Skip to main content
i-Perception logoLink to i-Perception
. 2018 Jan 19;9(1):2041669518754595. doi: 10.1177/2041669518754595

Transient Signals and Inattentional Blindness in a Multi-object Tracking Task

Dakota B Palmer 1, Yusuke Yamani 1,, Taylor L Bobrow 2, Nicole D Karpinsky 3, Dean J Krusienski 4
PMCID: PMC5777565  PMID: 29375755

Abstract

Inattentional blindness is a failure to notice an unexpected event when attention is directed elsewhere. The current study examined participants’ awareness of an unexpected object that maintained luminance contrast, switched the luminance once, or repetitively flashed. One hundred twenty participants performed a dynamic tracking task on a computer monitor for which they were instructed to count the number of movement deflections of an attended set of objects while ignoring other objects. On the critical trial, an unexpected cross that did not change its luminance (control condition), switched its luminance once (switch condition), or repetitively flashed (flash condition) traveled across the stimulus display. Participants noticed the unexpected cross more frequently when the luminance feature matched their attention set than when it did not match. Unexpectedly, however, a proportion of the participants who noticed the cross in the switch and flash conditions were statistically comparable. The results suggest that an unexpected object with even a single luminance change can break inattentional blindness in a multi-object tracking task.

Keywords: inattentional blindness, visual awareness, salience


Inattentional blindness (IB) occurs when an observer fails to notice conspicuous events or objects when their attention is directed elsewhere (Chabris, Weinberger, Fontaine, & Simons, 2011; Mack & Rock, 1998; Most, Scholl, Clifford, & Simons, 2005; Most, Scholl, Jimenez, Clifford, & Chabris, 2001; Simons & Chabris, 1999; Simons, 2000). IB can arise even with a direct fixation at an unexpected object (Drew, Võ, & Wolfe, 2013; Memmert, 2006), which suggests that IB arises not due to lack of direct fixations but from postsensory processes. Even expert observers and professional practitioners (Drew et al., 2013; Memmert, 2006), as well as those who are familiar with the phenomenon (Simons, 2010), are not immune to the effect of IB. In the real world, IB can lead to critical failures and have disastrous outcomes in professional tasks, such as piloting a vehicle (Fischer, Haines, & Price, 1980) or screening medical scans for abnormalities (Potchen, 2006). Thus, understanding the mechanisms underlying IB may help visual display designers for transportation and health-care systems select effective visual alerts that break operators’ IB.

Experimentally, Mack and Rock (1998) demonstrated IB in their static IB paradigm. In their experiment, observers were asked to judge which arm of a cross that was briefly presented at the center of the display was shorter. In a critical trial, an unexpected object appeared simultaneously with the cross, and observers reported whether they saw anything unusual after the completion of the trial. Approximately 25% of the observers failed to report the presence of the unexpected object, showing IB even in a simple perceptual task. Surprisingly, color changes and movements of the unexpected object did not entirely eliminate IB (Mack & Rock, 1998), suggesting that object saliency itself does not always capture observers’ attention (see Simons, 2000 for review).

More recently, Most et al. (2001; also 2005) showed that not only do bottom-up factors such as saliency of an unexpected object influence IB, but top-down factors such as attention sets influence IB in a dynamic multi-object tracking task. Participants were asked to view a dynamic visual display consisting of white and black circles and squares moving on random trajectories and to count the number of times either white or black objects deflected off the side of the display boundary while fixating at the center point of the display. Following two standard trials, on the third critical trial, an unexpected object which was visible for 5 seconds entered the display from the right side and traveled on a straight path through the center of the screen, exiting on the left side. The features of the unexpected critical stimulus such as shape, color, and luminance were systematically manipulated in a series of their experiments. After the critical trial, participants were asked if they noticed anything unusual and, if so, to describe what they saw. Depending on the unexpected object’s properties and their attention sets, as many as 100% of participants failed to notice it. Note that these participants detected the critical object in the following full attention trial, where they were asked to fixate on the display center without performing the tracking task. Strikingly, Most et al.’s (2001) showed that the probability of noticing an unexpected object depends on similarity between visual features of an unexpected object and those in participants’ attention set.

Thus, Most et al. (2001) showed that the probability of noticing unexpected objects depends on the degree of correspondence between an observer’s attention set and the stimulus property of an unexpected object. However, whether a highly salient, unexpected object overrides the top-down influence on attention and allows observers to notice the object remains unclear. Previous research indicates that highly salient and unexpected events do not necessarily promote visual awareness while performing a primary monitoring task (Simons & Chabris, 1999). Extending Mack and Rock’s (1998) study, Simons and Chabris (1999) asked participants to watch videos showing two teams of three players in black or white shirts (black vs. white teams) and silently count the number of passes made by one of two teams. In the middle of the tape, an experimenter wearing a gorilla costume (in black) entered from the left side of the action, thumped its chest, and then continued across the field of view. Results not only indicated that roughly half of participants failed to detect the Gorilla but also corroborated the attention set account of IB (Most et al., 2001) in that twice more number of participants noticed the Gorilla in the black condition than the white condition. Such results highlight the possibility that saliency of an unexpected event influences visual awareness more strongly when properties of the event match with participants’ attention set.

The current study aims to replicate the findings of Most et al. (2001) and examine the effects of levels of object saliency on visual awareness in a dynamic multi-object tracking task. We manipulated observers’ attention set (track black or white objects while ignoring white or black objects, respectively) and salience of an unexpected moving object (constant, switch, or flash). The unexpected object did not change its luminance (the constant condition), switched its luminance once (the switch condition), or flashed repetitively (the flash condition) in the tasks. We predicted that participants would notice the unexpected object more frequently in the flash condition than the switch condition, because the repetitive flashes create more frequent transient signals than the single switch. Furthermore, we predicted that participants would notice the unexpected object more frequently in the switch condition than the constant condition.

Method

Participants

One hundred twenty participants (96 females; mean age = 21.17 years, SD = 5.81; mean near visual acuity = 20/22.75; mean far visual acuity = 20/21.27) were recruited from the community of Old Dominion University. This study was reviewed and approved by the institutional review board at Old Dominion University. All participants were screened for normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal color perception. They received a research participation credit for their participation.

Stimuli

Following Most et al.’s (2001) methodology, each stimulus display contained four circle and four square objects on a gray background (luminance = 32.1 cd/m2) with a blue fixation point at the center of the display. One group of objects was colored black (luminance = 1.5 cd/m2) while the other was colored white (luminance = 88.0 cd/m2).

Figure 1 illustrates a sample of the stimulus display. During the task, the objects moved across random paths at a variable rate ranging from 2° to 5° per second. The moving objects deflected at the display boundaries such that the angle of reflection was equal to the angle of incidence with the boundary.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

An example of the visual display in a critical event trial. Circle and square objects move randomly while an unexpected object flickering at 7.5 Hz enters from the right side of the display, over the middle fixation point, and exits the left side of the display.

In the critical trials, a cross with the same horizontal and vertical dimensions as circles and squares (1.0° × 1.0°) moved horizontally and linearly across the center of the screen in front of the fixation point from right to left. The luminance value of the cross remained the same (control condition), changed from white to black or black to white (switch condition), or rapidly alternated between black and white (flashing condition; frequency = 7.5 Hz, mean luminance = 14.1 cd/m2) through its path. Each of precritical and critical trials lasted 15 seconds.

Procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three experimental conditions: Control, Switch, or Flashing. Within each condition, half of the participants attended to black objects while the others attended to white objects. Participants were instructed to maintain gaze on the fixation point, attend to target objects (black vs. white), and count the number of times target objects deflected off the display boundaries. Although participants were instructed to maintain their gaze on the central fixation point while completing the tracking task, we did not use eye-tracking to ensure this was the case, as IB is shown to arise even when observers directly fixate at unexpected objects (e.g., Drew et al., 2013; Mack & Rock, 1998).

Each trial began with the objects at random initial positions and central fixation point appearing in the visual display and motionless for 1 second. The objects then moved throughout the visual display until the trial ended. Participants completed a sequence of six experimental trials. The fifth and sixth trials contained the critical object. Five seconds after the beginning of the fifth and sixth trials, the critical cross entered and was visible for 5 seconds.

At the end of each critical trial, participants were asked to report whether an unusual event occurred during the trial to gauge their awareness of the critical event. Pressing “1” on the numeric keypad indicated that they detected an unusual event (presumably the cross) and “2” otherwise. If participants indicated that something unusual occurred, they were asked to report a description of the unusual event they perceived.

The sixth trial was a full attention trial, in which participants were instructed to passively view the display while fixating on the central fixation point, rather than counting the number of times objects deflected off the display boundaries. The full attention trial was performed to ensure that participants were able to perceive the object when attention was not diverted. Thus, data from those who failed to observe the critical object on the sixth trial were removed from the analysis. At the end of all trials except for the full attention trial, participants were asked to report the total number of times target objects deflected off the edges of the display by typing the number using the numeric keypad when prompted. Participants then received feedback regarding accuracy in the counting task after each trial for 750 milliseconds, consisting of correct or incorrect with the actual correct number of deflections.

Statistical Analyses

Instead of the standard null hypothesis significance tests, we employed default Bayesian tests (Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, & Iverson, 2009) using BayesFactor package in R (Morey, Rouder, & Jamil, 2015). The default Bayesian tests can provide evidence for the null hypothesis and Bayes factors provide a meaningful measure of evidence strength. The measure of evidence strength is B10, denoting the ratio of the likelihood that data support an alternative model that includes an effect of interest to the likelihood that data support the null model that excludes the effect. For example, a Bayes factor of 3 indicates that an alternative model is 3 times more likely resulting from the data than the null model, while a Bayes factor of 1/3 indicates substantial evidence for the null model over an alternative model. Descriptive terms for each comparison come from Jeffreys (1961) and Wetzels et al. (2011).

Results and Discussion

Data from 15 participants were removed from the analysis due to failure to report the unexpected cross in the full attention trial. Table 1 presents the number of participants who noticed the unexpected cross across the three conditions. Omnibus analysis showed comparable noticing rates across the three experimental conditions, χ2(2) = 3.46, B10 = 1/5.40. However, the results show that participants from the control condition noticed the unexpected cross more frequently when the luminance of the cross matched with their attention set, replicating Most et al. (2001). When attending to black objects, all the participants noticed the black cross while 57% of them noticed the white cross, and the data gave substantial evidence for a difference, χ2(1) = 2.35, B10 = 4.85. Similarly, when attending to white objects, all the participants noticed the white cross while 42% of them noticed the black cross, which again provided strong evidence for a difference, χ2(1) = 3.15, B10 = 7.80.

Table 1.

Number of Participants on the Three Experimental Conditions Who Noticed the Unexpected Cross in the Critical and Full-Attention Attention Trials.

Unexpected object Attention set n Trial type
Critical Full attention
Control
Black objects White objects 10 5 7
Black objects 10 10 9
White objects White objects 10 9 7
Black objects 10 5 7
Switch
Black to White White objects 10 7 9
Black objects 10 9 10
White to Black White objects 10 9 9
Black objects 10 9 10
Flashing
Flashing White objects 20 18 19
Black objects 20 19 18

Contrary to our expectation, the single transient signal of the unexpected luminance-switching cross produced rates of response comparable to those with the highly salient flashing cross (86% vs. 91% for the switch and flash conditions, respectively), χ2(1) = .50, B10 = 1/4.50. Furthermore, the data also gave substantial evidence that target type (white or black) does not influence rates of noticing (B10 = 1/3.16 and B10 = 1/3.96 for the switch and flash conditions, respectively). Within the switch condition, the data produced substantial evidence against the effect of the order of the luminance change (white to black vs. black to white) on rates of noticing, χ2(1) = .23, B10 = 1/3.38.

To compare between the control and switch conditions more directly, we compared the proportions of responders when the luminance of the cross matched with their attention set (the match condition) and when it did not (the mismatch condition) in the control condition separately to the proportion of responders in the switch condition. Data showed anecdotal trends in favor of the null when comparing the match condition to the switch condition, χ2(1) = .91, B10 = 1/2.02. However, data provided strong evidence that the switch condition produced a greater noticing rate than the mismatch condition, χ2(1) =5.88, B10 = 10.85.

There are three points to highlight from the results. First, using Most et al.’s (2001) multi-object tracking paradigm, we replicated the effect of attention set on visual awareness. Compared to the proportions of participants reported in Most et al. (2001, Experiment 1), the effect of attention set was, though statistically reliable, markedly smaller in the current study. The smaller number of participants in the control condition in the current study (N = 31), as opposed to Most et al.’s (2001) study (N = 145), could have contributed to the discrepancy between the two studies.

Second, the results imply that the participants noticed the cross because luminance of the object matched with the attention set for a half of the time during the critical trial in the switch and the flash conditions. Participants attending to white objects, for example, might have noticed the cross on the switch or flash condition because the luminance of the cross was white half of the time that the critical display contained the cross. Future research should investigate the relationship between visual awareness and the influence of the amount of time that features of an unexpected target match with those in participants’ attention set when the features of the target vary dynamically in a multi-object tracking task. One alternative account of the current results is that transient signals captured attention. Thus, additional research can directly investigate the effects of transient signals of an unexpected object by manipulating the number of transient signals on IB while controlling the amount of time that the luminance of the unexpected object matches with participants’ attention set.

Finally, it should be noted that the current results are not likely due to an inadequate sample size or a ceiling effect. The experiments reported in Most et al. (2005) used 20.5 participants in each group on average, which is comparable to the current study. Furthermore, the counting accuracy data showed that participants’ tracking performance was substantially poorer in the flash condition than the control condition, independent-samples t(70) = 2.45, B10 = 3.07, and that performance in the switch condition was statistically equivalent to that in the control condition (B10 = 1/4.18), indicating that the switch and flash conditions were no easier than the control condition. Nonetheless, future research may reduce noticing rates by making the unexpected object smaller and move faster.

Acknowledgements

Thanks are due to Andrei Gorea, two anonymous reviewers, and Noriyasu Li for helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript.

Author Biographies

graphic file with name 10.1177_2041669518754595-img1.jpg

Dakota B. Palmer is an undergraduate student pursuing a dual B.S. in Biology and Psychology at Old Dominion University. He is a research assistant in the Applied Cognitive Performance Lab and the Cellular Mechanobiology Lab at Old Dominion University. His research interests span basic and translational neuroscience.

graphic file with name 10.1177_2041669518754595-img2.jpg

Yusuke Yamani is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Psychology and the director of the Applied Cognitive Performance Lab at Old Dominion University. He earned his Ph.D. in Psychology (Visual Cognition and Human Performance) at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign in 2013. He was a post-doctoral fellow in the Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering at University of Massachusetts at Amherst and Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety. His research interests lie in the domain of basic and applied experimental psychology including studies of visual perception and attention.

graphic file with name 10.1177_2041669518754595-img3.jpg

Taylor L. Bobrow received his B.S. in Electrical Engineering with a focus in biomedical applications from Old Dominion University in 2017. He is currently a PhD student in Biomedical Engineering at the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine. His research interests are imaging hardware development, 3D imaging with structured light, and computer-aided detection.

graphic file with name 10.1177_2041669518754595-img4.jpg

Nicole D. Karpinsky earned her Bachelor’s degree in Psychology at Westminster College in 2014 and Master’s degree in Applied Experimental Psychology at Old Dominion University in 2016. She is a 4th-year doctoral student in Human Factors Psychology at Old Dominion University. Her research interests include human-automation interaction and trust.

graphic file with name 10.1177_2041669518754595-img5.jpg

Dean J. Krusienski is a Professor of Electrical and Computer Engineering at Old Dominion University (ODU), Norfolk, VA, USA, where he directs the Advanced Signal Processing in Engineering and Neuroscience (ASPEN) Lab. He is also the Graduate Program Director and a Founding Member of the Biomedical Engineering Program at ODU. He received the B.S., M.S., and Ph.D. degrees in Electrical Engineering from The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA. He conducted the postdoctoral research in the Brain-Computer Interface Laboratory, Wadsworth Center of the New York State Department of Health. His research interests include biomedical signal processing, pattern recognition, brain-computer interfaces, and neural engineering.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was partly supported by the National Institute of Aeronautics (NIA Activity No. 2A57) awarded to Dean Krusienski.

References

  1. Chabris, C. F., Weinberger, A., Fontaine, M., & Simons, D. J. (2011). You do not talk about Fight Club if you do not notice Fight Club: Inattentional blindness for a simulated real-world assault. i-Perception, 2(2), 150–153. doi: 10.1068/i0436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  2. Drew T., Võ M. L.-H., Wolfe J. M. (2013) The invisible gorilla strikes again: Sustained inattentional blindness in expert observers. Psychological Science 24: 1848–1853. doi: 10.1177/0956797613479386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Fischer, E., Haines, R. F., & Price, T. A. (1980). Cognitive issues in head-up displays (NASA Technical Paper 1711). Moffett Field, CA: National Aeronautics and Space Administration, Ames Research Center.
  4. Jeffreys H. (1961) The theory of probability, New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Mack A., Rock I. (1998) Inattentional blindness, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. Memmert D. (2006) The effects of eye movements, age, and expertise on inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition 15: 620–627. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Morey, R. D., Rouder, J. N., & Jamil, X. (2015). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs (Version 0.9.11). Retrieved from http://bayesfactorpcl.r-forge.r-project.org.
  8. Most S. B., Scholl B. J., Clifford E. R., Simons D. J. (2005) What you see is what you set: Sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. Psychological Review 112: 217–242. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Most S. B., Simons D. J., Scholl B. J., Jimenez R., Clifford E., Chabris C. F. (2001) How not to be seen: The contribution of similarity and selective ignoring to sustained inattentional blindness. Psychological Science 12: 9–17. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Potchen J. E. (2006) Measuring observer performance in chest radiology: Some experiences. Journal of the American College of Radiology 3: 423–432. doi: 10.1016/j.jacr.2006.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Rouder J. N., Speckman P. L., Sun D., Morey R. D. (2009) Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 16: 225–237. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Simons D. J. (2000) Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 147–155. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01455-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Simons D. J. (2010) Monkeying around with the gorillas in our midst: Familiarity with an inattentional-blindness task does not improve the detection of unexpected events. i-Perception 1: 3–6. doi: 10.1068/i0386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Simons D. J., Chabris C. F. (1999) Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28: 1059–1074. doi: 10.1068/p281059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Wetzels R., Matzke D., Lee M. D., Rouder J. N., Iverson G. J., Wagenmakers E. J. (2011) Statistical evidence in experimental psychology an empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6: 291–298. doi: 10.1177/1745691611406923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from i-Perception are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES