Abstract
Animals that experience situations likely to induce negative emotions show changes in judgment associated with pessimism. Few studies have focused on whether animals express stable differences in pessimism and whether these differences are related to personality traits. The first aim of this study was to explore if dairy calves are consistent over time in making judgments under ambiguous situations. Our second aim was to determine whether individual differences in judgment bias are related to conventional personality traits assessed using four standardized tests (Open field, Novel object, Human reactivity and Social motivation test). We subjected animals to two sessions of judgment bias and personality trait tests at 25 and 50 d of age. Individual differences in judgment bias were consistent over time with some animals persistently making more pessimistic choices compared to others. Two main dimensions of personality (Fearfulness and Sociability), obtained through principal component analysis, were also highly consistent over time. Pessimism was related to fearfulness, with more fearful calves making more pessimistic judgments. We conclude that dairy calves differ in the way they perceive and react to ambiguity and that this relates to individual differences in fearfulness.
Introduction
It is now well recognized that emotional states can induce attention, memory and judgment biases1. These affect-induced cognitive changes have been observed in many species2. Much interest has focused on the use of judgment biases, assessed by exposing animals to ambiguous situations. Response to ambiguity is believed to reflect pessimism or optimism, defined as an increased propensity to form negative or positive expectations, respectively3.
Cognitive biases have been extensively studied in relation to animal welfare4. For instance, following an experience predicted to elicit negative emotions such as fear5, pain6 or chronic stress7, farm animals showed a negative bias associated with pessimism. In contrast, subjecting farm animals to environmental enrichment reduced pessimistic judgments, and in some cases caused positive judgment biases8,9.
Judgment bias is most often considered as a state that is subjected to change according to the mood of the animal, a consequence of the accumulation of shorter-term emotional responses10, but there is a growing body of evidence suggesting that animals express stable individual differences in this response11. Pessimism and optimism are considered traits in humans12, but to our knowledge the consistency of individual differences in pessimism has received little attention in non-human animals other than rats13. More pessimistic rats were found to express higher vulnerability to stress-induced anhedonia13, to be more sensitive to negative feedbacks14, and to express an inflammatory immune profile compared to optimistic animals15, suggesting that trait pessimism might favor the development of depressive states. Recently, a study on dolphins has shown that individuals differ consistently over time in pessimism and that more pessimistic animals show fewer affiliative behaviors16. Although pessimism and optimism in humans are related to some of the ‘big five’ personality traits17 such as neuroticism and extraversion18, to date only one study has linked stable individual characteristics to differences in judgment bias in non-human animals, in this case work on pigs19.
Different terms are used to describe individual variation in animals, including personality traits20,21, temperament22, behavioral syndrome23 and coping styles24. In the current paper, we use the term trait, but regardless, the underlying idea is that behaviors show consistency over time and across contexts. In the case of farm animals, much of the initial work focused on differences in fearfulness or emotional reactivity25–27 defined as a “general susceptibility to react to a variety of potentially threatening situations”28. This work often addressed how fearfulness affects animal welfare29 and productivity30,31. Recent studies have found consistent differences in other traits such as sociability32,33 and aggressiveness34, but few studies have examined how individual characteristics may explain the variation in cognitive processes such as judgment biases.
The aims of this study were to explore whether dairy calves: express consistent differences in the way they perceive and react to ambiguous situations (i.e. judgment bias) and whether these differences are associated with more conventionally assessed personality traits. We predicted that calves would consistently differ in the way they reacted to ambiguous situations and that more fearful animals would make more pessimistic judgments.
Methods
The study was approved by the University of British Columbia’s Animal Care Committee (# A15-0117) and cared for according to the guidelines outlined by the Canadian Council of Animal Care (2009)35.
Animals
Twenty-two females Holstein calves were enrolled. Within 6 h of birth calves (BW 37.8 ± 4.4 kg) were separated from their dam and fed at least 4 L of >50 g/L of IgG colostrum. Calves were kept in individual pens until 5 d of age. On d 6 calves were moved to a 35 m2 group pen (n = 9 ± 1 calves/group). Calves had access to 12 L/d of whole pasteurized milk and ad libitum access to water, hay and grain. Fresh sawdust was added weekly to the group pen. Animals were kept with the same pen-mates for the entire experiment.
General procedure
The spatial learning task required for the Judgment Bias Test was completed first (described below). Training required approximately 12 d. Calves were tested for two consecutive days starting at d 25 and again at d 50.
Personality traits were characterized using four tests applied once per day starting at d 30 (Session 1) and then repeated on d 53 (Session 2). The order of the tests was: Open field (OF), Novel Object Test (NOT), Human’s Reactivity test (HRT), and lastly, the Social Motivation test (SMT) which was divided into two parts: 1) a social isolation and, 2) a runway test.
Judgment bias test
We used a spatial learning task adapted from Destrez et al.5. Groups of 2 to 3 calves were familiarized with the apparatus (see Fig. 1) for 10 min 1 d before the training phase began but all animals were trained alone. Five consecutive training trials took place each day. During this first 3 d of training, animals were only trained to associate one side of the apparatus (alternately assigned to calves) with a reward (i.e. milk). All animals successfully found the milk holder in less than 30s by the third day of training. Training then introduced negative events: an unrewarded (empty bottle) placed on the opposite side from where the calves had previously received a reward. To facilitate training of the negative event animals that touched the empty bottle received an air-puff in the face as a mild punishment5,7. Positive and negative events occurred alternatively during the 5 daily training trials. The last trial consisted of only rewarded events to avoid any negative association with the testing area. On each entry into the testing area, calves were allowed 30 s to approach and touch the milk holder before the response was deemed a ‘no-go’. For calves to be considered as trained they had to approach all positive milk holders (‘go’ response) and never approach the negative milk holder (‘no-go’ response) 10 consecutive times (i.e. 5 trials per day for 2 consecutive days). All animals were trained for 10 d, regardless of when they reached the learning criteria.
Once calves were trained they were tested by placing an empty milk bottle at one of three intermediate (ambiguous) locations: near positive (0.75 m from the positive location), middle (located directly half way (1.5 m) from the positive and negative locations) and near negative (0.75 m from the negative location). On day 1 of testing, animals were exposed first to the positive location (rewarded with milk) and then to the negative one (punished with the blower). Animals were then exposed to ambiguous locations one at a time, beginning with the near positive, followed by the near negative and finishing with the middle location. On day 2 of testing the order was changed to Negative, Positive, Middle, Near negative and Near positive. To ensure that calves remembered the task, we provided two additional days of training on the days before the second test session.
Personality tests
Except the social motivation test, all tests took place in the same experimental area (a 25 m2 test pen) and in all cases animals were tested individually. Animals were gently directed inside the test pen. The 10-min test began once the gate was closed. Behaviors were recorded using a camera (WV-CW504SP, Panasonic, Osaka, Japan) positioned 7 m above the pen. Ethovision XT 10 (Noldus, Netherlands) and BORIS36, version 2.05 (www.http://penelope.unito.it/boris) were used for the behavioral analysis.
For the open field test animals entered the experimental pen that was empty and unfamiliar37. Distance covered within the pen, time spent exploring and number of vocalizations were recorded. The novel object test involved placing a novel object (a black empty 50 L plastic bucket) in the center of the experimental pen. Latencies to contact the object, duration of contacts, time spent in proximity of the object, total distance covered and number of vocalizations were recorded. The human reactivity test involved an unfamiliar human standing immobile in the middle of the experimental pen. We recorded the latency to contact the human, duration of contacts, time spent at proximity, distance covered and number of vocalizations38. The social motivation test is a variant of the runway test used to assess social motivation of dairy cows32. This test was done in a familiar alley (25 m × 5 m) that was divided into two distinct spaces (i.e. a holding area and the alleyway) separated by a fence that did not prevent visual contact. For this test, the entire group of calves was moved to the holding pen and given 10 minutes to habituate. One calf was then randomly selected at a time (without replacement) and gently moved to the other end of the alleyway where she was placed in a start box (150 cm × 50 cm) for 5 min. Visual contact with the herd was maintained for the duration of the test. The number of vocalizations was recorded for 5 min, after which the calf was allowed to leave the start box. The time taken to return to within 5 m of the gate separating the rest of the group was measured.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were made using R version 3.2.139. Inter-observer reliability was evaluated using the intra-class-correlation coefficient (ICC, package ICC) applied to a random selection of 20 videos assessed by two observers. Only duration of exploration of the test environment and duration of contact with the object or the human were measured from video. Observers showed good agreement for both behaviors (duration of exploration: ICC = 0.78, CI: 0.58–0.98; duration of contact: ICC = 0,79, CI: 0.55–1,03). One animal was excluded from the study due to an extended period of illness.
Judgment biases
A linear mixed-effects model (LMM) was calculated with the R package lme440 and P values were extracted by Wald Chi-square tests (type III). The model included the latency to reach the bottle (as the response variable), and tested the effects of distance (in meters) from the rewarded side and session as fixed effects, as well as the interaction between distance and session. Calf was included as a random effect. Model residuals were scrutinized for outliers and normality.
To assess consistency over time, latency to reach each ambiguous location was averaged over the two days. To correct for activity differences, we subtracted the time needed to reach the rewarded bottle from the average latency to reach all ambiguous locations. Measures of latency from all three ambiguous locations were then averaged to get one overall measure of pessimism by session; this was then used to perform linear regression analyses by permutation to test consistency over time (package lmPerm41).
Personality tests
We used averages of the same behaviors expressed in the four different personality tests for each session (e.g. vocalizations in the OF, NO, HRT and SMT were averaged to give only one value per calf per session: Average vocalizations). Only the latency to reach the herd was not averaged with other latencies (latency to contact the object and the human) as the former was used as a proxy for social motivation; the other latencies were used as proxies for fearfulness. A principal component analysis (PCA: package FactoMineR42) that summarized correlated variables into principal components, was undertaken using the averages of behaviors expressed in Session 1. Then, to calculate individual PC coordinates for the second session, we used the function predict to perform another PCA on the same space created for the Session 1. This allowed us to obtain comparable PCs for both sessions.
Relationship between pessimism and personality traits
Measures from Session 1 and 2 were averaged to give an average value for each behavior and a new PCA was performed. Measures included in the PCA were: average number of vocalizations, average distance covered, average time spent in exploration, average latency to contact the object and the human, average time spent at proximity of the object and the human, average time spent in contact with the object and the human and the average latency to reach the herd. Individual coordinates on each of the two principal components were used for further analyses.
Similarly, we averaged the measures of judgment biases arising from Session 1 and 2 (average of latencies to reach all ambiguous locations) to get one individual measure of judgment bias per calf.
The relationship between personality dimensions (individual coordinates in the two components of the PCA) and individual levels of pessimism (mean value of both sessions) was assessed using permutation tests for regression.
Results
Judgment biases: Response to ambiguous locations
As expected, the latency to reach the ambiguous bottles was affected by the distance from the reward (Linear regression: Chisq = 192.6, P < 0.001), with calves taking longer to approach bottles positioned further from the positive training stimuli (Fig. 2). We found no difference in latency between sessions (Chisq = 0.78, P = 0.38). We also found no evidence of an interaction between session and distance (Chisq = 1.7, P = 0.19), suggesting that repetition did not affect the way calves responded to the different locations.
To assess consistency in judgment bias over time, we averaged the latencies to reach all ambiguous locations for each session. This average latency to reach the three ambiguous locations was consistent over time (Linear regression by permutation: R2 = 0.41 β = 0.57, P = 0.002, see Fig. 3).
Personality traits
Averages of similar behaviors expressed in the four different tests (Open Field, Novel Object, Human Reactivity, Social Motivation) were calculated and then used for further analysis. The Principal Component Analysis (PCA) performed for Session 1 revealed two principal components with eigenvalues ≥1. The first component explained 42.6% of the variation and the second component explained 25.6%. The first principal component was primarily explained by the average latency to contact (r = 0.91), the average time spent at proximity (r = −0.94) and the average time spent in contact with the human and the novel object (r = −0.86). The second component was mainly explained by the average number of vocalizations (r = 0.82) and by the latency to reach the herd during the social motivation test (r = −0.66). According to the variable loadings, we labeled the first dimension “Fearfulness” and the second “Sociability”. PCA coordinates have been calculated using the same space for Session 2 and individual PCA coordinates were used to assess the relationship across the two test sessions: Fearfulness (Linear regression by permutation: R2 = 0.70, β = 0.80, P < 0.0001; Fig. 4a) and Sociability (R2 = 0. 53, β = 0.73, P < 0.001; Fig. 4b) were both consistent across time.
Relationship between personality traits and individual measures of judgment bias
A new PCA was performed using averages of the behaviors performed during the personality tests across Session 1 and 2. Individual coordinates on the two first dimensions (with eigenvalues >1) were used to assess the relationship with judgment biases. According to the variable loadings, we again termed these “Fearfulness” and “Sociability” (see Table 1). Fearfulness showed a positive relationship with the average latency to reach the ambiguous locations (Linear regression by permutation: R2 = 0.24, β = 1.21, P = 0.023; Fig. 5), indicating that animals that made more pessimistic judgments were also the most fearful (i.e. more reluctant to make contact with the novel object and the human). There was no relationship between sociability and judgment bias (R2 = 0.06, P = 0.29).
Table 1.
Variables | Dimension 1 | Dimension 2 |
---|---|---|
Average_Vocalizations | −0.05 | 0.79 |
Average_Latency (NO + HR) | 0.87 | 0.18 |
Average_Latency to reach the herd (ST) | −0.12 | −0.76 |
Average_Exploration | 0.62 | −0.63 |
Average_Contact | −0.90 | −0.22 |
Average_Distance | −0.49 | 0.19 |
Average_Proximity | −0.97 | −0.08 |
Eigenvalue | 3.14 | 1.72 |
Variance explained (%) | 44.8 | 24.6 |
Cumulative variance explained (%) | 44.8 | 69.4 |
Discussion
The results of this study demonstrate that dairy calves are individually consistent in judgments when confronted with ambiguous situations, and that these individual differences are related to fearfulness as assessed using more conventional personality tests. Two main personality traits emerged from our analysis: Fearfulness and Sociability. Both were consistent over time, but we found no evidence that judgment bias was related to sociability.
Spatial learning tasks have been used to assess judgment bias in animals, but to the best of our knowledge this is the first study to use spatial learning to explore judgment bias in calves. Like many recent studies5,7,10, our results show that the latency to explore the probes increased when the distance between the probe and the rewarded side increased. Contrary to some studies7,43 we found no evidences of habituation across multiple tests, but in the current study calves were exposed to each probe just twice within each session.
Our results indicate that calves differ in their baseline levels of pessimism and optimism, and do so in a consistent way over a period of 3 weeks. To our knowledge, this is the first time that consistency in judgment bias over time has been demonstrated in farm animals. As recently discussed by Faustino et al.11, pessimism could be considered as both a state (i.e. changes according to the emotional context) and as a trait with some animals persistently judging ambiguous situations as potentially “negative” or “positive”. Judgment biases occur when humans and animals are subjected to mood changes1. Stability in individual levels of judgment bias (referred to here as the trait Pessimism) could be explained by relative time stability in mood state.
These findings have implications for animal welfare, as more pessimistic animals may fail to seize opportunities that are commonly assumed to provide good welfare (e.g. environmental enrichment). For instance, work on rodents has shown that pessimistic rats were less motivated to gain access to a reward44. Most importantly, animals that are inherently pessimistic may struggle when faced with emotional challenges. As demonstrated in rats13 and as well established in humans45, trait pessimism increases vulnerability to stressful events and is thought to contribute to the development of depression. In contrast, animals that are more optimistic may suffer when situations do not lead to reward. For instance, optimistic animals might be more sensitive to discrepancies in expectations and thus may be easily frustrated. This argument has been used to explain the link between optimism and loss of immunity when individuals were subjected to persistent uncontrollable situations46. Moreover, this variation may explain the inconsistent results found in some cognitive bias studies in animals47, and suggests that individual variation should be taken into account when possible.
According to Roelofs et al.48, an important validation of judgment bias tests in animals is the existence of correlations between judgment bias scores and personality traits. In the current study, calves that made more pessimistic judgments were also more fearful (i.e. more reluctant to contact the novel object and the unfamiliar human). Pessimistic people tend to engage in avoidance coping strategies when faced with challenging situations45 and similar results were recently obtained in pigs19; animals showing more pessimistic judgments after environmental change were the less proactive ones (i.e. they made fewer contacts with novel objects and were more distressed by social isolation). In addition, rats that were less anxious in unconditioned fear tests (i.e. open-field and elevated plus maze tests) made more optimistic judgments49, suggesting a link between emotional reactivity and judgment bias. Collectively these results suggest that individual differences in fearfulness modulate mood-related cognition in animals as is the case in humans where trait pessimism is positively associated with neuroticism and negatively to extraversion12.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate individual variation in animals using averages of similar behaviors expressed in four different tests rather than a single measure arising from one test. Recent work in the human personality literature makes use of aggregated measures across context to better represent personality traits and reports high consistency over time50–52. Animal studies investigating personality are often based on behaviors expressed in different situations but with similar causes. For instance, the different tests used in this study have been used to elicit fear responses in farm animals37, and tests exposed animals to different forms of social deprivation. Therefore, we expected fearfulness and sociability to be the two main traits explaining individual differences in these tests. Please note that we only aggregated behaviors that we had predicted were similarly motivated. For example, given that different underlying motivations are described with regards to the latency to reach the herd (reflecting sociability; see32) and the latency to contact the novel object and the human (reflecting fearfulness; see37), we did not average these variables. Although, many previous studies have used PCA53,54 and found consistent traits, the use of behavioral averages appears to increase the ability to capture individual differences across different contexts as suggested in the human psychology literature55. We encourage future studies to use averaged behaviors in conjunction with PCA.
Our results provide support for the idea that animal personality is multidimensional. Koolhaas and Van Reenen29 recently argued that personality traits in animals could be described using two (Fearfulness and Sociability) or three (Fearfulness, Coping styles and Sociability) dimensions. Our study confirms the existence of at least two personality dimensions in dairy calves. Our results did not show clear binomial responses generally associated with coping styles24, hence we used the term Fearfulness and Sociability to describe the individual traits in this study. As expected from previous studies53,56, latency to contact the human and the novel object and time spent in proximity or in contact accounted for the greatest proportion of variation. The novel object and human reactivity test have been frequently used to assess fearfulness of farm animals38,57,58 and usually trigger consistent behavioral and physiological response associated with fear56,59. These tests have long been considered reliable measures of fearfulness in farm animals28,37,38,60.
Vocalizations have been used as a measure of distress and fear in farm animals61,62. However, our results illustrate the social influence on vocal behaviors, given that the number of vocalizations loaded on the second dimension of the PCA, along with the latency to reach the herd after isolation (considered the gold standard to assess social motivation in farm animals32). This result agrees with previous work indicating that number of vocal responses of dairy cattle are independent of fearfulness when observed during standardized tests56. Similar results have also been reported in sheep63 and horses33 where vocalizations were related to the expression of social behaviors. These results add to the growing evidence showing stable individual differences in sociability in farm animals32,33,63,64.
Conclusion
Dairy calves show persistent differences in pessimism and in the more traditional personality traits of fearfulness and sociability. All three traits are consistent over time, and individual differences in pessimism are related to fearfulness.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a NSERC Discovery grant awarded to M.A.G.vK. The authors wish to thank Heather Neave for her great help at the beginning of the study and Joao Costa for his advice. We are grateful to Laura Whalin, Matheus Baylao, Julian Alberto Cortes Saavedra and Savannah Goldstein for their help during the experiments and to all the staff of the UBC Dairy Education and Research center.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiment (B.L., M.A.G.v.K., D.M.W.). Performed the experiment (B.L.). Statistical analyses (B.L.). Contributed to reagents, materials and analytical tools (M.A.G.v.K., D.M.W.). Contributed to the writing of the manuscript (B.L., M.A.G.v.K., D.M.W.).
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Change history
9/26/2018
A correction to this article has been published and is linked from the HTML and PDF versions of this paper. The error has not been fixed in the paper.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-017-17214-3.
References
- 1.Paul ES, Harding EJ, Mendl M. Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005;29:469–491. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mendl M, Burman OHP, Parker RMA, Paul ES. Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009;118:161–181. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Salmeto AL, et al. Cognitive bias in the chick anxiety-depression model. Brain Res. 2011;1373:124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Baciadonna L, McElligott AG. The use of judgement bias to assess welfare in farm livestock. Anim. Welf. 2015;24:81–91. doi: 10.7120/09627286.24.1.081. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Destrez A, Deiss V, Belzung C, Lee C, Boissy A. Does reduction of fearfulness tend to reduce pessimistic-like judgment in lambs? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012;139:233–241. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.04.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Neave HW, Daros RR, Costa JHC, von Keyserlingk MAG, Weary DM. Pain and pessimism: Dairy calves exhibit negative judgement bias following hot-iron disbudding. PLoS One. 2013;8:e80556. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Destrez A, et al. Chronic stress induces pessimistic-like judgment and learning deficits in sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013;148:28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.07.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Destrez A, Deiss V, Leterrier C, Calandreau L, Boissy A. Repeated exposure to positive events induces optimistic-like judgment and enhances fearfulness in chronically stressed sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014;154:30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.01.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Burman O, et al. Using judgement bias to measure positive affective state in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011;132:160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.04.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mendl M, et al. Dogs showing separation-related behaviour exhibit a ‘pessimistic’ cognitive bias. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:R839–R840. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Faustino AI, Oliveira GA, Oliveira RF. Linking appraisal to behavioral flexibility in animals: implications for stress research. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2015;9:104. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kam C, Meyer JP. Do optimism and pessimism have different relationships with personality dimensions? A re-examination. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2012;52:123–127. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.09.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rygula R, Papciak J, Popik P. Trait pessimism predicts vulnerability to stress-induced anhedonia in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:2188–96. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rygula R, Popik P. Trait ‘pessimism’ is associated with increased sensitivity to negative feedback in rats. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2016;16:516–526. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0410-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Curzytek, K. et al. The effects of pessimism on cell-mediated immunity in rats. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol. Psychiatry 0–1 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 16.Clegg ILK, Rödel HG, Delfour F. Bottlenose dolphins engaging in more social affiliative behaviour judge ambiguous cues more optimistically. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;322:115–122. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sharpe JP, Martin NR, Roth KA. Optimism and the Big Five factors of personality: Beyond neuroticism and extraversion. Personal. JPersonal. Individ. Differ. 2011;51:946–951. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.033. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Nettle D, Penke L. Personality: bridging the literatures from human psychology and behavioural ecology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2017;365:4043–4050. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Asher L, Friel M, Griffin K, Collins LM. Mood and personality interact to determine cognitive biases in pigs. Biol. Lett. 2016;12:20160402. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gosling SD, John OP. Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals a cross-species review. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1999;8:69–75. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bell AM, Hankison SJ, Laskowski KL. The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis. Anim. Behav. 2009;77:771–783. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol. Rev. 2007;82:291–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2004;19:372–378. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Koolhaas JM, et al. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1999;23:925–935. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.van Reenen CG, et al. Individual differences in behavioral and physiological responsiveness of primiparous dairy cows to machine milking. J. Dairy Sci. 2002;85:2551–2561. doi: 10.3168/jds.S0022-0302(02)74338-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.van Reenen CG, et al. Behavioural reactivity of heifer calves in potentially alarming test situations: a multivariate and correlational analysis. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004;85:11–30. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2003.09.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lansade L, Bouissou M-F, Erhard HW. Fearfulness in horses: A temperament trait stable across time and situations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008;115:182–200. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.06.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Boissy A, Bouissou MF. Assessment of individual differences in behavioural reactions of heifers exposed to various fear-eliciting situations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995;46:17–31. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00633-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Koolhaas JM, Van Reenen CG. Animal behavior and well-being symposium: Interaction between coping style/personality, stress, and welfare: Relevance for domestic farm animals. J. Anim. Sci. 2016;94:2284–2296. doi: 10.2527/jas.2015-0125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Müller R, von Keyserlingk MAG. Consistency of flight speed and its correlation to productivity and to personality in Bos taurus beef cattle. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006;99:193–204. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.05.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hemsworth PH, et al. The effects of cognitive behavioral intervention on the attitude and behavior of stockpersons and the behavior and productivity of commercial dairy cows. W. J. Anim. Sci. 2002;80:68–78. doi: 10.2527/2002.80168x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gibbons JM, Lawrence AB, Haskell MJ. Measuring sociability in dairy cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010;122:84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.11.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lansade L, Bouissou M-F, Erhard HW. Reactivity to isolation and association with conspecifics: A temperament trait stable across time and situations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008;109:355–373. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.03.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gibbons JM, Lawrence AB, Haskell MJ. Consistency of aggressive feeding behaviour in dairy cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009;121:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.CCAC. CCAC guidelines on the care and use of farm animals in research, teaching and testing. (2009).
- 36.Friard O, Gamba M, Fitzjohn R. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016;7:1325–1330. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12584. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Forkman B, Boissy A, Meunier-Salaun MC, Canali E, Jones RB. A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:340–374. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Waiblinger S, et al. Assessing the human-animal relationship in farmed species: A critical review. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006;101:185–242. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Team, T. R. C. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 1–3604 (2013).
- 40.Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R Packag. version 1–23 doi:citeulike-article-id:7112638 (2014).
- 41.Wheeler, R. E. lmPerm: Permutation tests for linear models. 24http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmPerm (2010).
- 42.Lê S, Josse J, Husson F. {{\tt FactoMineR}}: An {R} Package for Multivariate Analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 2008;25:1–18. doi: 10.18637/jss.v025.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Doyle RE, et al. The effect of repeated testing on judgement biases in sheep. Behav. Processes. 2010;83:349–352. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rygula, R., Golebiowska, J., Kregiel, J., Kubik, J. & Popik, P. Effects of optimism on motivation in rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 32 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Scheifer, C. S. & Carver, C. S. Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical overview and emperical update. Cognit. Ther. Res.16, 201–228 (1992).
- 46.Segerstrom SC. Optimism and immunity: Do positive thoughts always lead to positive effects? Brain. Behav. Immun. 2005;19:195–200. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Wichman A, Keeling LJ, Forkman B. Cognitive bias and anticipatory behaviour of laying hens housed in basic and enriched pens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012;140:62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.05.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Roelofs S, Boleij H, Nordquist R, Staay FJ. Van Der. Making decisions under ambiguity: judgment bias tasks for assessing emotional state in animals. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016;10:1–16. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Parker, R. M. A. Cognitive bias as an indicator of emotional state in animals. (2008).
- 50.Fleeson W. Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2001;80:1011–1027. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fleeson W. Moving personality beyond the person-situation debate the challenge and the opportunity of within-person variability. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2004;13:83–87. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00280.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Fleeson W. Situation-Based Contingencies Underlying Trait-Content Manifestation in Behavior. J. Pers. 2007;75:825–862. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00458.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Van Reenen CG, et al. Behavioural and physiological responses of heifer calves to acute stressors: Long-term consistency and relationship with adult reactivity to milking. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013;147:55–68. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.04.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Van Reenen CG, et al. Responses of calves to acute stress: Individual consistency and relations between behavioral and physiological measures. Physiol. Behav. 2005;85:557–570. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Nettle D, Penke L. Personality: bridging the literatures from human psychology and behavioural ecology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2017;365:4043–4050. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Romeyer A, Bouissou MF. Assessment of fear reactions in domestic sheep, and influence of breed and rearing conditions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1992;34:93–119. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(05)80060-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Visser EK, et al. Quantifying aspects of young horses’ temperament: consistency of behavioural variables. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001;74:241–258. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00177-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Visser EK, et al. Heart rate and heart rate variability during a novel object test and a handling test in young horses. Physiol. Behav. 2002;76:289–296. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00698-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Rushen J, Taylor AA, de Passillé AM. Domestic animals’ fear of humans and its effect on their welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1999;65:285–303. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00089-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Geverink NA, Schouten WGP, Gort G, Wiegant VM. Individual differences in behavioral and physiological responses to restraint stress in pigs. Physiol. Behav. 2002;77:451–457. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00877-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Manteuffel G, Puppe B, Schön PC. Vocalization of farm animals as a measure of welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004;88:163–182. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.02.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Ligout S, Foulquié D, Sèbe F, Bouix J, Boissy A. Assessment of sociability in farm animals: The use of arena test in lambs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011;135:57–62. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.09.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Sibbald AM, Elston DA, Smith DJF, Erhard HW. A method for assessing the relative sociability of individuals within groups: an example with grazing sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005;91:57–73. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Boissy A, Le Neindre P. Behavioral, cardiac and cortisol responses to brief peer separation and reunion in cattle. Physiol. Behav. 1997;61:693–699. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00521-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.