Abstract
This study explored the relationship between family functioning and trait emotional intelligence among 547 respondents, between the age of 16 and 24 years from Malaysia, Iran, China, Sudan, Somalia, Morocco, the United Kingdom, Germany and the Netherlands. The questionnaires were Family Adaptability and Cohesion Evaluation Scale III and Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire Short Form. Pearson correlation analysis revealed a significant relationship between family functioning and trait emotional intelligence. The higher the family functioning, the higher the trait emotional intelligence among youths. The findings provide a deeper understanding in the field of family functioning and trait emotional intelligence and have implications for parents, administrators and child relationships dealing with trait emotional intelligence.
Keywords: family adaptability, family cohesion, family functioning, trait emotional intelligence, youth
Introduction
Developmental theories describe adolescence as the period of growth when identity formation is addressed (Klimstra et al., 2010; Muuss, 1996). In other words, a lesser role is perceived for family in this period, as during adolescence, family plays a trivial role in young people’s lives. The youth gain physical, emotional and cognitive independence during and after adolescence while becoming more independent and continuing to grow at the same time (Muuss, 1996). However, they still need to maintain a balance in their home environment as well as an impregnable affecting base to delve into the world and attain more experience (Cavendish et al., 2014; Hong and Park, 2012). Such experience gives the adolescents the chance to return to the family and home when they need backing, unequivocal devotion and appreciation in the challenging times of their lives. Young individuals enhance their experience utilising their knowledge of esteem, consideration, unity, concurrence, affable and changeability while interacting with home conventions and family relationships (Galvin et al., 2015). These individuals have the advantage of testing how they think, feel or behave in the family environment, as well as examining and learning from relationships within their own and other families. Hence, it is natural that they fail sometimes, and the experience will teach them how to analyse the experience to help them handle their problems.
Furthermore, the constantly positive family relations and links are constructive factors against some health risk behaviours (Chen et al., 2017). Despite the continuously developing nature of relationships, the continuity of family connections and a secure emotional base is crucial for the positive development of young people which requires the ongoing family connections and a constructive emotional basis (Chandran and Nair, 2015).
People with higher emotional intelligence (EI) encounter less depression, are healthier and can build up better connections (Gugliandolo et al., 2015b). The EI is a type of social intelligence that enables the individuals to control feelings and emotions, distinguish them and utilise this information to mentor their thoughts and deeds (Salovey and Mayer, 1990). In other words, EI is the key element of ‘social maturity’ that enhances adolescents’ psychological health (Lau and Wu, 2012).
According to Petrides et al. (2007), Trait EI, or emotional self-efficacy, measures an individual’s belief in their emotional abilities. It is defined as ‘a constellation of emotional self-perceptions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies’ (p. 287). Trait EI consists of four indicators: emotionality, sociability, self-control and well-being. Emotionality refers to emotional expression, trait empathy, emotion perception and the quality of relationships. Sociability refers to emotion management, or ability to affect other people, assertiveness and social awareness. Self-control is impulsiveness and stress management. Well-being is described as optimism, self-esteem beliefs and trait happiness. These factors provide implications for the crucial life domains, such as health (Costa et al., 2014), parenting (Gugliandolo et al., 2015a), mental well-being (Andrei and Petrides, 2013), university performance (Sanchez-Ruiz et al., 2013), academic achievement in school (Azimifar, 2013), leadership and career decision-making (Di Fabio and Saklofske, 2014) and pro-social behaviour and self-management skills in early adolescent (Bar-On and Parker, 2000).
Family communication plays a key role in building constructive family relations. Family communication is the way verbal and nonverbal information is interchanged between family members to share needs and concerns, and find solutions for problems (Peterson and Green, 2009). As illustrated in the three-dimensional model of marital and family systems, a family has quality communication skills (communication dimension), the members of the family are more likely to be close (cohesion dimension) and they can resolve problems accordingly (adaptability dimension) (Olson and DeFrain, 2000; Olson et al., 1983). In addition, a good family communication is a key element of family functioning, and an indicator of children’s emotional and social progress (Manuel, 2002). Healthy family functioning is facilitated through open communication by forming positive change context, insight and development. Without proper communication, the family would have a very limited flexibility to change when it is necessary (Olson and DeFrain, 2000; Olson et al., 1983).
In this regard, family environment has an impact on children’s emotional base (Alegre, 2012; Alegre and Benson, 2010; Argyriou et al., 2016; Bhatia, 2012; Chandran and Nair, 2015; Ozbaci, 2006). Impact is observed in the emotional and social competencies when family relationships become secure and smooth (Chandran and Nair, 2015). Furthermore, there is a relationship between parenting and children’s trait EI (Alegre, 2012; Alegre and Benson, 2010; Gottman et al., 1997; Liau et al., 2003). The amount of time mothers spend with their children and the quality of their interactions are important factors in forming children’s trait EI. The more joint activity times represent better parenting, and the promotion of parent modelling, reinforcement, shared attention and social cooperation (Alegre, 2012). Consequently, parent–child interaction leads to less emotional and behavioural problems (Galboda Liyanage et al., 2003; Gardner et al., 2003; Gugliandolo et al., 2015b), better academic achievement (Mingyue et al., 2001) and adolescents’ mental health (Cousins et al., 2007). Therefore, parental psychological control predicts trait EI, which mutually affects behaviour problems in adolescent (Gugliandolo et al., 2015a). Argyriou et al. (2016) found a link between parenting styles and trait EI in adolescent, linking authoritarianism to a lower level of trait EI that may hinder the progress of trait EI. At factor level of trait EI, self-control predicts externalising problems, while well-being predicts internalising problems across all rating sources (Gugliandolo et al., 2015b). Emotionality and sociability distinctively predict problem behaviours based on the rating source, where the direct impact of specific trait EI variables on behavioural and emotional problems is observed during adolescence.
Another important factor contributing to a firm and active base in the development and progress of youth emotional well-being and trait EI is family adaptability that consists of family flexibility and family cohesion. Family flexibility is quality and expression of leadership, role relationship and relationship rules and negotiations (Koutra et al., 2016). Family cohesion is the family connectedness and emotional bonding towards one another (Koutra et al., 2016). Dominant healthy relationship in family makes the youth feel secure and well-adjusted, conceiving that they are accepted by the family, therefore, in the future, they will continue to have the well-adjusted feeling that society accepts them (Bhatia, 2012). Another study about the relationship between adolescents and family connectedness revealed that connectedness between adolescents and their family is a key element in differentiating collective and individualistic cultures (Dwairy and Achoui, 2010).
Only few studies have addressed family adaptability and cohesion and youth trait EI. This is the stage when the issues of emotional separation from family appear (Dwairy and Achoui, 2010). The sense of separation is a necessary step in the development of personal values, the evolution to self-sufficiency forces. Therefore, this study looked into the influence of family adaptability and cohesion and trait EI among youths.
Methodology
The sample was collected randomly from the youth after obtaining the informed consent from all participants. Out of 457 youths from Malaysia, Iran, China, Sudan, Somalia, Morocco, the United Kingdom, Germany and the Netherlands, 284 were female and 173 were male. Respondents’ age was between 16 and 24 years. The summary of the respondents’ demographic data is illustrated in Table 1.
Table 1.
Demographic data | Frequency | |
---|---|---|
Gender | Female | 284 |
Male | 173 | |
Age (years) | 16–18 | 160 |
19–21 | 112 | |
22–24 | 185 | |
Nationality | Malaysian | 70 |
Iranian | 65 | |
Chinese | 45 | |
Sudanese | 58 | |
Somalian | 55 | |
Moroccan | 44 | |
British | 35 | |
German | 38 | |
Dutch | 47 |
The respondents completed measures of family functioning and trait EI. Family Adaptability and Cohesion Evaluation Scale III (FACES III) by Olson et al. (1985) was used to measure family functioning. FACES III has 20 items to assess family adaptability and cohesion. It assesses the degree to which family members are adaptive and attached to their family. There are four levels of family adaptability: very flexible, flexible, structured and rigid levels. The central levels (flexible and structured) are considered the balanced levels of family adaptability. The two extreme levels (very flexible and rigid) are considered the unbalanced levels of family adaptability. On the other hand, the four levels of family cohesion consist of very connected, connected, separated and disengaged levels. Similarly, family adaptability has two central levels (connected and separated) which are the balanced levels of family cohesion, while the two extreme levels (very connected and disengaged) are the unbalanced levels of family cohesion. For FACES III, respondents were asked to indicate the frequency of the described behaviour in their families on a Likert scale from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). The reliability was acceptable for each domain when tested in sample of youths (Table 2). Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue-SF) from Petrides (2009) was adopted to measure youth trait EI. The TEIQue-SF consists of 30 items to measure global trait EI and the four factors of well-being, self-control, emotionality and sociability, on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). The reliability for the global trait EI is illustrated in Table 2.
Table 2.
Variables | Subscales | No of items | Cronbach’s alpha |
---|---|---|---|
Family Functioning | FA | 10 | .847 |
FC | 10 | .776 | |
Trait Emotional Intelligence | Global TEI | 30 | .711 |
FA: Family Adaptability; FC: Family Cohesion; TEI: Trait Emotional Intelligence.
The SPSS was used for the data analysis. A bivariate analysis was processed using Pearson’s correlation to measure the inter-correlations of family functioning and trait EI. As illustrated in Table 3, the family functioning components of family adaptability and family cohesion with trait EI were significantly correlated for all nationalities. The results indicated that higher family adaptability and cohesion in youths correlate positively with their trait EI.
Table 3.
Nationality | Variables (corresponding score) | Global TEI |
|
---|---|---|---|
r | Sig. (two-tailed) | ||
Malaysian | FC (5) | .431*** | .000 |
FA (6) | .301* | .040 | |
Iranian | FC (6) | .501** | .011 |
FA (3) | .229* | .035 | |
Chinese | FC (3) | .279* | .032 |
FA (3) | .611* | .048 | |
Sudanese | FC (7) | .428* | .040 |
FA (2) | .388* | .050 | |
Somalian | FC (7) | .165** | .001 |
FA (3) | .192* | .023 | |
Moroccan | FC (5) | .328* | .050 |
FA (3) | .043** | .015 | |
British | FC (3) | .042* | .045 |
FA (3) | .248* | .029 | |
German | FC (3) | .216* | .036 |
FA (4) | .428* | .030 | |
Dutch | FC (3) | .246* | .050 |
FA (6) | .513*** | .000 |
TEI: Trait Emotional Intelligence; FA: Family Adaptability; FC: Family Cohesion.
Corresponding score interpretation for FC: very connected (8–7), connected (6–5), separated (4–3) and disengaged (2–1). Corresponding score interpretation for FA: very flexible (8–7), flexible (6–5), structured (4–3) and rigid (2–1).
p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Discussion
Reviewing the limited studies conducted on trait EI, this study provides empirical evidence that family functioning significantly influences youth trait EI. Family is a key factor during all stages of an individual’s development. Providing an insight and strengthening family functioning patterns would be effective in enhancing an individual’s emotional development (Chandran and Nair, 2015). In the entire development stages, particularly since the beginning stage of adolescence, independence level from parents and dependence on peers and social interaction go up as adolescents grow older. Therefore, family adaptability (flexibility) and cohesion (connectedness) are effective in developing better trait EI at this age, where the youths and family healthy interactions result in trait EI (Alegre, 2012; Alegre and Benson, 2010; Argyriou et al., 2016; Bhatia, 2012; Chandran and Nair, 2015; Ozbaci, 2006). On the other hand, poor family adaptability and cohesion debilitates individual’s trait EI. Family functioning could be enhanced if the members of a family understand their and other people’s emotions and monitor their emotions (Ozbaci, 2006).
Implications from this study could be utilised by parents, children, policymakers and curriculum developers in forming a better understanding of having an effective relationship with children in the family. Emotional bonding and family’s ability to change its power structure will be passed down to subsequent relationships, for example, to one’s own children in their relationships with their kids. Therefore, parent awareness plays a key role and is advised to be considered more seriously by giving the priority to raising awareness in parents about the importance of adaptability and cohesion and children’s emotional well-being in families. One way is providing counsellors for families and for regular visits. Frequent visits and enhanced parent counsellor meetings lead to a better understanding about parents’ and child’s trait EI. Healthy family functioning could develop this clearer and deeper insight. Because of the importance of the role parents play in development of family members’ EI (Ozbaci, 2006), it could be concluded that if parents with low EI attend EI education programmes, improved results are obtainable in both family functioning and family members’ trait EI. However, the future studies should more investigate this. Finally, the ecological factors of a family, such as family size or demographic variables, including age and gender differences in the relationship between family functioning and trait EI, and discussion from a cross-country (variation) lens of view are recommended to be explored in future studies.
Footnotes
Declaration of conflicting interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.
References
- Alegre A. (2012) The relation between the time mothers and children spent together and the children’s trait emotional intelligence. Child & Youth Care Forum 41: 493–508. [Google Scholar]
- Alegre A, Benson MJ. (2010) Parental behaviors and adolescent adjustment: Mediation via adolescent trait emotional intelligence. Individual Differences Research 8: 83–96. [Google Scholar]
- Andrei F, Petrides K. (2013) Trait emotional intelligence and somatic complaints with reference to positive and negative mood. Psihologija 46: 5–15. [Google Scholar]
- Argyriou E, Bakoyannis G, Tantaros S. (2016) Parenting styles and trait emotional intelligence in adolescence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 57: 42–49. [Google Scholar]
- Azimifar M. (2013) The relationship between emotional intelligence and academic achievement among Iranian students in elementary schools. European Online Journal of Natural and Social Sciences 2: 216–222. [Google Scholar]
- Bar-On R, Parker JDA. (2000) Baron Emotional Quotient Inventory: Youth Version. Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health system, Incorporated. [Google Scholar]
- Bhatia G. (2012) A study of family relationship in relation to emotional intelligence of the students of secondary level. International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications 2: 1–5. [Google Scholar]
- Cavendish W, Montague M, Enders C, et al. (2014) Mothers and adolescents perceptions of family environment and adolescent social-emotional functioning. Journal of Child and Family Studies 23: 52–66. [Google Scholar]
- Chandran A, Nair BP. (2015) Family climate as a predictor of emotional intelligence in adolescents. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology 41: 167–173. [Google Scholar]
- Chen E, Brody GH, Miller GE. (2017) Childhood close family relationships and health. American Psychologist 72: 555–566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa S, Petrides K, Tillmann T. (2014) Trait emotional intelligence and inflammatory diseases. Psychology, Health & Medicine 19: 180–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cousins JC, Bootzin RR, Stevens SJ, et al. (2007) Parental involvement, psychological distress, and sleep: A preliminary examination in sleep-disturbed adolescents with a history of substance abuse. Journal of Family Psychology 21: 104–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Fabio A, Saklofske DH. (2014) Comparing ability and self-report trait emotional intelligence, fluid intelligence, and personality traits in career decision. Personality and Individual Differences 64: 174–178. [Google Scholar]
- Dwairy M, Achoui M. (2010) Adolescents-family connectedness: A first cross-cultural research on parenting and psychological adjustment of children. Journal of Child and Family Studies 19: 8–15. [Google Scholar]
- Galboda Liyanage KC, Prince MJ, Scott S. (2003) Mother–child joint activity and behaviour problems of pre-school children. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44: 1037–1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galvin KM, Braithwaite DO, Bylund CL. (2015) Family Communication: Cohesion and Change (Subscription). Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Gardner F, Ward S, Burton J, et al. (2003) The role of mother–child joint play in the early development of children’s conduct problems: A longitudinal observational study. Social Development 12: 361–378. [Google Scholar]
- Gottman JM, Katz LF, Hooven C. (1997) Meta-Emotion: How Families Communicate Emotionally. Abingdon: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gugliandolo MC, Costa S, Cuzzocrea F, et al. (2015. a) Trait emotional intelligence as mediator between psychological control and behaviour problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies 24: 2290–2300. [Google Scholar]
- Gugliandolo MC, Costa S, Cuzzocrea F, et al. (2015. b) Trait emotional intelligence and behavioral problems among adolescents: A cross-informant design. Personality and Individual Differences 74: 16–21. [Google Scholar]
- Hong YR, Park JS. (2012) Impact of attachment, temperament and parenting on human development. Korean Journal of Pediatrics 55: 449–454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimstra TA, Hale Iii WW, Raaijmakers QAW, et al. (2010) Identity formation in adolescence: Change or stability? Journal of Youth and Adolescence 39: 150–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koutra K, Simos P, Triliva S, et al. (2016) Linking family cohesion and flexibility with expressed emotion, family burden and psychological distress in caregivers of patients with psychosis: A path analytic model. Psychiatry Research 240: 66–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau PSY, Wu FKY. (2012) Emotional competence as a positive youth development construct: A conceptual review. The Scientific World Journal 2012: 975189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liau AK, Liau AW, Teoh GB, et al. (2003) The case for emotional literacy: The influence of emotional intelligence on problem behaviours in Malaysian secondary school students. Journal of Moral Education 32: 51–66. [Google Scholar]
- Manuel M. (2002) Parental inducement of emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, Personality 18: 3–23. [Google Scholar]
- Mingyue S, Rengang L, Jian W. (2001) Controlled study on behavior problems, family factors and parental rearing styles in students with poor academic achievement. Chinese Mental Health Journal 15: 391–393. [Google Scholar]
- Muuss RE. (1996) Theories of Adolescence (6th edn). New York, NY: McGraw Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Olson D, Portner J, Lavee Y. (1985) FACES III (Family Adaptation and Cohesion Scales). St. Paul, MN: University of Minnesota. [Google Scholar]
- Olson DH, DeFrain J. (2000) Marriage and the Family: Diversity and Strengths. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield. [Google Scholar]
- Olson DH, Russell CS, Sprenkle DH. (1983) Circumplex model of marital and family systems: VI. Theoretical update. Family Process 22: 69–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozbaci N. (2006) Emotional intelligence and family environment. Sosyal Bilimler Dergisi 16: 169–175. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson R, Green S. (2009) Families first-keys to successful family functioning: Communication. Virginia Tech: Virginia State University. [Google Scholar]
- Petrides KV. (2009) Psychometric properties of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue). In: Parker J, Saklofske D, Stough C. (eds) Assessing Emotional Intelligence. Boston, MA: Springer, pp. 85–101. [Google Scholar]
- Petrides KV, Pita R, Kokkinaki F. (2007) The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology 98: 273–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salovey P, Mayer JD. (1990) Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 9: 185–211. [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Ruiz M-J, Mavroveli S, Poullis J. (2013) Trait emotional intelligence and its links to university performance: An examination. Personality and Individual Differences 54: 658–662. [Google Scholar]