Abstract
This study examined whether social preference was a mechanism that explained the relation between proactive and reactive aggression and peer victimization. Participants were 494 children in grades 2–5. Proactive and reactive aggression was assessed via a self-report measure and indices of social preference and peer victimization were assessed via a peer nomination inventory. Data was collected during the fall and spring of two academic years. The relations among aggression, social preference, and peer victimization varied by the function of aggression and gender. For girls, reactive aggression was a significant negative predictor of social preference. Findings also revealed social preference mediated the relation between reactive aggression and peer victimization for girls. This pathway did not hold for boys. There was some evidence that proactive aggression was negatively associated with peer victimization, but only for girls. Findings from the current study suggest social preference may be a key mechanism through which reactive aggression is associated with future victimization for girls. Boys’ aggression was not related to subsequent peer victimization. Future research and intervention efforts should consider gender differences and the function of aggression when investigating children’s peer victimization experiences.
Keywords: Proactive and Reactive Aggression, Peer victimization, Teacher-student relationships, Peer relationships, Peer acceptance
Chronically victimized children are at risk for maladaptive outcomes in childhood and adolescence, including loneliness, school achievement (Buhs, Ladd, & Herald, 2006), internalizing symptoms, peer rejection, and low popularity (Bierman, Kalvin, & Heinrichs, 2015; Hanish & Guerra, 2002). Given these detrimental consequences, it is important to identify factors that are reliably associated with children’s peer victimization experiences. Early behavior problems have been shown to predict later victimization by peers, particularly for children with fewer friendships or poor quality peer relationships (Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 1999; Schwartz, McFadyen-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1999). Indeed, a subgroup of peer victimized children characterized by their own aggressive behaviors emerges when investigating risk factor for peer victimization.
Proactive and Reactive Aggression and Peer Victimization
The link between aggression and peer victimization has been well established, with some research suggesting a bidirectional relation between the two constructs (i.e., a subgroup of children repeatedly victimized by peers in turn respond with aggression; Salmivalli, Karhunen, & Lagerspetz, 1996). Childhood aggression is often subdivided by the motivation that underlies the aggressive act, namely whether it is proactive or reactive (Card & Little, 2006; Dodge & Coie, 1987). Although many children exhibit both functions of aggression (Vitaro & Brendgen, 2005), exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses indicate that the two functions are distinct (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Fite, Colder, & Pelham, 2006; Poulin & Boivin, 2000a). The ways in which proactive and reactive aggression differentially confer risk for peer victimization is less well understood (Card & Little, 2006), and we examined this issue in the present study.
Rooted in the frustration-aggression model, reactive aggression occurs due to provocation or perceived threat (Berkowitz, 1993). Reactive aggression is “hot-headed” or affective, impulsive, and defensive, and often the result of a hostile attribution bias (Dodge & Coie, 1987). It involves the instant gratification of anger or frustration driven impulses. Proactive aggression is goal-oriented and offensive, and best understood in terms of social learning theories of aggression (Bandura, 1973; Crick & Dodge, 1996). It is a non-provoked behavior motivated by the anticipation of a reward. In addition to the distinct social-cognitive processes underlying these behaviors, proactive and reactive aggression also show unique and differential relations with psychological outcomes (Card & Little, 2006).
Accumulating evidence indicates that children scoring high on reactive aggression are more likely to experience peer victimization than their proactively aggressive counterparts, although these associations have mostly been studied with boys (Poulin & Boivin, 2000a; Schwartz et al., 1998), using cross-sectional data (Lamarche et al., 2006; Salmivalli & Nieminen, 2002), or during early childhood (Ostrov, Kamper, Hart, Godleski, & Blakely-McClure, 2014). When examining aggression and victimization longitudinally among male and female pre-adolescents, researchers found that boys initially high on reactive aggression were more likely to be victimized by peers, and boys high on proactive aggression were less likely to be victimized by peers in the future. Neither effect was found for girls (Salmivalli & Helteenvuori, 2007). Moreover, in a sample of 7–8 year-old children, peer victimization was concurrently associated with reactive aggression and unrelated to proactive aggression, but no evidence of a gender difference emerged in these relations (Camodeca, Goossens, Meerum Terwogt, & Schuengel, 2002). Overall, the limited and mixed findings of studies examining the role of gender in the longitudinal link between functions of aggression and peer victimization represents a key limitation in the literature.
Proactive and Reactive Aggression and Peer Relations
Reactive and proactive aggression are distinct forms of aggressive behavior that elicit different responses from peers. For example, proactive aggression is often associated with popularity and a sense of humor, which may lead peers to respond more favorably to children displaying proactive aggression (Dodge & Coie, 1987; Poulin & Boivin, 2000a). There is also evidence that children scoring high on proactive aggression are more likely to have friendships, although their friends are more likely to also be proactively aggressive (Poulin & Boivin, 2000b). Therefore, these positive attributes may attenuate peers’ negative responses to proactive aggression, as peers may be hesitant to provide direct negative feedback for fear of retaliation, loss of friendship, or damage to their own social reputation.
Conversely, evidence suggests that children scoring high on reactive aggression are less socially preferred (Poulin & Boivin, 2000a; Price & Dodge, 1989; Prinstein & Cillessen, 2003). Hence, it is perhaps peers’ negative reaction to, or interpretation of, the aggressive behavior that explains a reactively aggressive child’s higher propensity for experiencing peer victimization and other relational difficulties, rather than the aggressive act itself (Hanish & Guerra, 2000). The impulsive, angry, and hostile nature of reactive aggression is not only aversive to peers, but may in fact influence the degree to which peers perceive victimization as a reasonable response (Card & Little, 2006). Consistent with this supposition is research finding emotionally-dsyregulated aggressive children are at increased risk for future peer rejection and victimization (Bierman et al., 2015). Feeling anger, a trait of reactive aggression, makes a child more likely to seek revenge against their aggressor, which in turn increases their likelihood of experiencing peer victimization (Hanish et al., 2004).
Social Preference as a Mediator between Aggression and Peer Victimization
Social preference is a measure of peer acceptance that takes into consideration the extent to which peers “like” and “dislike” a child in their classroom or social group. It is possible that social preference is one mechanism through which aggression confers developmental risk for peer victimization. Schwartz et al. (1999), for instance, found that the relation between externalizing problems in the first years of elementary school and later peer victimization was mediated by social preference in the intervening years. The social or relational cost of bullying a less accepted peer may be small. Bullies expect that their attacks on disliked children will go unpunished by other peers (Hodges et al., 1999), which is evidenced by the protective function of bystanders (e.g., children willing to intervene during conflict to aid the victim) among peer victimized children (Hawkins, Pepler, & Craig, 2001).
Boivin, Hymel, & Hodges (2001) sequential social process model provides a framework for understanding the relations among aggression, social preference, and peer victimization. The model first suggests that stable behavioral tendencies (i.e., aggression) may directly lead to problematic peer relationships, peer victimization, and ultimately negative social self-perceptions. Second, the model proposes that an indirect pathway may exist, whereby aggression first predicts low social preference and then indirectly peer victimization. Indeed, among 3rd–5th grade students, Boivin and colleagues found that aggression uniquely accounted for future victimization partly through low social preference, although this effect weakened with age (Boivin & Hymel, 1997; Boivin, Hymel, & Hodges, 2001). In addition, Ostrov (2008) found that, in young children, peer rejection partially mediated the association between relational aggression and later relational victimization (Ostrov, 2008). Few studies, however, have investigated the functions of aggression within this model, particularly with elementary school aged and older children (Ostrov & Godleski, 2013; Ostrov et al., 2014). Associations with social preference may develop differently over time for reactive aggression than proactive aggression, which highlights the important contribution of this study to the literature.
Bidirectional Relations Among Variables
In addition to relations specified by the sequential social processing model, past theory and research provides some support for the notion that aggression and victimization may have reciprocating enhancing effects over time (Ostrov & Godleski, 2013). Lamarche et al. (2007), for example, found that peer victimization predicted increases in future reactive-but not proactive- aggression for boys, suggesting that children may use reactive aggression as a defense or retaliation in response to being victimized by peers.
Similar temporal relations have also been found between aggression and social preference (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004) and between victimization and social preference (Kawabata, Tseng, & Crick, 2014), and thus will be examined in the present study. In one of the few studies known to assess the longitudinal interplay between these variables, Sentse, Kretschmer, & Salmivalli (2015) found that victimization contributed to lower social preference and vice versa, and peer rejection positively predicted bullying (i.e., proactive aggression) amongst 3–6th graders longitudinally. However, bullying was not associated with subsequent levels of peer rejection (Sentse et al., 2015).
Current Study
We examined the prospective links between functions of aggression, social preference, and peer victimization among elementary school children. While prior studies have investigated relations between functions of aggression and victimization, few have looked at associations longitudinally, included females, utilized multiple informants, or examined the mechanisms through which proactive and reactive aggression differentially predict risk for victimization (Card & Little, 2006). We predicted that reactive aggression would emerge as a stronger predictor of peer victimization than proactive aggression. A primary aim of the current study was to examine the extent to which social preference was a mechanism that explained the relation between proactive and reactive aggression and peer victimization. In light of previous theory and research linking reactive aggression to poor peer acceptance and peer victimization, and the well documented link between peer acceptance and peer victimization, it was hypothesized that social preference would mediate the relation between reactive aggression and peer victimization. Previous findings regarding the influence of proactive aggression on social adjustment are more speculative, but we predicted that social preference would also mediate the relation between proactive aggression and peer victimization, such that children scoring higher on proactive aggression would score higher on social preference and in turn experience lower level of peer victimization.
A secondary aim of the current study was to examine whether the pathway from proactive and reactive aggression to peer victimization was bidirectional. There is some evidence that the experience of peer victimization could lead to an increase in reactive aggression over time (Lamarche et al., 2007), but limited evidence suggests that peer victimization predicts changes in proactive aggression. Thus, it was predicted that peer victimization would emerge as a longitudinal predictor of reactive aggression but not proactive aggression. We also tested the extent to which social preference was a mechanism that explained the longitudinal relation between peer victimization and aggression, but this aim was largely exploratory.
Finally, this study examined the degree to which relations among aggression, social preference, and peer victimization vary as a function of gender. Despite theoretical support for gender-specific differences in the development and correlates of aggression (Ostrov & Godleski, 2010), few studies have considered the role of gender in the association between functions of aggression and social relations. Given the limited research available, we hypothesized that the prospective relation between reactive aggression and peer victimization would be stronger for boys than for girls, whereas the relation between proactive aggression and peer victimization would be similar across gender (Lamarche et al., 2007).
Method
Participants
Participants were 494 children recruited from eight schools located in the Midwestern United States. Schools were selected to represent the ethnic and socioeconomic diversity of the area. Fifty-five of the participating children were involved in an intervention trial testing the efficacy of a school-based mentoring program for children showing early signs of aggression, and 439 children were the classroom peers of those participating in this trial (N = 494). Intervention children were identified through a combination of teacher-nomination and questionnaires completed by parents and teachers assessing aggressive behavior. Eighty-four children were identified via teacher nomination, and 78.8% of those children’s parents consented to allow their child to participate in the intervention. Eligible for the intervention were 55 teacher-nominated children who scored at or above 60T on the Aggressive Behavior subscale of the Teacher Report Form or Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991). Children were randomized to the intervention (n =28) or the waitlist control (n = 27) condition.
To recruit classmates of children participating in the intervention arm, parental consent forms were sent home with 513 children. Parental consent forms were returned to schools by 390 children. Three hundred and fifty-one parents agreed to allow their children to participate in the larger study (Cohort 1); this number includes the 55 children participating in the intervention arm of the study. In the fall of the 2nd year, after children participating in the intervention arm of the study had transitioned to the next grade, 565 additional consent forms were sent home to the parents of classmates of the intervention children who previously had not partaken in the consent procedure in the fall of year one (i.e., these students were in classrooms without intervention children in the 1st year of the study) or with children who failed to return a consent form in the fall of year one. Parental consent forms were returned by 257 children, with 143 additional parents agreeing to allow their child to participate in year two (Cohort 2).
Data was collected at four time points. Self-report measures and a peer nomination inventory was administered in the early fall (September/October) and late spring (April/May) of project years one and two. Before completing the study measures, children were informed that their parents had given permission for them to participate in the study, that their participation was voluntary, and that they could choose not to participate by informing a research staff member of their decision. One student declined participation and was escorted back to their classroom. Data was available for 340 participating children in the fall of the 1st year (T1), 342 in the spring of the 1st year (T2), 393 in the fall of the 2nd year (T3), and 380 in the spring of the 2nd year (T4). Data was not collected from 87 participants at T2 and T3 because these students moved to classrooms that were not participating in our assessment procedure (i.e., classrooms without a child participating in the intervention arm) in year two of the study. Data was collected from 141 new participants at T3 and T4.
Analyses were based on a sample of 494 (236 girls, 215 boys; data on gender was missing for 43 children) 2nd- through 5th-grade children between the ages of 6 and 12 (M age at consent = 8.75 years, SD = .99 years). The sample was 61.8% white, but also included children whose parents identified them as African American (4.9%), Asian American (1.2%), Hispanic (3.0%), American Indian (3.7%), and multiracial (15.0%). Data on ethnicity was not available for 10.0% of participants and 0.4% of participants selected other ethnicity. Median family income was $35,000–$50,000, with 30.5% receiving reduced price lunch and 35.0% receiving free lunch.
Procedures
The University Institutional Review Board approved this project. An informational parental consent form and demographic questionnaire was sent home in children’s weekly folders, and written parental consent and child assent were obtained for all study participants prior to participation. Children completed self- and peer-report measures in class groups overseen by trained research assistants. For the peer nomination procedure, items were read aloud and children used a numerical roster to nominate classmates by circling the number corresponding to their name. To minimize discussion about ratings, children were spaced apart, instructed to keep answers covered, and allowed to work on distracter activities (e.g., mazes) between sets of questions and for approximately five minutes after completing all study measures.
Measures
Proactive and Reactive Aggression
Aggression was assessed using Dodge & Coie’s (1987) 6-item measure of proactive and reactive aggression. Proactive aggression items were “I get other kids to gang up on somebody that I do not like”, “I use physical force (or threaten to use physical force) in order to dominate other kids”, and “I threaten or bully other kids to get my way.” Reactive aggression items were “When I am teased or threatened, I get angry easily and strike back”, “I feel that other kids are to blame in a fight. I feel they started the trouble”, and “When a kids hurts me on accident (such as bumping into me), I think they meant to do it. I then react with anger or fighting.” Responses were made on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). Validity for the self- and teacher-report version of this measure has been established in prior investigations (Fite et al., 2016; Fite, Wimsatt, Elkins, & Grassetti, 2012; Waschbusch, Willoughby, & Pelham, 1998; Poulin & Boivin, 2000a). Previous research using the self-report version of this measure has demonstrated internal consistency estimates that range from low to high, with most estimates falling at or above the adequate range (Fite et al., 2016; Fite, Stoppelbein, & Greening, 2009; Fite et al., 2012). In the current investigation, internal consistency was low to adequate with estimates ranging from .49 to .72 across time points for proactive aggression and .47 to .59 across time points for reactive aggression.
Peer Victimization and Social Preference
A peer nomination inventory, similar to procedures outlined by Coie, Dodge, and Coppotelli (1982), was used to assess children’s peer victimization and social preference (Coie, Dodge, & Coppotelli, 1982). Peer victimization was assessed via two peer nomination items measuring overt (i.e., “Who in your class gets hit, pushed threatened, or teased by other children?”) and relational (i.e., “Who in your class gets gossiped about or left out of activities?”) victimization. Within time point correlations for overt and relational victimization were .48, .61, .76, and .77, respectively. Scores for overt and relational victimization at each time point were averaged to form a composite peer victimization score and then standardized by classroom. Children also nominated three classmates who they liked most and three classmates who they liked least. Following procedures developed by Coie, Dodge, and Coppotelli (1982), a social preference score was computed by subtracting “like least” from “like most” nominations, and then standardizing these scores by classroom. The validity and reliability of peer nomination procedures to assess social constructs is well established (Coie, Dodge, & Kupersmidt, 1990).
Data Analytic Plan
Descriptive statistics for and correlations among study variables as well as primary analyses were estimated using Mplus 7.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 2009). Cross-lagged longitudinal panel models using maximum likelihood estimation were used to examine longitudinal associations among proactive and reactive aggression, social preference, and peer victimization. Models were estimated separately for proactive and reactive aggression. Age, gender (dummy code 1 = male), and ethnicity (dummy code 1 = White) were included as covariates in our models, and each endogenous variable was regressed on to each covariate. Because some of the children in our sample were participating in the intervention arm of the larger study, we included a condition variable in each model (dummy code 1 = children assigned to the intervention or waitlist control; dummy code 0 = classroom peers of children participating in the intervention arm of the study) and regressed each endogenous variable in our model on to the condition variable. Although the condition variable did not emerge as a significant predictor of proactive or reactive aggression or peer victimization at any time point, there was evidence that children in the intervention arm of our study (i.e., identified as aggressive and assigned to the intervention or waitlist condition) scored lower on social preference. Thus, we retained this variable as a control variable in our final models. Because indirect effects are not normally distributed, a biased corrected bootstrap procedure (10,000 bootstrapped samples) was used to test the significance of indirect effects. A multiple group model was also estimated to examine whether relations varied as a function of gender. Model fit was evaluated using the following criteria, CFI > .90, RMSEA < .08, and SRMR < .08 (Bentler, 1990; Brown, 2015; Hu & Bentler, 1999; Little, 2013).
Treatment of Missing Data
Analyses were based on 494 participating children for whom data was available for at least one of the four data collections to minimize bias associated with case-wise deletion. Only 11 children (2%) had data at only one time point and 257 (52%) had data for at least 3 time points. Across variables, the percentage of missing values ranged from 9% to 35%. Little’s (1995) MCAR analysis was used to examine whether there was a systematic reason for missing data. Results indicated that the data were missing completely at random, χ2 (1775, N = 492) = 296.18, p = 1.00. Full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation was used to address missing data, which in the multivariate case uses all available information in the dataset to estimate model parameters.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Means and standard deviations for and correlations among primary study variables are reported in Tables 1 and 2. In general, constructs evidenced moderate stability overtime. Reactive and proactive aggression were significantly correlated across all measurement occasions for boys and girls. Reactive aggression was negatively correlated with social preference at Time 1, 2, and 3 for boys and at Time 1 and 3 for girls. Proactive aggression was negatively correlated with social preference at Time 2 and 3 for girls and T3 for boys. Reactive aggression was positively correlated with peer victimization at T4 for girls and Time 1 and 3 for boys. Girl’s proactive aggression was positively correlated with peer victimization at T4 only, and boy’s proactive aggression was positively correlated with peer victimization at T3 only. Social preference and peer victimization were negatively correlated across all time points. Overall, boys scored higher on both functions of aggression than girls. Girls were more socially preferred than boys, but scores on peer victimization were similar across genders.
Table 1.
Time 1 | Time 2 | Time 3 | Time 4 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | Boys | Girls | Boys | Girls | Boys | Girls | Boys | Girls |
RA | 1.16 (.85) | 1.07 (.80) | 1.10 (.91)* | .93 (.81) | 1.10 (.76)* | .84 (.72) | .93 (.73)* | .80 (.64) |
PA | .42 (.60)* | .22 (.46) | .27 (.59)* | .16 (.41) | .25 (.55)* | .14 (.35) | .19 (.41) | .12 (.36) |
SP | .33 (3.11)* | 1.14 (2.94) | .41 (3.30) | .72 (3.48) | .09 (3.20)* | .80 (3.23) | .33 (3.18) | .44 (3.21) |
PV | 1.02 (1.03) | .91 (1.00) | 1.01 (1.08) | 1.24 (1.60) | 1.05 (1.44) | 1.24 (1.64) | 1.17 (1.65) | 1.13 (1.54) |
Note. RA = Reactive Aggression; PA = Proactive Aggression; SP = Social Preference; PV = Peer Victimization. Time 1 = Fall Year 1; Time 2 = Spring Year 1; Time 3 = Fall Year 2; Time 4 = Spring Year 2.
Significant gender difference based on results of a t-test.
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. RA Tl | – | .33** | 40** | .14 | 46** | .07 | .18* | −.08 | −.08 | − 23** | −.12 | −.09 | .06 | .08 | .08 | −.01 |
2. RA T2 | .43** | – | .39** | 40** | .24** | .38** | .33** | .17 | −.16* | −.17* | −.26** | −.18* | .21* | .00 | −.02 | .01 |
3. RA T3 | .33** | .49** | – | .45** | .35** | .23* | .35** | .05 | −.08 | −.17* | −.13 | −.17* | .15 | .04 | .04 | .08 |
4. RA T4 | .24* | .34** | .51** | – | .28* | .33** | .28** | .29** | −.32** | −.19* | −.20* | −.22** | −.27** | .11 | .17* | .27** |
5. PA Tl | .50** | .29** | .43** | .32** | – | .05 | .44** | .16 | −.06 | −.12 | −.17* | −.13 | .03 | .11 | .02 | .02 |
6. PA T2 | .27** | .50** | 41** | .26* | .40** | – | .42** | .34** | −.29* | −.21** | −.20* | −.21* | .22* | .05 | −.04 | .04 |
7. PA T3 | .l5 | .32** | .44** | .24* | .37** | .67** | – | .26* | −.10 | −.13 | −.24** | −.20* | .17 | .00 | .04 | −.02 |
8. PA T4 | .28* | .26** | .35** | 47** | .60** | .40** | .37** | – | −.09 | −.04 | −.05 | −.04 | .07 | .11 | .08 | .19* |
9. SP Tl | −.14* | −.20* | − 27** | .00 | −.09 | −.04 | −.05 | −.13 | – | .65** | .51** | .53** | −.49** | −.33** | −.50** | −.55** |
10. SP T2 | −.l9 | −.25** | −.34** | −.05 | −.14 | −.05 | −.13 | −.12 | .65** | – | .60** | .62** | −.39** | − 41** | −.46** | −.47** |
11. SP T3 | −.l2 | −.l8* | −.25** | −.11 | −.11 | −.07 | −.19* | −.04 | .57** | .59** | – | .67** | −.44** | −.33** | −.44** | −.44** |
12. SP T4 | −.l7 | −.l6 | − 27** | −.09 | −.14 | −.06 | −.01 | −.02 | .54** | .62** | .68** | – | −.42** | −.30** | − 41** | −.46** |
13. PV Tl | .16* | .l5 | .22* | .08 | .11 | .02 | .01 | .25* | −.50** | −.37** | −.28** | −.30** | – | .29** | .55** | .51** |
14. PV T2 | .l4 | .08 | .05 | −.08 | .07 | .04 | .05 | .03 | −.30** | −.35** | −.33** | −.17 | .23** | – | .45** | .48** |
15. PV T3 | .08 | .11 | .30** | .03 | .07 | .13 | .28** | .15 | −.18* | −.28** | −.42** | −.27** | .39** | .41** | – | .79** |
16. PV T4 | .l3 | .02 | .18* | −.04 | .08 | .05 | .20* | .06 | −.25* | −.28** | − 41** | − 41** | .29** | .34** | .68** | – |
Note. Correlations for boys are presented below the diagonal; correlations for girls are presented above the diagonal. PA = Proactive Aggression; RA = Reactive Aggression; SP = Social Preference; PV = Peer Victimization; T1-4 = Time 1 – Time 4.
p < .05,
p < .01.
Primary Analyses
First, a single group model was estimated separately for proactive and reactive aggression. The fit for the initial model for reactive aggression, χ2 (19) = 71.80, p < .01, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .04, and proactive aggression, χ2 (19) = 76.41, p < .01, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .04, was acceptable. Next, a multiple group model was estimated to examine whether the associations between aggression, social preference, and peer victimization varied as a function of gender. For both reactive and proactive aggression, an unconstrained model in which all paths were freely estimated across gender was compared to a fully constrained model in which all paths were constrained to be equivalent across gender using a chi-squared difference test. The difference in model fit between the two models was significant (reactive aggression, Δχ2 (Δ 100) = 212.35, p < .01; proactive aggression, Δχ2 (Δ 100) = 235.88, p < .01), indicating that the paths varied by gender. Therefore, a multiple group model was estimated for both the proactive and reactive aggression models.
Reactive Aggression
The unconstrained multiple group reactive aggression model is presented in Figure 1. Full results of this model are presented in Table 3. The model fit was acceptable, χ2 (42) = 122.51, p < .01, CFI = .94, RMSEA = .09, SRMR = .05. Reactive aggression at T1 and T2 was a significant unique negative predictor of social preference at T2 and T3 respectively, for girls (β = −.19, SE = .07, p < .01, 95% CI [−.370, −.084]; β = −.16, SE = .07, p = .01, 95% CI [−.338, −.048]). Reactive aggression at T3 was a negative predictor of social preference at T4 for girls, but only at the level of a non-significant trend (β = −.10, SE = .08, p = .08, 95% CI [−.277, .017]). Conversely, reactive aggression did not emerge as a unique predictor of social preference for boys.
Table 3.
Boys | Girls | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
95% CI
|
95% CI
|
|||||||||
Variable | B | B | S.E. | Lower | Upper | β | B | S.E. | Lower | Upper |
Age ➔ RA2 | −.06 | −.06 | .07 | −.196 | .079 | −.10 | −.08 | .06 | −.207 | .046 |
White ➔ RA2 | .02 | .03 | .16 | −.286 | .350 | −.02 | −.03 | .12 | −.260 | .200 |
Inter ➔ RA2 | .07 | .18 | .24 | −.274 | .663 | −.03 | −.08 | .26 | −.564 | .447 |
RA1➔ RA2 | .41** | .43 | .10 | .224 | .608 | .33** | .33 | .09 | .135 | .502 |
SP1 ➔ RA2 | −.12 | −.11 | .08 | −.269 | .054 | −.06 | −.05 | .09 | −.217 | .129 |
PV1 ➔ RA2 | −.05 | −.05 | .08 | −.207 | .118 | .11 | .10 | .08 | −.055 | .241 |
Age ➔ SP2 | −.02 | −.02 | .07 | −.159 | .119 | −.06 | −.06 | .06 | −.180 | .065 |
White ➔ SP2 | −.00 | −.01 | .13 | −.265 | .242 | .08 | .16 | .12 | −.054 | .396 |
Inter ➔ SP2 | −.06 | −.17 | .16 | −.496 | .140 | − 17** | −.54 | .15 | −.823 | −.257 |
RA1 ➔ SP2 | −.08 | −.09 | .07 | −.231 | .055 | − 19** | −.23 | .07 | −.370 | −.094 |
SP1 ➔ SP2 | .62** | .59 | .06 | .474 | .719 | .53** | .56 | .06 | .489 | .682 |
PV1 ➔ SP2 | .04 | .04 | .07 | −.102 | .173 | −.12 | −.13 | .07 | −.259 | .005 |
Age ➔ PV2 | −.02 | −.02 | .07 | −.147 | .116 | .05 | .05 | .08 | −.100 | .209 |
White ➔ PV2 | −.07 | −.13 | .16 | −.439 | .173 | .09 | .20 | .15 | −.099 | .486 |
Inter ➔ PV2 | .01 | .02 | .20 | −.369 | .410 | .10 | .34 | .27 | −.177 | .902 |
RA1 ➔ PV2 | −.01 | −.01 | .10 | −.200 | .183 | .12 | .15 | .11 | −.046 | .368 |
SP1 ➔ PV2 | −.32** | −.29 | .09 | −.478 | −.132 | −.25** | −.28 | .09 | −.464 | −.096 |
PV1 ➔ PV2 | .02 | .02 | .09 | −.151 | .199 | .12 | .13 | .09 | −.054 | .303 |
Age ➔ RA3 | .22** | .17 | .06 | .057 | .279 | .05 | .04 | .05 | −.068 | .131 |
White ➔ RA3 | −.05 | −.07 | .13 | −.326 | .162 | −.10 | −.15 | .12 | −.416 | .070 |
Inter ➔ RA3 | .07 | .15 | .17 | −.169 | .507 | −.04 | −.10 | .20 | −.511 | .272 |
RA2 ➔ RA3 | .45** | .38 | .08 | .219 | .542 | .32* | .28 | .11 | .068 | .509 |
SP2 ➔ RA3 | −.21* | −.17 | .08 | −.317 | −.023 | −.04 | −.03 | .07 | −.174 | .107 |
PV2 ➔ RA3 | −.04 | −.04 | .09 | −.215 | .125 | .01 | .01 | .06 | −.123 | .134 |
Age ➔ SP3 | −.06 | −.06 | .07 | −.187 | .071 | .01 | .01 | .06 | −.094 | .123 |
White ➔ SP3 | .03 | .06 | .14 | −.210 | .337 | −.01 | −.03 | .12 | −.270 | .200 |
Inter ➔ SP3 | −.18* | −.49 | .21 | −.908 | −.094 | −.23** | −.74 | .19 | −1.12 | −.357 |
RA2 ➔ SP3 | −.01 | −.01 | .07 | −.149 | .127 | −.16* | −.19 | .07 | −.338 | −.048 |
SP2 ➔ SP3 | .32** | .32 | .09 | .119 | .492 | .44** | .43 | .09 | .258 | .610 |
PV2 ➔ SP3 | −.11 | −.12 | .10 | −.303 | .066 | −.13 | −.12 | .06 | −.251 | −.002 |
Age ➔ PV3 | .04 | .04 | .07 | −.094 | .171 | −.02 | −.02 | .07 | −.146 | .128 |
White ➔ PV3 | 24** | .49 | .15 | .207 | .786 | .11 | .23 | .14 | −.047 | .488 |
Inter ➔ PV3 | −.12 | −.33 | .20 | −.689 | .078 | .05 | .15 | .24 | −.327 | .618 |
RA2 ➔ PV3 | .07 | .07 | .09 | −.106 | .241 | −.12 | −.14 | .09 | −.311 | .034 |
SP2 ➔ PV3 | −.11 | −.11 | .10 | −.305 | .067 | −.33** | −.33 | .09 | −.505 | −.160 |
PV2 ➔ PV3 | 46** | .50 | .12 | .257 | .741 | .34** | .32 | .09 | .158 | .503 |
Age ➔ RA4 | −.11 | −.08 | .05 | −.185 | .026 | .04 | .02 | .04 | −.057 | .098 |
White ➔ RA4 | .04 | .06 | .13 | −.199 | .318 | −.13 | −.19 | .11 | −.407 | .032 |
Inter ➔ RA4 | −.15 | −.32 | .23 | −.753 | .134 | −.05 | −.11 | .23 | −.518 | .369 |
RA3 ➔ RA4 | .59** | .56 | .09 | .363 | .725 | .41** | .37 | .07 | .232 | .492 |
SP3 ➔ RA4 | .00 | .00 | .06 | −.125 | .128 | −.12 | −.08 | .05 | −.190 | .021 |
PV3 ➔ RA4 | −.16 | −.12 | .07 | −.265 | .012 | .09 | .06 | .06 | −.061 | .176 |
Age ➔ SP4 | −.01 | −.01 | .07 | −.132 | .121 | .00 | .00 | .06 | −.102 | .112 |
White ➔ SP4 | .07 | .13 | .13 | −.116 | .380 | −.02 | −.05 | .11 | −.259 | .175 |
Inter ➔ SP4 | −.11 | −.30 | .17 | −.648 | .038 | −.03 | −.09 | .23 | −.531 | .370 |
RA3 ➔ SP4 | −.06 | −.07 | .09 | −.236 | .121 | −.10 | −.13 | .08 | −.277 | .017 |
SP3 ➔ SP4 | .62** | .61 | .07 | .484 | .740 | .63** | .64 | .06 | .523 | .766 |
PV3 ➔ SP4 | −.00 | −.00 | .06 | −.112 | .122 | −.09 | −.09 | .07 | −.224 | .047 |
Age ➔ PV4 | .06 | .06 | .07 | −.073 | .206 | −.04 | −.04 | .06 | −.153 | .082 |
White ➔ PV4 | −.07 | −.15 | .14 | −.429 | .119 | .08 | .16 | .12 | −.080 | .399 |
Inter ➔ PV4 | .06 | .17 | .19 | −.191 | .545 | .04 | .14 | .27 | −.417 | .649 |
RA3 ➔ PV4 | −.08 | −.11 | .10 | −.323 | .083 | .04 | .05 | .09 | −.126 | .242 |
SP3 ➔ PV4 | −.20* | −.21 | .09 | −.381 | −.047 | −.19* | −.18 | .08 | −.337 | −.043 |
PV3 ➔ PV4 | .55** | .57 | .10 | .378 | .748 | .51** | .51 | .08 | .344 | .641 |
RA1 ➔ SP2 ➔ PV3 | − | .01 | .01 | −.005 | .059 | – | .08* | .03 | .028 | .157 |
RA2 ➔ SP3 ➔ PV4 | − | .00 | .01 | −.032 | .033 | – | .04* | .02 | .006 | .091 |
PV1 ➔ SP2 ➔ RA3 | − | −.01 | .01 | −.042 | .015 | – | .00 | .01 | −.013 | .030 |
PV2 ➔ SP3 ➔ RA4 | − | .00 | .01 | −.022 | .020 | – | .01 | .01 | −.001 | .037 |
Note. RA = Reactive Aggression; SP = Social Preference; PV = Peer Victimization; Inter = Participated in the Intervention Arm;
p < .05,
p < .01
Reactive aggression did not emerge as a unique predictor of subsequent peer victimization for either gender. Social preference was a significant negative predictor of peer victimization at subsequent time points for both girls and boys, with the exception of a non-significant association between T2 social preference and T3 peer victimization for boys. Boys and girls with lower social preference scores were more likely to experience subsequent peer victimization.
Examined next was whether the relation between reactive aggression and peer victimization was mediated by social preference. For girls, social preference at T2 was found to mediate the relation between reactive aggression at T1 and peer victimization at T3 (b = .08, SE = .03, p = .02, 95% CI [.028, .157]) . Social preference at T3 also mediated the relation between reactive aggression at T2 and peer victimization at T4 for girls (b = .04, SE = .02, p = .08, 95% CI [.006, .091]). The indirect effect was not significant for boys at either time point.
In the context of the same set of models, also tested was the possibility that the direction of the effect moved from peer victimization to social preference and reactive aggression. Findings revealed few significant effects in this direction. For boys, social preference at T2 emerged as a negative predictor of reactive aggression at T3 (β = −.21, SE = .08, p =.02, 95% CI [−.317, −.023]). For girls, a trend emerged in which peer victimization at T1 and T2 predicted lower social preference at T2 and T3, respectively (β = −.12, SE = .06, p =.06, 95% CI [−.259, .005]; β = −.13, SE = .06, p = .06, 95% CI [−.251, −.002]) .
Proactive Aggression
The unconstrained multiple group proactive aggression model is presented in Figure 2. Full results of this model are presented in Table 4. Collectively, the fit statistics indicate acceptable model fit, χ2 (42) = 127.78, p < .01, CFI = .93, RMSEA = .10, SRMR = .05. For boys and girls, proactive aggression was unrelated to subsequent social preference. For girls, proactive aggression at T2 was a negative predictor of peer victimization at T3 such that girls scoring higher on proactive aggression were subsequently less likely to experience peer victimization (β = −.18, SE = .16, p = .01, 95% CI [−.797, −.114]). This was the only significant association between proactive aggression and peer victimization to emerge for either gender, and social preference did not mediate the relation between proactive aggression and peer victimization . Moreover, social preference was again a negative predictor of peer victimization at subsequent time points across genders, with the exception of a non-significant association between boys’ T2 social preference and T3 peer victimization.
Table 4.
Boys | Girls | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
95% CI
|
95% CI
|
|||||||||
Variable | B | B | S.E. | Lower | Upper | β | B | S.E. | Lower | Upper |
Age ➔ PA2 | −.02 | −.01 | .04 | −.090 | .065 | −.16 | −.06 | .03 | −.140 | −.004 |
White ➔ PA2 | −.06 | −.07 | .10 | −.276 | .127 | .09 | .08 | .07 | −.074 | .186 |
Inter ➔ PA2 | .06 | .10 | .18 | −.203 | .496 | .01 | .02 | .12 | −.235 | .252 |
PA1 ➔ PA2 | .39* | .37 | .14 | .126 | .671 | .07 | .06 | .09 | −.051 | .362 |
SP1 ➔ PA2 | .02 | .01 | .05 | −.084 | .106 | −.15 | −.07 | .05 | −.211 | .009 |
PV1 ➔ PA2 | .01 | .01 | .06 | −.100 | .127 | .09 | .04 | .06 | −.055 | .164 |
Age ➔ SP2 | −.03 | −.03 | .07 | −.171 | .112 | −.05 | −.05 | .06 | −.179 | .070 |
White ➔ SP2 | −.00 | −.01 | .13 | −.263 | .237 | .06 | .13 | .12 | −.088 | .378 |
Inter ➔ SP2 | −.06 | −.17 | .17 | −.505 | .144 | −.15 | −.49 | .15 | −.781 | -.177 |
PA1 ➔ SP2 | −.08 | −.13 | .10 | −.317 | .089 | −.09 | −.18 | .17 | −.516 | .139 |
SP1 ➔ SP2 | .63** | .59 | .06 | .472 | .718 | .54** | .57 | .07 | .431 | .695 |
PV1 ➔ SP2 | .03 | .03 | .07 | −.105 | .167 | −.14* | −.14 | .07 | −.282 | −.005 |
Age ➔ PV2 | −.02 | −.02 | .07 | −.154 | .114 | .05 | .05 | .08 | −.101 | .212 |
White ➔ PV2 | −.07 | −.14 | .16 | −.445 | .175 | .09 | .21 | .15 | −.098 | .499 |
Inter ➔ PV2 | .01 | .02 | .20 | −.374 | .402 | .09 | .30 | .28 | −.227 | .865 |
PA1 ➔ PV2 | .00 | .00 | .13 | −.276 | .245 | .08 | .17 | .22 | −.216 | .654 |
SP1 ➔ PV2 | −.32** | −.29 | .09 | −.476 | −.132 | −.26** | −.28 | .10 | −.473 | −.093 |
PV1 ➔ PV2 | .02 | .02 | .09 | −.155 | .194 | .13 | .14 | .09 | −.041 | .321 |
Age ➔ PA3 | .09 | .05 | .03 | −.007 | .116 | −.14* | −.05 | .02 | −.105 | −.009 |
White ➔ PA3 | .02 | .02 | .08 | −.142 | .165 | −.09 | −.07 | .06 | −.197 | .037 |
Inter ➔ PA3 | −.06 | −.10 | .11 | −.308 | .124 | .01 | .01 | .12 | −.231 | .256 |
PA2 ➔ PA3 | .63** | .58 | .15 | .252 | .848 | .40* | .35 | .14 | .031 | .574 |
SP2 ➔ PA3 | −.09 | −.05 | .04 | −.130 | .029 | −.02 | −.01 | .03 | −.055 | .044 |
PV2 ➔ PA3 | .07 | .04 | .06 | −.059 | .167 | .02 | .01 | .03 | −.051 | .066 |
Age ➔ SP3 | −.06 | −.06 | .07 | −.183 | .070 | .03 | .03 | .06 | −.079 | .145 |
White ➔ SP3 | .02 | .05 | .14 | −.227 | .323 | −.01 | −.02 | .13 | −.269 | .223 |
Inter ➔ SP3 | −.18* | −.49 | .21 | −.892 | −.071 | −.21** | −.69 | .19 | −1.06 | −.300 |
PA2 ➔ SP3 | −.01 | −.01 | .11 | −.233 | .196 | −.04 | −.10 | .20 | −.395 | .380 |
SP2 ➔ SP3 | .33** | .33 | .09 | .133 | .503 | .48** | .47 | .09 | .275 | .641 |
PV2 ➔ SP3 | −.11 | −.11 | .10 | −.307 | .071 | −.12 | −.11 | .07 | −.253 | .006 |
Age ➔ PV3 | .04 | .04 | .07 | −.095 | .172 | −.03 | −.03 | .07 | −.155 | .116 |
White ➔ PV3 | .25** | .52 | .14 | .243 | .806 | .12* | .26 | .13 | −.008 | .516 |
Inter ➔ PV3 | −.13 | −.35 | .20 | −.727 | .034 | .06 | .18 | .23 | −.272 | .630 |
PA2 ➔ PV3 | .13 | .21 | .14 | −.067 | .489 | −.19* | −.43 | .16 | −.797 | −.114 |
SP2 ➔ PV3 | −.14 | −.15 | .10 | −.336 | .034 | −.35** | −.34 | .08 | −.512 | −.186 |
PV2 ➔ PV3 | .43** | .46 | .12 | .235 | .713 | .34** | .31 | .09 | .150 | .497 |
Age ➔ PA4 | −.04 | −.02 | .02 | −.061 | .034 | −.12 | −.04 | .03 | −.109 | .001 |
White ➔ PA4 | .00 | .00 | .06 | −.115 | .115 | .02 | .01 | .05 | −.091 | .120 |
Inter ➔ PA4 | .05 | .05 | .14 | −.205 | .311 | .12 | .15 | .15 | −.085 | .500 |
PA3 ➔ PA4 | .27 | .20 | .23 | −.168 | .656 | .32 | .32 | .18 | .020 | .722 |
SP3 ➔ PA4 | .13 | .06 | .03 | −.012 | .119 | .11 | .04 | .02 | −.001 | .089 |
PV3 ➔ PA4 | −.10 | −.04 | .04 | −.114 | .039 | .06 | .02 | .04 | −.04 | .114 |
Age ➔ SP4 | −.02 | −.02 | .06 | −.138 | .098 | −.01 | −.01 | .06 | −.112 | .105 |
White ➔ SP4 | .09 | .18 | .13 | −.072 | .421 | −.02 | −.04 | .11 | −.242 | .182 |
Inter ➔ SP4 | −.12* | −.33 | .16 | −.646 | −.014 | −.02 | −.07 | .23 | −.490 | .404 |
PA3 ➔ SP4 | .11 | .20 | .13 | −.060 | .437 | −.04 | −.10 | .20 | −.424 | .353 |
SP3 ➔ SP4 | .63** | .63 | .06 | .502 | .748 | .63** | .65 | .06 | .524 | .772 |
PV3 ➔ SP4 | −.05 | −.05 | .06 | −.163 | .067 | −.09 | −.09 | .07 | −.229 | .050 |
Age ➔ PV4 | .05 | .05 | .07 | −.083 | .192 | −.06 | −.05 | .06 | −.174 | .062 |
White ➔ PV4 | −.06 | −.13 | .14 | −.406 | .128 | .07 | .15 | .12 | −.107 | .365 |
Inter ➔ PV4 | .04 | .11 | .20 | −.259 | .517 | .03 | .10 | .26 | −.449 | .587 |
PA3 ➔ PV4 | −.02 | −.03 | .16 | −.407 | .253 | −.11 | −.28 | .25 | −.823 | .171 |
SP3 ➔ PV4 | −.19* | −.20 | .09 | −.381 | −.045 | −.22** | −.22 | .08 | −.372 | −.065 |
PV3 ➔ PV4 | .53** | .55 | .10 | .366 | .745 | .51** | .51 | .08 | .349 | .642 |
PA1 ➔ SP2 ➔ PV3 | – | .02 | .02 | −.007 | .090 | – | .06 | .06 | −.031 | .199 |
PA2 ➔ SP3 ➔ PV4 | – | .00 | .02 | −.039 | .054 | – | .02 | .05 | −.073 | .109 |
PV1 ➔ SP2 ➔ PA3 | – | −.00 | .00 | −.032 | .204 | – | .00 | .00 | −.017 | .004 |
PV1 ➔ SP2 ➔ PA3 | – | −.01 | .01 | −.071 | .106 | – | −.01 | .00 | −.028 | .003 |
Note. PA = Proactive Aggression; SP = Social Preference; PV = Peer Victimization; Inter = Participated in the Intervention Arm;
p < .05,
p < .01
Similar to the reactive aggression model, the possibility that the direction of effects moved from peer victimization to social preference and proactive aggression was considered. The few significant effects found in this direction applied to girls only. T1 peer victimization was a significant negative predictor of T2 social preference for girls (β = −.14, SE = .07, p =.05, 95% CI [−.282, −.005]), and a non-significant trend emerged in which peer victimization at T2 was negatively related to social preference at T3 (β = −.12, SE = .07, p = .09, 95% CI [−.253, .006]). For girls, social preference at T3 was marginally positively associated with proactive aggression at T4 (β = .11, SE = .02, p = .06, 95% CI [−.001, .089]).
Discussion
Based on a review of existing research, it was reasoned that children scoring higher on reactive aggression would experience higher levels of peer victimization than their proactively aggressive counterparts (Lamarche et al., 2007; Lamarche et al., 2006; Ostrov et al., 2014; Poulin & Boivin, 2000a; Salmivalli & Nieminen, 2002; Schwartz et al., 1998). We also considered the possibility that social preference was one mechanism that might explain these relations. Finally, the current investigation examined whether these associations were bidirectional and varied as a function of gender.
The current study provided little evidence that the pathway from aggression to peer victimization is bidirectional. Instead, when significant effects were found, they suggested an influence of aggression on social preference and peer victimization. Results provided partial support for the sequential social process model (i.e., aggression predicting peer victimization partly through social preference; Boivin et al., 2001), particularly for girls. Reactive aggression emerged as a longitudinal predictor of peer victimization and that association was explained through the effect of reactive aggression on girls’ social preference. Girls who scored high on reactive aggression were less socially preferred overtime and subsequently more likely to experience peer victimization. Our findings suggest that peers react negatively to girls who respond to perceived threats with reactive aggression, and this negative effect on their social relations may make them easy targets for victimization.
Displays of reactive aggression identify those children who struggle to articulate and regulate their emotions well in social situations (Little, Jones, Henrich, & Hawley, 2003). Although emotion-dysregulation and ineffectuality within the social domain is typical of boys and girls who are reactively aggressive, these attributes may be particularly problematic for girls (Hanish et al., 2004). Girls who use aggression in their peer interactions may violate traditional gender norms, ostracizing themselves from the peer group and placing them at greater risk for social maladjustment (Crick, 1997). Consistent with this supposition is evidence linking externalizing behaviors in girls to peer victimization (Cillessen & Lansu, 2015) or low social preference (Prinstein & La Greca, 2004). Hanish et al. (2004) argue that peers may be more sensitive to expressions of anger (i.e., a characteristic closely tied to reactive aggression) among girls, despite displays of anger being more common among boys (Hanish et al., 2004). Moreover, difficulty articulating and expressing emotion may interfere with girls’ ability to form intimate, dyadic relationships, which is particularly detrimental at this age (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003). Indeed, the ability to develop social ties within the peer group appears to be a stronger predictor of maladjustment for girls than for boys (Sentse et al., 2015), which is further corroborated by our finding that, for girls only, low social preference predicts subsequent peer victimization and vice versa.
Contrary to our hypotheses, reactive aggression was not found to predict social preference or peer victimization for boys, which is at odds with the existing literature. In attempts to explain these null findings, it is important to consider methodological differences between the current study and previous work. This study utilized a self-report measure of aggression and a peer-report measure of peer victimization, whereas the majority of research has assessed proactive and reactive aggression via teacher-report (Camodeca et al., 2002; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Fite, Colder, Lochman, & Wells, 2007; Lamarche et al., 2007; Poulin & Boivin, 2000b) and, to a lesser extent, peer-report (Prinstein & Cillessen, 2003; Salmivalli & Helteenvuori, 2007; Salmivalli & Nieminen, 2002). In one of the few known studies to examine victimization and functions of aggression via self-report, Little, Jones, et al. (2003) found that both functions of aggression in adolescents were unrelated to their perceptions of their own victimization, and negatively related to peer nominations of their victimization. Most of the studies relying on self-report measures of aggression have included older children and adolescents (e.g., Raine et al., 2006), and this study extends the assessment of self-reported proactive and reactive aggression downward to elementary-school children. It is important to note that some have questioned whether self-reports of aggression among elementary school-aged children are valid (Hubbard, McAuliffe, Morrow, & Romano, 2010). However, given that intentionality defines the function of aggression, children may be the best informants of the distinct motivations that underlie their behaviors. While the accuracy with which elementary-school children disclose their aggressiveness may vary (Lochman & Dodge, 1998), our findings suggest that self-report data provide an important perspective that might be overlooked utilizing only teacher- or peer-report measures.
The current investigation did find some evidence that girls scoring higher on proactive aggression were at reduced risk for peer victimization, but this finding did not hold for boys. Among studies that have examined gender differences in the relation between proactive aggression and victimization, findings are mixed. Some evidence suggests that proactive aggression is negatively related to peer victimization in boys only (Salmivalli & Helteenvuori, 2007), while other studies have found correlates of proactive aggression to be similar across gender (Camodeca et al., 2002). Results from the current study add to the mixed literature on the role of proactive aggression in children’s peer victimization experiences. Still, there is evidence suggesting that proactively aggressive children are often themselves bullies, feared by other students, or are socially competent (Little, Brauner, Jones, Nock, & Hawley, 2003), which indicates that this behavior may deter peers from selecting these children as targets of peer victimization. It is also important to note that, unlike reactive aggression, results suggest that girls’ proactive aggression did not predict social preference, which suggests that the pathway from proactive aggression to victimization may be explained by factors other than likeability. Indeed, female bullies, or proactive aggressors, tend to have a controversial status (i.e., both liked and disliked) in the peer group (Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman, & Kaukiainen, 1996).
This study also replicates and extends prior research examining the association between social preference and peer victimization (e.g., Sentse et al., 2015). For both boys and girls, social preference scores in the fall were uniquely related to their peer victimization experiences the following spring; girls’ social preference scores in the spring were also predictive of their peer victimization scores in the fall of the following school year. This bidirectional relation suggests a spiraling down process for girls, such that girls who are accepted less well by their peers are more victimized, victimized girls are then subsequently less accepted by their peers, and then girls scoring lower on peer acceptance are once again at risk for future peer victimization. This may be particularly detrimental for girls’ long-term psychosocial adjustment given the heightened value girls place on relationship status and peer perceptions relative to boys (Rose & Rudolph, 2006).
Strength and Limitations
The current investigation had a number of strengths. Data were collected using a longitudinal design spanning two academic years with four measurement occasions, strengthening our ability to discern predictive relations and possible mechanisms of action. Different report sources were used to assess aggression, peer victimization, and social preference, which allowed us to examine the extent to which our findings generalized beyond a single report source. The investigation also considered the possibility that relations among constructs varied as a function of gender or were reciprocal. This represents an area that has received less attention in the research literature, particularly in terms of examining the pathways from functions of aggression to peer victimization (Ostrov et al., 2014; Salmivalli & Helteenvuori, 2007).
Several limitations of this study also deserve consideration. When interpreting gender effects, it is important to consider the characteristics of the aggression measure used in the current study (i.e., Dodge & Coie, 1987). The overt, rather than relational, nature of the proactive/reactive aggression items may shed some light on the current findings, particularly with regard to correlates of girls’ aggressive behavior. Others have also criticized this measure for confounding the form and function of aggression (Little, Jones, et al., 2003; Vitaro & Brendgen, 2005). It would be important to examine both the form and function of aggression in order to disentangle these complex relations (Ostrov et al., 2014). Estimates of internal consistency for proactive and reactive aggression were low to adequate (.49 to .72 and .47 to .59, respectively), and approximately 70–85% of girls and 51–73% of boys endorsed zero for proactive aggression across different time points. It is possible that both of these measurement limitations attenuated relations between aggression and other constructs. Thus, null associations, in particular, should be interpreted with caution.
Conclusions
Overall, our findings highlight the complex nature of the longitudinal link between aggression and peer victimization. To the author’s knowledge, this is the first study to examine whether social preference is a mechanism that explains the relation between proactive and reactive aggression and peer victimization separately for boys and girls. Results from this study reflect initial evidence in support of both direct and indirect pathways of the sequential social process model in girls for reactive aggression and peer victimization. Not only do these findings suggest the need to distinguish functions of aggression in future research, they also reflect the importance of examining gender differences within the aggression construct. Indeed, given the limited and relatively inconsistent literature, the differences found in this study for reactive and proactive aggression among boys and girls are hardly definitive and represent a direction for future research. A more nuanced understanding of the individual and contextual factors differentially associated with the correlates of proactive and reactive aggression could allow researchers to better tailor intervention procedures that are aimed at reducing aggression and peer victimization, and ultimately deflect children off a trajectory toward later maladjustment. It is possible, for example, that cognitive behavioral interventions that target girls’ displays of reactive aggression within the peer group could promote greater social acceptance, and thereby reduce incidences of peer victimization. Social skills training may facilitate more prosocial interactions between reactively aggressive children and their peers. Children who can learn to respond to peer victimization or peer conflict in a nonaggressive way that demonstrates emotional control may be more likely to maintain or enhance their status or level of acceptance in the peer group, disrupting the transactional cycle from low peer acceptance to peer victimization. This is clearly an important area for future research.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institute of Health (F32HD066833). We would also like to thank the Lawrence Public School District and its students, families, and faculty for their participation and support.
Footnotes
Ethical Standards
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
- Achenbach TM. Manual for Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Dept. of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkowitz L. Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York, NY: Mcgraw-Hill Book Company; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Bierman KL, Kalvin CB, Heinrichs BS. Early childhood precursors and adolescent sequelae of grade school peer rejection and victimization. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2015;44:367–379. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.873983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boivin M, Hymel S. Peer experiences and social self-perceptions: A sequential model. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:135–145. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boivin M, Hymel S, Hodges EVE. Toward a process view of peer rejection and harassment. In: Juvonen J, Graham S, editors. Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2001. pp. 265–289. [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. 2nd. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Buhs ES, Ladd GW, Herald SL. Peer exclusion and victimization: Processes that mediate the relation between peer group rejection and children’s classroom engagement and achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006;98:1–13. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Camodeca M, Goossens FA, Meerum Terwogt M, Schuengel C. Bullying and victimization among school-age children: Stability and links to proactive and reactive aggression. Social Development. 2002;11:332–345. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00203. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Card NA, Little TD. Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2006;30:466–480. doi: 10.1177/0165025406071904. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cillessen AHN, Lansu TAM. Stability, correlates, and time-covarying associations of peer victimization from grade 4 to 12. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2015;44:456–470. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.958841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cillessen AHN, Mayeux L. From censure to reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association between aggression and social status. Child Development. 2004;75:147–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00660.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coie JD, Dodge KA, Coppotelli H. Dimensions and types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology. 1982;18:557–570. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.4.557. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Coie JD, Dodge KA, Kupersmidt JB. Peer group behavior and social status. In: Asher SR, Coie JD, editors. Peer rejection in childhood. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 17–59. [Google Scholar]
- Crick NR. Engagement in gender normative versus nonnormative forms of aggression: Links to social–psychological adjustment. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:610–617. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick NR, Dodge KA. Social information-processing mechanisms on reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development. 1996;67:993–1002. doi: 10.2307/1131875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick NR, Zahn-Waxler C. The development of psychopathology in females and males: Current progress and future challenges. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:719–742. doi: 10.1017/S095457940300035X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA, Coie JD. Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;53:1146–1158. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fite PJ, Poquiz J, Cooley JL, Stoppelbein L, Becker SP, Luebbe AM, Greening L. Risk factors associated with proactive and reactive aggression in a child psychiatric inpatient sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2016;38:56–65. doi: 10.1007/s10862-015-9503-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fite PJ, Wimsatt A, Elkins S, Grassetti S. Contextual influences of proactive and reactive subtypes of aggression. Child Indicators Research. 2012;5:123–133. [Google Scholar]
- Fite PJ, Stoppelbein L, Greening L. Proactive and reactive aggression in a child psychiatric inpatient population: Relations to psychopathic characteristics. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 2009;36:481–493. doi: 10.1007/s10862-015-9503-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fite PJ, Colder CR, Lochman JE, Wells KC. Pathways from proactive and reactive aggression to substance use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21:355–364. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fite PJ, Colder CR, Pelham WE. A factor analytic approach to distinguish pure and co-occurring dimensions of proactive and reactive aggression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:578–582. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanish LD, Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Spinrad TL, Ryan P, Schmidt S. The expression and regulation of negative emotions: Risk factors for young children’s peer victimization. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:335–353. doi: 10.1017/S0954579404044542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanish LD, Guerra NG. Predictors of peer victimization among urban youth. Social Development. 2000;9:521–543. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00141. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hanish LD, Guerra NG. A longitudinal analysis of patterns of adjustment following peer victimization. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14:69–89. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402001049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins DL, Pepler DJ, Craig WM. Naturalistic observations of peer interventions in bullying. Social Development. 2001;10:512–527. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00178. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hodges EVE, Boivin M, Vitaro F, Bukowski WM. The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:94–101. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hubbard JA, McAuliffe MD, Morrow MT, Romano LJ. Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: Precursors, outcomes, processes, experiences, and measurement. Journal of Personality. 2010;78:95–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00610.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawabata Y, Tseng WL, Crick NR. Adaptive, maladaptive, mediational, and bidirectional processes of relational and physical aggression, relational and physical victimization, and peer liking. Aggressive Behavior. 2014;40:273–287. doi: 10.1002/ab.21517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamarche V, Brendgen M, Boivin M, Vitaro F, Dionne G, Pérusse D. Do friends’ characteristics moderate the prospective links between peer victimization and reactive and proactive aggression? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2007;35:665–680. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9122-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamarche V, Brendgen M, Boivin M, Vitaro F, Pérusse D, Dionne G. Do friendships and sibling relationships provide protection against peer victimization in a similar way? Social Development. 2006;15:373–393. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00347.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Little RJA. Modeling the drop-out mechanism in longitudinal studies. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1995;90:1112–1121. [Google Scholar]
- Little TD. Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Little TD, Brauner J, Jones SM, Nock MK, Hawley PH. Rethinking aggression: A typological examination of the functions of aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2003;49:343–369. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2003.0014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Little TD, Jones SM, Henrich CC, Hawley PH. Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2003;27:122–133. doi: 10.1080/01650250244000128. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lochman JE, Dodge KA. Distorted perceptions in dyadic interactions of aggressive and nonaggressive boys: Effects of prior expectations, context, and boys’ age. Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10:495–512. doi: 10.1017/S0954579498001710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Sixth. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2011. [Google Scholar]
- Ostrov JM. Forms of aggression and peer victimization during early childhood: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:311–322. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9179-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostrov JM, Godleski SA. Toward an integrated gender-linked model of aggression subtypes in early and middle childhood. Psychological Review. 2010;117:233–242. doi: 10.1037/a0018070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostrov JM, Godleski SA. Relational aggression, victimization, and adjustment during middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:801–815. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostrov JM, Kamper KE, Hart EJ, Godleski SA, Blakely-McClure SJ. A gender-balanced approach to the study of peer victimization and aggression subtypes in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2014;26:575–587. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin F, Boivin M. Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two-factor model. Psychological Assessment. 2000a;12:115–122. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin F, Boivin M. The role of proactive and reactive aggression in the formation and development of boys’ friendships. Developmental Psychology. 2000b;36:233–240. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JM, Dodge KA. Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood: Relations to peer status and social context dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1989;17:455–471. doi: 10.1007/BF00915038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prinstein MJ, Cillessen AHN. Forms and functions of adolescent peer aggression associated with high levels of peer status. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2003;49:310–342. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2003.0015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Prinstein MJ, La Greca AM. Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of adolescent girls’ externalizing and health risk behaviors: A 6-year longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:103–112. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raine A, Dodge K, Loeber R, Gatzke-Kopp L, Lynam D, Reynolds C, Liu J. The reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32:159–171. doi: 10.1002/ab.20115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose AJ, Rudolph KD. A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:98–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmivalli C, Helteenvuori T. Reactive, but not proactive aggression predicts victimization among boys. Aggressive Behavior. 2007;33:198–206. doi: 10.1002/ab.20210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmivalli C, Karhunen J, Lagerspetz KMJ. How do the victims respond to bullying? Aggressive Behavior. 1996;22:99–109. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:2%3C99::AID-AB3%3E3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Salmivalli C, Lagerspetz K, Björkqvist K, Österman K, Kaukiainen A. Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggressive Behavior. 1996;22:1–15. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:1%3C1::AID-AB1%3E3.0.CO;2-T. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Salmivalli C, Nieminen E. Proactive and reactive aggression among school bullies, victims, and bully-victims. Aggressive Behavior. 2002;28:30–44. doi: 10.1002/ab.90004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz D, Dodge KA, Coie JD, Hubbard JA, Cillessen AHN, Lemerise EA, Bateman H. Social-cognitive and behavioral correlates of aggression and victimization in boys’ play groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998;26:431–440. doi: 10.1023/A:1022695601088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz D, McFadyen-Ketchum S, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Early behavior problems as a predictor of later peer group victimization: Moderators and mediators in the pathways of social risk. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27:191–201. doi: 10.1023/A:1021948206165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sentse M, Kretschmer T, Salmivalli C. The longitudinal interplay between bullying, victimization, and social status: Age-related and gender differences. Social Development. 2015;24:659–677. doi: 10.1111/sode.12115. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vitaro F, Brendgen M. Proactive and reactive aggression: A developmental perspective. In: Tremblay RE, Hartup WW, Archer J, editors. Developmental origins of aggression. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2005. pp. 178–201. [Google Scholar]
- Waschbusch DA, Willoughby MT, Pelham WE., Jr Criterion validity and the utility of reactive and proactive aggression: Comparisons to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and other measures of functioning. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1998;27:396–405. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]