered reliable by others. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to directly compare elderly populations from 16 European countries and provides evidence that cardiovascular risk factors are strongly associated with impaired cognitive function, even after accounting for sociodemographic and health status. Moreover, we report important results about the heterogeneity across Europe, with valuable policy implications on late life cognitive decline prevention that could be implemented in the clinical setting.

In conclusion, impaired cognitive function in terms of verbal fluency, numeracy and perceived memory is highly prevalent but unequally distributed across Europe. This is associated with multiple factors, namely cardiovascular risk factors, most of which can be prevented or treated.

Acknowledgments

This paper used data from the SHARE Wave 4 release 1.1.1, as of March 28th, 2013 (DOI: 10.6103/SHARE.w4.111). The SHARE data collection has been primarily funded by the European Commission through the 5th Framework Programme (project QLK6-CT-2001-00360 in the Thematic Programme Quality of Life), through the 6th Framework Programme (projects SHARE-I3, RII-CT-2006-062193, COMPARE, CIT5- CT-2005-028857, and SHARELIFE, CIT4-CT-2006-028812) and through the 7th Framework Programme (SHARE-PREP, N° 211909, SHARE-LEAP, N° 227822 and SHARE M4, N° 261982). Additional funding from the U.S. National Institute on Aging (U01 AG09740-13S2, P01 AG005842, P01 AG08291, P30 AG12815, R21 AG025169, Y1-AG-4553-01, IAG BSR06-11 and OGHA 04-064) and the German Ministry of Education and Research as well as from various national sources is gratefully acknowledged (see www.share-project.org for a full list of funding institutions). This study was also supported by FCT (UID/Multi/04378/2013), COMPETE-FEDER.

References

  • [1].United Nations DESA, Population Division. World Population Ageing 2015. Available at:http://www.unpopulation.org.
  • [2].Clegg A, Young J, Iliffe S, Rikkert MO, Rockwood K (2013). Frailty in elderly people. Lancet, 381:752-62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Gorelick PB, Nyenhuis D (2015). Stroke and cognitive decline. JAMA, 314:29-30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Beard JR, Bloom DE (2015). Towards a comprehensive public health response to population ageing. Lancet, 385:658-61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Gorelick PB, Counts SE, Nyenhuis D (2016). Vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta, 1862:860-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Lutomski JE, Baars MA, Boter H, Buurman BM, den Elzen WP, Jansen AP, et al. (2014). [Frailty, disability and multi-morbidity: the relationship with quality of life and healthcare costs in elderly people]. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd, 158: A7297. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Lim SL, Gao Q, Nyunt MS, Gong L, Lunaria JB, Lim ML, et al. (2015). Vascular health indices and cognitive domain function: Singapore longitudinal ageing studies. J Alzheimers Dis, 50:27-40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Gorelick PB, Scuteri A, Black SE, Decarli C, Greenberg SM, Iadecola C, et al. (2011) Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke, 42:2672-713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].Malter F B-S. SHARE Wave 4: Innovations & Methodology. Munich: MEA, Max Planck Institute for Social Law and Social Policy; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Vannier-Nitenberg C, Dauphinot V, Bongue B, Sass C, Rouch I, Beauchet O, et al. (2013). Early detection of memory impairment in people over 65 years old consulting at Health Examination Centers for the French health insurance: the EVATEM protocol. BMC Geriatr, 13:55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].Litwin H, Schwartz E, Damri N (2016). Cognitively stimulating leisure activity and subsequent cognitive function: A SHARE-based analysis. Gerontologist,in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Schneeweis N, Skirbekk V, Winter-Ebmer R (2012). Does schooling improve cognitive functioning at older ages? IZA Discussion Paper No. 6958 Available at: http://ftp.iza.org/dp6958.pdf.
  • [13].Sterniczuk R, Theou O, Rusak B, Rockwood K (2015). Cognitive test performance in relation to health and function in 12 European countries: The SHARE study. Can Geriatr J, 18:144-151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Eurostat: Revision of the European Standard Population - Report of Eurostat’s Task Force (2013). Available at: http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/documents/3859598/5926869/KS-RA-13-028-EN.PDF/e713fa79-1add-44e8-b23d-5e8fa09b3f8f.
  • [15].Skirbekk V, Loichinger E, Weber D (2012). Variation in cognitive functioning as a refined approach to comparing aging across countries. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 109:770-74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Scholes Sea. [update at:2009]. Technical Report (Wave 3). Living in the 21st Century: Older People in England. The 2006 English Longitudinal Study of Ageing.
  • [17].See ST, Ryan EB (1995). Cognitive mediation of adult age differences in language performance. Psychol Aging, 10:458-68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Peters R (2006). Ageing and the brain. Postgrad Med J, 82:84-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Weber D, Skirbekk V, Freund I, Herlitz A (2014). The changing face of cognitive gender differences in Europe. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 111:11673-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Herlitz A, Airaksinen E, Nordstrom E (1999). Sex differences in episodic memory: the impact of verbal and visuospatial ability. Neuropsychology, 13:590-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Richards M, Hatch SL (2011). A life course approach to the development of mental skills. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 66 Suppl 1: i26-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Nisbett RE, Aronson J, Blair C, Dickens W, Flynn J, Halpern DF, et al. (2012). Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. Am Psychol, 67:130-59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].Sterniczuk R, Theou O, Rusak B, Rockwood K (2015). Cognitive test performance in relation to health and function in 12 European countries: the SHARE study. Can Geriatr J, 18:144-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Bell JF, Fitzpatrick AL, Copeland C, Chi G, Steinman L, Whitney RL, et al. (2015). Existing data sets to support studies of dementia or significant cognitive impairment and comorbid chronic conditions. Alzheimers Dement, 11:622-38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [25].Koyama A (2013). The role of peripheral inflammatory markers in dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 68:433-40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Leys D, Henon H, Mackowiak-Cordoliani MA, Pasquier F (2005). Poststroke dementia. Lancet Neurol, 4:752-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [27].de Bruijn RF, Ikram MA (2014). Cardiovascular risk factors and future risk of Alzheimer's disease. BMC Med, 12:130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Altman R, Rutledge JC (2010). The vascular contribution to Alzheimer's disease. Clin Sci (Lond), 119:407-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [29].Zhou J, Yu JT, Wang HF, Meng XF, Tan CC, Wang J, et al. (2015). Association between stroke and Alzheimer's disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis, 43:479-89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Jagust WJ, Becker JT, DeKosky ST, Lyketsos C, et al. (2005). Determinants of vascular dementia in the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. Neurology, 64:1548-52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [31].Craig MC, Maki PM, Murphy DGM (2005). The Women's Health Initiative Memory Study: findings and implications for treatment. Lancet Neurol, 4:190-94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [32].Ikram MA, van Oijen M, de Jong FJ, Kors JA, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, et al. (2008). Unrecognised myocardial infarction in relation to risk of dementia and cerebral small vessel disease. Stroke, 39:1421-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [33].Vidal JS, Sigurdsson S, Jonsdottir MK, Eiriksdottir G, Thorgeirsson G, Kjartansson O, et al. (2010). Coronary artery calcium, brain function and structure: the AGES-Reykjavik Study. Stroke, 41:891-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [34].Crane PK, Walker R, Hubbard RA, Li G, Nathan DM, Zheng H, et al. (2013). Glucose levels and risk of dementia. N Engl J Med, 369:540-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [35].Downer B, Raji MA, Markides KS (2016). Relationship between metabolic and vascular conditions and cognitive decline among older Mexican Americans. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 31:213-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [36].Ho N, Sommers MS, Lucki I (2013). Effects of diabetes on hippocampal neurogenesis: links to cognition and depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 37:1346-62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [37].Bloemer J, Bhattacharya S, Amin R, Suppiramaniam V (2014). Impaired insulin signaling and mechanisms of memory loss. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci, 121:413-49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [38].Kivipelto M, Helkala EL, Hanninen T, Laakso MP, Hallikainen M, Alhainen K, et al. (2001). Midlife vascular risk factors and late-life mild cognitive impairment: A population-based study. Neurology, 56:1683-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [39].Elias MF, Elias PK, Sullivan LM, Wolf PA, D'Agostino RB (2003). Lower cognitive function in the presence of obesity and hypertension: the Framingham heart study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord, 27: 260-268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [40].Ninomiya T, Ohara T, Hirakawa Y, Yoshida D, Doi Y, Hata J, et al. (2011). Midlife and late-life blood pressure and dementia in Japanese elderly: the Hisayama study. Hypertension, 58:22-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [41].Launer LJ, Hughes T, Yu B, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, Ross GW, et al. (2010). Lowering midlife levels of systolic blood pressure as a public health strategy to reduce late-life dementia: perspective from the Honolulu Heart Program/Honolulu Asia Aging Study. Hypertension, 55:1352-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [42].O'Rourke MF, Safar ME (2005). Relationship between aortic stiffening and microvascular disease in brain and kidney: cause and logic of therapy. Hypertension, 46:200-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [43].Launer LJ, Ross GW, Petrovitch H, Masaki K, Foley D, White LR, et al. (2000). Midlife blood pressure and dementia: the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Neurobiol Aging, 21:49-55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [44].Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L (2005). The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol, 4:487-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [45].Tyas SL, White LR, Petrovitch H, Webster Ross G, Foley DJ, Heimovitz HK, et al. (2003). Mid-life smoking and late-life dementia: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Neurobiol Aging, 24:589-96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [46].Sabia S (2008). Smoking history and cognitive function in middle age from the Whitehall II study. Arch Intern Med,168:1165-73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [47].Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Schupf N, et al. (2010). Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA, 302:627-637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [48].Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ (2009). The neurobiology of brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity as modulators of brain disorders. Prog Neurobiol, 89:369-382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [49].Lautenschlager N T, et al. (2010). Physical Activity and Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 10:352-358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Aging and Disease are provided here courtesy of JKL International LLC

RESOURCES