Skip to main content
PeerJ logoLink to PeerJ
. 2018 Jan 24;6:e4293. doi: 10.7717/peerj.4293

Are pumas subordinate carnivores, and does it matter?

L Mark Elbroch 1,, Anna Kusler 1,2
Editor: Jan Schipper
PMCID: PMC5786880  PMID: 29379688

Abstract

Background

Interspecific competition affects species fitness, community assemblages and structure, and the geographic distributions of species. Established dominance hierarchies among species mitigate the need for fighting and contribute to the realized niche for subordinate species. This is especially important for apex predators, many of which simultaneous contend with the costs of competition with more dominant species and the costs associated with human hunting and lethal management.

Methods

Pumas are a widespread solitary felid heavily regulated through hunting to reduce conflicts with livestock and people. Across their range, pumas overlap with six apex predators (gray wolf, grizzly bear, American black bear, jaguar, coyote, maned wolf), two of which (gray wolf, grizzly bear) are currently expanding in North America following recovery efforts. We conducted a literature search to assess whether pumas were subordinate or dominant with sympatric apex predators, as well as with three felid mesocarnivores with similar ecology (ocelot, bobcat, Canada lynx). We also conducted an analysis of the spatial distributions of pumas and their dominant sympatric competitors to estimate in what part of their range, pumas are dominant versus subordinate.

Results

We used 64 sources to assess dominance among pumas and other apex predators, and 13 sources to assess their relationships with felid mesocarnivores. Evidence suggested that wolves, grizzly bears, black bears, and jaguars are dominant over pumas, but that pumas are dominant over coyotes and maned wolves. Evidence suggested that pumas are also dominant over all three felid mesocarnivores with which they share range. More broadly, pumas are subordinate to at least one other apex carnivore in 10,799,252 (47.5%) of their 22,735,268 km2 range across North and South America.

Discussion

Subordinate pumas change their habitat use, suffer displacement at food sources, likely experience increased energetic demands from harassment, exhibit increased starvation, and are sometimes directly killed in competitive interactions with dominant competitors. Nevertheless, we lack research clearly linking the costs of competition to puma fitness. Further, we lack research that assesses the influence of human effects simultaneous with the negative effects of competition with other sympatric carnivores. Until the time that we understand whether competitive effects are additive with human management, or even potentially synergistic, we encourage caution among managers responsible for determining harvest limits for pumas and other subordinate, apex carnivores in areas where they are sympatric with dominant species. This may be especially important information for managers working in regions where wolves and brown bears are recolonizing and recovering, and historic competition scenarios among multiple apex predators are being realized.

Keywords: Puma concolor, Subordinate, Dominance, Competition, Gray wolf, Grizzly bear, American black bear, Jaguar, Apex predators

Introduction

The outcome of interspecific competition affects species fitness, community assemblages and structure, and the geographic distributions of species (Schoener, 1983; Polis & Holt, 1992; Le Bourlot, Tully & Claessen, 2014; Dröge et al., 2017). Much of competition is indirect, and competitors generally assess the competitive abilities (e.g., resource holding potential; Parker & Stuart, 1976) of their opponents before deciding to engage them, resulting in established species hierarchies that mitigate the need for fighting (Arnott & Elwood, 2008). In fact, the established “superiority” (Crombie, 1947) of one species over another contributes to Hutchinson’s (1957) differentiation between the fundamental and realized niche. A species’ enemies include both predators and competitors, both of which limit an animal’s distribution to its realized niche (e.g., wild dogs, Lycaon pictus, in Dröge et al., 2017). This is especially important for carnivores in multi-carnivore guilds, in which competition contributes to intraguild killing (Polis & Holt, 1992; Donadio & Buskirk, 2006) and whether a species self-regulates its population through various social mechanisms or is instead regulated by another predator (Wallach et al., 2015).

Carnivores disproportionately influence ecological functions in natural systems and are culturally important (Ripple et al., 2014). Most carnivores, however, are also feared and persecuted because of perceived and real threats to human safety and because they kill livestock, household pets, and game species we value (Ordiz, Bischof & Swenson, 2013; Ripple et al., 2014; Elbroch, Feltner & Quigley, 2017). Not all carnivores are ecologically equivalent, however, and size betrays stark differences in small versus large carnivores. Carbone, Teacher & Rowcliffe (2007) found that carnivores <20 kg exhibited different hunting strategies and energetic budgets than carnivores >20 kg, and Wallach et al. (2015) found that carnivores <15 kg exhibited additional differences in life history strategies than those species >15 kg. Specifically, Wallach et al. (2015) reported that carnivores >15 kg self-regulate through social mechanisms and density-dependence, whereas smaller carnivores are generally limited by predation by larger carnivores. Wallach et al. (2015) subsequently used 15 kg to differentiate between “apex predators” and smaller carnivores. For our review, we also utilized the 15-kg cutoff.

Apex predators, too, are not ecologically equivalent, and vary in size, morphology, weapons, and life history strategies (Kleiman & Eisenberg, 1973; Bekoff, Daniels & Gittleman, 1984; Sandell, 1989). These differences have resulted in established, though sometimes variable, dominance hierarchies, as well as the unique morphology and behaviors utilized by subordinate species coping with the pressures of interspecific competition. For example, pumas (Puma concolor) and cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) appear to withstand high levels of kleptoparatism without obvious influences on their fitness (Scantlebury et al., 2014; Elbroch et al., 2017a), and coyotes (Canis latrans), wild dogs (Lycaon pictus), leopards (Panthera pardus), and cheetahs utilize temporal and spatial competition refuges and other movement strategies to mitigate interactions with dominant competitors (Durant, 1998; Arjo & Pletscher, 1999; Vanak et al., 2013; Dröge et al., 2017).

The position a species occupies in a dominance hierarchy of a multi-carnivore guild influences its ecology (e.g., foraging strategies and temporal space use; Dröge et al., 2017), but external ecological factors, such as resource distributions, also influence the strategies subordinate species utilize to coexist with dominant competitors, or whether they exhibit subordinate behaviors at all (Karanth et al., 2017). Evidence strongly supports the theory that size determines the outcomes of competitive interactions and the structure of dominance hierarchies; in general, larger carnivores dominate smaller carnivores (Schoener, 1983; Donadio & Buskirk, 2006; De Oliveira & Pereira, 2014). Nevertheless, this is not always the case. Striped skunks (Mephitis mephitis), bobcats (Lynx rufus), and wolverines (Gulo gulo) compensate for their smaller size by being more aggressive and wielding unique weapons (Krebs et al., 2004; Allen et al., 2016).

Determining whether or not a species is dominant or subordinate may also be important in applied conservation and management. Most of the world’s apex predators are hunted by humans, often for trophies (Ordiz, Bischof & Swenson, 2013), and whereas carnivores have evolved strategies to cope with interspecific competition, they are less prepared to withstand the intensity of modern human hunting (Cooley et al., 2009; Darimont et al., 2015). Humans differ from other predators in that they disproportionately target other carnivores and adult animals, which constitute the reproductive capital of populations (Darimont et al., 2015). In the case of subordinate species already under pressures of interspecific competition, human impacts may lead to unexpected population fluctuations or declines. For example, following the recovery of a tiger population in India, subordinate leopards shifted their diet, increased their conflicts with people through livestock depredation, and subsequently suffered rapid declines in their numbers (Harihar, Pandav & Goyal, 2011).

Pumas are a widespread, solitary felid heavily regulated through sport hunting to mitigate conflicts with livestock and people (Treves, 2009; Murphy & MacDonald, 2010; Mattson, 2014). Across their range in North and South America, pumas overlap with six other apex predators (as defined by Wallach et al., 2015). Grizzly (or “brown”) bear (Ursus arctos) and gray wolf (Canis lupus) populations are currently expanding in North America and increasingly overlapping managed puma populations already under pressure from human harvest. If pumas are dominant over bears and wolves, the expansion of these potential competitors should not influence puma fitness or population dynamics significantly. But, if pumas are subordinate to one or both of these recovering species, conservation mangers may need to adjust human harvest to accommodate for the additional ecological stress of new competition. The first step, then, is to determine whether pumas are indeed a subordinate carnivore, and if so, ascertain to which apex carnivores they are subordinate.

In this paper, we had three objectives: First, we reviewed the published literature to gather empirical evidence for whether pumas are dominant, subordinate, or equal in their competitive interactions with the following apex predators: grizzly bear, American black bear (Ursus americanus), gray wolf, coyote, maned wolf (Chrysocyon brachyurus), and jaguar (Panthera unca). Following established theory, we predicted that size would determine competitive outcomes, and that social carnivores and larger carnivores would dominate pumas (Donadio & Buskirk, 2006; De Oliveira & Pereira, 2014). Second, we gathered information on interactions with three ecologically-similar felid mesocarnivores (bobcat, Canada lynx, Lynx canadensis, and ocelot, Leopardus pardalis) with which the potential for competition with pumas over resources is also likely. Third, we assessed the extant spatial distribution of pumas, and quantified in what portion of their range they are the dominant predator versus a subordinate under other sympatric apex predators. Managers and scientists can then refer to this map as they consider hunting limits, conservation priorities, and research questions for the species.

Survey Methodology

Literature search

We searched for evidence of dominance and subordination among species interactions in the published literature. We conducted independent searches using Google Scholar and Web of Science, utilizing puma-species pairings and the following keywords: puma, puma concolor, cougar, mountain lion, jaguar, Panthera onca, bear, black bear, grizzly bear, Ursus, wolf, wolves, gray wolf, Canis lupus, coyote, C. latrans, ocelot, Leopardus pardalis, bobcat, Lynx rufus, maned wolf, Chrysocyon brachyurus, Canada lynx, L. canadensis. We are confident this approach captured most publications in English, but we are less sure what percent of Spanish publications were identified successfully.

Types of competition and awarding dominance

We sought evidence for outcomes from the following four types of interspecific interactions:

  • (1)

    Exploitation competition (EC) describes indirect interactions in which a dominant species superior at gathering, using, or acquiring a shared resource, reduces the availability of the shared resource for the subordinate species (Tilman, 1982). For example, if a subordinate species shifts its diet in the presence of a dominant, the change may be interpreted as exploitation competition (Hayward & Kerley, 2008).

  • (2)

    Interference competition (IC), defined as aggression or direct contests in which a dominant species blocks a subordinate species from a resource (Case & Gilpin, 1974). In natural systems, it can be difficult to interpret between exploitive and interference competition (Schoener, 1983). For example, pumas reduce their intake of elk (Cervus canadensis) in the presence of wolves (e.g., Kortello, Hurd & Murray, 2007), but it is unclear whether wolves are better at killing this prey or whether wolves are directly (chasing pumas) or indirectly blocking pumas from elk, by forcing elk to shift to habitats in which pumas hunt less often. IC may also elevate to physical contests in which the subordinate species is killed, described next.

  • (3)

    Interspecific competitive killing (CK), defined as a dominant species killing a subordinate species, but not eating them (Lourenço et al., 2013).

  • (4)

    Intraguild predation (IGP), defined as a dominant predator killing and consuming a subordinate predator (Holt & Polis, 1997; Lourenço et al., 2013).

Differentiating between these four categories, however, proved challenging. Schoener (1983) emphasized the difficulty in interpreting mechanisms of competition (EC vs. IC) from field data and acknowledged that both mechanisms may contribute to specific animal interactions and behaviors. Likewise, differentiating CK from IGP required knowledge on whether the prey (subordinate carnivore) was consumed in its entirety (Lourenço et al., 2013), and this information was lacking in most published papers we read. Therefore, we simplified the four categories to two: (1) non-lethal competition (NLC), inclusive of all forms of EC and IC; and (2) lethal competition (LC), inclusive of CK and IGP.

We tallied the number of publications that provided evidence for whether pumas were dominant or subordinate in interactions with each sympatric carnivore species, and awarded dominance to the species in the pairing with the most evidence (highest number of publications) supporting dominance.

Geographic analysis to determine where pumas are dominant versus subordinate

Based upon our results determining which species were dominant over pumas and which were subordinate, we assessed coarse spatial patterns across the extant range of pumas. We acquired distribution maps for pumas and the species to which they were subordinate from the International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) (Wildlife Conservation Society, 2008; IUCN, 2010; IUCN, 2015; IUCN, 2016; IUCN SSC Bear Specialist Group, IUCN and IBA, 2017). IUCN range maps are polygons for Geographic Information Systems (GIS) that represent extant ranges where a species is likely to occur, based upon known occurrences of the species, habitat preferences and subsequent modeling, and other expert knowledge of the species and its range (IUCN, 2017). We utilized the Union tool in ArcGIS 10.0 to create a single distribution for the dominant species, removing any redundancy where species overlapped. Then we used the Overlay tool to quantify in what proportion of a their range pumas are dominant versus subordinate to one or more competitors.

Results

Our initial searches in Web of Science and Google Scholar yielded 72 potential papers that in turn yielded another 15 scientific accounts (e.g., old graduate theses and state reports) for our review of competition between pumas and sympatric apex predators. In the end, we utilized a refined list of 64 distinct sources in our review; those that were discarded did not address competition between apex predators or were redundant publications of earlier findings. We found evidence that wolves dominated pumas in 78% (n = 18) of 23 sources, grizzly bears dominated pumas in 100% of 4 sources, black bears dominated pumas in 67% (n = 6) of 9 sources, jaguars dominated pumas in 60% (n = 15) of 25 sources, coyotes dominated pumas in 19% (n = 3) of 16 sources, and zero examples in which maned wolves dominated pumas (Table 1; Fig. 1). In contrast, we found evidence that pumas dominated wolves in 22% (n = 5) of 23 sources, black bears in 44% (n = 4) of 9 sources, coyotes in 81% (n = 13) of 16 sources, and maned wolves in 67% (n = 2) of 3 sources (Table 2; Fig. 1). We also found 10 sources that did not detect dominance in either species where jaguars and pumas were sympatric (Table 2).

Table 1. Puma subordination.

Evidence of puma subordination to sympatric apex carnivores, the number of sources (n) supporting the conjecture, and the citations of the sources.

Species pairing n NLC LC
Wolf dominant over puma 18 Puma spatial displacement: Riley, Nesslage & Maurer (2004), Kortello, Hurd & Murray (2007), Bartnick et al. (2013), Lendrum et al. (2014) Puma displacement at carcasses and kleptoparasitism of puma kills: USFW (1995), Ruth & Hornocker (1996), Kunkel et al. (1999), Hornocker Wildlife Institute (2001), Akenson, Akenson & Quigley (2005), Kortello, Hurd & Murray (2007), Ruth & Buotte (2007), Elbroch et al. (2017c), Puma prey switching due to wolves: Kortello, Hurd & Murray (2007), Bartnick et al. (2013), Elbroch et al. (2015) Direct killing of pumas: Schmidt & Gunson (1985), White & Boyd (1989), Boyd & Neal (1992), Kunkel et al. (1999), Jimenez et al. (2006), Kortello, Hurd & Murray (2007), Ruth (2004a), Ruth & Buotte (2007), Elbroch et al. (2015) Puma starvation attributed to competition: Ruth (2004a), Kortello, Hurd & Murray (2007) and Elbroch et al. (2015)
Grizzly bear dominant over puma 4 Puma displacement at carcasses and kleptoparasitism of puma kills: Murphy et al. (1998), Ruth & Buotte (2007); LM Elbroch & HB Quigley, 2018, unpublished data Direct killing of pumas: Ruth (2004b)
Black bear dominant over puma 6 Puma displacement at carcasses and kleptoparasitism of puma kills Murphy et al. (1998), Ruth (2004b), Allen et al. (2014a), Elbroch et al. (2014) and Allen et al. (2015) Direct killing of pumas: LM Elbroch & HB Quigley, 2018, unpublished data (black bear killed and consumed three kittens in same litter)
Jaguar dominant over puma 15 Spatial and temporal displacement of pumas: Schaller & Crawshaw Jr (1980), Emmons (1987), Scognamillo et al. (2003), Moreno, Kays & Samudio Jr (2006),De Azevedo (2008), Harmsen et al. (2009), Paviolo et al. (2009), Romero-Munoz et al. (2010), Sollmann et al. (2012), Palomares et al. (2016), Palomares et al. (2017) Change in puma diet when sympatric with jaguars: Moreno, Kays & Samudio Jr (2006) Direct killing of pumas: Crawsha Jr & Quigley (1984), Crawshaw & Quigley (1991), Harmsen et al. (2009), Ruth & Murphy (2010), De Oliveira & Pereira (2014)
Coyote dominant over puma 3 Puma displacement at carcasses and kleptoparasitism of puma kills: Harrison (1990), Stanford (2013) Direct killing of pumas: Logan & Sweanor (2001)

Notes.

NLC
non-lethal competition
LC
lethal competition

Figure 1. The apex predators of North and South America and their relative competitive relationship with pumas (E).

Figure 1

Bold arrows denote dominance, and point from the dominant species to the subordinate. Thin arrows denote some evidence to the contrary. (A) gray wolf (Canis lupus), (B) grizzly bear (Ursus arctos), (C) American black bear (Ursus americanus), (D) jaguar (Panthera unca), (E) puma (Puma concolor), (F) maned wolf (Chrysocyon brachyurus), (G) coyote (Canis latrans). Drawings by Mark Elbroch.

Table 2. Puma dominance and equality.

Evidence of (1) puma dominance over and (2) equality to sympatric apex carnivores, the number of sources (n) supporting the conjecture, and the citations of the sources.

Species pairing n NLC LC
Dominance
Puma dominant over wolf 5 Direct killing of wolves: Schmidt & Gunson (1985), Ruth (2004a), Mexican Wolf Blue Range Adaptive Management Oversight Committee and Interagency Field Team (2005), Jimenez et al. (2008); LM Elbroch & HB Quigley, 2018, unpublished data (1 subadult wolf killed and consumed by an adult female puma)
Puma dominant over grizzly bear 0
Puma dominant over black bear 4 Direct killing of black bears: Cunningham, Gustavson & Ballard (1999), Elbroch et al. (2014), Nichols (2017); LM Elbroch & HB Quigley, 2018, unpublished data (adult female puma kills and consumes black bear cub of the year)
Puma dominant over jaguar 0 -
Puma dominant over coyote 13 Spatial and temporal displacement of coyotes: Koehler & Hornocker (1991), Wang, Allen & Wilmers (2015), Mahoney (2017) Direct killing of coyotes Ackerman, Lindzey & Hemker (1984), Boyd & O’Gara (1985), Arjo (1998), Murphy et al. (1998), Koehler & Hornocker (1991), Atwood (2006), Ruth & Buotte (2007), De la Torre & De la Riva (2009), Knopff et al. (2010), Lowrey, Elbroch & Broberg (2016); LM Elbroch & HB Quigley, 2018, unpublished data (multiple pumas killing and consuming multiple coyotes)
Puma dominant over maned wolf 2 Direct killing of maned wolves: Mazzolli (2009), De Oliveira & Pereira (2014)
Equality
Jaguar and puma equal 10 Inferred because spatial/temporal activity patterns were similar: Taber et al. (1997), Scognamillo et al. (2003), Foster, Harmsen & Doncaster (2010), Davis, Kelly & Stauffer (2011), Foster et al. (2013), Hernández-SaintMartín (2013), Gutiérrez-González & López-González (2017) Inferred because diet patterns were dissimilar: Scognamillo et al. (2003), Novack et al. (2005), De Azevedo (2008), Hernández-SaintMartín (2015)
Maned wolf and puma equal 1 Inferred because spatial/temporal activity patterns were similar: Trolle et al. (2007)

Notes.

NLC
non-lethal competition
LC
lethal competition

We utilized 13 distinct sources to assess puma interactions with three large sympatric felid mesocarnivores and the literature suggested that pumas were dominant over all three species. We found three sources reporting pumas killing ocelots (Crawshaw, 1995; López-González & González-Romero, 1998; Martins, Quadros & Mazzolli, 2008), but two additional references that indicated pumas and ocelots may not influence each other (Davis, 2009; Massara et al., 2016). We only found one reference to pumas killing Canada lynx (Ruth & Murphy, 2010), and nothing further about puma-lynx competition. We found six papers documenting puma predation of bobcats (Ackerman, Lindzey & Hemker, 1984; Burton, Perez & Tovar, 2003; Hass, 2009; Koehler & Hornocker, 1991; Lopez-González, 1994; Lowrey, Elbroch & Broberg, 2016), and three references reporting spatial displacement of bobcats by pumas (Koehler & Hornocker, 1991; Burton, Perez & Tovar, 2003; Lewis et al., 2015).

We were unable to analyze whether weight ratios correlated with dominance because of complications due to extreme sexual dimorphism among pumas and other carnivores (and sources not always reporting the sex of the animals involved in interactions), and the fact that social carnivores should likely be analyzed as the combined weight of all members in an interaction (e.g., wolf pack); group size in interactions with social species was often lacking as well. Overall, the general pattern was suggestive that the size difference between pumas and other apex predators does correlate with dominance. For example, evidence that jaguars are dominant is strongest in areas where jaguars are large and weigh considerably more than pumas, but more ambiguous where the two species are more similar in size (López-González & González-Romero, 1998; Gutiérrez-González & López-González, 2017). Wolf packs also appear to have a clear advantage over individual pumas, but one-on-one, the outcome of competitive interactions between a wolf and a puma is less certain (Ruth & Murphy, 2010) or dependent upon differences in age (e.g., wolves killing puma kittens; Elbroch et al., 2015).

Geographic analysis to determine where pumas are dominant versus subordinate

Pumas are subordinate to another apex carnivore in 10,799,252 (47.5%) of their 22,735,268 km2 range (International Union for Conservation of Nature, 2015) across North and South America (Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Extant puma range.

Figure 2

The extant range of pumas in North and South America. The light green denotes the portion of puma range where they are the only or dominant apex predator, and the orange denotes the portion of puma range where they are subordinate to at minimum one other apex predator. Source: ESRI, DigitalGlobe, GeoEye, Earthstar Geographics, CNES/Airbus DS, USDA, USGS, AE, Getmapping, Aerogrid, IGN, IGP, swisstopo, CIS User Community.

Discussion

Competition among carnivores in multi-species guilds is complex and difficult to disentangle (Schoener, 1983; Donadio & Buskirk, 2006; Lourenço et al., 2013). Our review provided strong evidence that pumas are both subordinate to some apex carnivores with which they are sympatric, but dominant over others. Lethal outcomes (LC) certainly provided compelling evidence of competition and subordination, but we did not rank them above NLC as non-lethal indirect effects can equally impact fitness (Ritchie & Johnson, 2009). Subordinate pumas switched their habitat use, suffered displacement at food sources, likely experienced increased energetic demands from harassment, exhibited increased starvation, and were sometimes directly killed in competitive interactions with dominant competitors. In short, pumas suffered costs when sympatric with carnivores to which they are subordinate. Thus, we suggest managers and researchers consider the potentially-additive or synergistic effects of competition and lethal management on puma population dynamics.

We found strong evidence that gray wolves are dominant over pumas, likely due to their social structure and the fact that they often have the numerical advantage over pumas (Ruth & Murphy, 2010). The effects of wolves on pumas appear numerous. Pumas exhibit habitat (Riley, Nesslage & Maurer, 2004) and dietary shifts (Elbroch et al., 2015) in the presence of wolves, and experience increased starvation as wolves re-establish in systems in which they were absent for some time (e.g., Kortello, Hurd & Murray, 2007; Elbroch et al., 2015). Wolves also harass pumas, the energy expense of which is approximately five times greater than the energy expenditure of normal hunting (Bryce, Wilmers & Williams, 2017), and kleptoparatize and displace pumas from their kills (Bartnick et al., 2013; Elbroch et al., 2017c). Wolves also directly kill all age classes of pumas (e.g., Boyd & Neal, 1992; Ruth, 2004a; Elbroch et al., 2015), even while pumas too occasionally kill wolves (e.g., Jimenez et al., 2008). Yet even with so much suggestive evidence amassing from multiple studies, we still lack for research that clearly links competition with wolves to puma fitness (e.g., survivorship, fecundity).

The influence of grizzly and black bears on pumas is less studied, but appears to be less severe than that of wolves due to the seasonal dormancy of bears that provide pumas a temporal reprieve from competition with these species. The most significant competition between bears and pumas documented in the literature is that bears displace pumas from their kills, costing pumas energetic calories in food lost and the additional efforts of hunting to procure more prey. If bears displace pumas from their kills often enough, pumas may increase their kill rates and influence other trophic levels (Elbroch et al., 2014). Research directly linking bear kleptoparatism to puma fitness, however, is lacking. For example, kleptoparatism of leopard (Panthera pardus) kills negatively impacts leopard reproductive success (e.g., Balme et al., 2017). At this time, we cannot currently predict whether there exists an energetic threshold at which point bear kleptoparatism impacts puma fitness, or say definitively that it does. Bears also occasionally kill puma kittens (Ruth, 2004b; LM Elbroch & HB Quigley, 2018, unpublished data), and rarely adult pumas (M Jorgensen, pers. comm., 2007). Pumas also occasionally kill black bears (e.g., Elbroch et al., 2014; Nichols, 2017; LM Elbroch & HB Quigley, 2018, unpublished data).

Jaguar-puma interactions were the most frequently studied competitive interaction among apex predators in the Americas. In contrast to wolf-puma research which has been conducted with marked individuals, telemetry, and GPS technology, most jaguar-puma research has been conducted with non-invasive camera trapping assessing spatial and temporal time-sharing of habitats, and to a lesser extent, dietary overlap through scat analysis. Such studies offer insights into competitive interactions and mechanisms of coexistence, however, this difference in methods likely accounts for the large number of publications in which researchers report that they did not detect any competition between jaguars and pumas (Table 2). Whereas some authors were certain they found evidence that jaguars were dominant over pumas (e.g., Harmsen et al., 2009), other researchers, from areas where jaguars are smaller and sometimes numerically fewer than pumas, suggested the two species may be at least equal in competitive interactions, or even that pumas may be dominant over jaguars (e.g., Gutiérrez-González & López-González, 2017). We did discover five references that report incidents of jaguars killing pumas (e.g., Crawsha Jr & Quigley, 1984; Harmsen et al., 2009), whereas we did not find a single documentation of the reverse.

Most research we uncovered suggested that pumas were dominant over coyotes, maned wolves, and felid mesocarnivores. This is not unexpected, given that pumas outweigh these species by a large margin (Donadio & Buskirk, 2006; De Oliveira & Pereira, 2014). Packs of coyotes, however, do occasionally push pumas from their kills (Harrison, 1990), as well as harass and kill puma kittens (Logan & Sweanor, 2001; Stanford, 2013). Nevertheless, there is very little research on the outcomes of competition between pumas and smaller carnivores.

Humans, of course, are also dominant over pumas. Human hunting negatively impacts puma population dynamics, as well as puma dispersal and metapopulation dynamics (Cooley et al., 2009; Stoner et al., 2013). Humans are generally the leading cause of death for pumas in non-hunted populations as well (Thompson, Jenks & , 2014; Vickers et al., 2015). At finer scales, pumas shift their activity patterns and habitat use to avoid humans and human infrastructure (Sweanor et al., 2008; Wilmers et al., 2013), as well as abandon food sources when humans are detected (Smith, Wang & Wilmers, 2015).

In the absence of human hunting, pumas self-regulate (Seidensticker et al., 1973; Wallach et al., 2015), and exhibit the complex social structures (Wallach et al., 2015; Elbroch et al., 2017b) and hunting strategies (Carbone, Teacher & Rowcliffe, 2007) that identify them as apex predators. We are, however, clearly lacking research to help us understand how pumas cope with the stressors of competition with dominant, apex carnivores. Further, we lack research that assesses the influence of human non-lethal and lethal effects on pumas simultaneous with the negative effects of competition with other sympatric carnivores. Apex predators did not evolve like prey species to contend with top-down selection pressures (Darimont et al., 2015), and therefore human-caused mortalities may be additive to other natural causes. Hunting may also influence the outcomes of competition scenarios among sympatric carnivores through the removal of key individuals (sensu Bischof et al., 2017). Perhaps pumas are better adapted to handle human hunting because they are already subordinate to other species. It is also possible, however, that the combination of competition with dominant carnivores combined with human harvest will lead to rapid puma declines in some populations.

Conclusions

Managing species in complex systems is difficult, especially with cryptic species like pumas that are laborious and expensive to monitor in order to track population dynamics. Elucidating the direct and indirect effects of competition in species interactions in natural, multi-species systems is equally challenging, and a topic very much pursued in current ecology (e.g., Dröge et al., 2017; Karanth et al., 2017). Our review, if nothing else, highlights how little we know about the competitive interactions between pumas and other apex carnivores, or more generally, the costs accrued by apex predators during competitive interactions with dominant species. Therefore we encourage further research into competition among apex predators, as well as research assessing the effects of competition in managed systems where pumas, and perhaps other predators, are hunted as well. Until the time that we understand whether competitive effects are additive with human management, or even potentially synergistic resulting in unexpected, disproportionate declines in subordinate predators, we encourage caution among managers responsible for determining harvest limits for pumas and other subordinate, apex carnivores in areas where they are sympatric with dominant species. Our review, for example, suggested that pumas are negatively impacted by wolves more than grizzly bears. This is important information for managers working in regions where wolves and brown bears are recolonizing and recovering, and historic competition scenarios among multiple apex predators are being realized.

Acknowledgments

Thank you to our team members studying pumas in the southern Yellowstone Ecosystem for lively discussions relevant to this paper. In particular, we thank Jennifer Feltner for her assistance gathering references in the early stages of this research.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by The Summerlee Foundation, National Science Foundation (graduate fellowship), the Community Foundation of Jackson Hole, Pace University, EcoTour Adventures, PC Fund for Animals Charitable Trust, The Tapeats Fund, The Lee and Juliet Folger Fund, L. Westbrook, the Scully Family, R. and L. Haberfeld, Hogan LLC, L. and R. Holder, S. and L. Robertson, R. and L. Heskett, F. and B. Burgess, J. Morgan, A. Smith, D. Bainbridge, T. Thomas and many additional donors that contributed through the Community Foundation of Jackson Hole’s Old Bill’s Fun Run, many of which were anonymous. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Additional Information and Declarations

Competing Interests

Mark Elbroch is an employee of Panthera. Anna Kusler is a graduate student researcher with Panthera.

Author Contributions

L. Mark Elbroch conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Anna Kusler performed the experiments, analyzed the data, wrote the paper, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Data Availability

The following information was supplied regarding data availability:

This is a review article, and the resources exist in the public domain.

References

  • Ackerman, Lindzey & Hemker (1984).Ackerman BB, Lindzey FG, Hemker TP. Cougar food habits in southern Utah. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1984;48:147–155. doi: 10.2307/3808462. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Akenson, Akenson & Quigley (2005).Akenson J, Akenson H, Quigley H. Effects of wolf reintroduction on a couar population in the central Idaho wilderness. In: Beausoleil RA, Martorello DA, editors. Proceedings of the 8th mountain lion workshop. Washington Department of Fish and Wildlife; Olympia: 2005. pp. 177–187. [Google Scholar]
  • Allen et al. (2014a).Allen ML, Elbroch LM, Casady DS, Wittmer HU. Seasonal variation in the feeding ecology of pumas (Puma concolor) in northern California. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 2014a;92:397–403. doi: 10.1139/cjz-2013-0284. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Allen et al. (2015).Allen ML, Elbroch LM, Wilmers CC, Wittmer HU. The comparative effects of large carnivores on the acquisition of carrion by scavengers. American Naturalist. 2015;185:822–833. doi: 10.1086/681004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Allen et al. (2016).Allen ML, Wilmers CC, Elbroch LM, Golla JM, Wittmer HU. The importance of motivation, weapons and foul odors in driving encounter competition in carnivores. Ecology. 2016;97:1905–1912. doi: 10.1002/ecy.1462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Arjo (1998).Arjo WM. D Phil. Thesis. 1998. The effects of recolonizing wolves on coyote populations, movements, behaviors, and food habits. [Google Scholar]
  • Arjo & Pletscher (1999).Arjo WM, Pletscher DH. Behavioral responses of coyotes to wolf recolonization in northwestern Montana. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1999;77:1919–1927. doi: 10.1139/z99-177. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Arnott & Elwood (2008).Arnott G, Elwood RW. Information gathering and decision making about resource value in animal contests. Animal Behaviour. 2008;76:529–542. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Atwood (2006).Atwood TC. D Phil. Thesis. 2006. Wolves, coyotes, elk, and mule deer: predator–prey behavioral ecology in southwest Montana. [Google Scholar]
  • Balme et al. (2017).Balme GA, Robinson HS, Pitman RT, Hunter LT. Caching reduces kleptoparasitism in a solitary, large felid. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2017;86:634–644. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bartnick et al. (2013).Bartnick TD, Van Deelen TR, Quigley HB, Craighead D. Variation in cougar (Puma concolor) predation habits during wolf (Canis lupus) recovery in the southern Greater Yellowstone Ecosystem. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 2013;91:82–93. doi: 10.1139/cjz-2012-0147. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Bekoff, Daniels & Gittleman (1984).Bekoff M, Daniels TJ, Gittleman JL. Life history patterns and the comparative social ecology of carnivores. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics. 1984;15:191–232. doi: 10.1146/annurev.es.15.110184.001203. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Bischof et al. (2017).Bischof R, Bonenfant C, Rivrud IM, Zedrosser A, Friebe A, Coulson T, Mysterud A, Swenson JE. Regulated hunting re-shapes the life history of brown bears. Nature Ecology & Evolution. 2017;2:116–123. doi: 10.1038/s41559-017-0400-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Boyd & Neal (1992).Boyd DK, Neal GK. An adult Cougar, Felis concolor, killed by Gray Wolves, Canis lupus, in Glacier National Park, Montana. Canadian Field-Naturalist. 1992;106:524–525. [Google Scholar]
  • Boyd & O’Gara (1985).Boyd D, O’Gara B. Cougar predation on coyotes. Murrelet. 1985;66:17. [Google Scholar]
  • Bryce, Wilmers & Williams (2017).Bryce CM, Wilmers CC, Williams TM. Energetics and evasion dynamics of large predators and prey: pumas vs. hounds. PeerJ. 2017;5:e3701. doi: 10.7717/peerj.3701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Burton, Perez & Tovar (2003).Burton AM, Navarro Perez S, Tovar CC. Bobcat ranging behavior in relation to small mammal abundance on Colima Volcano, Mexico. Anales Del Instituto de Biología. Serie Zoología. 2003;74:67–82. [Google Scholar]
  • Carbone, Teacher & Rowcliffe (2007).Carbone C, Teacher A, Rowcliffe JM. The costs of carnivory. PLOS Biology. 2007;5:e22. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Case & Gilpin (1974).Case TJ, Gilpin ME. Interference competition and niche theory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1974;71:3073–3077. doi: 10.1073/pnas.71.8.3073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cooley et al. (2009).Cooley HS, Wielgus RB, Koehler GM, Robinson HS, Maletzke BT. Does hunting regulate cougar populations? A test of the compensatory mortality hypothesis. Ecology. 2009;90:2913–2921. doi: 10.1890/08-1805.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Crawsha Jr & Quigley (1984).Crawsha Jr PG, Quigley HB. Relatório Final. Instituto Brasileiro de Desenvolvimento Florestal; Brasília: 1984. A ecologia do jaguar ou onça pintada no Pantanal; p. 112. [Google Scholar]
  • Crawshaw (1995).Crawshaw PG. D Phil. Thesis. 1995. Comparative ecology of ocelot Felis pardalis and jaguar Panthera onca in a protected subtropical forest in Brazil and Argentina. [Google Scholar]
  • Crawshaw & Quigley (1991).Crawshaw PG, Quigley HB. Jaguar spacing, activity and habitat use in a seasonally flooded environment in Brazil. Journal of Zoology. 1991;223:357–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1991.tb04770.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Crombie (1947).Crombie AC. Interspecific competition. Journal of Animal Ecology. 1947;16:44–73. doi: 10.2307/1506. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Cunningham, Gustavson & Ballard (1999).Cunningham SC, Gustavson CR, Ballard WB. Diet selection of mountain lions in southeastern Arizona. Journal of Range Management. 1999;52:202–207. doi: 10.2307/4003681. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Darimont et al. (2015).Darimont CT, Fox CH, Bryan HM, Reimchen TE. The unique ecology of human predators. Science. 2015;349:858–860. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Davis (2009).Davis ML. MS Thesis. 2009. Densities, habitat-use, and mesopredator release of the ocelot in Belize. [Google Scholar]
  • Davis, Kelly & Stauffer (2011).Davis ML, Kelly MJ, Stauffer DF. Carnivore co-existence and habitat use in the Mountain Pine Ridge Forest Reserve, Belize. Animal Conservation. 2011;14:56–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-1795.2010.00389.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • De Azevedo (2008).De Azevedo FCC. Food habits and livestock depredation of sympatric jaguars and pumas in the Iguacu National Park area, south Brazil. Biotropica. 2008;40:494–500. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2008.00404.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • De la Torre & De la Riva (2009).De la Torre JA, De la Riva G. Food habits of pumas (Puma concolor) in a semiarid region of central Mexico. Mastozoología Neotropical. 2009;16:211–216. [Google Scholar]
  • De Oliveira & Pereira (2014).De Oliveira TG, Pereira JA. Intraguild predation and interspecific killing as structuring forces of carnivoran communities in South America. Journal of Mammalian Evolution. 2014;21:427–436. [Google Scholar]
  • Donadio & Buskirk (2006).Donadio E, Buskirk SW. Diet, morphology, and interspecific killing in Carnivora. American Naturalist. 2006;167:524–536. doi: 10.1086/501033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dröge et al. (2017).Dröge E, Creel S, Becker MS, M’soka J. Spatial and temporal avoidance of risk within a large carnivore guild. Ecology and Evolution. 2017;7:189–199. doi: 10.1002/ece3.2616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Durant (1998).Durant SM. Competition refuges and coexistence: an example from Serengeti carnivores. Journal of Animal Ecology. 1998;67:370–386. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2656.1998.00202.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Elbroch, Feltner & Quigley (2017).Elbroch LM, Feltner J, Quigley H. Human-carnivore competition for antlered ungulates: do pumas select for bulls and bucks? Wildlife Research. 2017;44:523–533. doi: 10.1071/WR17006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Elbroch et al. (2014).Elbroch LM, Lendrum PE, Allen ML, Wittmer HU. Nowhere to hide: pumas, black bears, and competition refuges. Behavioral Ecology. 2014;26(1):247–254. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Elbroch et al. (2015).Elbroch LM, Lendrum PE, Newby J, Quigley H, Thompson DJ. Recolonizing wolves impact the realized niche of resident cougars. Zoological Studies. 2015;54 doi: 10.1186/s40555-015-0122-y. Article 41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Elbroch et al. (2017b).Elbroch LM, Levy M, Lubell M, Quigley H, Caragiulo A. Adaptive social behaviors in a solitary carnivore. Science Advances. 2017b;3:e1701218. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1701218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Elbroch et al. (2017c).Elbroch LM, López-González C, Fitzgerald F, Kusler A, Quigley H. Attraction–repulsion among top predators following reintroduction efforts. Mammalian Biology. 2017c;86:66–69. doi: 10.1016/j.mambio.2017.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Elbroch et al. (2017a).Elbroch LM, O’Malley C, Peziol M, Quigley HB. Vertebrate diversity benefiting from carrion provided by pumas and other subordinate, apex felids. Biological Conservation. 2017a;215:123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2017.08.026. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Emmons (1987).Emmons LH. Comparative feeding ecology of felids in a Neotropical rainforest. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1987;20:271–283. doi: 10.1007/BF00292180. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Foster, Harmsen & Doncaster (2010).Foster RJ, Harmsen BJ, Doncaster CP. Habitat use by sympatric jaguars and pumas across a gradient of human disturbance in Belize. Biotropica. 2010;42:724–731. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2010.00641.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Foster et al. (2013).Foster VC, Sarmento P, Sollmann R, Tôrres N, Jácomo AT, Negrões N, Fonseca C, Silveira L. Jaguar and puma activity patterns and predator–prey interactions in four Brazilian biomes. Biotropica. 2013;45:373–379. doi: 10.1111/btp.12021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Gutiérrez-González & López-González (2017).Gutiérrez-González CE, López-González CA. Jaguar interactions with pumas and prey at the northern edge of jaguars’ range. PeerJ. 2017;5:e2886. doi: 10.7717/peerj.2886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harihar, Pandav & Goyal (2011).Harihar A, Pandav B, Goyal SP. Responses of leopard Panthera pardus to the recovery of a tiger Panthera tigris population. Journal of Applied Ecology. 2011;48:806–814. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2664.2011.01981.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Harmsen et al. (2009).Harmsen BJ, Foster RJ, Silver SC, Ostro LE, Doncaster CP. Spatial and temporal interactions of sympatric jaguars (Panthera onca) and pumas (Puma concolor) in a neotropical forest. Journal of Mammalogy. 2009;90:612–620. doi: 10.1644/08-MAMM-A-140R.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Harrison (1990).Harrison S. Cougar predation on bighorn sheep in the Junction Wildlife Management Area, British Columbia. 1990. MS Thesis, University of British Columbia.
  • Hass (2009).Hass CC. Competition and coexistence in sympatric bobcats and pumas. Journal of Zoology. 2009;278:174–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2009.00565.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Hayward & Kerley (2008).Hayward MW, Kerley GI. Prey preferences and dietary overlap amongst Africa’s large predators. South African Journal of Wildlife Research. 2008;38:93–108. doi: 10.3957/0379-4369-38.2.93. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Hernández-SaintMartín (2013).Hernández-SaintMartín AD, Rosas-Rosas OC, Palacio-Núñez J, Tarango-Arambula LA, Clemente-Sánchez F, Hoogesteijn AL. Activity patterns of jaguar, puma and their potential prey in San Luis Potosi, Mexico. Acta Zoológica Mexicana. 2013;29:520–533. [Google Scholar]
  • Hernández-SaintMartín (2015).Hernández-SaintMartín AD, Rosas-Rosas OC, Palacio-Núñez J, Tarango-Arambula LA, Clemente-Sánchez F, Hoogesteijn AL. Food habits of jaguar and puma in a protected area and adjacent fragmented landscape of Northeastern Mexico. Natural Areas Journal. 2015;35:308–317. doi: 10.3375/043.035.0213. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Holt & Polis (1997).Holt RD, Polis GA. A theoretical framework for intraguild predation. American Naturalist. 1997;149:745–764. doi: 10.1086/286018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Hornocker Wildlife Institute (2001).Hornocker Wildlife Institute Bozeman, MontanaCougar-wolf interactions in Yellowstone National Park: competition, demographics and spatial relationships. Cumulative annual technical report. 2001
  • Hutchinson (1957).Hutchinson GE. Concluding remarks. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology. 1957;22:415–427. doi: 10.1101/SQB.1957.022.01.039. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • IUCN (2010).International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) Canis lupus. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2010. [06 Jue 2017]. Version 2017-2. http://www.iucnredlist.org .
  • IUCN (2015).International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) Puma concolor. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2015. [11 December 2016]. Version 2016-3. http://www.iucnredlist.org .
  • IUCN (2016).International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) Ursus americanus. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2016. [06 Jue 2017]. Version 2017-2. http://www.iucnredlist.org .
  • IUCN (2017).International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) IUCN Spatial Data Resources. 2017. http://www.iucnredlist.org/technical-documents/red-list-training/iucnspatialresources. [18 December 2017]. http://www.iucnredlist.org/technical-documents/red-list-training/iucnspatialresources
  • IUCN SSC Bear Specialist Group, IUCN and IBA (2017).International Union for Conservation of Nature SSC Bear Specialist Group, IUCN and IBA Ursus arctos. 2017. [6 June 2017]. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2017-2. http://www.iucnredlist.org .
  • Jimenez et al. (2008).Jimenez MD, Asher VJ, Bergman C, Bangs EE, Woodruff SP. Gray Wolves, Canis lupus, Killed by Cougars, Puma concolor, and a Grizzly Bear, Ursus arctos, in Montana, Alberta, and Wyoming. The Canadian Field-Naturalist. 2008;122(1):76–78. doi: 10.22621/cfn.v122i1.550. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Jimenez et al. (2006).Jimenez MD, Woodruff SP, Cain S, Dewey S. Wolf-elk interactions on winter range and state-managed feed grounds in Wyoming. US Fish and Wildlife Service, JacksonProgress report. 2006
  • Karanth et al. (2017).Karanth KU, Srivathsa A, Vasudev D, Puri M, Parameshwaran R, Kumar NS. Spatio-temporal interactions facilitate large carnivore sympatry across a resource gradient. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 2017;284 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.1860. Article 2016186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kleiman & Eisenberg (1973).Kleiman DG, Eisenberg JF. Comparisons of canid and felid social systems from an evolutionary perspective. Animal Behaviour. 1973;21:637–659. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(73)80088-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Knopff et al. (2010).Knopff KH, Knopff AA, Kortello A, Boyce MS. Cougar kill rate and prey composition in a multiprey system. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2010;74:1435–1447. doi: 10.1111/j.1937-2817.2010.tb01270.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Koehler & Hornocker (1991).Koehler GM, Hornocker MG. Seasonal resource use among mountain lions, bobcats, and coyotes. Journal of Mammalogy. 1991;72:391–396. doi: 10.2307/1382112. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Kortello, Hurd & Murray (2007).Kortello AD, Hurd TE, Murray DL. Interactions between cougars (Puma concolor) and gray wolves (Canis lupus) in Banff National Park, Alberta. Ecoscience. 2007;14:214–222. doi: 10.2980/1195-6860(2007)14[214:IBCPCA]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Krebs et al. (2004).Krebs J, Lofroth EC, Copeland J, Banci V, Cooley D, Golden H, Magoun A, Mulders R, Shults B. Synthesis of survival rates and causes of mortality in North American wolverines. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2004;68:3493–3502. [Google Scholar]
  • Kunkel et al. (1999).Kunkel KE, Ruth TK, Pletscher DH, Hornocker MG. Winter prey selection by wolves and cougars in and near Glacier National Park, Montana. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1999;63:901–910. doi: 10.2307/3802804. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Le Bourlot, Tully & Claessen (2014).Le Bourlot V, Tully T, Claessen D. Interference versus exploitative competition in the regulation of size-structured populations. American Naturalist. 2014;184:609–623. doi: 10.1086/678083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lendrum et al. (2014).Lendrum PE, Elbroch LM, Quigley H, Thompson DJ, Jimenez M, Craighead D. Home range characteristics of a subordinate predator: selection for refugia or hunt opportunity? Journal of Zoology. 2014;294:58–66. doi: 10.1111/jzo.12153. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Lewis et al. (2015).Lewis JS, Bailey LL, VandeWoude S, Crooks KR. Interspecific interactions between wild felids vary across scales and levels of urbanization. Ecology and Evolution. 2015;5:5946–5961. doi: 10.1002/ece3.1812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Logan & Sweanor (2001).Logan KA, Sweanor LL. Desert puma: evolutionary ecology and conservation of an enduring carnivore. Island Press; Washington, D.C.: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • Lopez-González (1994).Lopez-González CA. MS Thesis. 1994. Ecologia y comportamiento del puma (Puma concolor) en un habitat fragmentado. [Google Scholar]
  • López-González & González-Romero (1998).López-González C, González-Romero G. A synthesis of current literature and knowledge about the ecology of the puma (Puma concolor) Acta Zoologica Mexicana. 1998;75:171–190. [Google Scholar]
  • Lourenço et al. (2013).Lourenço R, Penteriani V, Rabaça JE, Korpimäki E. Lethal interactions among vertebrate top predators: a review of concepts, assumptions and terminology. Biological Reviews. 2013;89:270–283. doi: 10.1111/brv.12054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lowrey, Elbroch & Broberg (2016).Lowrey B, Elbroch LM, Broberg L. Is individual prey selection driven by chance or choice? A case study in cougars (Puma concolor) Mammal Research. 2016;61:353–359. doi: 10.1007/s13364-016-0292-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Mahoney (2017).Mahoney PJ. D Phil. Thesis. 2017. Spatial ecology of coyotes and cougars: understanding the influence of multiple prey on the spatial interactions of two predators. [Google Scholar]
  • Martins, Quadros & Mazzolli (2008).Martins R, Quadros J, Mazzolli M. Habito alimentar e interferência antrópica na atividade de marcação territorial do Puma concolor e Leopardus pardalis (Carnívora, Felidae) e outros carnívoros na Estação Ecológica de Juréia-Itatins, São Paulo, Brasil. Revista Brasileira de Zoologia. 2008;25:427–435. doi: 10.1590/S0101-81752008000300007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Massara et al. (2016).Massara RL, Paschoal AM, Bailey LL, Doherty PF, Chiarello AG. Ecological interactions between ocelots and sympatric mesocarnivores in protected areas of the Atlantic Forest, southeastern Brazil. Journal of Mammalogy. 2016;97:1634–1644. doi: 10.1093/jmammal/gyw129. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Mattson (2014).Mattson DJ. State-level management of a common charismatic predator: mountain lions of the west. In: Clark SG, Rutherford MB, editors. Large carnivore conservation: integrating science and policy in North America. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 2014. pp. 29–64. [Google Scholar]
  • Mazzolli (2009).Mazzolli M. Mountain lion Puma concolor attacks on a maned wolf Chrysocyon brachyurus and a domestic dog in a forestry system. Mastozoología Neotropical. 2009;16:465–470. [Google Scholar]
  • Mexican Wolf Blue Range Adaptive Management Oversight Committee and Interagency Field Team (2005).Mexican Wolf Blue Range Adaptive Management Oversight Committee and Interagency Field Team . Mexican Wolf Blue Range Reintroduction Project, 5-Year Review. US Fish and Wildlife Service; Albuquerque: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • Moreno, Kays & Samudio Jr (2006).Moreno RS, Kays RW, Samudio Jr R. Competitive release in diets of ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) and puma (Puma concolor) after jaguar (Panthera onca) decline. Journal of Mammalogy. 2006;87:808–816. doi: 10.1644/05-MAMM-A-360R2.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Murphy et al. (1998).Murphy KM, Felzien GS, Hornocker MG, Ruth TK. Encounter competition between bears and cougars: some ecological implications. Ursus. 1998;1:55–60. [Google Scholar]
  • Murphy & MacDonald (2010).Murphy T, MacDonald DW. Pumas and people: lessons in the landscape of tolerance from a widely distributed felid. In: MacDonald DW, Loveridge AJ, editors. Biology and conservation of wild felids. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2010. pp. 431–452. [Google Scholar]
  • Nichols (2017).Nichols B. MS Thesis. 2017. Foraging ecology of mountain lions in the Sierra National Forest California. [Google Scholar]
  • Novack et al. (2005).Novack AJ, Main MB, Sunquist ME, Labisky RF. Foraging ecology of jaguar (Panthera onca) and puma (Puma concolor) in hunted and non-hunted sites within the Maya Biosphere Reserve, Guatemala. Journal of Zoology. 2005;267:167–178. doi: 10.1017/S0952836905007338. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Ordiz, Bischof & Swenson (2013).Ordiz A, Bischof R, Swenson JE. Saving large carnivores, but losing the apex predator? Biological Conservation. 2013;168:128–133. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2013.09.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Palomares et al. (2017).Palomares F, Adrados B, Zanin M, Silveira L, Keller C. A non-invasive faecal survey for the study of spatial ecology and kinship of solitary felids in the Viruá National Park, Amazon Basin. Mammal Research. 2017;62:241–249. doi: 10.1007/s13364-017-0311-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Palomares et al. (2016).Palomares F, Fernández N, Roques S, Chávez C, Silveira L, Keller C, Adrados B. Fine-scale habitat segregation between two ecologically similar top predators. PLOS ONE. 2016;11:e0155626. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0155626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Parker & Stuart (1976).Parker GA, Stuart RA. Animal behavior as a strategy optimizer: evolution of resource assessment strategies and optimal emigration thresholds. American Naturalist. 1976;110:1055–1076. doi: 10.1086/283126. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Paviolo et al. (2009).Paviolo A, Blanco YE, De Angelo CD, Di Bitetti MS. Protection affects the abundance and activity patterns of pumas in the Atlantic Forest. Journal of Mammalogy. 2009;90:926–934. doi: 10.1644/08-MAMM-A-128.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Polis & Holt (1992).Polis GA, Holt RD. Intraguild predation: the dynamics of complex trophic interactions. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 1992;7:151–155. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(92)90208-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Riley, Nesslage & Maurer (2004).Riley SJ, Nesslage GM, Maurer BA. Dynamics of early wolf and cougar eradication efforts in Montana: implications for conservation. Biological Conservation. 2004;119:575–579. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2004.01.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Ripple et al. (2014).Ripple WJ, Estes JA, Beschta RL, Wilmers CC, Ritchie EG, Hebblewhite M, Berger J, Elmhagen B, Letnic M, Nelson MP, Schmitz OJ, Smith DW, Wallach AD, Wirsing AJ. Status and ecological effects of the world’s largest carnivores. Science. 2014;343 doi: 10.1126/science.1241484. Article 1241484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ritchie & Johnson (2009).Ritchie EG, Johnson CN. Predator interactions, mesopredator release and biodiversity conservation. Ecology Letters. 2009;12:982–998. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01347.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Romero-Munoz et al. (2010).Romero-Munoz A, Maffei L, Cuéllar E, Noss AJ. Temporal separation between jaguar and puma in the dry forests of southern Bolivia. Journal of Tropical Ecology. 2010;26:303–311. doi: 10.1017/S0266467410000052. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Ruth (2004a).Ruth TK. Ghost of the Rockies: the Yellowstone cougar project. Yellowstone Science. 2004a;12:13–24. [Google Scholar]
  • Ruth (2004b).Ruth TS. D Phil. Thesis. 2004b. Patterns of resource use among cougars and wolves in northwestern Montana and southeastern British Columbia. [Google Scholar]
  • Ruth & Buotte (2007).Ruth TK, Buotte PC. Hornocker Wildlife Institute, BozemanCougar ecology and cougar-carnivore interactions in Yellowstone National Park. Final technical report. 2007
  • Ruth & Hornocker (1996).Ruth TK, Hornocker MG. Mountain Lion Workshop. California Department of Fish and Game; Sacramento: 1996. Interactions between cougars and wolves (and a bear or two) in the North Fork of the Flathead River, Montana. [Google Scholar]
  • Ruth & Murphy (2010).Ruth TK, Murphy K. Competition with other carnivores for prey. In: Hornocker M, Negri S, editors. Cougar: ecology and conservation. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 2010. pp. 138–162. [Google Scholar]
  • Sandell (1989).Sandell M. The mating tactics and spacing patterns of solitary carnivores. In: Gittleman JL, editor. Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution. Cornell University Press; Ithaca: 1989. pp. 164–182. [Google Scholar]
  • Scantlebury et al. (2014).Scantlebury DM, Mills MG, Wilson RP, Wilson JW, Mills ME, Durant SM, Bennett NC, Bradford P, Marks NJ, Speakman JR. Flexible energetics of cheetah hunting strategies provide resistance against kleptoparasitism. Science. 2014;346:79–81. doi: 10.1126/science.1256424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schaller & Crawshaw Jr (1980).Schaller GB, Crawshaw Jr PG. Movement patterns of jaguar. Biotropica. 1980;12:161–168. doi: 10.2307/2387967. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Schmidt & Gunson (1985).Schmidt KP, Gunson JR. Evaluation of wolf-ungulate predation near Nordegg, Alberta. Alberta Energy and Natural Resources, Fish and Wildlife Division, EdmontonSecond Year Progress Report, 1984–85. 1985
  • Schoener (1983).Schoener TW. Field experiments on interspecific competition. American Naturalist. 1983;122:240–285. doi: 10.1086/284133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Scognamillo et al. (2003).Scognamillo D, Maxit IE, Sunquist M, Polisar J. Coexistence of jaguar (Panthera onca) and puma (Puma concolor) in a mosaic landscape in the Venezuelan llanos. Journal of Zoology. 2003;259:269–279. doi: 10.1017/S0952836902003230. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Seidensticker et al. (1973).Seidensticker JCJ, Hornocker MG, Wiles WV, Messick JP. Mountain lion social organization in the Idaho primitive area. Wildlife Monographs. 1973;35:1–60. [Google Scholar]
  • Smith, Wang & Wilmers (2015).Smith JA, Wang Y, Wilmers CC. Top carnivores increase their kill rates on prey as a response to human-induced fear. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2015;282 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2711. Article 20142711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sollmann et al. (2012).Sollmann R, Furtado MM, Hofer H, Jacomo AT, Tôrres NM, Silveira L. Using occupancy models to investigate space partitioning between two sympatric large predators, the jaguar and puma in central Brazil. Mammalian Biology. 2012;77:41–46. doi: 10.1016/j.mambio.2011.06.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Stanford (2013).Stanford J. Coyotes tame lions on refuge. JHunderground [Blog Post] 2013. http://www.jhunderground.com/2013/03/31/coyotes-tame-lions-on-refuge/ http://www.jhunderground.com/2013/03/31/coyotes-tame-lions-on-refuge/
  • Stoner et al. (2013).Stoner DC, Wolfe ML, Rieth WR, Bunnell KD, Durham SL, Stoner LL. De facto refugia, ecological traps and the biogeography of anthropogenic cougar mortality in Utah. Diversity and Distributions. 2013;19:1114–1124. doi: 10.1111/ddi.12035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Sweanor et al. (2008).Sweanor LL, Logan KA, Bauer JW, Millsap B, Boyce WM. Puma–human relationships in Cuyamaca Rancho State Park California. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2008;72:1076–1084. doi: 10.2193/2007-024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Taber et al. (1997).Taber AB, Novaro AJ, Neris N, Colman FH. The food habits of sympatric jaguar and puma in the Paraguayan Chaco. Biotropica. 1997;29:204–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.1997.tb00025.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Thompson, Jenks & (2014).Thompson DJ, Jenks JA, Fecske DM. Prevalence of human-caused mortality in an unhunted cougar population and potential impacts to management. Wildlife Society Bulletin. 2014;38:341–347. doi: 10.1002/wsb.390. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Tilman (1982).Tilman D. Resource competition and community structure. Princeton University Press; Princeton: 1982. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Treves (2009).Treves A. Hunting for large carnivore conservation. Journal of Applied Ecology. 2009;46:1350–1356. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2664.2009.01729.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Trolle et al. (2007).Trolle M, Noss AJ, Lima ED, Dalponte JC. Camera-trap studies of maned wolf density in the Cerrado and the Pantanal of Brazil. Biodiversity and Conservation. 2007;16:1197–1204. doi: 10.1007/s10531-006-9105-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • USFW (1995).US Fish and Wildlife Service Helena, Montana, Washington, D.C., US Fish and Wildlife ServiceAnnual report of the Rocky Mountain Interagency Wolf Recovery Program. 1995
  • Vanak et al. (2013).Vanak AT, Fortin D, Thaker M, Ogden M, Owen C, Greatwood S, Slotow R. Moving to stay in place: behavioral mechanisms for coexistence of African Large carnivores. Ecology. 2013;94:2619–2631. doi: 10.1890/13-0217.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vickers et al. (2015).Vickers TW, Sanchez JN, Johnson CK, Morrison SA, Botta R, Smith T, Cohen BS, Huber PR, Ernest HB, Boyce WM. Survival and mortality of pumas (Puma concolor) in a fragmented, urbanizing landscape. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:0131490. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wallach et al. (2015).Wallach AD, Izhaki I, Toms JD, Ripple WJ, Shanas U. What is an apex predator? Oikos. 2015;124:1453–1461. doi: 10.1111/oik.01977. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • Wang, Allen & Wilmers (2015).Wang Y, Allen ML, Wilmers CC. Mesopredator spatial and temporal responses to large predators and human development in the Santa Cruz Mountains of California. Biological Conservation. 2015;190:23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2015.05.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • White & Boyd (1989).White PA, Boyd DK. A cougar, Felis concolor, kitten killed and eaten by gray wolves, Canis lupus, in Glacier National Park, Montana. Canadian Field-Naturalist. 1989;103:408–409. [Google Scholar]
  • Wildlife Conservation Society (2008).Wildlife Conservation Society Panthera onca. 2008. [06 June 2017]. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2017-2. http://www.iucnredlist.org .
  • Wilmers et al. (2013).Wilmers CC, Wang Y, Nickel B, Houghtaling P, Shakeri Y, Allen ML, Kermish-Wells J, Yovovich V, Williams T. Scale dependent behavioral responses to human development by a large predator, the puma. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:60590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The following information was supplied regarding data availability:

This is a review article, and the resources exist in the public domain.


Articles from PeerJ are provided here courtesy of PeerJ, Inc

RESOURCES