Abstract
Background
Stress contributes to reactivation of feline herpesvirus‐1 (FHV‐1). The usage of pheromones to decrease stress in FHV‐1 experimentally inoculated kittens has not previously been investigated.
Hypothesis/Objectives
To determine whether a feline pheromone would lessen stress, resulting in decreased recurrence of FHV‐1‐associated illness in kittens.
Animals
Twelve 5‐month‐old, purpose‐bred kittens.
Methods
Randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled clinical trial. Kittens previously infected with the same dose of FHV‐1 were randomized into 2 separate but identical group rooms. After a 2‐week equilibration period, a diffuser containing either the pheromone or placebo was placed in each of the rooms, and the kittens acclimated for an additional 2 weeks. Every 2 weeks thereafter, for the 8‐week study period, housing was alternated between kennel‐ and group housing. Blinded observers applied a standardized clinical and behavioral scoring rubric daily. After each 2‐week period, serum cortisol concentrations and quantitative PCR for FHV‐1 and glyceraldehyde 3‐phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH) ratios were evaluated. Clinical, behavioral, and laboratory test results were compared between groups within individual and combined study periods.
Results
Sneezing occurred more frequently in the placebo group during individual (P = 0.006) and combined study periods (P = 0.001). Sleep at the end of observation periods occurred more frequently in the pheromone group during individual (P = 0.006) and combined study periods (P < 0.001).
Conclusions and Clinical Importance
The findings suggest that the pheromone decreased stress, and the decrease in stress response may have resulted in decreased sneezing associated with FHV‐1.
Keywords: Behavior, Cat, Feliway, FHV‐1, Respiratory, Rhinotracheitis
Abbreviations
- CI
confidence interval
- DNA
deoxyribonucleic acid
- FHV‐1
feline herpesvirus‐1
- GAPDH
glyceraldehyde 3‐phosphate dehydrogenase
- OR
odds ratio
- Period E
equilibration period
- Period G#
group‐housing period number
- Period K#
kennel‐housing period number
- qPCR
quantitative polymerase chain reaction
- SS#
snapshot time point number
- URI
upper respiratory infection
Feline herpesvirus‐1 (FHV‐1) is a common infectious disease of cats. Infection can be subclinical, or it may result in clinical signs of disease including pyrexia, conjunctivitis, keratitis, sneezing, cough, dyspnea, inappetence, lethargy, and occasionally, pneumonia and death.1, 2, 3, 4, 5 Morbidity and mortality in crowded or stressful environments, such as shelters, can be high.1, 3, 4
After acute exposure, most cats develop persistent infection, with the trigeminal ganglia serving as the main site of viral latency.5, 6, 7, 8 Reactivation of FHV‐1 then can recur, resulting in clinical signs, and FHV‐1 shedding increases.2, 6, 8, 9 Stressful events may precipitate FHV‐1 reactivation in some instances.5, 8, 10, 11 In a shelter study, cats with the highest stress scores during the first week in the shelter were more likely to develop upper respiratory infection (URI).10 Unfamiliar handlers and environments, altered feeding schedules or husbandry activities, kennel confinement, impoverished or nonstimulating environments, aversive stimuli such as noise, odors, uncomfortable temperatures, and lack of hiding resources all can cause stress in cats.8, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18 Furthermore, changing housing from group housing to kennels also has been shown to trigger FHV‐1‐associated disease.8, 19 Stressful events are believed to result in FHV‐1 reactivation within the first 3 weeks, with an approximate lag phase of 4–11 days after the stress.5, 8, 10
The mechanisms by which stress induces reactivation of FHV‐1 are unclear. Although acute stress can be adaptive, allowing the animal to cope with and avoid or lessen the impact of the stressor, persistent distress can lead to a damaging pathophysiological reaction in the animal, leading to faulty immune response and disease susceptibility.20, 21 Activation of the stress response system, however, is dependent on individual history, the context in which the stressor occurs, and the expectation the individual has for the outcome of the event.22, 23, 24
Several strategies with variable outcomes have been employed in an attempt to mitigate FHV‐1 reactivation in cats.9, 19, 25, 26, 27 Lessening stress is a strategy that may decrease signs of URI and viral shedding in shelter cats.11, 28 In shelters, stress reduction methods have included gentle stroking and speaking to the cat, grooming, playing, and use of hiding enrichment and minimally invasive daily kennel cleaning.11, 18, 28, 29
A feline facial pheromone fractiona contained in a commercial preparation has been assessed in a variety of studies as another potential stress reducing modality.30, 31, 32 Use of this product has been evaluated in the management of feline behaviors sometimes associated with stress, such as urine spraying, as well as stress‐associated diseases such as feline idiopathic cystitis. Use of the product also has been shown to decrease signs of stress during transportation or when visiting a veterinary clinic and to improve appetite in hospitalized patients.30, 31, 33, 34, 35, 36
Our study was designed to determine whether experimentally induced FHV‐1‐infected kittens housed in equivalent rooms containing either a pheromonea diffuser or a placebo diffuser and subjected to housing change‐induced stress would differ in behavioral scores, clinical scores, FHV‐1 shedding, or serum cortisol concentrations.
Materials and Methods
Cats
Six neutered male and 6 spayed female, 5‐month‐old, mixed breed kittens bred for use in research projects were used in this 12‐week pilot study. Eight weeks before the study, each of the 12 kittens was infected with FHV‐1 by intranasal instillation for a study in which the 12 kittens were the group‐housed control group (Contreras et al., unpublished data, 2017). Using these control kittens from a prior study for the purpose of our study eliminated the need for experimental inoculation of additional kittens, an objective of both the investigators and the sponsor. In that previous study, FHV‐1 infection was confirmed in all kittens by quantitative polymerase chain reaction (qPCR), and each developed clinical signs consisting of fever, sneezing, ocular discharge, nasal discharge, nasal congestion, conjunctivitis, blepharospasm, or some combination of these. At the time of the study, none were undergoing treatment for URI, and occasional sneezing, serous nasal discharge, or serous ocular discharge were the only potential manifestations of FHV‐1 present intermittently in the kittens. Both the prior study and our study were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Housing
Kittens were randomized into a pheromonea group or a placebo group (placebo diffuser); each group consisted of 3 males and 3 females. The kittens were housed in 2 separate but similarly sized rooms (pheromone group = 8′6″ width × 10′ 3″ length × 9′ height; placebo group = 9′7″ width × 9′ length × 9′ height) within the research facility; each room had a separate air exchanger and 2 similarly sized litter boxes. During kennel housing, each room contained 3 sets of top and bottom individual wire kennels (36″ × 25.5″ × 22″); kittens spent 1 2‐week kennel period in a top kennel and the other 2‐week kennel period in a bottom kennel. During kennel housing, kittens were in visual contact with the other kittens in kennels, other than the 1 kitten in a respective top or bottom kennel. Some of the kittens also had physical contact with the other kittens through the bars of the kennels. All kittens were provided dry food and water ad libitum and were provided a table with 2 levels that were approximately 36″ height × 15″ width × 36″ length during group housing and a similarly sized kennel perch during kennel housing. Kittens were provided 2 white ping‐pong balls during group housing and one white ping‐pong ball per kennel during kennel housing. Between 2 and 4 cardboard boxes of various shapes and sizes also were provided during group housing; boxes were identical between rooms and were exchanged for new, different boxes every week. The litter boxes, table, and enrichment devices were movable and not always in the same position within the rooms. While observers were interacting with the kittens during group housing, novel objects such as paper balls, writing implements, and the observers’ outer garments were used as toys and enrichment devices by the kittens; the novel objects were removed when the observers departed after scoring. Enrichment was kept to a minimum because the study was designed to evaluate the effect of the pheromone on stress.
Clinical scoring
A clinical score sheet adapted from other FHV‐1 vaccination and treatment studies, including the previous study of which these kittens were a part, was used in our study (Table 1).9, 27, 37 A total clinical score was calculated for each kitten each day by adding the individual clinical score variables recorded for that day. Body temperatures were estimated by microchip.38 Increased body temperature was defined as >102.5°F. Heart rates were measured daily when auscultation was not obscured by purring. Body weights were measured weekly.
Table 1.
Clinical scoring rubric
Clinical Sign | Score |
---|---|
Conjunctivitisa | 0 = None |
1 = Mild | |
2 = Moderate | |
3 = Severe | |
Blepharospasma | 0 = None |
1 = Eye<25% closed | |
2 = Eye 25–50% closed | |
3 = Eye 50–75% closed | |
4 = Eye completely closed | |
Ocular dischargea | 0 = None |
1 = Mild serous (clear) discharge | |
2 = Moderate mucoid (white) discharge | |
3 = Severe mucopurulent (moist yellow‐green) discharge | |
Body temperature (microchip) | 0: ≤102.5 |
1: >102.5 | |
Cough | 0 = None |
1 = Observed | |
Sneezing (yes/no) | 0 = None |
1 = Observed | |
Nasal dischargea | 0 = None |
1 = Mild serous (clear) discharge | |
2 = Moderate mucoid (white) discharge | |
3 = Severe mucopurulent (moist yellow‐green) discharge or hemorrhagic (bloody/red) discharge | |
Nasal congestiona (if score varies during observation period, record highest score observed) | 0 = None (no congestion present; able to breathe through both nares without difficulty) |
1 = Mild/Minor congestion (barely audible; audible on close listening, subtle snoring sounds on inhalation ANY time during the observation period | |
2 = Moderate congestion (easily audible; consistently audible throughout observation period; audible snoring sounds on inhalation or expiration that are likely to originate from the nasal cavity) | |
3 = Severe congestion (audible across the room, with or without open mouth breathing; minimal nasal air flow noted from1 or both nares after local debris is cleared away) |
Due to statistically low occurrence of scores >1, binomial analyses were performed using 0 or 1 to indicate presence or absence of clinical sign.
The protocol included a rescue clause for kittens that developed moderate to severe signs of FHV‐1 infection and a loss of appetite for 48 hours. Supportive care and treatment that could be administered included SC administration of fluids, buprenorphine for discomfort, topical cidofovir, or PO famciclovir, as needed and determined by the investigators.
Behavioral scoring
Several different behavioral assessment scales used in previous shelter, and other studies were reviewed as tools to assess stress and behavior in the kittens.12, 13, 18, 39, 40, 41, 42 Based on observations of the kittens during the study in which they were inoculated with FHV‐1, and because these purpose‐bred research kittens already were habituated to the research facility, housing, each other, and human interactions, a modified scale was designed (Table 2). The behavioral observation metrics also were designed to accommodate ease and efficiency and to avoid distracting from FHV‐1 clinical scoring. The rubric contained lists of typical feline postures, vocalizations, and actions that represented either normal, relaxed calm, or stress‐related behaviors that could be observed and objectively scored (Table 2). Because of the overall engaging personalities and temperaments of the kittens in the study, the rubric was further adapted before and during the equilibration period. The final rubric that was applied when the diffusers were introduced into the rooms contained 28 individual behaviors, recorded at five different specified time points during the 45‐minute scoring period per room each morning (Table 2). Snapshot observations were performed for 15 seconds per kitten at the following 4 time points: upon entry into room (SS1 time point), during clinical scoring handling (SS2 time point), immediately after clinical scoring handling (SS3 time point), and at the 45‐minute mark (SS4 time point). Behaviors also were recorded throughout the 20 to 30 minutes between SS3 and SS4 (Long time point; Table 2).
Table 2.
Behavioral scoring rubric
Experimental design
Kittens were housed in their respective group rooms on weeks 1, 2 (period E); 3, 4 (period G0); 7, 8 (period G1); and 11, 12 (period G2; Fig. 1). Kittens were housed in kennels in their respective rooms on weeks 5, 6 (period K1) and 9, 10 (period K2). Two trained scorers, blinded regarding treatment allocation, applied the standardized clinical and behavioral scoring system at approximately the same time and order every morning, for 45 minutes per room, throughout the study (Fig. 1).
Figure 1.
Study timeline by week and corresponding group‐ or kennel‐housing period number.
Assays
At the beginning of the study and after each of the 6 periods (E, G0, K1, G1, K2, G2), kittens were sedated; proparacaine was applied to their corneas; and blood, caudal pharynx mucosal cells, and conjunctival swabs were collected. Sera, oropharyngeal, and conjunctival swabs were stored at −80°C until assayed in batches. Total DNA was extracted from the oropharyngeal and conjunctival samples and evaluated for DNA of FHV‐1 and DNA of the glyceraldehyde 3‐phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH) gene, by quantitative PCR (qPCR) as previously described.43 Results of the GAPDH assay were used as a positive control for sample adequacy because this house‐keeping gene is present in all feline cells. Results of the FHV‐1 qPCR assay were expressed as the ratio of FHV‐1 DNA/GAPDH DNA to standardize specimens. Serum cortisol concentrations were measured at a commercial laboratory.b
Statistical evaluation
After randomization but before starting the study, the total clinical scores associated with FHV‐1 that developed after primary infection in the previous study were compared between the pheromone group and the placebo group using the Wilcoxon rank sum test, and the groups were found to not have different median scores (P = 0.9).
Because there were no manipulations of the kittens in G0 other than adding the diffusers, this was not considered a stress period for the final comparisons between groups. Only the results from periods K1, G1, K2, and G2, in which the kittens’ routines were disrupted by housing changes potentially associated with stress, were evaluated individually and in combinations. Descriptive statistics were calculated, and categorical data were expressed as frequencies, whereas continuous data were expressed as means, medians, and ranges. The Shapiro‐Wilk test was used to assess normalcy of data. Because of non‐normality of all variables, the Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to compare group median results for total clinical score, total stress score, FHV‐1/GAPDH ratios, heart rate, and weekly body weight changes in the pheromone group as compared to the placebo group. The Wilcoxon rank sum test also was used to compare median heart rates in kenneled periods as compared to group‐housed periods, median serum cortisol concentrations at the beginning of the study as compared to the end of the study, and median serum cortisol concentrations in kittens that shed FHV‐1 during any study period as compared to serum cortisol concentrations in those kittens that did not shed FHV‐1 during any study period. Individual clinical and behavioral variables were categorized into dichotomous variables of presence or absence of these variables each day. The proportions of observations of dichotomous clinical or behavioral variables were compared between the pheromone group and placebo group by use of the 2‐tailed Fisher exact test. The 2‐tailed Fisher exact test also was used to compare dichotomous variables at the beginning of the study with dichotomous variables at the end of the study. To control for lack of independence among observations because of repeated measurements on the same kitten over time, mixed model regression analyses were used, and odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated.
Commercially available softwarec was used for all comparisons. Significance was defined as P < 0.05.
Results
Clinical findings
At the end of the equilibration period, no significant differences were found between groups (P = 0.58). All kittens had normal appetites, gained weight consistently, and had soft, groomed hair coats each day throughout the study. None of the cats required medical intervention for FHV‐1 infection. Weight changes did not differ significantly between groups. Heart rates did not differ significantly between groups. Heart rates, however, were significantly higher during kennel‐housed periods when compared to group‐housed periods in the pheromone group (P < 0.001), placebo group (P < 0.001), and both groups combined (P < 0.001); significant differences were retained after adjustment for lack of independence, but no clinical sequelae were noted.
For 1 of the 12 kittens, the temperature sensing microchip malfunctioned, and body temperature in this kitten was measured in the axillary space. Median, mean, range, and group comparison results for the total clinical score listed by groups and study period are presented in Table 3.
Table 3.
Median, mean, and range for the total clinical score by group and study period
Study Period | G0a | K1a | G1a | K2a | G2a | K1, G1, K2, G2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | Median, Mean (Range) | Median, Mean (Range) | Median, Mean (Range) | Median, Mean (Range) | Median, Mean (Range) | Median, Mean |
Placebo | 13, 13.8 (5–31) | 14.5, 15.3 (2–29) | 12.5, 16.8 (8–36) | 14, 15.5 (3–33) | 13.5, 15.3 (4–34) | 58, 63 |
Pheromone | 9.5, 12.8 (3–31) | 15, 15 (2–28) | 12, 12.3 (3–28) | 16, 16 (5–29) | 11, 13.2 (3–31) | 55.5, 56.5 |
P value | 0.58 | 0.92 | 0.06 | 0.66 | 0.32 | 0.23 |
G#, group‐housed period number; K#, kennel‐housed period number. G1, G2: the 2, 2‐week group‐housing periods after placement of the diffusers in G0; K1, K2: the 2, 2‐week kenneled periods. The means and ranges are shown to demonstrate variation between the 6 kittens per group; the group medians are compared by Wilcoxon rank sum test. Statistical significance: P < 0.05.
n = 84 observations: 14 observations per 6 kittens per 2‐week study period per group.
Coughing was rarely heard during the study. Mild ocular discharge (score = 1) was recorded at many observation points for several kittens before and during the study periods and potentially associated with stress, but it was never associated with conjunctivitis. Mild nasal discharge (score = 1) was recorded frequently, but moderate mucoid discharge (score = 2) associated with nasal congestion was not detected during the study. Thus, coughing, ocular discharge, and nasal discharge were not evaluated further. Mild nasal congestion (score = 1) was reported commonly, but moderate congestion was rarely reported, and this variable was not evaluated further. Sneezing was the most common finding that likely was associated with FHV‐1 infection. After the diffusers were placed during G0 but before the induction of stress, no differences were observed (P = 0.39) between the placebo group (32%; n = 27/84 observations) and the pheromone group (25%; n = 21/84 observations) in occurrence of sneezing (Fig. 2).
Figure 2.
Proportion of sneezing occurrences (presence) by group and individual and combined study periods. K#, kennel‐housed period number; G#, group‐housed period number. *Statistical significance of P < 0.05 for group comparisons using multivariate logistic regression, adjusted for lack of independence.
After adjusting for lack of independence, sneezing (presence) occurred less frequently in kittens in the pheromone group when compared to the placebo group during period K1 (P = 0.006), period G2 (P = 0.005), and when the study periods potentially associated with stress were combined (P < 0.001, Fig. 2). In the combined study periods, kittens in the placebo group were 2.7 (95% confidence interval [CI], 1.7–4.6) times more likely to have a sneezing occurrence than kittens in the pheromone group (Fig. 2). Kittens in the pheromone group also were 3.3 (95% CI, 1.3–8.0; P = 0.009) times more likely to have a sneezing occurrence at the beginning of the study (G0) when compared to the end of the study (G2; Fig. 2). In contrast, no significant differences were identified in sneezing occurrence among these study periods for the placebo group (P = 0.61).
Behavioral findings
Based on review of the literature and agreement by the clinical scorers at the end of the study, four behaviors were believed to be objective indicators of stress in this cohort of cats: kennel pacing, kennel pawing, kennel disarray, and excessive vocalization. Kennel pacing and kennel pawing were evaluated in the SS1, SS3, SS4, and Long time points (Table 2), but there were too few occurrences of kennel pacing in SS4 to be evaluated. Kennel disarray and excessive vocalization were evaluated in the Long time point (Table 2).
Differences were detected between the 2 groups during the kenneled periods (Table 4). More frequent kennel pacing in placebo group kittens was borderline significant during the SS3 time point (P = 0.05). A total stress score for each kitten was calculated by adding the four stress‐related behavior scores within the K1 and K2 study periods; the total stress score then was compared between groups. The pheromone group had significantly lower total stress scores as compared to the placebo group in univariate analyses (Table 5). However, after controlling for lack of independence between kitten observations over repeated measures, the total kitten stress scores were not significantly different between groups.
Table 4.
Frequency of potential stress‐related behaviors during time points within kenneled periods
Table 5.
Median and range for the total stress score by group and kennel study period
Study Period | K1a | K2a | K1 and K2 Combined |
---|---|---|---|
Group | Median (Range) | Median (Range) | Median (Range) |
Placebo | 36 (19–52) | 19 (6–45) | 55 (26–97) |
Pheromone | 25 (19–46) | 16 (4–27) | 42 (23–73) |
P valueb | 0.04c | 0.04c | 0.004c |
K#, kennel‐housed period number. Median represents the 6 kittens’ total stress scores in each group, per kennel period and per both kennel periods combined. Range represents the lowest (minimum) and highest (maximum) among the 6 kittens’ total stress scores in each group.
n = 84 observations: 14 observations per 6 kittens per 2‐week study period per group.
Unadjusted P values from Wilcoxon rank sum test are displayed for the comparison between groups.
Statistical significance: P < 0.05.
Sleep was used as a correlate for calm/relaxed behavior, as it was observed and recorded at the SS4 time point, after the room gradually quieted after the previous 45 minutes of activity. At that last time point, if the kitten had a relaxed body posture and closed eyes, sleep was recorded.13, 16, 18, 41 During the equilibration period, none of the kittens in either group had any occurrences of sleeping (Table 6).
Table 6.
Frequency of observations of kittens sleeping during SS4 time point, by group and study period
Study Period Group | Equilibration | G0 a | K1a | G1a | K2a | G2a |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Placebo | 0% | 0% | 0% | 0% | 0% | 1% |
Pheromone | 0% | 2% | 10% | 4% | 13% | 20% |
P valueb | n/a | 0.5 | 0.007c | 0.25 | 0.0007c | 0.0001c |
G#, group‐housed period number; K#, kennel‐housed period number; SS4, snapshot‐4 time point. Bold G0: diffusers placed at the start of G0.
n = 84 observations: 14 observations per 6 kittens per 2‐week study period per group; n/a=not applicable due to too few observations.
Unadjusted P values from Fisher 2‐tailed exact test are displayed for the comparison between groups.
Statistical significance achieved at P < 0.05.
During study period G0, there were only two occurrences of sleeping in the pheromone group and no occurrences of sleeping in the placebo group. In univariate analyses, the pheromone group had significantly more sleeping events during the K1, K2, and G2 study periods (Table 6), when K1 and K2 were combined (P < 0.001) and when all 4 of the study periods (K1, G1, K2, and G2) potentially associated with stress were combined (P < 0.001). After controlling for lack of independence between kitten observations over repeated measures, the pheromone group had significantly more sleeping occurrences during the final G2 study period (P = 0.006) and when all 4 of the study periods potentially associated with stress were combined (P = 0.002). In the combined study periods, kittens in the pheromone group were 3.9 (95% CI, 1.4–6.4) times more likely to sleep than kittens in the placebo group (Table 6).
Other potential behavioral indicators of stress occurred infrequently in this kitten cohort. Isolated hissing events occurred between kittens during group play with novel objects on 4 occasions in both groups during period E, twice in both groups during period G0, and twice in the pheromone group and on 6 occasions in the placebo group during G1 and G2 combined. Similarly, fighting was observed during group play with novel objects, twice during period E and once during period G0 in the pheromone group, and it occurred on four occasions during period G1 in the placebo group. During the kenneled periods when fecal character could be ascribed to an individual kitten, diarrhea was reported on one occasion in 3 placebo group kittens and 2 pheromone group kittens and on four occasions in 1 placebo group kitten. When group‐housed, all kittens greeted scorers at the door daily. Hiding was not observed among the kittens during the study.
Cortisol results
All serum cortisol concentrations were within the reference range reported by the laboratory. The results for the pheromone group and the placebo group did not differ over the course of the study. However, the median cortisol results were higher at the end of the last study period (G2) when compared with the start of the first study period (K1) in the pheromone group (P = 0.02), placebo group (P = 0.05), and the two groups combined (P = 0.001).
FHV‐1/GAPDH ratios
GAPDH DNA, as an indicator of viable feline cells (GAPDH‐positive), was amplified from almost all swabs collected from the kittens, and thus, adequate sample collection was obtained. However, FHV‐1 DNA was only amplified from 2 kittens in the placebo group and 4 kittens in the pheromone group after starting the first study period (K1) potentially associated with stress. Because of the low number of samples with detectable FHV‐1 DNA, comparisons of numbers of positive or negative samples between groups or comparison of the FHV‐1/GAPDH ratios as a measure of viral shedding magnitude between groups were considered inaccurate, and thus, results are not presented. Serum cortisol concentrations at the end of the study, after period G2, did not differ significantly when comparing kittens that did shed FHV‐1 (n = 6) to kittens that did not shed FHV‐1 (n = 6; P = 0.09).
Discussion
After controlling for lack of independence between kitten observations over repeated measures, kittens in the room with the pheromonea diffuser still had increased sleeping and less sneezing when compared to kittens in the room with the placebo diffuser; findings support a treatment effect. Even when a standardized dose of FHV‐1 is used to inoculate kittens born to FHV‐1 naïve queens, variations occur in the clinical signs of disease.43, 44 In addition, collection of objective data to assess behavior in cats can be difficult, and individual cats respond to stress differently.13, 14, 39 Reactivation of FHV‐1‐associated illness in response to stress may not occur in some cats, and when reactivation does occur, time until recognition of clinical signs can vary.5, 8, 10 Thus, combining the results from different study periods likely to be associated with stress (K1, G1, K2, G2) seemed the most accurate way to evaluate for differences between groups in our study. We believe the most important limitation to this pilot study was the inclusion of only 6 cats per group, which may have lessened the chances of detecting significant differences between groups.45
This stress model to attempt to reactivate FHV‐1 was used in another 12‐cat study that evaluated a probiotic with presumed immunomodulating activity.19 Similar to the results described in the current study, evidence for reactivation of FHV‐1 varied among cats, and a treatment effect was documented.19 In the previous study, conjunctivitis was common, and sneezing was rare, whereas in the study described here, conjunctivitis was rare, but sneezing was common. These differences between studies likely arose from the use of 2 different FHV‐1 strains and inoculation methods. In the previous study, the field strain of FHV‐1 was administered into the conjunctiva fornix, whereas the different FHV‐1 strain used in the study described here was administered by nasal inoculation.
In our study, although mild, subjective clinical signs could not be evaluated individually, the objective clinical sign of sneezing was reliably scored and measured by two scorers present at the same time during the observation periods. All kittens still had intermittent sneezing from the primary FHV‐1 infection at the time they entered our study, and sneezing still occurred in 32.1% of the observations for the placebo group and in 25.0% of the observations for the pheromone group during G0 when the diffusers were first introduced (Fig. 2). However, over time in the four study periods that may have been associated with stress (K1, G1, K2, G2), the pheromone group had decreased proportions of observation points with sneezing, whereas the placebo group did not (Fig. 2). These findings could indicate reactivation or maintenance of FHV‐1‐associated sneezing in the placebo group, presumably because of greater receptivity to stress exposure. If a similar study were to be performed again in the future, splitting the FHV‐1 inoculum between the nose and eyes may better mimic a natural infection, potentially resulting in clearer reactivation of disease.
GAPDH was amplified from almost all of the oropharyngeal swabs and conjunctival swabs, suggesting that sample collections were adequate. However, FHV‐1 DNA was rarely amplified. Because FHV‐1 is not eliminated after inoculation, it is likely that many of the FHV‐1 PCR assay results were falsely negative. Several studies have shown that FHV‐1 PCR assay results can be negative even in the presence of disease, because numbers of infectious viral particles are suppressed by the immune responses.1, 46, 47 Use of small biopsy specimens of the conjunctiva may be more sensitive than use of swabs, and this approach may be considered for use in future studies.48
Another limitation was the use of kittens that were socialized, affectionate, attention‐seeking of people, and habituated to the research environment, each other, and the observers. All kittens accepted and often sought gentle handling and human contact, petting, and play with each other and the observers. Therefore, the typical indicators of stress and fear such as hiding, freezing, stiffness, crouching, hissing (unrelated to guarding of novel objects), dilated pupils, and holding ears back could not be evaluated in our study because those behaviors were not displayed by these particular kittens.13, 14, 16, 40, 41, 42 Although data were collected for 28 behaviors recorded during multiple time points, only kennel pacing, kennel pawing, kennel disarray, and excessive vocalization (Tables 4 and 5) could be evaluated as stress indicators for statistical comparisons between groups. Although these stress indicative scores and total stress scores usually were numerically higher in the placebo group, statistical differences were lost when analyses were adjusted for individual kitten variations. This failure to find differences between groups in the multivariate analyses could have been a result of limited sample size.45 Individuals experience wide variations in stress responses, even when exposed to the same stressor, or they differ in their immune response to stress.22, 24, 49 Thus, individual variations in our kittens’ responses to stress also could have resulted in individual variations in reactivation of FHV‐1 and outward clinical signs, because coping mechanisms and coping efficiencies differ among individuals.
Our study attempted to induce stress by housing change, minimal interaction during kenneled periods, and venipuncture. However, stress amount, type, frequency, and duration necessary to reactivate FHV‐1 still are unclear. The kenneled study periods resulted in acute stress in the kittens, reflected in the analyzed behaviors as well as increased body temperature, and significantly increased heart rates during kenneled observation periods. However, the study only evaluated 45 minutes; behaviors occurring in the other 23 hours of the day were unknown. Because all kittens retained well‐groomed hair coats, ate, and gained weight appropriately, and no clinically relevant diarrhea or vomiting was observed, it is possible that kittens did not exhibit stress behaviors or experience prolonged stress during the rest of the days when kenneled, without the observers present.16, 18, 39, 41
Although serum cortisol concentrations remained in the reference range of the reference laboratory, a significant difference between samples collected before attempting to induce housing change stress and the end of study was observed in both groups of kittens, which may suggest that stress occurred over time. In addition, other events in the study beyond housing changes likely were associated with stress including blood collection, witnessing other kittens in distress during blood collection, and occasional variations in feeding and husbandry times.12, 17, 50, 51, 52, 53 In addition, the placebo group always was scored first, and the pheromone group kittens may have heard the kitten‐human interactions, potentially leading to an additional 45‐minute anticipatory state for the pheromone group. These potential confounding factors should be addressed in future studies. Also, the changes in serum cortisol concentration within the normal reference range may have been merely related to aging of the kittens over the course of the study.
In the kittens of our study, the best indicator of a relaxed state was sleeping at the end of the 45‐minute observation period (SS4). We believe this behavior differed from feigning sleep, which has been used as an indicator of stress in cats, particularly shelter and kenneled cats and described as a defensive sleeping posture in captive felids in zoos, for example.16 In contrast, the kittens recorded as sleeping in our study did not have a tense, immobile, or defensive posture, but rather a relaxed posture with closed eyes, and the kittens responded positively if awakened. At the end of the 45‐minute observation time when the observers rose to depart the room, the sleeping kittens awakened, occasionally stretched, and actively sought engagement again from the observers. This finding supported our assessment of this behavior as an indicator of a relaxed state and not a fear or stress state. Furthermore, the SS4 time point recorded behaviors after the kittens were accustomed to the observers’ presence in the room for 45 minutes, after the room gradually quieted subsequent to other activities. Sleeping observations did not occur during the equilibration period, during times of activity such as clinical scoring times, or when observers first entered the room or if facility noise was audible outside of the room. Sleeping observations were recorded during the quietest time, the SS4 time point. Sleeping also was observed with gradually increasing frequency in the room with the pheromone diffuser after the diffusers were in place (Table 6).
Conclusions
We believe our data support reactivation or maintenance of sneezing associated with FHV‐1 in the kittens in the placebo group when compared to the kittens in the pheromone group. This difference may have resulted from a response to stress associated with the study design. The evidence for increased relaxing sleep and decreased sneezing in the pheromone group compared to the placebo group, supports the hypothesis that exposure to the pheromone lessened stress and consequently clinical signs associated with FHV‐1.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Jamie Bunkers, Claire Hovenga, Kimberly Kern, Kristine Kofron, Serena Mancha, Karla Schultz, and Amy Zug for being the clinical scorers that also aided in the development and assessment of the behavioral scoring rubric and provided data entry and quality assurance. Jennifer Hawley and Arianne Morris were responsible for the performance of the cortisol assays and the FHV‐1/GAPDH ratios.
Conflict of Interest Declaration
Three authors are employees of Ceva Animal Health (Beck, Hodgkins, and Tynes), which also funded the study. However, the data management was controlled by the researchers at Colorado State University.
Off‐label Antimicrobial Declaration
Authors declare no off‐label use of antimicrobials.
This work was conducted at Colorado State University and High Quality Research, Fort Collins, CO. The study was funded by Ceva Animal Health.
Footnotes
aFeliway®; Ceva Santé Animale, Libourne, France.
bEndocrinology Laboratory, Michigan State University, Lansing, Michigan.
cStataCorp. 2015. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX, StataCorp LP.
References
- 1. Low HC, Powell CC, Veir JK, et al. Prevalence of feline herpesvirus 1, Chlamydophila felis, and Mycoplasma spp DNA in conjunctival cells collected from cats with and without conjunctivitis. Am J Vet Res 2007;68:643–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Maggs DJ, Lappin MR, Reif JS, et al. Evaluation of serologic and viral detection methods for diagnosing feline herpesvirus‐1 infection in cats with acute respiratory tract or chronic ocular disease. J Am Vet Med Assoc 1999;214:502–507. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Dinnage JD, Scarlett JM, Richards JR. Descriptive epidemiology of feline upper respiratory tract disease in an animal shelter. J Feline Med Surg 2009;11:816–825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Veir JK, Ruch‐Gallie R, Spindel ME, et al. Prevalence of selected infectious organisms and comparison of two anatomic sampling sites in shelter cats with upper respiratory tract disease. J Feline Med Surg 2008;10:551–557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Gaskell R, Dawson S, Radford A, et al. Feline herpesvirus. Vet Res 2007;38:337–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Gaskell RM, Dennis PE, Goddard LE, et al. Isolation of felid herpesvirus I from the trigeminal ganglia of latently infected cats. J Gen Virol 1985;66:391–394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Townsend WM, Stiles J, Guptill‐Yoran L, et al. Development of a reverse transcriptase polymerase chain reaction assay to detect feline herpesvirus‐1 latency‐associated transcripts in the trigeminal ganglia and corneas of cats that did not have clinical signs of ocular disease. Am J Vet Res 2004;65:314–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Gaskell RM, Povey RC. Experimental induction of feline viral rhinotracheitis virus re‐excretion in FVR‐recovered cats. Vet Rec 1977;100:128–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Contreras ET, Hawley JR, Lappin MR. Effects of administration of Carnivora on clinical signs in cats after repeat challenge with feline herpesvirus 1. Int J Appl Res Vet Med 2016;14:208–216. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Tanaka A, Wagner DC, Kass PH, et al. Associations among weight loss, stress, and upper respiratory tract infection in shelter cats. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2012;240:570–576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Gourkow N, Phillips CJC. Effect of interactions with humans on behaviour, mucosal immunity and upper respiratory disease of shelter cats rated as contented on arrival. Prev Vet Med 2015;121:288–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Carlstead K, Brown JL, Strawn W. Behavioral and physiological correlates of stress in laboratory cats. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1993;38:143–158. [Google Scholar]
- 13. McCobb EC, Patronek GJ, Marder A, et al. Assessment of stress levels among cats in four animal shelters. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2005;226:548–555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Stella J, Croney C, Buffington T. Effects of stressors on the behavior and physiology of domestic cats. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2013;143:157–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Stella J, Croney C, Buffington T. Environmental factors that affect the behavior and welfare of domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus) housed in cages. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2014;160:94–105. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Kessler MR, Turner DC. Stress and adaptation of cats (Felis silvestris catus) housed singly, in pairs and in groups in boarding catteries. Anim Welf 1997;6:243–254. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Bassett L, Buchanan‐Smith HM. Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2007;102:223–245. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kessler MR, Turner DC. Effects of density and cage size on stress in domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus) housed in animal shelters and boarding catteries. Anim Welf 1999;8:259–267. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lappin MR, Veir JK, Satyaraj E, et al. Pilot study to evaluate the effect of oral supplementation of Enterococcus faecium SF68 on cats with latent feline herpesvirus 1. J Feline Med Surg 2009;11:650–654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Griffin JFT. Stress and immunity: a unifying concept. Vet Immunol Immunopathol 1989;20:263–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Dohms JE, Metz A. Stress—mechanisms of immunosuppression. Vet Immunol Immunopathol 1991;30:89–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Buffington CAT. External and internal influences on disease risk in cats. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2002;220:994–1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev 2007;87:873–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, et al. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress‐physiology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1999;23:925–935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Maggs DJ, Nasisse MP, Kass PH. Efficacy of oral supplementation with L‐lysine in cats latently infected with feline herpesvirus. Am J Vet Res 2003;64:37–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Maggs DJ. Antiviral therapy for feline herpesvirus infections. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract 2010;40:1055–1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Fenimore A, Carter K, Fankhauser J, et al. Evaluation of intranasal vaccine administration and high‐dose interferon‐ α2b therapy for treatment of chronic upper respiratory tract infections in shelter cats. J Feline Med Surg 2016;18:603–611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Gourkow N, Hamon SC, Phillips CJC. Effect of gentle stroking and vocalization on behaviour, mucosal immunity and upper respiratory disease in anxious shelter cats. Prev Vet Med 2014;117:266–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Kry K, Casey R. The effect of hiding enrichment on stress levels and behaviour of domestic cats (Felis sylvestris catus) in a shelter setting and the implications for adoption potential. Anim Welf 2007;16:375–383. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Pheromonatherapy MD. Theory and applications. In Pract 2005;27:368–377. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Pereira JS, Fragoso S, Beck A, et al. Improving the feline veterinary consultation: the usefulness of Feliway spray in reducing cats’ stress. J Feline Med Surg 2016;18:959–964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Beck A. Use of pheromones to reduce stress in sheltered cats. J Feline Med Surg 2013;15:829–830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Gaultier E, Pageat P, Tessier Y. Effect of a feline appeasing pheromone analogue on manifestations of stress in cats during transport. In: Proceedings of the 32nd Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology, Clermont‐Ferrand. 1998.
- 34. Gunn‐Moore DA, Cameron ME. A pilot study using synthetic feline facial pheromone for the management of feline idiopathic cystitis. J Feline Med Surg 2004;6:133–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Pageat P, Tessier Y. F4 synthetic pheromone: a means to enable handling of cats with a phobia of the veterinarian during consultations. 1997. Available at: http://agris.fao.org/agris-search/search.do?recordID=GB1997038280. Accessed February 1, 2017.
- 36. Pageat P, Gaultier E. Current research in canine and feline pheromones. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract 2003;33:187–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Summers SC, Ruch‐Gallie R, Hawley JR, et al. Effect of modified live or inactivated feline herpesvirus‐1 parenteral vaccines on clinical and laboratory findings following viral challenge. J Feline Med Surg 2017;19:824–830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Quimby JM, Olea‐Popelka F, Lappin MR. Comparison of digital rectal and microchip transponder thermometry in cats. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2009;48:402–404. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Finka LR, Ellis SL, Stavisky J. A critically appraised topic (CAT) to compare the effects of single and multi‐cat housing on physiological and behavioural measures of stress in domestic cats in confined environments. BMC Vet Res 2014;10:73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Seksel K. Fear, aggression, communication, body language and social relationships in cats. Eur J Companion Anim Pract 2014;24:20–27. [Google Scholar]
- 41. McCune S. Caged cats: avoiding problems and providing solutions. Newsl Companion Anim Study Group 1994;7:1–9. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Weiss E, Gramann S, Drain N, et al. Modification of the feline‐alityTM assessment and the ability to predict adopted cats’ behaviors in their new homes. Animals 2015;5:71–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Fontenelle JP, Powell CC, Veir JK, et al. Effect of topical ophthalmic application of cidofovir on experimentally induced primary ocular feline herpesvirus‐1 infection in cats. Am J Vet Res 2008;69:289–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Gaskell RM, Povey RC. The dose response of cats to experimental infection with feline viral rhinotracheitis virus. J Comp Pathol 1979;89:179–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Giuffrida MA. Type II error and statistical power in reports of small animal clinical trials. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2014;244:1075–1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Powell CC, McInnis CL, Fontenelle JP, et al. Bartonella species, feline herpesvirus‐1, and Toxoplasma gondii PCR assay results from blood and aqueous humor samples from 104 cats with naturally occurring endogenous uveitis. J Feline Med Surg 2010;12:923–928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Burgesser KM, Hotaling S, Schiebel A, et al. Comparison of PCR, virus isolation, and indirect fluorescent antibody staining in the detection of naturally occurring feline herpesvirus infections. J Vet Diagn Invest 1999;11:122–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Stiles J, McDermott M, Willis M, et al. Comparison of nested polymerase chain reaction, virus isolation, and fluorescent antibody testing for identifying feline herpesvirus in cats with conjunctivitis. Am J Vet Res 1997;58:804–807. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Negrao AB, Deuster PA, Gold PW, et al. Individual reactivity and physiology of the stress response. Biomed Pharmacother 2000;54:122–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Carstens E, Moberg GP. Recognizing pain and distress in laboratory animals. Inst Lab Anim Res J 2000;41:62–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Overall KL, Dyer D. Enrichment strategies for laboratory animals from the viewpoint of clinical veterinary behavioral medicine: emphasis on cats and dogs. Inst Lab Anim Res J 2005;46:202–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Balcombe JP, Barnard ND, Sandusky C. Laboratory routines cause animal stress. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2004;43:42–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Willemse T, Vroom MW, Mol JA, et al. Changes in plasma cortisol, corticotropin, and alpha‐melanocyte‐stimulating hormone concentrations in cats before and after physical restraint and intradermal testing. Am J Vet Res 1993;54:69–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]