Abstract
Background
In the general population, sleep problems have an impact on daytime performance. Despite sleep problems being common among children with Down syndrome, the impact of sleep problems on daytime behaviours in school-age children with Down syndrome is an understudied topic. Our study examined the relationship between parent-reported and actigraphy-measured sleep duration and sleep quality with parent- and teacher-reports of daytime behaviour problems among school-age children with Down syndrome.
Method
Thirty school-age children with Down syndrome wore an actigraph watch for a week at home at night. Their parent completed ratings of the child’s sleep during that same week. Their parent and teacher completed a battery of measures to assess daytime behaviour.
Results
Parent reports of restless sleep behaviours on the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ), but not actigraph-measured sleep efficiency, was predictive of parent and teacher behavioural concerns on the Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form (NCBRF) and the Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales. Actigraph-measured sleep period and parent-reported sleep duration on the CSHQ was predictive of daytime parent-reported inattention. Actigraph measured sleep period was predictive of parent-reported hyperactivity/impulsivity.
Conclusion
The study findings suggest that sleep problems have complex relationships to both parent- and teacher-reported daytime behaviour concerns in children with Down syndrome. These findings have implications for understanding the factors impacting behavioural concerns and their treatment in school-age children with Down syndrome.
Keywords: Down syndrome, trisomy 21, sleep, behaviour, children
Children with Down syndrome demonstrate a variety of sleep problems, including obstructive sleep apnoea (OSA), sleep onset difficulties, frequent night awakenings, and premature awakening (Carter, McCaughey, Annaz, & Hill, 2009; Churchill, Kieckhefer, Bjornson, & Herting, 2014; de Miguel-Diez, Villa-Asensi, & Alvarez-Sala, 2003; Epstein, Pillar, Tzichinsky, Here, & Lavie, 1992; Esbensen, 2016; Marcus, Keens, Bautista, von Pechman, & Ward, 1991; Maris, Verhulst, Wojciechowski, Van de Heyning, & Boudewyns, 2016; Stebbens, Dennis, Samuels, Croft, & Southall, 1991). OSA affects 31–66% of individuals with Down syndrome (de Miguel-Diez et al., 2003; Stebbens et al., 1991), and behavioural sleep disturbances affect 52–69% of children with Down syndrome (Carter et al., 2009; Esbensen & Hoffman, 2017).
In the general population, paediatric sleep problems are associated with deficits in neurobehavioural functioning including attention, impulse control, cognitive abilities (e.g., learning/memory), and behavioural regulation (Beebe, 2011; Dewald, 2010; Fallone, 2002; Paavonen, Porkka-Heiskanen, & Lahikainen, 2009; Paavonen, Raikkonen, et al., 2009; Steenari, 2003) as well as adverse outcomes for parents and the family (Beebe, 2006; Dahl, 2006; Sadeh & Gruber, 2002; Sadeh & Raviv, 2000). Among children with intellectual and developmental disabilities, preliminary studies suggest that sleep problems may similarly be associated with maladaptive behaviours (G. Stores & Wiggs, 2001). Parent reported sleep concerns in children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) demonstrate a positive correlation with parent-reports of behaviour problems on general behaviour rating scales (Malow et al., 2006), and there is some evidence that improving sleep in children with ASD and severe insomnia results in improved behavioural functioning (Wright et al., 2011). Further, parent reported sleep concerns in children with idiopathic intellectual disability also demonstrate a positive correlation with maladaptive behaviours (Didden, Korzilius, Aperlo, Overloop, & Vries, 2002; Quine, 1991; Richdale, Francis, Gavidia-Payne, & Cotton, 2000; Rzepecka, McKenzie, McClure, & Murphy, 2011). Specifically, parent-reports of sleep problems on the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ) are associated with parent reports of both anxiety and challenging behaviour (Rzepecka et al., 2011). Additionally, sleep problems in children with intellectual and developmental disabilities are related to parental stress and well-being, parenting behaviours (e.g., discipline, demonstrating affection) and parental sleep (Quine, 1991, 1992; Richdale et al., 2000). While poorer attention has been speculated to be associated with sleep difficulties in children with intellectual and developmental disabilities (Harvey & Kennedy, 2002), at least one study found that inattention was unrelated to sleep problems, as measured by actigraphy and pulse oximetry (Ashworth, Hill, Karmiloff-Smith, & Dimitriou, 2015).
The relationship between sleep and daytime behaviour has particular relevance among children with Down syndrome, for two primary reasons. First, children with Down syndrome are at an elevated risk for daytime behaviour problems that are associated with sleep in other clinical populations. For example, children with Down syndrome often engage in noncompliant behaviours (McGuire & Chicoine, 2006) and are characterised by symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder at rates 2–3 times higher than similarly aged children with intellectual and developmental disabilities (Dekker & Koot, 2003; Ekstein, Glick, Weill, Kay, & Berger, 2011; Froehlich et al., 2007). Second, both physiologically-based sleep problems [e.g., OSA (Bull & Genetics, 2011)] and behaviourally-based sleep problems (e.g., bedtime resistance) have high prevalence in Down syndrome, with more concerns observed in children than adults (Esbensen, 2016), and in pre-adolescents than adolescents (Carter et al., 2009). Individuals with Down syndrome also frequently demonstrate abnormal polysomnography with high apnoea hypopnoea index and sleep fragmentation (Levanon, Tarasiuk, & Tal, 1999; Trois et al., 2009).
To date, very few studies have examined the relationship between sleep and daytime behaviour in children with Down syndrome. In studies among school-age children with Down syndrome, disturbed sleep was associated with both the child’s daily functioning (irritability and over-activity), and parental well-being (R. Stores, 1993; R. Stores, Stores, Fellows, & Buckley, 1998). Parent-reported sleep concerns among children with Down syndrome have also been associated with worse adaptive functioning compared to typically developing children, particularly with respect to personal care, relationships, school, fitness, and communication (Churchill et al., 2014). These studies have exclusively relied on parent-reports of sleep and behavioural concerns, which can tend to inflate associations due to shared reporter variance, and with limited external validation of their measures. Using a small sample, actigraphy measures of total sleep time, activity level and snoring are correlated with several measures of inappropriate behaviour in children with Down syndrome (R. Stores & Stores, 2014). These preliminary studies beg for additional research to confirm the relationship between sleep and behaviour in other contexts, and to confirm the importance of evaluation and management of sleep problems in the clinical care for children with Down syndrome.
Psychometrically validated measures are available to assess sleep and maladaptive behaviour in children with Down syndrome. The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ) is commonly used to assess sleep in children with intellectual and developmental disabilities (Carter et al., 2009). The CSHQ has been demonstrated to be psychometrically sound for children with Down syndrome (Esbensen & Hoffman, 2017). Children with Down syndrome demonstrate elevated concerns relative to typically developing peers for bedtime resistance, sleep onset delay, sleep anxiety, night waking, parasomnias, sleep disordered breathing and daytime sleepiness subscales of the CSHQ (Carter et al., 2009; Esbensen & Hoffman, 2017). However, parent reports of disordered breathing do not correspond with abnormal polysomnography findings (Shott et al., 2006). The Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form (NCBRF) is currently recommended for use in children with Down syndrome for assessing comorbid behaviours of concern (Edgin et al., 2010). Both the NCBRF and the Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales are considered appropriate for children with intellectual and developmental disabilities (Esbensen et al., 2017). Currently, no measure of maladaptive behaviour has been evaluated specifically for its psychometric properties with children with Down syndrome (Esbensen et al., 2017).
This study used multi-method assessments of sleep and multi-informant assessments of daytime behaviour to clarify the relationship between common sleep problems and daytime behaviours in children with Down syndrome and expand findings beyond parent report. We focus on sleep problems of shorter sleep duration and poor sleep quality as actigraph measures of these two constructs demonstrate convergent validity with gold standard polysomnography measures of Total Sleep Time and Sleep Efficiency in children with Down syndrome (Hoffman & Esbensen, 2017). We hypothesised that sleep would be related to daytime functioning, specifically that parental-report and actigraphy-based measures indicating shorter sleep duration and poorer sleep quality would be associated with parent and teacher reports of higher levels of problematic behaviour. Based on findings with children with intellectual and developmental disabilities and children with Down syndrome, we hypothesised that sleep would be related to parent and teacher reports of inattention, anxiety and challenging behaviour, specifically conduct problems (Harvey & Kennedy, 2002; Rzepecka et al., 2011). We also examined the relationship between sleep and hyperactivity given the high prevalence of hyperactivity in children with Down syndrome.
Method
Participants
All study activities were approved and overseen by the Institutional Review Board at the medical centre. Community recruitment targeted school-age children with trisomy 21 Down syndrome. Families were recruited through distribution of fliers at the medical centre, a Down syndrome clinic, and newsletters distributed by the local Down syndrome association. Study participants included 30 children with Down syndrome and their parents participating in a community-based pilot study examining sleep and associated daytime behaviour and cognition. One additional family consented but did not complete the study battery. Genetic diagnoses of Trisomy 21 were confirmed via medical record. Children with Down syndrome ranged in age from 6 to 17 years (M = 11.68 years, SD = 2.73) and were predominantly male (60%) and Caucasian (93%; 7% African-American). Full scale IQ on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-2 ranged from 40–65 (M = 44.57, SD = 6.46) (Kaufman, 2004). The Broad Index score of the Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised ranged from 15–93 (M = 51.26, SD = 21.62) (Bruininks, Woodcock, Weatherman, & Hill, 1996). Parent respondents were predominantly mothers (97%). The following medical conditions were reported: congenital heart defect 50%, overweight/obesity 48.2%, recurrent otitis media 36.7%, OSA 33.3%, ADHD 23.3%, GI concerns 20%, and anxiety 6.7%.
Measures
Sleep
Sleep duration and quality of sleep were assessed in a multimodal fashion. To obtain objective sleep data, children wore a Micro-mini Motionlogger Actigraph (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc.), which is a battery operated device that closely resembles a watch and measures movement. The actigraph was placed on the non-dominant wrist of the participant 30 minutes before bedtime and removed from the wrist 30 minutes after rising in the morning. Parents completed a companion sleep diary which was used to corroborate actigraphy estimates. Movement data were processed using a validated sleep scoring algorithm, which differentiates between sleep and wake states (Micro-Mini Motionlogger Instruction Manual, 2000; Sadeh, Sharkey, & Carskadon, 1994). Specific actigraph measures of sleep duration and sleep quality used for the current analyses included: (1) Sleep Duration: sleep period, the time from when the child fell asleep to when the child woke up, ignoring waking times within that period; and (2) Sleep Quality: sleep efficiency, defined as the percent of the sleep period that the child spent in sleep, which offers an objective index of sleep quality. Sleep period and sleep efficiency were determined for each night children wore the actigraph, then averaged across the week to obtain more stable indexes for current analyses. Children demonstrated high compliance during the 7 consecutive day actigraph period; 89% of children wore the actigraph for 6 or 7 nights. One child wore the watch for only 3 nights, and two children wore the watch for 4 nights. The actigraphs were worn on average for 6.4 nights (SD = 1.1). The mean percentage of nights actigraphs were worn was 91.8%.
While objective, actigraph results are limited to inferences that can be drawn from movement patterns during the recording period. Questionnaires, though collecting subjective impressions, can provide input from a longer-term parent observation perspective (Beebe, 2012). The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ) is a 33-item sleep screening instrument for children and assesses major childhood medical and behavioural sleep disorders during a typical week (Owens, Spirito, & McGuinn, 2000). Items are rated on a 3-point Likert-type scale from (1) rarely (0–1 time/week) to (3) usually (5–7 times/week). Although designed for use in paediatric populations under 10 years of age without intellectual disabilities, the CSHQ demonstrates strong psychometric properties and convergence in identifying behavioural sleep problems in school-age children with Down syndrome ages 6–17 years (Esbensen & Hoffman, 2017) and has demonstrated validity in other paediatric populations characterised by intellectual and developmental disabilities (Goldman, Bichell, Surdyka, & Malow, 2012; Richdale & Baker, 2014; Veatch et al., 2016). Two CSHQ subscales assessing sleep duration and quality were used in the current analyses. The CSHQ includes a three-item Sleep Duration subscale that assesses parent perception of child’s sleep efficiency and consistency. The CSHQ has several other subscales that could relate to sleep quality. After inspection of the most common sleep problems reported in the sample, we elected to use the Parasomnias subscale as the parent-reported measure of sleep quality. Although this seven-item subscale incorporates actual parasomnias (e.g., sleep-talking), it also includes an item related to restlessness/movements during sleep that was most often endorsed by parents (83%). Half of parents reported restlessness to occur sometimes, and a third reported restlessness to occur 5–7 nights of the week.
Daytime Behaviour
Two sets of measures were also used to assess daytime behaviour. We administered the parent- and teacher-report versions of the Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form (NCBRF-P and NCBRF-T); (Aman, Tassé, Rojahn, & Hammer, 1996), which was designed specifically for use in populations with intellectual disabilities. Items are rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale from behaviour did not occur or was not a problem (scored 0) to behaviour occurred a lot or was a severe problem (scored 3). This scale demonstrates strong psychometric properties and, unlike some other broad-band behavioural measures, does not directly ask about sleep. The NCBRF demonstrates high inter-rater reliability between parent and teacher forms on all scales and high internal consistency for multiple subscales (Aman et al., 1996). The conduct problems and insecure/anxious subscales from the NCBRF were selected based on prior research with children with intellectual disabilities showing relationships between poor sleep and these behavioural domains of challenging behaviour and anxiety (Didden et al., 2002; Rzepecka et al., 2011).
In order to assess inattention and hyperactivity, which are distinct constructs that could be impacted differentially by sleep difficulties (Beebe, 2012), we used the Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales – Parent and Teacher Forms (VADPRS and VADTRS). The VADPRS and VADTRS are DSM-IV-based scales that provide clinical information regarding the frequency and severity of symptoms related to ADHD across the home and school domains (Wolraich et al., 2003). Internal consistency and reliability are excellent (Wolraich et al., 2003) across the 9-item inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity subscales. Items are rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (very often) but, as is typical for this measure, a symptom count was tallied for how many items out of 9 items were scored 2 or 3 on Inattention, and similarly for the 9-item Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale.
Procedure
Children wore an actigraph for seven consecutive nights at home immediately prior to an office visit, during which parents provided information on the child’s demographics, daily living skills, and completed sleep and behavioural rating forms. During this office visit, children participated in a neuropsychological battery that included brief IQ testing. During that same week teachers also completed behavioural rating forms that were distributed and collected by parents. Teacher reports were collected from 25 teachers. Four teacher reports were not obtained as the child was on school break. One teacher report was not returned.
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics and correlational analyses were completed for sleep and daytime behaviour measures. After confirmation that the assumptions of regression (e.g., normality of residuals, multicollinearity of predictors) were not violated, hierarchical linear regressions were used to test whether sleep duration and quality predicted parent- and teacher-reports of daytime behaviour. Separate regressions were run for each parent- and teacher-rated subscale (i.e., NCBRF-Conduct, NCBRF-Insecure, VADPRS and VADTRS attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity subscales). Age of the child and gender were entered as covariates as sleep concerns vary with age in individuals with Down syndrome and as behaviour concerns vary with gender and age in individuals with intellectual disability (Ashworth, Hill, Karmiloff-Smith, & Dimitriou, 2013; Schroeder, Tessel, Loupe, & Stodgell, 1997).
Results
Descriptive data for sleep and behaviour measures are presented in Table 1. Descriptive data for the NCBRF are presented as item means, reflecting the total score divided by the number of items on the subscale to support comparison across subscales containing different number of items. Inter-correlations between measures of sleep and behaviours are presented in Table 2. Despite a moderate bivariate correlation between the two CSHQ subscales, multivariate collinearity was found not to be a concern in subsequent regression analyses.
Table 1.
Mean (SD) | Range | |
---|---|---|
Sleep measures | ||
Parent reporta | ||
CSHQ Sleep Duration | 4.27 (1.80) | 3 – 9 |
CSHQ Parasomnias | 9.38 (2.03) | 7 – 14 |
Actigraphyb | ||
Sleep period (minutes) | 553.47 (39.37) | 466.43 – 618.64 |
Sleep efficiency (percent) | 87.41 (6.04) | 72.49 – 96.41 |
Parent Behaviour measures | ||
NCBRF (subscale mean 0–3)c | ||
Conduct Problem | 0.54 (0.50) | 0 – 2.31 |
Insecure/Anxious | 0.18 (0.20) | 0 – 0.81 |
Vanderbilt (# items endorsed 2 or 3) | ||
Inattention | 2.97 (2.86) | 0 – 9 |
Hyperactivity/Impulsivity | 1.34 (2.07) | 0 – 7 |
Teacher Behaviour measures | ||
NCBRF (subscale mean 0–3)c | ||
Conduct Problem | 0.49 (0.55) | 0 – 2.08 |
Insecure/Anxious | 0.24 (0.26) | 0 – 1.13 |
Vanderbilt (# items endorsed 2 or 3) | ||
Inattention | 3.88 (3.11) | 0 – 9 |
Hyperactivity/Impulsivity | 1.48 (2.02) | 0 – 7 |
Possible range of scores on Sleep Duration is 3–9. Possible range of scores on Parasomnias is 7–21.
Actigraphy measures are averaged over the week the actigraph is worn.
NCBRF scores reflect the total score divided by the number of items to achieve an item mean.
Note: CSHQ = Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire, NCBRF = Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 CSHQ Sleep Duration | - | ||||||||||
2 CSHQ Parasomnias | .49** | - | |||||||||
3 Sleep period | −.20 | .07 | - | ||||||||
4 Sleep efficiency | .03 | −.23 | .26 | - | |||||||
5 NCBRF-P Conduct | .23 | .65** | .10 | .03 | - | ||||||
6 NCBRF-P Insecure/Anxious | .40* | .64** | .06 | .15 | .82** | - | |||||
7 NCBRF-T Conduct | .06 | .62** | .03 | −.10 | .81** | .55** | - | ||||
8 NCBRF-T Insecure/Anxious | −.03 | .33 | .16 | −.02 | .47* | .26 | .74** | - | |||
9 VADPRS Inattentive | .56** | .49** | −.35 | .02 | .45* | .63** | .42* | .03 | - | ||
10 VADPRS Hyperactive/Impulsivity | .46* | .55** | −.39* | .00 | .73** | .55** | .68** | .38 | .53** | - | |
11 VADTRS Inattentive | .09 | .29 | −.14 | .12 | .47* | .36 | .59** | .30 | .45* | .44* | - |
12 VADTRS Hyperactive/Impulsivity | −.03 | .62** | −.16 | −.22 | .79** | .49* | .82** | .64** | .33 | .72** | .54** |
p < .05,
p < .01
Note: CSHQ = Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire, NCBRF-P = Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form – Parent, NCBRF-T = Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form - Teacher, VADPRS = Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales – Parent, VADTRS = Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales – Teacher.
Sleep predicting parent reports of behaviour
Table 3 summarises results of regressions in which sleep measures were entered as predictors of parent-reported daytime behaviours. Neither age nor gender was significantly related to any of the analysed parent-report daytime behaviour measures. In contrast, roughly half of the variance on each parent-report behaviour measure was statistically predicted by the collective sleep variables (R2 change = .42 – .53, p < .05). The CSHQ parasomnia subscale was related to the parent-reported Conduct problems (β = .78, p < .01), Insecure/Anxious behaviours (β = .73, p < .01), and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (β = .62, p < .01), and had a modest relationship to inattention that did not reach statistical significance. Overall, poorer parent-reported parasomnias were associated with more daytime behavioural concerns. Parent-reported sleep duration was related to Inattention (β = .40, p < .05), but not to other parent reports of daytime behaviour problems. Further, a shorter sleep period, as measured by actigraphy, was related to higher parent ratings of both Inattention (β = −.36, p < .05) and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (β = −.50, p < .01), but not Conduct Problems or Insecure/Anxious behaviours. Actigraphy-measured sleep efficiency did not significantly predict any of the four parent-reported daytime behaviour scales.
Table 3.
NCBRF-P Conduct |
NCBRF-P Insecure/Anxious |
VADPRS Inattention |
VADPRS Hyperactive/Impulsivity |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
R2 change | β | R2 change | β | R2 change | β | R2 change | β | |
Step 1 | .06 | .02 | .07 | .06 | ||||
Age | −.23 | −.13 | −.24 | −.23 | ||||
Gender | −.06 | .01 | .08 | −.09 | ||||
| ||||||||
Step 2 | .42* | .51** | .53** | .50** | ||||
Age | −.01 | .13 | −.10 | −.06 | ||||
Gender | .00 | .12 | .31 | .01 | ||||
CSHQ Duration | −.14 | .09 | .40* | .04 | ||||
CSHQ Parasomnia | .78** | .73** | .38 | .62** | ||||
Sleep period | −.05 | −.04 | −.36* | −.50** | ||||
Sleep efficiency | .23 | .31 | .11 | .28 |
p < .05,
p < .01
Note: CSHQ = Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire, NCBRF-P = Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form – Parent, VADPRS = Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales – Parent.
Sleep predicting teacher reports of behaviour
Table 4 summarises results of regressions in which sleep measures were entered as predictors of teacher-reported daytime behaviours. Neither age nor gender were significant predictors of teacher-reported outcomes. Subsequent entry of the sleep variables again resulted in marginal or substantial prediction of two out of four teacher-reported outcomes. Here, parent-report of sleep duration was not predictive of teacher-report of daytime behavioural concerns, but more parent-reported problems on the CSHQ parasomnia subscale significantly predicted teacher-reported daytime Conduct Problems (β = .66, p < .01), and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (β = .75, p < .01). No sleep index significantly predicted teacher-reported Inattention. Actigraphy-based indexes generally were not significantly associated with teacher-reported behavioural concerns.
Table 4.
NCBRF-T Conduct |
NCBRF-T Insecure/Anxious |
VADTRS Inattention |
VADTRS Hyperactive/Impulsivity |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
R2 change | β | R2 change | β | R2 change | β | R2 change | β | |
Step 1 | .12 | .07 | .17 | .05 | ||||
Age | −.32 | .16 | −.39 | −.26 | ||||
Gender | −.15 | −.21 | .15 | .01 | ||||
| ||||||||
Step 2 | .32 | .21 | .08 | .47* | ||||
Age | −.14 | .34 | −.31 | −.06 | ||||
Gender | −.17 | −.27 | .12 | −.05 | ||||
CSHQ Duration | −.21 | −.16 | −.13 | −.32 | ||||
CSHQ Parasomnia | .66** | .50 | .30 | .75** | ||||
Sleep period | −.13 | .02 | −.24 | −.35 | ||||
Sleep efficiency | −.11 | .25 | .12 | .07 |
p < .05,
p < .01
Note: CSHQ = Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire, NCBRF-T = Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form - Teacher, VADTRS = Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales – Teacher.
Discussion
The current study examined the relationship between parent- and actigraphy-reports of sleep duration and quality with parent- and teacher-reports of daytime behaviour problems among school-age children with Down syndrome. Parent report of poor sleep quality (as measured by the CSHQ Parasomnia subscale), but not actigraph-measured activity during sleep, was predictive of increased conduct problems, insecure and anxious behaviours, and daytime hyperactivity/impulsivity. This pattern was similar for both parent- and teacher-reported behaviour concerns, indicating that restless sleep as reported by parents has relevance for daytime functioning both at home and at school as observed by independent informants (parent and teacher). These findings parallel findings in children with autism spectrum disorders, where sleep problems (sleep efficiency, restlessness) are related to oppositional behaviour, anxiety and hyperactivity (Goldman et al., 2009; Limoges, Mottron, Bolduc, Berthiaume, & Godbout, 2005; Mayes & Calhoun, 2009). Both, parent-report and actigraph-measured shorter sleep period was predictive of daytime inattention as reported by parents, and actigraphy-measured sleep period predictive of hyperactivity/impulsivity as reported by both parents. These findings again parallel those in youth with autism spectrum disorder where objective measures of sleep problems (sleep latency) are related to an omnibus indicator of maladaptive behaviour problems (Bruni et al., 2007; Goldman et al., 2009). The present findings support the previous literature that sleep problems are related to daytime behaviour concerns in children with Down syndrome, in children with intellectual and developmental disabilities (Didden et al., 2002; Richdale et al., 2000), and children with autism spectrum disorders (Goldman et al., 2011; Sikora, Johnson, Clemons, & Katz, 2012).
These correlational findings cannot definitively establish causation – i.e., that sleep problems cause daytime behaviour problems, or that daytime behaviour problems contribute to poor sleep. Further research is needed to determine causality and inform interventions. However, based on the literature from typically developing children, others suggest that insufficient sleep duration or quality may serve as a setting event for noncompliant behaviours displayed at home and at school (Didden et al., 2002; R. Stores et al., 1998; Wiggs & Stores, 1996). Thus, interventions to improve daytime behaviour may need to also target or address current sleeping patterns. Communication between home and school regarding current sleeping patterns may be a helpful tool to understanding the role of sleep in behaviours observed at school and the implementation of appropriate behavioural strategies. Empirically-supported interventions for behaviours associated with sleep and sleep hygiene may be beneficial to improving sleep duration and quality. Currently, the first randomised clinical trial is underway to evaluate behavioural sleep strategies for improving sleep in children and adolescents with Down syndrome (NCT02996175).
Current findings also highlight that the relationship between sleep problems and behaviour problems can vary depending on how sleep problems are assessed; using parent reports or actigraphy. Indeed, despite our intent to perform multimodal assessments of the same constructs, it is noteworthy that parent-reported sleep duration correlated poorly with actigraphy-measured sleep period, while parent-reported sleep disruption (as measured by questions about nocturnal restlessness and parasomnias) correlated poorly with actigraphy-measured sleep efficiency or activity during sleep. Future research is needed to better understand this lack of correlation. Potential causes could include errors in parental estimates (e.g., due to selective awareness of events that disrupt parents’ sleep), movement-related artefacts on actigraphy in children with Down syndrome (a possibility that has not yet been examined), or subtle but important differences in the constructs being measured. On the latter point, whereas actigraphy provides an objective estimate of sleep duration, the CSHQ Sleep Duration subscale also asks about consistency of sleep length and perception of sleep relative to appropriate sleep length. This introduces an element of parental subjectivity and judgment that may reduce associations with daytime behaviours. Conversely, whereas actigraphy-measured sleep efficiency or activity during sleep is affected by both brief and lengthy waking bouts, it may be that parental report on the CSHQ assesses aspects of sleep disruption that are more selectively related to daytime behaviours. It may not be that one measurement source is “better” than the other, but that each has unique information to contribute. These findings underscore the importance of measuring behavioural sleep problems with multiple methods that are hypothesis-driven.
The sample in this pilot study was small and presented with a low rate of behavioural concerns, and thus provided limited power for conducting additional statistical analyses. For example, with this smaller sample size, it would be difficult to evaluate the impact of medical comorbidities on the relationship between sleep and daytime behaviour problems. In addition, though the CSHQ demonstrates convergent validity with polysomnography, the gold standard for assessing sleep problems remains assessing sleep with polysomnography (Hoffman & Esbensen, 2017). Despite these limitations, our findings replicate and extend the initial findings in the literature of a relationship between sleep problems and daytime behaviour in children with Down syndrome. Further work is needed to explore the bidirectional relationship between behaviours and how they may impact sleep. Our findings demonstrate a pattern of poor sleep contributing to a pattern of poor behaviour. Further work is needed to explore the immediate relationship between sleep and daytime behaviour on the following day. Future studies would benefit from examining the impact of medical comorbidities (obesity, obstructive sleep apnoea, ADHD, and autism spectrum disorders) on the relationship between sleep and daytime behaviour problems.
Children with Down syndrome are at increased risk for sleep problems (Esbensen & Schwichtenberg, 2016). Our study corroborates that sleep problems are a concern, and also that these sleep problems are associated with daytime problem behaviours, particularly conduct behaviours, anxious behaviours, inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. Thus, while screening for OSA is well-recognised in the Down syndrome community (Bull & Genetics, 2011), there also is a need for appropriate screening for other sleep problems in children with Down syndrome, as well as a need for behavioural interventions to understand the role of sleep in contributing to these behaviours. Future research is needed to understand how sleep problems may hinder behavioural interventions, and how sleep interventions may improve daytime behaviours.
Acknowledgments
This manuscript was prepared with support from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (R21 HD082307, A. Esbensen, PI), the Jack H Rubinstein Foundation, and the Emily Ann Hayes Research Fund. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This research would not have been possible without the contributions of the participating families.
References
- Aman MG, Tassé MJ, Rojahn J, Hammer D. The Nisonger CBRF: A child behavior rating form for children with developmental disabilities. Research in developmental disabilities. 1996;17(1):41–57. doi: 10.1016/0891-4222(95)00039-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashworth A, Hill CM, Karmiloff-Smith A, Dimitriou D. Cross syndrome comparison of sleep problems in children with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome. Research in developmental disabilities. 2013;34(5):1572–1580. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashworth A, Hill CM, Karmiloff-Smith A, Dimitriou D. The importance of sleep: Attentional problems in school-aged children with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome. Behavioral sleep medicine. 2015;13(6):455–471. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2014.940107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beebe D. Neurobehavioral effects of childhood sleep-disordered breathing (SDB): A comprehensive review. Sleep. 2006;29:1115–1134. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beebe D. Cognitive, behavioral, and functional consequences of inadequate sleep in children and adolescents. Pediatric Clinics of North America. 2011;58(3):649. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2011.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beebe D. A brief primer on sleep for pediatric and child clinical neuropsychologists. Child Neuropsychology. 2012;18(4):313–338. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.602014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruininks RH, Woodcock R, Weatherman R, Hill B. SIB-R: Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Bruni O, Ferri R, Vittori E, Novelli L, Vignati M, Porfirio MC, … Curatolo P. Sleep architecture and NREM alterations in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Sleep. 2007;30(11):1577–1585. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.11.1577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bull MJ, Genetics Co. Health supervision for children with Down syndrome. Pediatrics. 2011;128(2):393–406. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter M, McCaughey E, Annaz D, Hill CM. Sleep problems in a Down syndrome population. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2009;94(4):308–310. doi: 10.1136/adc.2008.146845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchill SS, Kieckhefer GM, Bjornson KF, Herting JR. Relationship between sleep disturbance and functional outcomes in daily life habits of children with Down syndrome. Sleep. 2014;38(1):61–71. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahl RE. Sleeplessness and aggression in youth. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;38:641–642. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Miguel-Diez J, Villa-Asensi JR, Alvarez-Sala JL. Prevalence of sleep-disordered breathing in children with Down syndrome: polygraphic findings in 108 children. Sleep. 2003;26(8):1006–1009. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.8.1006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dekker MC, Koot HM. DSM-IV disorders in children with borderline to moderate intellectual disability. I: Prevalence and impact. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42(8):915–922. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046892.27264.1A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewald FJ, Meijer AM, Oort FJ, Kerkhof GA, Bögels SM. The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Sleep medicine reviews. 2010;14:179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Didden R, Korzilius H, Aperlo Bv, Overloop Cv, Vries Md. Sleep problems and daytime problem behaviours in children with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2002;46(7):537–547. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2002.00404.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edgin JO, Mason GM, Allman MJ, Capone GT, DeLeon I, Maslen C, … Nadel L. Development and validation of the Arizona Cognitive Test Battery for Down syndrome. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2010;2:149–164. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9054-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekstein S, Glick B, Weill M, Kay B, Berger I. Down syndrome and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Child Neurology. 2011;26:1290–1295. doi: 10.1177/0883073811405201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein R, Pillar D, Tzichinsky O, Here P, Lavie P. Sleep disturbances in children with Downs’ syndrome. Journal of Sleep Research. 1992;1:68. [Google Scholar]
- Esbensen AJ. Sleep problems and associated comorbidities among adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2016;60(1):68–79. doi: 10.1111/jir.12236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esbensen AJ, Hoffman E. Reliability of parent report measures of sleep in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2017;61(3):210–220. doi: 10.1111/jir.12315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esbensen AJ, Hooper SR, Fidler D, Hartley SL, Edgin J, d’Ardhuy XL, … Abbeduto L. Outcome measures for clinical trials in Down syndrome. American journal on intellectual and developmental disabilities. 2017;122(3):247–281. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-122.3.247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esbensen AJ, Schwichtenberg A. Sleep in neurodevelopmental disorders. International Review of Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2016;51:153–191. doi: 10.1016/bs.irrdd.2016.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fallone G, Owens JA, Deane J. Sleepiness in children and adolescents: Clinical implications. Sleep medicine reviews. 2002;6:287–306. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN, Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Kahn RS. Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of US children. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2007;161(9):857–864. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.9.857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman SE, Bichell T, Surdyka K, Malow B. Sleep in children and adolescents with Angelman syndrome: Association with parent sleep and stress. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2012;56(6):600–608. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01499.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman SE, McGrew S, Johnson KP, Richdale AL, Clemons T, Malow BA. Sleep is associated with problem behaviors in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5(3):1223–1229. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman SE, Surdyka K, Cuevas R, Adkins K, Wang L, Malow BA. Defining the sleep phenotype in children with autism. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2009;34(5):560–573. doi: 10.1080/87565640903133509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey MT, Kennedy CH. Polysomnographic phenotypes in developmental disabilities. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2002;20(3):443–448. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(02)00008-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman E, Esbensen AJ. Measurement of Sleep in School-Age Children with Down Syndrome. Paper presented at the Gatlinburg Conference on Research and Theory in Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities; San Antonio, TX. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman A. KBIT-2: Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. 2. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Levanon A, Tarasiuk A, Tal A. Sleep characteristics in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Pediatrics. 1999;134:755–760. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(99)70293-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Limoges É, Mottron L, Bolduc C, Berthiaume C, Godbout R. Atypical sleep architecture and the autism phenotype. Brain. 2005;128(5):1049–1061. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malow BA, Marzec ML, McGrew SG, Wang L, Henderson LM, Stone WL. Characterizing sleep in children with autism spectrum disorders: A multidimensional approach. Sleep. 2006;29(12):1563. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.12.1563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcus C, Keens T, Bautista D, von Pechman W, Ward S. Obstructive sleep apnea in children with Down syndrome. Pediatrics. 1991;88:132–139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maris M, Verhulst S, Wojciechowski M, Van de Heyning P, Boudewyns A. Sleep problems and obstructive sleep apnea in children with down syndrome, an overwiew. International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2015.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Variables related to sleep problems in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2009;3(4):931–941. [Google Scholar]
- McGuire D, Chicoine B. Mental Wellness in Adults with Down Syndrome. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Micro-Mini Motionlogger Instruction Manual. Ardsley, NY: Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Owens JA, Spirito A, McGuinn M. The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep. 2000;23(8):1043–1052. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paavonen E, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Lahikainen AR. Sleep quality, duration and behavioral symptoms among 5–6 year old children. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;18:747–754. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0033-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paavonen E, Raikkonen K, Lahti J, Komsi N, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK. Short sleep duration and behavioral symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in healthy 7- to 8-year old children. Pediatric. 2009;123:e857–864. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quine L. Sleep problems in children with mental handicap. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 1991;35(4):269–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1991.tb00402.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quine L. Severity of sleep problems in children with severe learning difficulties: description and correlates. Journal of community & applied social psychology. 1992;2(4):247–268. [Google Scholar]
- Richdale AL, Baker EK. Sleep in individuals with an intellectual or developmental disability: Recent research reports. Current Developmental Disorders Reports. 2014;1(2):74–85. [Google Scholar]
- Richdale AL, Francis A, Gavidia-Payne S, Cotton S. Stress, behaviour, and sleep problems in children with an intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability. 2000;25(2):147–161. [Google Scholar]
- Rzepecka H, McKenzie K, McClure I, Murphy S. Sleep, anxiety and challenging behaviour in children with intellectual disability and/or autism spectrum disorder. Research in developmental disabilities. 2011;32(6):2758–2766. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. Sleep, Neurobehavioral Functioning, and Behavior Problems in School-Age Children. Child development. 2002;73:405–417. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh A, Raviv A, Gruber R. Sleep patterns and sleep disruptions in school-age children. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:291–301. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.3.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh A, Sharkey KM, Carskadon MA. Activity-Based Sleep—Wake Identification: An Empirical Test of Methodological Issues. Sleep. 1994;17(3):201–207. doi: 10.1093/sleep/17.3.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder SR, Tessel RE, Loupe PS, Stodgell CJ. Severe behavior problems among people with developmental disabilities. In: MacLean WE, editor. Ellis’ Handbook of Mental Deficiency, Psychological Theory and Research. 3. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1997. pp. 439–464. [Google Scholar]
- Shott S, Amin R, Chini B, Heubi C, Hotze S, Akers R. Obstructive sleep apnea: Should children with Down syndrome be tested? Archives of Otolaryngology Head Neck and Surgery. 2006;132:432–436. doi: 10.1001/archotol.132.4.432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sikora DM, Johnson K, Clemons T, Katz T. The relationship between sleep problems and daytime behavior in children of different ages with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2012;130(Supplement 2):S83–S90. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0900F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stebbens V, Dennis J, Samuels M, Croft C, Southall D. Sleep related upper airway obstruction in a cohort with Down’s syndrome. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 1991;66:1333–1338. doi: 10.1136/adc.66.11.1333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steenari MR, Vuontela V, Paavonen EJ, Carlson S, Fjalberg M, Aronen E. Working memory and sleep in 6- to 13- year old school children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:85–92. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200301000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stores G, Wiggs LE. Sleep disturbance in children and adolescents with disorders of development: Its significance and management. Cambridge University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Stores R. A preliminary study of sleep disorders and daytime behaviour problems in children with Down’s syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 1993;1:29–33. [Google Scholar]
- Stores R, Stores G. The significance of aspects of screening for obstructive sleep apnoea in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2014;58(4):381–392. doi: 10.1111/jir.12033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stores R, Stores G, Fellows B, Buckley S. A factor analysis of sleep problems and their psychological associations in children with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disability. 1998;17:345–354. [Google Scholar]
- Trois MS, Capone GT, Lutz JA, Melendres MC, Schwartz AR, Collop NA, Marcus CL. Obstructive sleep apnea in adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine. 2009;5(4):317–323. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veatch OJ, Reynolds A, Katz T, Weiss SK, Loh A, Wang L, Malow BA. Sleep in children with autism spectrum disorders: How are measures of parent report and actigraphy related and affected by sleep education? Behavioral sleep medicine. 2016;14(6):665–676. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2015.1065408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiggs L, Stores G. Severe sleep disturbance and daytime challenging behaviour in children with severe learning disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 1996;40(6):518–528. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.1996.799799.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolraich ML, Lambert W, Doffing MA, Bickman L, Simmons T, Worley K. Psychometric properties of the Vanderbilt ADHD diagnostic parent rating scale in a referred population. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2003;28(8):559–568. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright B, Sims D, Smart S, Alwazeer A, Alderson-Day B, Allgar V, … Jardine J. Melatonin versus placebo in children with autism spectrum conditions and severe sleep problems not amenable to behaviour management strategies: A randomised controlled crossover trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011;41(2):175–184. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1036-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]