Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jan 24.
Published in final edited form as: J Health Soc Behav. 2007 Dec;48(4):335–351. doi: 10.1177/002214650704800401

Did Socioeconomic Inequalities in Morbidity and Mortality Change in the United States over the Course of the Twentieth Century?*

John Robert Warren 1, Elaine M Hernandez 1
PMCID: PMC5781232  NIHMSID: NIHMS934567  PMID: 18198683

Abstract

In this article we present two sets of empirical analyses that consider the extent to which socioeconomic gradients in self-assessed health and child mortality changed since the beginning of the twentieth century in the United States. This empirical issue has important and wide-ranging research and policy implications. In particular, our results speak to the value of considering the role of broader social, economic, and political inequalities in generating and maintaining socioeconomic disparities in morbidity and mortality. Despite dramatic declines in morbidity and mortality rates in the United States across the twentieth century, we find that socioeconomic-status gradients in morbidity and mortality declined only modestly (if at all) during that period.


For nearly 200 years, researchers have recognized inverse relationships between socioeconomic status (SES) and aggregate morbidity and mortality rates (Antonovsky 1967; Chaplin 1924; Coombs 1941; Kawachi and Kennedy 2002; Link and Phelan 2004; Robert and House 2003; Villerme 1840; Virchow 1848). Over those 200 years, the United States and other developed countries have also witnessed dramatic declines in overall morbidity and mortality rates. In this article, we ask whether the overall declines in morbidity and mortality rates in the United States across the twentieth century were accompanied by reductions in the SES gradients in these outcomes. As we describe below, there is relatively little evidence to suggest that SES gradients in morbidity and mortality declined in the United States across the twentieth century. This straightforward empirical question has important and wide-ranging research and policy implications.

BACKGROUND

Biomedical and public health research has typically sought to explain socioeconomic disparities in morbidity and mortality rates by looking closely at the downstream or proximate factors that link socioeconomic position to these outcomes, including biomedical, psychosocial, behavioral, and physiological mechanisms (Berkman and Macintyre 1997; Deaton 2002; Marmot, Kogevinas, and Elston 1987; McKinley and Marceau 2000; Robert and House 2003). These efforts have clearly led to a better understanding of the ways in which the effects of social, economic, behavioral, and psychological factors are mediated by biological processes and behaviors to create health inequalities (McKinlay and McKinlay 1977; Robert and House 2003).

In recent years, some research has moved toward contextualizing these downstream or proximate mechanisms within the framework of upstream or macro-social factors, such as education, occupation, income, and other environmental factors (Hayward et al. 2000; Link and Phelan 1995; Lutfey and Freese 2005; Mackenbach et al. 2003; Marmot et al. 1987; Robert and House 2003; Wilson 2001). The result is a growing interest in conceptualizing SES as a “basic cause” (Lieberson 1985) of morbidity and mortality and a reassessment of the long-term efficacy of exclusively focusing on the proximate mechanisms that link SES to morbidity and mortality. Downstream approaches are not inherently opposed to perspectives that focus on upstream factors; both are located on a spectrum of theoretical and practical approaches to understanding health inequalities (Berkman and Macintyre 1997; McKinley and Marceau 2000; Robert and House 2003). Understanding this spectrum of mechanisms associated with health inequalities has important implications for research, policy, and public health interventions.

Perhaps the most prominent example of an approach to understanding the spectrum of upstream and downstream mechanisms associated with SES disparities in morbidity and mortality is Link and Phelan’s (1995, 1996) theory of “fundamental social causes.” This theory begins with the observation that the mechanisms or risk factors that account for socioeconomic gradients in morbidity and mortality rates change over time. In the early twentieth century, issues like sanitation, water quality, and food safety may have been the key mechanisms linking SES and health, but now researchers focus more on factors like smoking, obesity, and access to health insurance. Despite changes over time in the intervening mechanisms, these authors contend that the associations between SES and aggregate morbidity and mortality rates have nonetheless persisted. Link and Phelan (2000) note that resources associated with SES help individuals avoid exposure to deleterious health effects:

[T]he reason SES has been so consistently associated with disease is that it embodies resources like knowledge, money, power, and prestige that can be used in different ways in different situations to avoid risks for disease and death (Link and Phelan, 1995, 1996). People who are relatively better off use their advantage to avoid risks and to adopt protective strategies that enhance health and well-being no matter what the risk and protective factors happen to be at a given point in time. (P. 39)

Lutfey and Freese (2005) explain further:

[Link and Phelan’s] fundamental cause concept implies not a theory of the specific proximate mechanisms responsible for a persistent association, but rather that some metamechanism(s) is responsible for how specific and varied mechanisms are continuously generated over historical time in such a way that the direction of the enduring association is preserved…|. If an explanatory variable is a fundamental cause of an outcome, then the association cannot be successfully reduced to a set of more proximate, intervening causes because the association persists even while the relative influence of various proximate mechanisms changes. (P. 1327–28)

As exemplified by the two quotations above, research that argues for contextualizing the more proximate mechanisms that link SES to health within the framework of broader social and economic inequalities is frequently motivated by a striking empirical claim: Despite dramatic long-term improvements in public health, changes in the types of diseases that are most prevalent, and changes in health risk factors, the magnitude of socioeconomic disparities in morbidity and mortality rates has persisted for decades or longer. Indeed, Link and Phelan (1996) orient their theory to account for “enduring associations between socio-demographic factors and disease” (p. 472).

In fact, as we review below, there is relatively little systematic evidence to support (or refute) the claim that the magnitude of socioeconomic disparities in morbidity and mortality rates in the United States has persisted over the long term. Consequently, in this article we ask how socioeconomic gradients in morbidity and mortality changed in the United States over the course of the twentieth century. Our goal is to provide empirical evidence that documents long-term SES gradients in health inequalities. We then discuss the role of traditional downstream efforts to improve morbidity and mortality within the context of this empirical evidence.

What relevance does this empirical question have for research and practice? We argue that if socioeconomic inequalities in morbidity and mortality rates did not decline in the United States over the last century, then there is greater motivation for contextualizing downstream proximate mechanisms that link SES to health within the framework of upstream socioeconomic inequalities. Although traditional public health and biomedical research has made major advances in reducing overall morbidity and mortality rates, it may have had little or no impact on SES inequalities in those rates. Indeed, reducing socioeconomic inequalities in those rates has not typically been an explicit goal of such research. On the other hand, if SES inequalities in morbidity and mortality rates are on the wane, then there is less urgency for researchers to contextualize their research within the framework of broader social and economic inequalities. We intend our research to help shape future research and policies related to health inequalities.

PREVIOUS EVIDENCE

Researchers in the United States consistently find that associations between income and mortality rates have either remained steady (Duleep 1989) or have increased since the 1960s (Duncan 1996; Pappas et al. 1993), particularly for men. Likewise, since at least 1960 educational gradients in aggregate mortality rates have either held steady (Duleep 1989) or increased (Crimmins and Saito 2001; Lauderdale 2001; Lynch 2003; Pappas et al. 1993). Although most U.S. research operationalizes SES in terms of income or educational attainment, there are exceptions. Steenland, Hu, and Walker (2004), for example, found that mortality differences by occupation increased for men between 1984 and 1997.

One limitation of most research on U.S. trends in SES inequalities in aggregate mortality or morbidity rates is the limited time horizon. Most research builds on the Matched Record Study of 1960 (Kitagawa and Hauser 1973), which matched death certificates registered between May and August of 1960 to records from the 1960 U.S. Census. While much of this research includes individuals who were born across the span of the twentieth century, their analyses are typically not organized in such a way as to model changes across birth cohorts in age-group-specific SES inequalities in mortality or morbidity rates (e.g., Pappas et al. 1993; Preston and Elo 1995), and their findings are thus restricted to changes in health disparities since 1960. Two exceptions include the work of Lauderdale (2001) and Lynch (2003), who each draw conclusions about how age-group-specific educational inequalities in morbidity and mortality rates changed over the course of the twentieth century; both authors found increasing health disparities by education across birth cohorts. However, as discussed below, it is worth wondering whether these findings may conflate changes across the twentieth century in the extent to which education is an important component of socioeconomic status with real changes across birth cohorts in socioeconomic disparities in morbidity and mortality rates.

While research on SES gradients in aggregate morbidity and mortality rates in the United States typically operationalizes SES in terms of income or education, such research in Great Britain typically operationalizes SES in terms of the Register-General’s Social Class scheme (which is fundamentally an occupational classification; Marmot et al. 1987). British researchers using this social class scheme have almost uniformly concluded that SES gradients in mortality have remained stable or increased since at least World War II (Antonovsky 1967; Black et al. 1982; Marang-van de Mheen et al. 1998; Wilkinson 1986; Williams 1990). Mackenbach et al. (2003) provide similar evidence for England and Wales, Italy, Finland, Sweden, Norway, and Denmark in the 1980s. Although some researchers have considered British data from as early as the 1920s (Koskinen 1985; Pamuk 1985), pre–World War II trends in Western European SES gradients in mortality or morbidity are not well understood. In the end, the bulk of evidence from Western Europe is subject to the same limited time horizon as most of the U.S. evidence.

CONCEPTUAL ISSUES IN MEASURING SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS

Beyond the limited time horizon of most research on trends in socioeconomic disparities in morbidity and mortality rates, much of this research suffers from inadequate conceptualizations of SES. To be sure, a number of observers have carefully explicated the theoretical and practical issues involved in operationalizing and measuring SES for use in health research (Oakes and Rossi 2003), sociological research (Hauser and Warren 1997), and elsewhere. Beyond the overarching need to conceptualize and measure SES with the same care that typically goes into the measurement of morbidity and mortality, research in this area also faces three additional challenges.

First, at a minimum researchers need to measure both SES and mortality or morbidity in a strictly consistent manner over time. The basic structures of the U.S. educational or occupational systems have not changed dramatically in the last several decades (although the distribution of people within those structures has changed); nor has the basic racial classification system changed. Therefore, at first glance it seems possible to use consistently measured indicators of education, income, occupation, or race/ethnicity as proxies for SES in studying long-term trends in the association between SES and aggregate mortality or morbidity rates in the United States.

Second, even if researchers measure SES in a technically consistent manner over time, the social meaning and consequences of some components of SES have likely changed in important ways over time. For example, in the early twentieth century people without formal educational credentials could still be socially and economically successful through agriculture, skilled blue-collar work, or other means. In the late twentieth century, people without formal educational credentials face severely dimmer prospects for socioeconomic success. This change over time in the extent to which educational attainment reliably signals socioeconomic success raises real questions about how to interpret findings that educational disparities in morbidity and mortality rates increased in the United States across the twentieth century (Lauderdale 2001; Lynch 2003). It may be that socioeconomic disparities in morbidity and mortality remained constant and that education became a better indicator of socioeconomic status over time.

A third measurement challenge faced by researchers seeking to operationalize SES in analyses of trends over time in SES-health relationships stems from a debate about whether absolute or relative measures of SES have greater causal impact on morbidity and mortality. Essentially, this debate concerns whether absolute material deprivation (for instance, not having enough money to purchase adequate food or medical care) or relative deprivation (for instance, having an income that falls well below the median income in a society) matters most for morbidity and mortality. We recognize that there are both theoretical and practical implications of this debate (Deaton 2002; Lynch et al. 2004; Marmot 2003; Wagstaff and Doorslaer 2000; Wilkinson 1997), but the aim of this research is not to resolve the debate between absolute and relative measures. We seek to understand how both absolute and relative deprivation are associated with morbidity.

Our approach to dealing with these measurement issues is to use a variety of indicators of SES in our analyses. All of our SES indicators are measured in a technically consistent manner over time. While some of them might have changed in their conceptual meaning over time, other measures that we use arguably mean the same thing over time. Finally, we utilize two relative measures and two absolute measures of SES in our analyses.

We proceed with our empirical analyses in two parts. In Part I we model the impact of four measures of SES on self-reported overall health using data that include respondents between the ages of 18 and 89 who were born between 1883 and 1986. In Part II we estimate and compare occupation group–specific child-mortality rates in 1910 and in the 1990s. Together, the two parts provide evidence about whether and in what way SES gradients in morbidity and mortality changed in the United States during the twentieth century. As described below, both parts of our analyses are limited in certain respects; neither analysis is based on ideal data or measures. Nonetheless, we contend that, taken together, the two sections of our analyses provide valuable empirical information that speaks to broader policy and research issues.

PART I

Data and Methods

We begin with analyses of data from the 1972 through 2004 General Social Surveys (GSS). The GSS is administered by the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) at the University of Chicago. This multipurpose survey of members of households across the United States has been conducted annually between 1972 and 1994 (except in 1979, 1981, and 1992) and biennially since then (Davis, Smith, and Marsden 2005). Each survey year, NORC selects a fresh cross-sectional random sample of Americans living in households. Prior to 1994, the GSS was conducted with about 1,500 respondents each year; beginning in 1994, NORC began interviewing approximately 3,000 people biennially. Respondents vary in age from 18 to 89. The content of the interviews ranges broadly, covering such areas as attitudes about a variety of social issues, questions about socioeconomic and demographic characteristics, and a variety of behavioral measures. Core questions—including those at the heart of our analyses—have been asked the same way in each year in order to facilitate comparisons over time (Davis et al. 2005). Table 1 includes descriptive statistics for our measures of SES, self-assessed overall health, age, and year of birth. This table also indicates the years in which particular survey items were administered.

TABLE 1.

Descriptive Statistics for Variables in the Analyses of GSS Data

% n Years Observed
Self-assessed overall health
  Poor 6% 1,958 1972 to 2004 (except 1978, 1983, and 1986)
  Fair 18% 6,425
  Good 45% 15,531
  Excellent 31% 10,943
Relative childhood family income
  Far below average 8% 2,802 1972 to 2004 (except 1996, 1998, and 2000)
  Below average 24% 8,332
  Average 52% 17,947
  Above average 15% 5,096
  Far above average 2% 603
Father’s occupation
  White collar 46% 17,609 1972 to 2004
  Skilled blue collar 22% 8,327
  Unskilled blue collar, service, and farm 32% 12,315
Educational attainment
  Not a high school graduate 25% 11,597 1972 to 2004
  High school graduate 32% 14,562
  Some college completed 43% 20,061
Relative adult family income
  Far below average 5% 2,241 1972 to 2004
  Below average 24% 10,138
  Average 51% 21,875
  Above average 18% 7,952
  Far above average 2% 843
Age group
  18 to 19 2% 814 1972 to 2004
  20 to 29 20% 9,486
  30 to 39 22% 10,249
  40 to 49 18% 8,365
  50 to 59 14% 6,571
  60 to 69 12% 5,434
  70 to 79 8% 3,849
  80 to 89 3% 1,576
Birth cohort
  1883 to 1889 0% 60 1972 to 2004
  1890 to 1899 1% 608
  1900 to 1909 4% 2,051
  1910 to 1919 9% 4,068
  1920 to 1929 12% 5,489
  1930 to 1939 12% 5,701
  1940 to 1949 18% 8,332
  1950 to 1959 21% 9,958
  1960 to 1969 14% 6,587
  1970 to 1979 6% 2,983
  1980 to 1986 1% 507

Note: GSS = General Social Survey.

In all survey years, NORC ascertained each respondent’s age at the time of their interview. Using this information, we have computed each respondent’s year of birth. Because we have a wide age range in each survey year and because we have more than three decades of cross-sectional survey data, respondents’ years of birth range from 1883 to 1986. Respondents’ ages range from 18 to 89.

Our measure of self-assessed overall health—the dependent variable for this portion of our analyses—is derived from a survey item that asks, “Would you say your own health, in general, is excellent, good, fair, or poor?” Entirely subjective measures of self-assessed overall health are closely linked to more objective measures of morbidity and mortality (Idler and Benyamini 1997; Idler and Kasl 1991; Wilson 2001); what is more, the available evidence suggests that the association between this measure and mortality has remained more or less constant over time (Idler and Benyamini 1997).

In our analyses of GSS data, we employ four distinct measures of SES, two of which are “absolute” measures of SES, two of which are “relative” measures, two of which pertain to SES in childhood, and two of which pertain to SES in adulthood. First, relative childhood family income is derived from a survey item that asks, “Thinking about the time when you were 16 years old, compared with American families in general then, would you say your family income was far below average, below average, average, above average, or far above average?” Second, in each GSS survey, respondents have been asked about the occupation that their father (or “father substitute”) held while they were growing up (an absolute measure of SES in childhood). For our purposes we have collapsed this detailed occupational information into a three-category scheme: white collar, skilled blue collar, and unskilled (which includes unskilled blue collar, farm/forestry/ fishing occupations, and service work). Third, in each GSS survey respondents were asked about their educational attainment (an absolute measure of SES in adulthood). For our purposes, we have divided the sample into those who (1) did not complete high school, (2) completed high school but no post-secondary education, or (3) completed at least some post-secondary education.1 Finally, relative adult family income is derived from a survey question that asks, “Compared with American families in general, would you say your family income is far below average, below average, average, above average, or far above average?”

Each of our measures of health and SES has its own weaknesses for our purposes. We would certainly prefer a more objective measure of health. In addition, the validity and reliability of retrospective reports of relative childhood family income have not been well established; there is a fair amount of missing data on the father’s occupation measure;2 and the education measure may have conceptual weaknesses, as described in the above discussion of education as an indicator of SES. Consequently, we caution against placing too much faith in the results of models using any one of these SES measures. However, the fact that the results of our analyses are substantively similar across SES measures and that the results from the analyses of the GSS data parallel those from Part II of our analyses give us confidence in the broader validity of our empirical findings.

Descriptive Results

Table 2 reports the percentage of individuals who report their health as excellent or good, separately by 10-year age groups, decade of birth, and relative childhood family income. We choose this SES measure simply as an example, and we hasten to note that we do not collapse categories of the SES measures in our multivariate analyses.3 Table 2 is designed to demonstrate that we do not observe members of all age groups in all birth cohorts, and we do not observe members of all birth cohorts in all age groups. The empty cells in the upper-left portion of Table 2 are a function of the GSS not beginning until 1972; people born in the 1890s could only be in the 20 to 29 age group if they were interviewed in the 1910s. The empty cells in the lower right portion of the table cannot be filled in yet; individuals born in the 1980s will not be 80 to 89 years old until the 2060s.

TABLE 2.

Percentage of Respondents in Excellent or Good Health, by Decade of Birth, Age Group, and Childhood Family Income, 1972–2004 General Social Surveys (sample sizes given below percentages)

Age Group Relative Family Income
at Age 16
Decade of Birth

1880s 1890s 1900s 1910s 1920s 1930s 1940s 1950s 1960s 1970s 1980s
20 to 29 Average or above average 90% 88% 89% 85% 87%
716 1,708 1,340 407 107
Below average 83% 80% 84% 81% 78%
254 508 361 113 32
30 to 39 Average or above average 83% 88% 87% 89% 87%
462 1,323 1,465 479 166
Below average 79% 77% 82% 81% 88%
258 562 573 196 69
40 to 49 Average or above average 79% 79% 85% 82% 88%
454 894 987 452 130
Below average 65% 71% 77% 81% 77%
234 473 492 189 66
50 to 59 Average or above average 68% 71% 77% 83% 88%
393 846 621 272 115
Below average 59% 59% 60% 70% 77%
258 593 365 141 70
60 to 69 Average or above average 56% 61% 68% 77% 80%
342 726 582 137 65
Below average 43% 50% 57% 69% 84%
164 472 476 107 51
70 to 79 Average or above average 56% 55% 56% 64% 70%
161 487 474 101 23
Below average 45% 39% 51% 53% 88%
96 258 322 66 17
80 to 89 Average or above average 53% 55% 54% 67% 67%
38 173 218 42 9
Below average 53% 40% 44% 42% 56%
17 82 100 33 9

Note: Figures in bold are for cells with fewer than 30 observations.

Table 2 provides assurance of the quality of the GSS data and of our SES and self-assessed overall health measures. First, as expected, we observe that respondents’ self-reported overall health declines with age within every birth cohort. Second, also as expected, we observe that respondents’ self-reported overall health improves across birth cohorts within specific age groups. Third, as expected, we observe SES gradients in self-assessed overall health.

Multivariate Results

In Table 3 we present fit statistics from a series of logistic regression models in which self-assessed overall health (“excellent/good” = 1, “fair/poor” = 0) is modeled as a function of SES, year of birth, and age. We have also estimated a series of ordered logistic regression models that make use of the full range of variability in the self-assessed overall health measure. Although the results of those models are substantively quite similar, we prefer the results that we present because the proportional odds assumption is clearly violated in the ordered logistic regression models. We estimate a separate series of models for each of the SES measures.

TABLE 3.

Logistic Regression Models of Excellent or Good Health on SES, Cohort, and Age Group

Relative Childhood
Family Income
Father’s
Occupation
Education Relative Adult
Family Income
Model 1: Logistic regressions of self-reported overall health on SES, year of birth, and age
  χ2 (df) 2253.8 (6) 2440.7 (4) 3917.5 (4) 3552.7 (6)
Model 2: Model 1 + year of birth × age interaction term
  Improvement in χ2 vs. model 1 (df) 15.3 (1) 14.4 (1) .0 (1) 1.0 (1)
  Change in BIC vs. model 1 −5.2 4.1 10.5 9.4
Model 3: Model 2 + SES × age interaction terms
  Improvement in χ2 vs. model 2 (df) 6.2 (4) 2.7 (2) 8.3 (2) 4.8 (4)
  Change in BIC vs. model 2 34.4 17.8 12.6 36.8
Model 4: Model 2 + SES × year of birth interaction terms
  Improvement in χ2 vs. model 2 (df) 7.5 (4) 5.6 (2) 13.8 (2) 6.4 (4)
  Change in BIC vs. model 2 33.1 14.9 7.1 35.2
Model 5: Model 2 + all two-way interaction terms
  Improvement in χ2 vs. model 2 (df) 11.6 (8) 8.7 (4) 15.3 (4) 15.8 (8)
  Change in BIC vs. model 2 69.6 32.4 26.5 67.4

Notes: SES = socioeconomic status. BIC = Bayesian information criterion. Figures in bold indicate the preferred model for each measure of socioeconomic status.

The baseline model (model 1) for each SES measure simply includes the SES measure (as a series of dummy variables indicating the SES category to which respondents belong), a continuous measure of age, and a continuous measure of year of birth. To simplify the process of computing predicted values, we have subtracted 18 (the minimum age of sample members) from each respondent’s age and 1883 (the earliest year of birth among those included in the sample) from each respondent’s year of birth.

Models 2 through 6 add a series of interaction terms to the baseline model. Our strategy is to test hypotheses about whether particular interaction terms in the full model add to the predictive power of the reduced model (which does not include those interaction terms). Because our sample sizes are large—more than 25,000 for each series of models—we base our decisions about improvement in model fit on the Bayesian information criterion (BIC; Raftery 1995). Whereas traditional χ2 comparisons are especially likely to show improvement in model fit with large sample sizes, BIC provides a better-calibrated assessment of improvement in model fit that accounts for sample size. Reductions in BIC of 10 or greater are associated with strong evidence of improved model fit.

Model 2 begins with model 1 and adds an interaction term for age by year of birth. For two of the four measures for SES, BIC declines by at least 10. This means that the effect of age on self-reported overall health varies as a function of year of birth. Specifically, people of a particular age are healthier in more recent cohorts than same-aged people in less recent cohorts.

Model 3 begins with model 2 and adds an interaction term for SES and age. The BIC statistic never indicates improved model fit. Model 4 returns to model 2 and adds an interaction term for SES and birth cohort; again, the addition of this interaction never improves the fit of the model. Finally, model 5 again begins with model 2 and adds interaction terms for both SES and age and SES and birth cohort. Once again, according to BIC, model 5 never fits the data significantly better than model 2. Consequently, our preferred model (shown in bold in Table 3) includes only the interaction between age and year of birth when SES is measured as childhood relative family income or father’s occupation; it includes none of these interactions when SES is measured as adult relative family income or education. These technical differences in model specification are substantively important, as they provide evidence about whether SES differences in self-assessed overall health grow, diminish, or remain unchanged over time. If the association between SES and self-assessed overall health changed across birth cohorts, then we would have expected to find significant interaction effects involving the SES and year of birth covariates.4

Table 4 reports parameter estimates and standard errors for our preferred model for each measure of SES. The patterns of results are similar across SES measures. As expected, health (1) declines with age for people in the same birth cohort, (2) improves across birth cohorts for people of the same age, and (3) is always better for people in higher SES categories. Most importantly for our purposes, the best-fitting models specify stability in age-specific SES inequalities across cohorts.

TABLE 4.

Results for Best–Fitting Models in Analyses of 1972–2004 General Social Survey Data

Relative Childhood
Family Income
Father’s
Occupation
Education Relative Adult
Family Income




b (s.e.) b (s.e.) b (s.e.) b (s.e.)
Socioeconomic status, year of birth, and age main effects
  Lowest SES category −.59 (.12)*** −.63 (.04)*** −1.32 (.03)*** −1.47 (.13)***
  Second SES category −.17 (.12) −.45 (.04)*** −.51 (.03)*** −.99 (.12)***
  Third (or highest) SES category .17 (.12) Reference group Reference group −.36 (.12)***
  Fourth SES category .45 (.12)*** .32 (.12)**
  Highest SES category Reference group Reference group
  Year of birth (× 100) 1.24 (.23)*** .86 (.21)*** .13 (.14) 1.47 (.14)***
  Age (× 100) −2.51 (.30)*** −2.88 (.28)*** −2.57 (.15)*** −2.01 (.16)***
Year of birth by age interaction term
  Year of birth × age (× 1,000) .18 (.05)*** .17 (.04)***
Intercepts
  Constant .93 (.21)*** 1.77 (.16)*** 2.42 (.13)*** 1.41 (.17)***
*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

PART II

Data and Measures

In the second part of our analyses, we use harmonized SES and child mortality information from the 1910 U.S. Census and the June Fertility and Marital History Supplements to the 1985, 1990, and 1995 Current Population Surveys (CPS). As in our analyses of GSS data, our central research question is whether the association between SES and mortality (here measured as parental occupation and child mortality) changed across the twentieth century. We utilize the 1910 Census Public Use Microdata Sample provided by the Minnesota Population Center (Ruggles and Sobek 1995); this 1-in-250 national random sample of the population includes approximately 89,000 households and 366,000 individuals. The CPS has been conducted since 1948 by the Bureau of Labor Statistics under the auspices of the Bureau of the Census; more than 50,000 nationally representative households are interviewed monthly. Individuals in the CPS are broadly representative of the civilian, non-institutionalized population of the United States. In addition to the basic demographic and labor force questions that are included in each monthly CPS, questions on selected topics are included in most months. The 1985, 1990, and 1995 June CPS files (like the 1910 U.S. Census) include survey items sufficient to produce indirect estimates of child mortality using the Brass (1975; Brass and Coale 1968) method and its extensions (Trussell 1975; United Nations 1983). The key difference in sampling procedures between the Census and CPS concerns the treatment of institutionalized individuals: Whereas the 1910 Census represents the full population, the CPS samples exclude institutionalized individuals. While rates of institutionalization were certainly higher in the latter part of the twentieth century, evidence from 1990 Census data indicate that only about 3 percent of individuals were institutionalized in 1990. While this difference in sample coverage may have some bearing on our empirical results, we doubt that our results are seriously biased by these differences.

Specifically, our child mortality estimates are derived from the children ever born, children surviving (CEBCS) method, which is based entirely on information about the number of children ever born to each woman, the number of those children who were still surviving at the time of interview, and women’s mean age at childbearing. In the 1910 Census, ever-married women were asked to report the number of children they had ever given birth to and the number of those children who were still surviving. They were instructed to include children born in previous marriages, but not to include their current spouse’s children born in previous marriages. In the 1985, 1990, and 1995 June CPS, all women older than 14 were asked how many children they had ever given birth to (excluding stillbirths). They were then asked about the current place of residence of the first five children to which they gave birth; among the response options was “deceased.” Although we are forced to restrict the CPS analyses to the first five babies born to each woman, this restriction has a minimal impact; only 0.3 percent of women in these data had more than 5 children.

The CEBCS method indirectly estimates child mortality using data on the average number of children ever born and average number of children surviving, calculated by the five-year age group of mother.5 Brass (1975) demonstrated that the probability of dying between birth and age a, denoted as q(a), can be estimated by:

q(a)=M(x,5)×D(x,5)

where D(x,5) refers to the proportion of children dead among women in age group (x, x + 5) and M(x,5) is an age-specific multiplier that depends on indices of the age pattern of fertility. Under this system, the proportion of children dead among women in five-year age groups ranging from 15–19 to 45–49 are used to calculate q(1), q(2), q(3), q(5), q(10), q(15), and q(20), respectively. All of the results presented below are from Coale-Demeny West models with Trussell equations.

We begin by selecting all women between the ages of 15 and 49 (for reasons outlined above). We then measure SES using information about the occupation of the head of household of these women. In some cases the woman herself is the head of household, but in the majority of cases the head of household is the woman’s husband. That is, our child mortality measure is based on the woman’s number of children ever born and surviving, while our SES measure is based on the occupation of her head of household. Data from the 1985, 1990, and 1995 censuses were coded to the standards of the 1980 U.S. Census Occupational Classification. The Minnesota Population Center’s release of Public Use Microdata from the 1910 Census also includes a recode of all respondents’ occupations to the standards of the 1980 U.S. Census Occupational Classification. Consequently, all of these data from the 1910 U.S. Census and from the late-twentieth-century June CPS surveys are consistently coded to the same standards. In these analyses we have collapsed detailed occupational information into a four-category scheme: white-collar, skilled-blue-collar, service, and unskilled (which includes unskilled blue-collar and farm/forestry/ fishing occupations). The difference between this occupational classification and the one used in the GSS analyses is that we have made service occupations a distinct category. Because q(1) and q(2) are best identified for younger women (ages 15–19 and 20–24, respectively), and because so few women in these young age groups have heads of household who are in white-collar or skilled-blue-collar occupations, our analyses are restricted to deriving estimates of q(3) and q(5).

Results

Because the separate 1985, 1990, and 1995 June CPS samples are too small to be analyzed separately, we combine those three samples into one that we will henceforth refer to as “the 1990s.” Table 5 reports the mean number of children ever born, the mean number of children surviving, and sample sizes, by the age group of mother and the major occupation group of the head of household in 1910 and in the 1990s. As expected, the table makes clear that both fertility rates and child mortality rates declined between 1910 and the 1990s across each of the SES groups. The data in this table are all that is required to produce estimates of q(3) and q(5) using the CEBCS method.

TABLE 5.

Mean Number of Children Ever Born, Mean Number of Children Surviving, and Sample Sizes, by Age of Mother and Major Occupation Group of Head of Household, 1910 and 1985–95

White Collar Skilled Blue Collar Service Unskilled Blue Collar/Farm




Mean Number
of Children …
Mean Number
of Children …
Mean Number
of Children …
Mean Number
of Children …




… Ever Born … Surviving n … Ever Born … Surviving n … Ever Born … Surviving n … Ever Born … Surviving n
1910 (U.S. Census)
Age of mother
  15–19 .086 .081 2,920 .169 .155 2,658 .162 .151 1,023 .267 .251 9,915
  20–24 .731 .694 3,328 1.206 1.129 2,611 .901 .803 1,086 1.528 1.392 8,920
  25–29 1.677 1.559 3,250 2.398 2.181 2,375 1.699 1.499 1,028 2.861 2.542 7,610
  30–34 2.461 2.219 2,842 3.395 2.951 2,140 2.623 2.197 828 3.988 3.457 6,128
  35–39 3.116 2.734 2,725 4.236 3.631 2,007 3.373 2.805 770 5.087 4.299 5,684
  40–44 3.660 3.137 2,063 4.922 4.025 1,546 4.117 3.237 607 5.706 4.694 4,580
  45–49 3.857 3.222 1,574 5.195 4.161 1,252 4.311 3.286 518 6.298 5.063 3,938
1985–1995 (1985, 1990, 1995 June Current Population Surveys)
Age of mother
  15–19 .070 .068 3,942 .095 .095 84 .064 .064 3,270 .089 .089 751
  20–24 .295 .292 8,019 .590 .586 251 .484 .477 2,881 .610 .601 1,189
  25–29 .673 .666 9,688 .954 .945 307 1.116 1.099 2,464 1.230 1.211 1,332
  30–34 1.195 1.180 10,194 1.351 1.320 353 1.692 1.671 2,313 1.656 1.632 1,424
  35–39 1.552 1.532 9,850 1.757 1.713 338 1.975 1.940 2,084 1.948 1.914 1,491
  40–44 1.787 1.756 8,762 1.876 1.846 299 2.139 2.097 1,846 2.215 2.160 1,216
  45–49 2.013 1.965 7,012 2.181 2.130 216 2.485 2.425 1,531 2.432 2.357 1,052

Figures 1 and 2 report estimates of q(3) and q(5), respectively, by SES and separately for 1910 and the 1990s. For ease of presentation, probabilities of dying by age x have been multiplied by 1,000 so that the resulting number can be interpreted as the expected number of deceased children per 1,000 births. The top panels in both figures reflect dramatic declines in child mortality across the twentieth century and reproduce evidence about SES inequalities in child mortality: At each time point, and for both q(3) and q(5), child mortality rates are lowest among those with heads of household who have white-collar or service occupations.

FIGURE 1. Probability of Dying Before Age 3 (q3) by Major Occupation Group of Head of Household, 1910 and 1985–95.

FIGURE 1

Notes: q3 is derived from “Children Ever Born, Children Surviving” indirect life table estimates. The reported results are from the Coale-Demeny West model (with Trussell equations). See text for details regarding sample and variable construction.

FIGURE 2. Probability of Dying Before Age 5 (q5) by Major Occupation Group of Head of Household, 1910 and 1985–95.

FIGURE 2

Note: q5 is derived from “Children Ever Born, Children Surviving” indirect life table estimates. The reported results are from the Coale-Demeny West model (with Trussell equations). See text for details regarding sample and variable construction.

However, the real focus of our investigation is on the bottom panels of Figures 1 and 2. There we compute the relative risk of dying before age x for the skilled-blue-collar, service, and unskilled-blue-collar/ farm groups as compared to the white-collar group. For example, the bottom panel of Figure 1 shows that in 1910 the risk of a child dying before age 3 was 52 percent higher in the unskilled-blue-collar/farm group as compared to the white-collar group. The most relevant thing to note about this figure is that, in general, these relative risks declined only modestly by the 1990s—for example, to 1.33 for the unskilled-blue-collar/farm group. The same general pattern is observed in Figure 2 for q(5). The only result that does not conform with this general pattern has to do with service occupations in the 1990s. Here, q(5) is lower in the 1990s than for white-collar occupations. This may have to do with fundamental changes in the nature of service work across the twentieth century. Whereas there has been more stability in the social meaning of the other occupational categories, service occupations more closely resembled unskilled-blue-collar work in 1910 but more closely resembled white-collar work by the 1990s. In any case, the general pattern of results in the second part of our analysis reflects modest declines in SES disparities in child mortality across the twentieth century in the United States. While it is encouraging that these inequalities have declined, it is worth reiterating that these declines are generally fairly modest and that substantial inequalities remain in the 1990s.

DISCUSSION

In this article, we presented two sets of empirical analyses that considered the extent to which SES gradients in self-assessed overall health (Part I) and child mortality (Part II) changed in the United States since the beginning of the twentieth century. In the first part of our analyses we observed no changes in age-specific SES inequalities in self-reported overall health across birth cohorts, regardless of our operationalization of SES. In the second part of our analyses we found SES inequalities in child mortality in both 1910 and the 1990s, and we generally observed only modest declines in those inequalities across those two time points. In general, our results suggest that SES gradients in morbidity and mortality remained stable or declined only modestly in the United States across the twentieth century.

As described above, prior research that has addressed our empirical question has typically been limited by restricted time horizons: Research in the United States generally only goes back as far as 1960, whereas research in Europe goes back only to World War II. What is more, we take the measurement of SES quite seriously in our analyses. We employ multiple absolute and relative indicators of SES that are measured in a technically consistent manner over time, including some that arguably have the same conceptual meaning over time. Our aim was not to distinguish between the salience of relative or absolute inequalities (e.g., Wagstaff and van Doorslaer 2000), but our results demonstrate that both are relevant measures in our individual-level analysis of long-term trends in health inequalities.

With this said, both parts of our analyses are limited in a number of important ways. The analyses of GSS data are limited in at least three respects. First, survivorship biases may be limiting our ability to observe cross-cohort changes in age-specific socioeconomic gradients in morbidity among older Americans. The skeptical reader may choose to ignore our results for people beyond midlife; we would note, however, that our results are generally consistent across age groups. Second, for methodological reasons we are forced to rely on a dichotomous measure of self-assessed overall health (although models that utilize the full variability of the original self-assessed overall health measure, which violate the proportional odds assumption, yield substantively similar results). Third, for reasons described above, none of the SES measures used in our analyses of GSS data are ideal. Although we take comfort from the fact that our diverse SES measures—two of which are absolute measures, two of which are relative measures, two of which are measures of childhood SES, and two of which are measures of adult SES—generally yield the same substantive findings, we recognize that each of these SES measures is imperfect. Finally, the basic structure of the GSS data—repeated cross-sections of the U.S. population—is less than ideal for our purposes.

Our analyses of 1910 U.S. Census data and 1985–1995 CPS data are also limited in a number of respects. First, on a technical note, the sampling schemes changed between 1910 and the 1990s. The 1910 sample includes ever-married women, whereas the 1990s data include all women. There is no way to reconcile this difference between these samples, but because rates of non-marital childbearing were low in the early twentieth century, we do not expect that this difference severely biases our results. Second, we would have preferred to have multiple measures of SES (some relative and some absolute) at our disposal in this part of our analyses. Third, the relatively small sample sizes in the CPS forced us to aggregate the 1985, 1990, and 1995 data; even after this aggregation, there are an insufficient number of child deaths to employ a more detailed occupational classification in our analyses. Finally, our choice of methodology allows no formal test of the statistical significance of changes between 1910 and the 1990s in occupation-group-specific probabilities of dying by ages three or five. Point estimates of the relative risks of dying by ages three and five generally declined modestly between 1910 and the 1990s, but we do not know whether these declines can simply be attributed to sampling variability.

The results of the second half of our analyses indicate that SES disparities in child mortality generally remained constant or declined modestly across the twentieth century. We cannot say whether any reduction in disparities was due to downstream proximate interventions. As in the first half of our analyses, these results document persisting SES inequalities in an important health outcome in the face of substantial reductions in the incidence rate of that health problem. All of these results provide further motivation to consider the ways in which upstream macro-social factors help perpetuate persistent SES inequalities in health. While traditional downstream proximate interventions dramatically reduced mortality and morbidity rates across the twentieth century, they did much less to reduce SES inequalities in those rates.

CONCLUSION

Our results and others’ (Lutfey and Freese 2005; Lynch et al. 2004) support the contention that research and policy efforts to reduce health inequalities would benefit from considering broader social, economic, and political inequalities alongside more downstream proximate factors that give rise to health inequalities. The U.S. public health infrastructure is in part designed and funded to alleviate health inequalities; indeed, the second objective of the Department of Health and Human Services’ Healthy People 2010 initiative is to eliminate health disparities in this decade. It is clear that there is important work to be done to tackle the proximate causes of health disparities, such as inequalities in access to care, differentials in health risk behaviors, and inequalities in environmental exposures. We argue that researchers doing this work should be cognizant of the upstream macro-social factors that influence the distributions of individuals at risk of downstream proximate causes of morbidity and mortality.

Beyond the need to contextualize traditional public health research, we suggest that more research and policy attention should be paid to the effects of macro-social inequalities on health disparities. There is evidence that country-level variability in health inequalities is related to country-level variability in socioeconomic inequalities (e.g., Mackenbach and Howden-Chapman 2003). This evidence supports the notion that reducing broader social, economic, and political inequalities in the United States might have the consequence of reducing health inequalities. It may be—as Link and Phelan (1995) and others argue—that in order to alleviate socioeconomic gradients in health we need to focus our research and policy efforts more squarely on reducing broader social and economic inequalities. The extent to which this is true—and the extent to which alleviating macro-level social and economic inequalities might be more or less effective in reducing health disparities than traditional approaches—is an open empirical question that is deserving of further investigation.

Acknowledgments

Elaine Hernandez received support for this project from a predoctoral National Research Service Award from the National Institute of Mental Health (#T32-MH19893). We are very grateful to Krista Jenkins for providing important research assistance and to Jennie Brand, Andrew Halpern-Manners, Mary Jackman, Carolyn Liebler, Bruce Link, Karen Lutfey, Ross Macmillan, J. Michael Oakes, Chris Seplaki, Christopher Uggen, and several anonymous reviewers for their help in developing this project. However, errors and omissions are the responsibility of the authors.

Biographies

John Robert Warren is an Associate Professor of Sociology and an affiliate of the Minnesota Population Center at the University of Minnesota. His work focuses on the sociology of education, social stratification, medical sociology, and quantitative research methods.

Elaine M. Hernandez is a Ph.D. candidate in the Department of Sociology at the University of Minnesota. Her research focuses on health inequalities and integrates medical sociology, social stratification, and life course perspectives.

Footnotes

*

An earlier version of this research was presented at the 2006 meeting of the Population Association of America in Los Angeles, CA.

1

We have repeated all analyses using a continuous measure of years of schooling completed. The results, not presented here, are substantively the same.

2

There is very little missing data (less than 5 percent of cases) on all measures used in our analyses except for father’s occupation. About one in six respondents did not grow up with their fathers and are missing on the father’s occupation measure. All analyses presented below simply use listwise deletion to handle missing data. We experimented with more elegant techniques for handling missing data on father’s occupation— for example, including a new “missing” category of father’s occupation—but our results remain substantially the same.

3

We do, however, dichotomize the self-assessed overall health measure in our multivariate analyses. This eases interpretation, but it also helps us (as described below) to avoid violating methodological assumptions associated with regression models for ordinal dependent variables.

4

We also estimate a model with all two-way interactions and a three-way interaction. As expected, this model does not fit significantly better than model 2.

5

The children ever born, children surviving method also requires information about mean age of mothers at childbearing. We have used the application FERTCB in MORTPAK 4.0 to estimate mean age of mothers at childbearing. FERTCB produces estimates of age-specific fertility rates and mean age at childbearing from data on children ever born tabulated by age of mother.

References

  1. Antonovsky Aaron. Social Class Life Expectancy and Overall Mortality. Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. 1967;45:31–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berkman Lisa F, Macintyre Sally. The Measurement of Social Class in Health Studies: Old Measures and New Formulations. In: Kogevinas Manolis, Pearce Neil, Susser Mervyn, Boffetta Paolo., editors. Social Inequalities and Cancer. Lyon, France: International Agency for Research on Cancer; 1997. pp. 51–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Black Sir Douglas, Morris JN, Smith Cyril, Townsend Peter. Inequalities in Health: The Black Report. Middlesex, England: Penguin Books; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  4. Brass William. Methods for Estimating Fertility and Mortality from Limited and Defective Data. Chapel Hill, NC: Laboratory for Population Statistics, Carolina Population Center; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  5. Brass William, Coale Ansley J. Methods of Analysis and Estimation. In: Brass W, editor. The Demography of Tropical Africa. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1968. pp. 88–139. [Google Scholar]
  6. Chaplin Charles V. Deaths among Taxpayers and Non-taxpayers, Providence, 1865. American Journal of Public Health. 1924;14:647–51. doi: 10.2105/ajph.14.8.647-a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Coombs Lolagene. Economic Differentials in Causes of Death. Medical Care. 1941;1:246–55. [Google Scholar]
  8. Crimmins Eileen M, Saito Yasuhiko. Trends in Healthy Life Expectancy in the United States, 1970–1990: Gender, Racial, and Educational Differences. Social Science & Medicine. 2001;52:1629–41. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00273-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Davis James A, Smith Tom W, Marsden Peter V. General Social Surveys, 1972–2004. Chicago, IL: National Opinion Research Center [producer]; Storrs, CT: Roper Center for Public Opinion Research, University of Connecticut/Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributors]; 2005. [2005-09-02]. [CUMULATIVE FILE] [Computer file]. ICPSR04295-v1. 2005. [Google Scholar]
  10. Deaton Angus. Policy Implications of The Gradient of Health and Wealth. Health Affairs. 2002;21:13–20. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.21.2.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Duleep Harriet Orcutt. Measuring Socioeconomic Mortality Differentials Over Time. Demography. 1989;26:345–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Duncan Greg J. Income Dynamics and Health. International Journal of Health Services. 1996;26:419–44. doi: 10.2190/1KU0-4Y3K-ACFL-BYU7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hauser Robert M, Warren John R. Socioeconomic Indexes of Occupational Status: A Review, Update, and Critique. In: Raftery A, editor. Sociological Methodology. Cambridge, England: Blackwell Publishers; 1997. pp. 177–298. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hayward Mark D, Miles Toni P, Crimmins Eileen M, Yang Yu. The Significance of Socioeconomic Status in Explaining the Racial Gap in Chronic Health Conditions. American Sociological Review. 2000;65:910–30. [Google Scholar]
  15. Idler Ellen, Benyamini Yael. Self-Rated Health and Mortality: A Review of Twenty- Seven Community Studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1997;38:21–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Idler Ellen L, Kasl Stanislav. Health Perceptions and Survival: Do Global Evaluations of Health Status Really Predict Mortality? Journal of Gerontology. 1991;46:S55–65. doi: 10.1093/geronj/46.2.s55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kawachi Ichiro, Kennedy Bruce P. The Health of Nations: Why Inequality is Harmful to Your Health. New York: New Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  18. Kitagawa Evelyn M, Hauser Philip M. Differential Mortality in the United States: A Study in Socioeconomic Epidemiology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  19. Koskinen Seppo. Proceedings of 20th IUSSP Conference. Florence, Italy: IUSSP; 1985. Time Trends in Cause- Specific Mortality by Occupational Class in England and Wales. [Google Scholar]
  20. Lauderdale Diane S. Education and Survival: Birth Cohort, Period, and Age Effects. Demography. 2001;38:551–61. doi: 10.1353/dem.2001.0035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lieberson Stanley. Making it Count: The Improvement of Social Research and Theory. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  22. Link Bruce G, Phelan Jo C. Social Conditions as Fundamental Causes of Disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1995;35:80–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Link Bruce G, Phelan Jo C. Understanding Sociodemographic Differences in Health—The Role of Fundamental Social Causes. American Journal of Public Health. 1996;86:471–73. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.4.471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Link Bruce G, Phelan Jo C. Evaluating the Fundamental Cause Explanation for Social Disparities in Health. In: Bird Chloe, Conrad P, Fremont A., editors. Handbook of Medical Sociology. Upper Saddler River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 2000. pp. 33–46. [Google Scholar]
  25. Link Bruce G, Phelan Jo C. Fundamental Sources of Health Inequalities. Columbia University; New York, NY: 2004. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lutfey Karen, Freese Jeremy. Toward Some Fundamentals of Fundamental Causality: Socioeconomic Status and Health in the Routine Clinic Visit for Diabetes. American Journal of Sociology. 2005;110:1326–72. [Google Scholar]
  27. Lynch John, Smith George Davey, Harper Sam, Hillemeier Marianne, Ross Nancy, Kaplan George A, Wolfson Michael. Is Income Inequality a Determinant of Population Health? Part 1. A Systematic Review. Milbank Quarterly. 2004;82:5–99. doi: 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00302.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lynch Scott M. Cohort and Life-Course Patterns in the Relationship between Education and Health: A Hierarchical Approach. Demography. 2003;40:309–31. doi: 10.1353/dem.2003.0016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mackenbach Johan P, Bos Vivian, Andersen Otto, Cardano Mario, Costa Giuseppe, Harding Seeromanie, Reid Alison, Hemström Örjan, Valkonen Tapani, Kunst Anton E. Widening Socioeconomic Inequalities in Mortality in Six Western European Countries. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2003;32:830–37. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyg209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mackenbach Johan P, Howden-Chapman Philippa. New Perspectives on Socioeconomic Inequalities in Health. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 2003;46:428–44. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2003.0039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Marang-van de Mheen PJ, Davey Smith G, Hart CL, Gunning-Schepers LJ. Socioeconomic Differentials in Mortality among Men within Great Britain: Time Trends and Contributory Causes. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 1998;52:214–18. doi: 10.1136/jech.52.4.214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Marmot Michael G. Understanding Social Inequalities in Health. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 2003;46:S9–S23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Marmot Michael G, Kogevinas Manolis, Elston Mary Ann. Social/Economic Status and Disease. Annual Review of Public Health. 1987;8:111–35. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pu.08.050187.000551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McKinlay John B, McKinlay Sonja M. The Questionable Contribution of Medical Measures to the Decline of Mortality in the United States in the Twentieth Century. Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. 1977;55:405–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McLinley J, Marceau L. U.S. Public Health and the 21st Century: Diabetes Mellitus. Lancet. 2000;356:757–61. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02641-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Oakes J Michael, Rossi Peter H. The Measurement of SES in Health Research: Current Practice and Steps toward a New Approach. Social Science & Medicine. 2003;56:769–84. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00073-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pamuk Elsie. Social Class Inequality in Mortality from 1921 to 1972 in England and Wales. Population Studies. 1985;39:17–31. doi: 10.1080/0032472031000141256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pappas Gregory, Queen Susan, Hadden Wilbur, Fisher Gail. The Increasing Disparity in Mortality between Socioeconomic Groups in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine. 1993;329:103–109. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199307083290207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Preston Samuel H, Elo Irma T. Are Educational Differentials in Adult Mortality Increasing in the United States? Journal of Aging and Health. 1995;7:476–96. doi: 10.1177/089826439500700402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raftery Adrian E. Bayesian Model Selection in Social Research. Sociological Methodology 1995. 1995;25:111–63. [Google Scholar]
  41. Robert Stephanie A, House James S. Socioeconomic Inequalities in Health: An Enduring Sociological Problem. In: Bird Chloe E, Conrad P, Fremont AM., editors. Handbook of Medical Sociology. Vol. 5. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 2003. pp. 79–97. [Google Scholar]
  42. Ruggles Steven, Sobek Matt. Integrated Public Use Microdata Series: Version 1.0. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  43. Steenland Kyle, Hu Sherry, Walker James. All-Cause and Cause-Specific Mortality by Socioeconomic Status among Employed Persons in 27 U.S. States, 1984–1997. American Journal of Public Health. 2004;94:1037–42. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.6.1037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Trussell T James. A Re-estimation of the Multiplying Factors for the Brass Technique for Determining Childhood Survivorship Rates. Population Studies. 1975;29:97–107. doi: 10.1080/00324728.1975.10410187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. United Nations. Indirect Techniques for Demographic Estimation, Manual X. New York: United Nations; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  46. Villerme Louis-René. Tableau d’ Etat Physique et Moral des Ouvriers. Vol. 2. Paris: Renouard; 1840. [Google Scholar]
  47. Virchow Rudolf. The Public Health Service (in German) Medizinische Reform. 1848;5:21–22. [Google Scholar]
  48. Wagstaff Adam, van Doorslaer Eddy. Income Inequality and Health: What Does the Literature Tell Us? Annual Review of Public Health. 2000;21:543–67. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.21.1.543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wilkinson Richard G. In: Class and Health: Research and Longitudinal Data. Wilkinson RG, editor. London: Tavistock Publications; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  50. Wilkinson Richard G. Socioeconomic Determinants of Health: Health Inequalities: Relative or Absolute Material Standards? British Medical Journal. 1997;314:591–95. doi: 10.1136/bmj.314.7080.591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Williams David R. Socioeconomic Differentials in Health: A Review and Redirection. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1990;53:81–99. [Google Scholar]
  52. Wilson Sven E. Socioeconomic Status and the Prevalence of Health Problems among Married Couples in Late Midlife. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91:131–35. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.1.131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES